You are on page 1of 18

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/259910733

Measurement of situation awareness for automobile technologies of the future

Chapter · January 2010

CITATIONS READS
14 3,024

5 authors, including:

Cheryl Bolstad Mica R. Endsley


Sandia National Laboratories SA Technologies
66 PUBLICATIONS   1,173 CITATIONS    216 PUBLICATIONS   28,317 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Linda Sala Angell


Touchstone Evaluations, Inc.
76 PUBLICATIONS   770 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Collaboration Tools for the Advanced Decision Architecture View project

Careers View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Cheryl Bolstad on 01 April 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


DVM- 11

Measurement of Situation Awareness for Automobile Technologies of the Future

Cheryl A. Bolstad, Haydee M. Cuevas, Jingjing Wang-Costello, and Mica R. Endsley


SA Technologies, Inc.

Linda S. Angell
Touchstone Evaluations, Inc.

Copyright © 2009 SAE International

ABSTRACT projection of their status in the near future” [2, p.36]. For
many years, SA has been primarily associated with
We present a theoretical model of situation awareness aviation and military performance. Yet, it is now
(SA) to highlight how driver SA may be influenced by considered to be a central organizing construct that
characteristics and limitations of both the human driver forms a key foundation for effective decision making and
(e.g., attention, memory) as well as the technologies that human performance in a wide variety of dynamic
have been increasingly introduced into private and systems and challenging environments [3].
commercial vehicles. Such technologies include devices
that are integrated with the vehicle, such as navigation One could argue that the driving domain is such a
and telematics systems, as well as those that are carried dynamic system consisting of human, technological, and
in by drivers, such as cell phones, PDAs, etc., and also environmental elements [4]. A driver must be aware of
include more advanced automation technologies, such road conditions, the presence of other cars or obstacles,
as adaptive cruise control, lane centering systems, and the rate of change of traffic in the area in order to
autonomous braking, and other systems. We then predict whether it is safe to change lanes or to pass
provide a brief overview of the different approaches for another car. Thus, important research questions that
measuring SA, citing the advantages and disadvantages must be addressed by safety researchers and
of each approach. These measurement techniques are automobile developers include, for example, determining
presented as guidelines for automotive researchers and if drivers are capable of perceiving and comprehending
others interested in assessing SA. all that is occurring around them, and if they are able to
guide their future actions based on their understanding
INTRODUCTION of the current situation. In addition to these primary tasks
of driving, secondary devices used in the vehicle can
Driving a car is a common activity that many of us take further complicate the driving task and have the potential
for granted. However, safe and effective driving to impact SA. These devices are of two broad types:
performance requires the ability to attend to important those that are integrated with the vehicle (e.g., built-in
information, process this information, and then react navigation or telematics systems) and those that are
accordingly. This complex and cognitively challenging carried into the vehicle by drivers (e.g., cell phones,
ability is encompassed within the construct of situation PDAs). The introduction of advanced automation
awareness (SA). SA can be broadly defined as the technologies (e.g., adaptive cruise control, lane
internal conceptualization of the current situation [1]. centering systems, autonomous braking systems) may
More formally, SA involves “…the perception of the hold still further implications for SA. Accordingly, a better
elements in the environment within a volume of time and understanding of how SA is influenced by both human
space, the comprehension of their meaning and the and technological characteristics and limitations can

The Engineering Meetings Board has approved this paper for publication. It has successfully completed SAE’s peer review process under the supervision of the
session organizer. This process requires a minimum of three (3) reviews by industry experts.
All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, electronic,
mechanical, photocopying, recording, or otherwise, without the prior written permission of SAE.
ISSN 0148-7191
Positions and opinions advanced in this paper are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of SAE. The author is solely responsible for the content of
the paper.
SAE Customer Service: Tel: 877-606-7323 (inside USA and Canada)
Tel: 724-776-4970 (outside USA)
Fax: 724-776-0790
Email: CustomerService@sae.org
SAE Web Address: http://www.sae.org
Printed in USA
guide the design of systems (e.g., cars, equipment, etc.) disregarding nonessential items. Comprehension (Level
and intervention techniques (e.g., driver safety 2 SA) involves integrating this information in working
programs, infrastructure changes) to improve driving memory to understand how the information will impact
performance. the individual’s goals and objectives [7]. This involves
combining separate pieces of information together to
Next, we present a theoretical model of SA to explain form a comprehensive picture of the world, or of that
how SA is formed and also discuss how technology may portion of the world of concern to the individual, such as
impact driver SA. We then provide a brief overview of the a change in car direction or sudden loss of speed.
different approaches for measuring SA, citing the
advantages and disadvantages of each approach. These Projection (Level 3 SA) involves extrapolating this
measurement techniques are presented as guidelines for information forward in time to determine how it will affect
automotive researchers and others interested in future states of the operating environment [8, 9]. Level 3
assessing SA. We conclude with directions for future SA combines what the individual knows about the
research. current situation with his/her mental model or schemata
of similar events to predict what might happen next. For
MODEL OF SITUATION AWARENESS instance, a driver approaching a red light might predict
that ignoring the light and proceeding into the
A considerable body of research supports the notion that intersection would result in a crash based on his/her
SA is formed through several steps or stages [2, 5, 6, 7]. prior driving experience. Thus, mental models and
As noted earlier, Endsley’s [2] definition of SA identifies schemata help ease the cognitive workload required to
three distinct levels: perception, comprehension and develop Levels 2 and 3 SA. These cognitive processes
projection. Perception (Level 1 SA) involves the sensory all form part of the larger human information-processing
detection of significant environmental cues. Perception is model in which SA is used to make decisions and is, in
an active process whereby individuals extract salient turn, reflected in subsequent actions (see Figure 1). As
cues from their environment. By selectively directing will be discussed next, applying this model to driving can
attention to the incoming stimuli, the individual attends to highlight the criticality of SA to this common albeit
important information, such as speed and location, while inherently complex task.
Figure 1: Theoretical model of situation awareness (adapted from [2]).
SITUATION AWARENESS IN DRIVING the changing spatial ‘picture’ over time), goal awareness
(driver’s intention of navigating to the destination, and
The construct of SA in dynamic systems applies readily the maintenance of speed and direction), and system
to the complex task of driving. For example, Ma and awareness (relevant information on the vehicle within the
Kaber [4] argued that driving tasks generally involve five driving environment, which may also be viewed as a
time-phased information processing functions, including system). In-vehicle system interaction knowledge has
perception, comprehension, and projection, as well as a also been shown to be important in the driving
decision on a course of action and implementing the environment. For example, when a car traveling at a
action. In their studies, they observed that all levels of constant speed under cruise control enters a higher
SA (perception, comprehension, and projection) appear speed limit area, driver awareness of their vehicle
to have an impact on operational driving behaviors in speed, the speed limit, and knowledge of how to set a
following tasks, and consequently, performance. Further, higher speed all represent good SA [11, 12].
both Ward [10] and Matthews, Bryant, Webb, and
Harbluk [11] related three general types of driving tasks, Other studies have also shown the criticality of SA in
including operational, tactical, and strategic tasks, to the driver performance. Indeed, poor SA has often been
three levels of SA. These research findings fit well with implicated in vehicle crashes [13]. For example, two of
the model of SA illustrated in Figure 1. Specific the leading causes of driving accidents, ‘improper
examples of how the three levels of SA can be applied to lookout’ and ‘inattention,’ might be thought of as failures
the driving task are shown in Figure 2. to maintain SA [14]. Often, drivers fail to ‘look ahead’
(and behind) and to keep track of traffic and
Offering a slightly different perspective, Matthews et al. environmental changes. Moreover, in-depth studies of
[11] outlined multiple elements of awareness defining SA crash causation have highlighted attentional and
in driving, including spatial awareness (appreciation of perceptual errors (two processes underlying Level 1 SA)
the location of all relevant features of the environment), as important direct causes of road accidents [15]. Next,
identity awareness (knowledge of salient items in the we discuss several examples of how technology can
driving environment), temporal awareness (knowledge of potentially impact driver SA.

Figure 2: Illustration of three levels of situation awareness in driving.


SITUATION AWARENESS AND AUTOMATION crashes from driver distraction when performing more in-
TECHNOLOGY vehicle secondary tasks [18]. As such, one might
hypothesize that the use of other technologies, such as
Although the increasing use of automation in cell phones, while driving may diminish the benefits
automobiles, such as adaptive cruise control and offered by ACC [4].
autonomous steering and braking systems, may offer
tremendous promise to improve driver SA and However, the results of another study on ACC warrant
performance, such technological aids are not without consideration in this context. The Automotive Collision
limitations and may even present potential unintended Avoidance System (ACAS) Field Operational Trial (FOT)
risks to safety. These technologies are designed for was the largest operational test within the USDOT
different purposes, depending on the specific system. In Intelligent Vehicle Initiative at the time it was conducted.
some cases, automation and advanced technology is It was carried out through a partnership between
applied to enhance driver-vehicle safety; in other USDOT, General Motors, and others over a 5-year
instances, systems are intended to improve convenience period. ACC was examined in the context of a Forward
and comfort for drivers. However, whatever their Collision Warning (FCW) technology in a small fleet of
purpose, such technologies may also generate instrumented vehicles equipped with these advanced
unintended negative effects on driver behavior, including systems. Participants in the study were given a vehicle
increasing the monitoring workload of the driver and from the fleet to drive as their own for a period of 3 to 4
distracting attention away from driving task performance. weeks. Several key findings were noted by Kiefer and
As evident in other complex domains, automation can Salinger [19] in 2006, at the conclusion of the ACAS
change the nature of the demands and responsibilities FOT. ACC reduced tailgating behavior, increased lane
on the operator, often in ways that were unintended or dwelling, and led to no unintended safety consequences.
unanticipated by the designer [16]. For example, Kiefer and Salinger [19] reported that the ACC system
automation can hinder SA by creating an out-of-the-loop was found to be broadly used, readily learned, and
experience for operators [3]. In such situations, attentively supervised by its drivers (including over
operators will have difficulty intervening if they need to relatively lengthy periods of sustained engagement and
take over or override the automation [17]. However, not vehicle-following). Responses to events (e.g., cut-ins)
all automation is bad; in fact, automation, when well were characterized by short reaction times and modest
designed, can help reduce the complexities of the driving manual decelerations). No pattern of dependence on the
task. It is in recognition of these issues that so much braking portion of the system could be discerned. FCW
research has been initiated and is underway on these also reduced tailgating behavior, produced valuable
automotive technologies – namely, to identify and alerts, led to no unintended safety consequences, but
accurately quantify the effects of these systems. also produced no broad effect on “closing conflicts.” With
respect to engagement in secondary tasks, there was no
One notable example of automation technology statistically significant difference in behaviors between
integrated into vehicles is adaptive cruise control (ACC). manual and ACC-assisted driving; secondary tasks were
ACC automates one part of the driving task (namely, engaged in about 20% of driving time for both manual
controlling headway to a lead vehicle) by adjusting and ACC-assisted conditions. Data for comparison with
vehicle speed based upon algorithms that typically use a use of conventional cruise control were sparse. Similarly,
defined minimum following distance and information under conditions of FCW only, frequency and pattern of
obtained from monitoring traffic – and which may lead to secondary task engagement was largely the same, even
the instigation of braking or acceleration when the across weeks of use (with a slight increase in
detected headway is smaller or larger than the set conversation with passengers in week 2 only).
criteria [4]. ACC has been shown to improve driving
safety by reducing instances of unsafe headway This set of studies demonstrates both the importance
distance in following tasks, thereby reducing the and the complexity of examining SA effects that may
frequency of ‘tailgating’ and the severity of rear-end arise from the introduction of advanced technologies and
collisions [10]. Setting ACC to a long headway distance automation. It illustrates the potential value of having
(e.g., 2.4s) has also been shown to significantly lower metrics for each of the levels of SA, in building an
driving workload, as compared to driving without ACC. understanding of how a system affects the driver-vehicle
However, the use of ACC in one study was shown to system. And these studies also illustrate that knowledge
present potential disadvantages, notably in terms of the builds across studies, and requires many, many studies
secondary effects of reduced attention on driver SA. – at least some of them involving naturalistic driving over
Ward [10] observed that the use of ACC led to poorer an extended period –before a full picture begins to
attention to lane positioning and failure to yield to traffic emerge, since there is variability between drivers,
as well as slower response times to unexpected events. between system implementations, and between driving
Another study also showed that drivers may direct their conditions and maneuvers.
attention away from driving while using ACC and that the
use of ACC may lead to unexpected increases in
SITUATION AWARENESS AND IN-VEHICLE instructions, paper map, and paper text instructions. One
SECONDARY SYSTEMS of the most important findings to have emerged was that
turn-by-turn guidance information enhanced
In-vehicle secondary systems such as navigation aids performance, usability, and safety. Among the electronic
based on Global Positioning Systems (GPS) potentially systems tested, the turn-by-turn display with voiced
improve driver performance by supporting navigation guidance instructions led to the best overall
tasks during driving, particularly in unfamiliar performance. The paper map proved least usable.
environments. In models of driving, navigation and route-
following are often regarded as part-and-parcel of the The performance measures taken were extensive,
driving task – at the strategic and tactical levels (e.g., setting a high standard of excellence for on-road studies.
[20]). In such models, the strategic level would include They included measures of lateral control, longitudinal
activities related to determining trip goals and planning a control, visual scanning, and navigation performance
route to a destination, whereas the tactical level of (from time to plan a trip, drive a trip, total trip time,
driving allows for drivers to maneuver through existing number of stops, time stopped, and number of errors).
circumstances on the road, and the operational level With the current availability of SA measures, it would be
provides for moment-to-moment operation of the vehicle. interesting in future studies of navigation and route
guidance to also gather data on Level 2 and 3 SA, where
However, when a navigation aid is available to support certain system features may manifest themselves as
the driver in route selection and route following (whether especially beneficial – and others less so.
it is a paper map, or an electronic display embedded in
the vehicle, or a hand-held unit carried into the vehicle), SITUATION AWARENESS AND CARRIED-IN
it can represent a secondary task (depending on its PORTABLE DEVICES
implementation) that must be time-shared with the
primary task of controlling the vehicle. As such, some One portable device that is commonly carried into the
implementations of navigation aids have the potential to vehicle by drivers and used while on the road is the cell
decrease driver attention to the roadway and SA, phone. It comes in many types – from the simple
particularly Level 1 SA, which, in turn, may lead to handheld, to handheld with hands-free features,
performance problems [21]. For example, the use of including headsets, to completely hands-free (fully
paper maps by drivers has been studied, and difficulties embedded hands-free is not typically portable or carried-
of doing so documented in comparison to alternatives in, but installed in-vehicle).
(e.g., [22]). Navigation aids may facilitate achieving a
The controversy regarding the effects of cell phone tasks
high level of SA for navigation tasks but may
on driving – both in the scientific literature and in the
inadvertently undermine attention to operational or
public policy arena – largely focuses on issues centering
tactical driving behaviors (e.g., braking, passing, etc.) at
on the extent to which phoning tasks affect SA while
certain points in the driving task [21]. Increased
driving. The evidence in the literature contains conflicting
demands on visual attention allocation to a navigation
evidence on this point – which is not surprising in light of
aid that has chosen to display a visually-intensive
the variation in devices and the variation in driving
moving map screen may negatively impact drivers’
conditions that have been examined. Further, while a
ability to achieve SA of the external driving environment
variety of methods have been employed to examine this
– yet a different implementation (such as voiced route
question, it does not appear that formal SA
guidance instructions) may not. In addition, the reliability
measurement techniques (of the type described later)
and accuracy of navigation systems in providing
have yet been applied.
information support to the driver may differentially
influence driver SA and possibly driver performance in A brief review of the literature reveals that an early
terms of strategic behaviors. More specifically, whereas epidemiological and statistical analyses of crash data
higher reliability leads to higher driver SA, systematic estimated that drivers with cell phones were four times
decreases in navigation aid reliability has been shown to as likely to be involved in a crash -- and made the claim
decrease SA by diverting driver attention from the that hands-free telephones provided no safety benefit
roadway [21]. (though the variety of hands-free phones studied did not
include any fully-embedded systems) [23, 24].
One of the most notable programs of research on Subsequent studies [25, 26, 27] addressed some of the
navigation aids was the Travtek program [22]. In one of methodological issues of that first early epidemiological
its studies, an instrumented car was used to obtain a study, and have produced findings that contrast with the
comprehensive set of measures about driving early application of the case-crossover method. For
performance and behavior. Six types of navigation/route- example, Young and Schreiner [26] examined personal
guidance implementations were examined: (visual) map conversations which were carried out using a hands-free
display alone, (visual) map display with voiced guidance embedded device (which also allowed precise time
instructions, (visual) symbolic guidance display alone, relationships to be established) and found the risk of an
(visual) symbolic guidance display with voiced guidance airbag crash was somewhere in a range from a
moderately lower risk to a risk near that of driving conversation tasks had in studies done on the road –
without a recent personal conversation. These results they correspond well – and it furthermore becomes
are not consistent with the large increase in crash risk apparent that miss rates and reaction times for
reported in using the case-crossover method. Hahn and conversation of the type studied in these experiments
Prieger [27] used data on number of crashes together leads to much smaller effects on event detection miss
with data from a national Internet survey on cell phone rates and reaction times than do visual-manual tasks
use to develop a regression model estimating the effects (which produced miss rates in the range of 24-40% and
of cell phone use on crash risk. Their analysis indicated slowed reaction times by ~200 ms or more relative to
that the effect of cell phone use on crashes varied “just driving” for events occurring in the driver’s forward
across segments of the population. When corrections for view) [33].
self-selection bias and endogeneity were made (notions
related to the idea that drivers who use the cell phone Thus, given the landscape of findings on phoning and
while driving may also be inherently less safe drivers conversation tasks, it is interesting to consider whether
even when not on the phone), there was no significant research which would apply measures developed
effect on risk for hand-held or hands-free phone use. specifically to examine SA would bring new insights to
this body of work (and to the larger body of work on
This issue has also been studied from the perspective of
device use beyond cell phones). Such a research
driving performance (e.g., in simulators, on test tracks,
enterprise may well illuminate new dimensions of these
and on-the-road). Several studies [28, 29] have indicated
issues, and extend the state of knowledge.
that drivers exhibit slower responses to traffic light
changes in the presence of an experimentally-produced
SUMMARY - The preceding discussion, although brief,
in-vehicle distracter (artificial memory tasks performed
highlights how automation technologies and other
using a touchpad that looked like a telephone keypad),
devices used in the vehicle (whether integrated with the
and have shown that tasks with a visual-manual
vehicle or carried into the vehicle) may sometimes
component can have effects at crucial points in a driving
negatively impact driver SA through the introduction of
maneuver using basic automatic transmission cars. In
‘SA Demons’ (i.e., factors that work to undermine SA).
another study, it was shown that in comparison to
These ‘SA Demons’ affecting driving performance
conversing with a passenger, cell phone conversations
include increased cognitive workload, attention
may differ in terms of effects on SA and performance
tunneling, misplaced salience, and data overload,
since the caller cannot visualize the driving situation to
among others (for a detailed discussion on ‘SA Demons,’
assist the driver, and the driver may need to use one
see [34]). Part of the reason for identifying these is to
hand to hold the telephone [4].
illustrate that the negative effects of these problems can
be minimized through careful system design and
While this research on driving performance suggests the
training. For example, to prevent technology-induced
possibility of decrements in SA at multiple levels for
performance problems [35] in lower level (operational
some visual-manual devices used during driving, it has
and tactical) driving behaviors, the reliability and manner
also been hypothesized that conversations alone may
of delivery of information is critical in the design of in-
compromise drivers’ comprehension of stimuli perceived
vehicle navigation systems. This has already been
from the driving environment relative to the goal of
illustrated through the discussion of the Travtek findings
maintaining vehicle safety, specifically sudden stopping
– which served to demonstrate that systems can be
situations. The notion underlying these hypotheses is
designed such that information is presented reliably,
that cell phone conversations when driving may compete
clearly, and meaningfully to drivers without distracting
for limited driver cognitive resources, possibly leading to
their attention away from the main driving task (operating
SA decrements and decreases in driving performance.
the vehicle) or increasing the cognitive workload
associated with understanding the information provided
Interestingly, again the empirical record provides
by the system. In addition, driver safety programs can be
conflicting data. In one study, speech-based interactions
developed to increase awareness of the dangers and
with in-vehicle computers were observed to have poorly
risks associated with device usage during driving. As will
designed interfaces that may distract drivers and
be discussed next, to evaluate the effectiveness of
increase reaction time [30], effects similar to some cell
proposed technology design concepts and training
phone interfaces. Other studies, which have used both
interventions, researchers must be able to validly and
behavioral and brain imaging measures to examine the
reliably measure SA.
effects of conversation on reaction time to events in a
driving-like environment reveal that conversations have
a negligible effect on rates of missed detections (which
MEASURING SITUATION AWARENESS
were not distinguishable from “just driving/no-
Although we can build vehicles and equipment to
conversation” epochs) and resulted in reliable but small
support SA and even develop intervention strategies
increases in reaction times to events (<~100 ms) [31,
32]. When these differences for conversation in the such as training programs to improve SA, understanding
laboratory are compared with the effects that similar what factors influence driver SA and how and where SA
impacts driving performance is a critical requirement for into perception and comprehension. In the context of
research and design. As one example of how important assessing driver SA, eye tracking devices can be used
this is, when new, back-up cameras and related backing to monitor where the driver is directing his/her attention.
crash countermeasure systems were introduced, More specifically, is the saliency of important cues
extensive research was undertaken to study how drivers sufficient or are nonessential cues drawing away the
behaved during backing. These studies examined a driver’s attention? Is the driver’s gaze focused more on
range of SA-related topics, from where drivers looked the external environment or on the interface of an in-
during backing, to what they attended to and noticed, to vehicle device (e.g., advanced information display) or on
what they were aware of, and how they responded to the rear view mirror (a display to support SA)? Analyzing
unexpected obstacles placed out-of-view in the backing communications can also serve as process indices of
path. See [36] and [37] as two examples of the kind of driver SA. For example, verbalizations made by drivers
studies that are important in the early development of during a navigation task can be analyzed to determine
new technologies, particularly those that may affect SA. how well information is being acquired from an in-vehicle
Further, it is important to recognize that a range of navigation system.
fundamental studies are required to empirically
determine the impact of any particular countermeasure Process indices are advantageous in that these offer
system on driver SA in a driving task, as well as to objective assessment of driver SA and provide an
identify any unintended consequences. indication of information access and utilization. However,
process indices create large amounts of data to analyze
Therefore, in this section, the focus is not on whether to and are difficult to implement in the real world
measure SA – but how. Applying the model of SA environment (e.g., eye tracking devices, if head-
discussed earlier, researchers can determine if a new mounted, can be cumbersome and intrusive). Further,
device or modification to an existing one is helping or process indices do not indicate what is actually done
hindering the driver’s ability to perceive critical with the information acquired (processing) nor whether
information (Level 1 SA), comprehend the relevance of the information is registered correctly or what is retained
this information to the driving task (Level 2), and use this in memory. Instead, these measures simply indicate that
information to predict what will happen next (Level 3 SA) the driver looked at the information. Given these
as well as to evaluate how these effects on driver SA limitations, process indices are more suitable for
influence decision making and, ultimately, driving safety investigating specific research questions of information
and performance. Thus, measurement of SA provides a acquisition and for examining the processes underlying
degree of diagnosticity that is useful in training and SA (e.g., perception, attention) rather than the final
system design and evaluation. SA measurement can be product.
used to identify the source of the problem as well as
establish a baseline for comparison of the effects of Studies using process indices have yielded mixed
different training or design solutions. results, in part, because it is so difficult to tease out what
is SA and what are indicators of perception, information
In general, methodologies vary in terms of direct processing, and workload. In one of the earliest studies
measurement of SA (e.g., objective real-time probes or used to examine SA and process indices, Poe,
subjective questionnaires assessing perceived SA) or Suyenobu, Bolstad, Endsley, and Sterman [40] did not
indirect methods (e.g., process indices) that infer SA find any correlation between EEG activity and SA as
based on operator behavior or performance. Direct measured by the SAGAT methodology (described next).
measures are typically considered to be ‘product- Results indicated that individual differences played a
oriented’ in that these techniques assess an SA large role in correlating between EEG metrics and
outcome; indirect measures are considered to be workload and performance. However, this study used
‘process-oriented,’ focusing on the underlying processes technology that is now considered outdated and newer
or mechanisms required to achieve SA [38]. The measurement systems may yield different results.
selection of measurement choice depends upon what Results of a study of combat pilots conducted by
the researcher or developer wants to know about SA and Vidulich, Stratton, Crabtree, and Wilson [41] revealed
what facilities or setup is available to implement the that EEG and eye blinks were related to the
measures. Examples of each of these SA measurement maintenance of SA. Essentially, the metrics were able to
approaches will be further described next. detect when pilots had to work harder (i.e., experienced
higher workload) to maintain their SA.
PROCESS INDICES - Simply stated, process indices,
such as psycho-physiological measures, examine how STATE-OF-KNOWLEDGE METRICS – State-of-
individuals process information in their environment [39]. knowledge metrics include both objective and subjective
Such measures include PET, MRI, EEG, eye blinks, eye direct measures of SA. Objective measures directly
tracking and cardiac activity, just to name a few. assess SA by comparing an individual’s perceptions of
Tracking eye movements is one of the more common the situation or environment to some ‘ground truth’
psycho-physiological approaches for providing insight reality. Specifically, objective measures can be used to
collect data from drivers’ perceptions of the situation and ordered questions about their current perceptions of the
compare this to what is actually happening at a given situation. These responses are then compared to
moment in time. Thus, this type of assessment provides ‘ground truth’ (i.e., actual data on the real situation) to
a direct measure of SA and does not require drivers or assess the accuracy of the individuals’ SA. SAGAT
experimenters to make judgments about situational offers several advantages including: requires no post
knowledge on the basis of incomplete information. data collection, provides a direct objective measure of
Objective measures can be gathered in one of three SA minimizing potential biasing of operator SA, utilizes
ways: during an interruption in task performance (e.g., random sampling of queries to provide an unbiased
SAGAT), real time as the task is completed (e.g., measure of SA, and has been extensively validated in
probes), or post-test following completion of the task. numerous complex domains [42]. SAGAT, however,
does have its limitations. The temporary halt in the
SAGAT Queries - One common approach to directly and exercise may be perceived as disruptive to task
objectively measure SA is the Situation Awareness performance and may also potentially increase cognitive
Global Assessment Technique (SAGAT) [42]. SAGAT workload. In addition, because it involves ‘freezing’ task
utilizes a concurrent memory probe technique that performance, SAGAT is better suited for assessing SA in
presents queries related to the current task environment. simulation exercises and may not be practical for real-
The SAGAT queries are derived from a goal directed time measurement of SA. Finally, while the SAGAT
task analysis (GDTA) of the domain being trained or methodology is relatively straightforward, the greatest
evaluated during the exercise. A GDTA is a unique form challenge is creating the appropriate queries. Generation
of cognitive task analysis that involves conducting of the queries requires conducting a detailed task
extensive knowledge elicitation sessions with domain analysis to identify domain-relevant SA information
subject matter experts (for a detailed description of this requirements. Care should be taken to ensure the
methodology; see [34]). The objective of the GDTA is to queries address all levels of SA and that these can be
identify the major goals, subgoals, and decisions that objectively scored.
drive performance in a particular job or position as well
as to delineate the critical, dynamic information Bolstad [44] used the SAGAT methodology to assess
requirements associated with each goal and decision. To age-related changes associated with driving and how
illustrate this methodology, consider that an important this affects SA formation. She found that SA did decline
subgoal in driving is to “Avoid road hazards.” A key with age, but like other performance metrics, the
decision for this subgoal is “What type of hazard is variability increased significantly. Additionally, the
present?” The associated SA information requirements SAGAT methodology provides a degree of diagnosticity
for this decision are listed in Table 1. Note that this not found with the other measures. For example, Bolstad
GDTA example only illustrates one subgoal for the and Endsley [45] were asked to assess the ability of a
driving task. The actual GDTA for this domain is much new military force structure to support data fusion and
more comprehensive and detailed, involving multiple analysis. Using the model of SA cited earlier, Levels 2
subgoals, decisions, and associated SA information and 3 SA best represent this ability. The SAGAT
requirements (see [43]). methodology was used to measure SA of Army officers
while performing a command and control task. Results
Administration of the SAGAT involves freezing a showed that SA scores were higher for Levels 2 and 3
simulation exercise at randomly selected times and SA than for Level 1 SA, thus, supporting the study’s
hiding task information sources (e.g., blanking visual hypothesis.
displays) while individuals quickly answer randomly

Table 1: Example of GDTA for the Driving Task Listing SA Information Requirements for the Subgoal “Avoid Road
Hazards” (adapted from [43]).

Subgoal: Avoid Road Hazards


Decision: What type of hazard is present?
SA Information Requirements: Assess the type of hazards
• Pedestrians • Road conditions
• Crosswalks • Detours
• Signs present • Signs present
• School zones • Car direction of travel
• Signs • Curves
• Presence of children • Signs present
• Construction zones • Car direction of travel
• Signs present • Speed limit changes
• Speed limit changes • Unsafe roads
• Road lane changes (2 to 1) • Road condition
• Presence of road equipment • Signs present
• Behavior of other drivers

Real-time probes - Real-time probes (open questions occurred, on average, once every 2 minutes and did not
embedded as verbal communications during the task) appear to interfere with performance, suggesting that
can be utilized to naturally and unobtrusively measure probes could be administered even more often without
driver SA during task performance [46]. Real-time posing problems.
probes are similar to SAGAT in that these can be used
to query drivers on their knowledge of key task-relevant Post-test questionnaires and post-test analysis - As the
information in the environment; however, this approach name implies, post-test questionnaires are administered
offers an important advantage over the SAGAT in that following task completion, and, as such, do not interfere
task performance is not disrupted (i.e., the simulation or with driving performance and, thus, are less intrusive. In
task is not stopped) but rather the queries are addition, because the driver has completed the task and
incorporated as a natural part of the task. Thus, real-time time is less of an issue, researchers can administer a
probes are more ideally suited for assessing SA in the more detailed yet still objective assessment of SA. This
real world environment. In addition, driver SA can be methodology is relatively easy to implement in either
evaluated both in terms of response times as well as simulated or real world environments. A major drawback
accuracy. As with the SAGAT, however, real-time probes to this approach is the risk of rationalization or
do have some limitations. The probes must be created generalization after the fact, that is, responses are based
using a cognitive task analysis in order to specifically on the driver’s memory of what occurred in the past
assess SA requirements. Further, if not seamlessly rather then his/her current understanding of the situation
integrated within the task, probes may interfere with at that given moment in time. Accordingly, post-test
driver performance. Because of their specificity and link questionnaires are limited to providing reliable
to the current situation, probes also limit the amount of assessment of SA only with regard to the end of the task
information that can be collected. Thus, administration of or event. Still, this methodology can be useful for
real-time probes requires careful synchronization with determining the overall success of a new technology.
tasks or events and should only be presented one
question at a time. Finally, multiple measures are Subjective measures (described next) are commonly
needed to ensure reliability. used to assess SA on a post-test basis. Additionally, it is
not uncommon to utilize SAGAT at the end of a scenario
In an investigation of the sensitivity and validity of real- or test. However, SA in our work has been shown to vary
time probes, Jones and Endsley [46] compared the use throughout an experiment and assessing it only at the
of questions directly posed to operators of an air defense end does not provide the researcher with a clear picture
system during simulated peace and wartime operations of what is actually occurring. Typically, we find that SA
with the SAGAT. The probes used in this study were improves over time in our longer experimental studies
based on an SA requirements analysis for the air (e.g., [47]).
defense system and were verbally administered one-at-a
time during task performance. Results showed the SA Post-test analysis can be performed on existing sets of
probes to be sensitive to workload manipulations in data to determine their causal factors. For example in
terms of overall response time and accuracy of the driving domain, detailed databases on crash data
responses. (Response time was measured because all can be examined to determine which SA errors led to the
information displays remained visible to participants specified incidents. The greatest drawback to this
during probes.) With respect to the validity of the real- approach is the fact that the analysis is being performed
time probes for assessing SA, results indicated in retrospect. Thus, the analysis can only be performed
significant, but weak, correlations of response accuracy at a very broad level and the actual series of events that
with the accuracy of responses to the SAGAT queries, led up the crash may never be known.
which were also found to be a sensitive measure of SA.
Additionally, real-time probes showed differences A number of studies have been performed that employ
between scenarios, but not by query while SAGAT this methodology. For example, an analysis of SA errors
showed differences at the individual query level, thereby in aviation was conducted by Jones and Endsley [48]
providing diagnostic detail as to the types of SA using reports from the NASA Aviation Safety Reporting
differences that existed between the two scenarios. System (ASRS). They used an SA error taxonomy based
Jones and Endsley recommended that at least 6 to 8 on Endsley’s [2] model of SA. The aviation errors cited in
repeats of each probe type be used in order to obtain this database were primarily due to Level 1 SA problems
sufficient reliability and sensitivity in assessing (76.3%) including pilot’s failure to monitor or observe the
participant cognitive states. In their experiment, a probe needed data. Level 2 errors occurred only 20.3% of the
time. Rodgers, Mogford and Strauch [49] also performed scale, with different anchors for each item. The utility of
a post-hoc assessment of SA with regard to Air Traffic MARS as a subjective measure of SA was evaluated
Controller incidents. They initially determined that loss of during an experiment designed to assess four different
awareness was the primary reason for the events, but approaches to simulating night vision goggles in a virtual
further analyses using Endsley’s model of SA allowed for environment [51]. Sixteen enlisted soldiers, working in
a more detailed description. Three causal factors were four-man teams, completed four MOUT (Military
then used to reassess the loss of awareness incidents: Operations in Urban Terrain) missions in a virtual
failure to detect displayed information, failure to environment. After each mission, participants completed
comprehend displayed information, and failure to project a MARS questionnaire. The results indicated that MARS
the future status of displayed information. significantly and robustly differentiated among the four
means of simulating night vision goggles, and that the
Subjective measures - Subjective measures directly MARS data were consistent with other objective and
assess SA by asking individuals (or experienced subjective indexes designed to assess the four different
observers) to rate their SA on an anchored scale (for a approaches to night simulations.
detailed review, see [50]). These ratings can be
collected during task performance or following task PERFORMANCE AND BEHAVIORAL-BASED
completion. Subjective measures of SA are attractive in MEASURES - Performance measures ‘infer’ SA from the
that these are relatively straightforward, inexpensive, end result (i.e., task performance outcomes), based on
and easy to administer. However, several important the assumption that better performance indicates better
limitations should be noted. Individuals making SA. Common performance metrics include quantity of
subjective assessments of their own SA are often output or productivity level, time to perform the task or
unaware of information they do not know. Further, self- respond to an event, and the accuracy of the response
ratings may be tainted by performance outcomes. or, conversely, the number of errors committed. The
Subjective measures also tend to be global in nature, main advantage of performance measures is that these
and, as such, do not fully exploit the multivariate nature can be collected objectively and without disrupting task
of SA to provide the detailed diagnostics available with performance. However, although evidence exists to
objective measures. Nevertheless, self-ratings may be suggest a positive relation between SA and
useful in that these can provide an assessment of performance, this connection is probabilistic and not
drivers’ degree of confidence in their SA. always direct and unequivocal [2]. In other words, good
SA does not always lead to good performance and poor
Subjective estimates of an individual’s SA may also be SA does not always lead to poor performance [52].
made by experienced observers (e.g., supervisors or
trained external experts). These observer ratings may be Behavioral measures also ‘infer’ SA from the actions that
somewhat superior to self-ratings of SA because more individuals choose to take, based on the assumption that
information about the true state of the environment is good actions will follow from good SA and vice-versa.
usually available to the observer than to the driver, who Behavioral measures rely primarily on observer ratings,
may be focused on performing the driving task (i.e., and, thus, are somewhat subjective in nature. To
trained observers may have more complete knowledge address this limitation, observers can be asked to
of the situation). However, observers have only limited evaluate the degree to which individuals are carrying out
knowledge about the driver’s concept of the situation and actions and exhibiting behaviors that would be expected
cannot have complete insight into the mental state of the to promote the achievement of higher levels of SA. This
individual being evaluated. Thus, observers are forced to approach removes some of the subjectivity associated
rely more on drivers’ observable actions and with making judgments about an individual’s internal
verbalizations in order to infer their level of SA. Given the state of knowledge by allowing them to make judgments
aforementioned limitations, subjective measures, about SA indicators that are more readily observable.
whether self or observer ratings, should not be used in Within the context of driving, observers can be asked to
isolation, but rather to complement objective techniques. evaluate observable behaviors such as, for example,
whether or not the driver brakes in time to avoid a
An example of a domain-specific subjective measure of collision, notices a ball rolling across the street,
SA is the Mission Awareness Rating Survey (MARS), maintains a consistent speed or following distance, or
which consists of two 4-item subscales [51]. The SA responds to an inaccurate display setting.
Content subscale requires participants to rate how well
they can identify, comprehend, and predict (the three A significant limitation to the utility of performance and
levels of SA as defined by Endsley [2]) as well as decide behavioral-based measures is that these methodologies
in the given mission. The SA Workload subscale ‘infer’ rather than directly assess SA and thus, lack
requires participants to rate their difficulty, in terms of sensitivity and diagnosticity. This also makes the
how much mental effort is required, to identify, interpretability of the data collected from these measures
comprehend, predict, and decide in the given mission. problematic and subjective. Thus, performance and
Responses are recorded using a four-point anchored behavioral-based measures should be used in
conjunction with others measures of SA that directly Although evidence exists to support the utility of
assess this construct. It is also recommended that these communication analysis for predicting team SA [57], time
measures cover a wide range of tasks or scenarios to constraints and technological limitations (e.g., cost and
increase generalizability. availability of speech recording systems and speech-to-
text translation software) may make this approach less
The Situation Awareness Behaviorally Anchored Rating practical and viable in time-pressured, fast paced
Scale (SABARS) is a behavioral measurement approach operations. In addition, the models generated using this
consisting of 27 items, derived from an SA requirements approach are domain and task specific; thus, unique
analysis, that identify key observable behaviors related models must be created for each environment or
to developing SA in a MOUT environment, such as application. Lastly, this measure is only effective for
communicating clearly with higher echelons, measuring SA in a team environment and would not be
subordinates, and adjacent units [53]. One additional suitable for situations in which a single operator is being
item concerns the platoon leader’s overall SA. Expert evaluated.
trained observers rate participants on the SA-related
behaviors assessed by the 28 items using a 5-point SUMMARY - Improving driver performance requires
scale, with performance on specified behaviors rated as carefully evaluating proposed design options for the
very poor, poor, borderline, good, or very good (a “not displays and interfaces of in-vehicle devices and
applicable” option is also available). Strater et al. [53] automation technologies. The measurement of driver SA
conducted a comparison of SA measures to determine if provides critical information with greater sensitivity than
the SABARS was predictive of the level of SA measured simple performance measures. However, the choice of
using the SAGAT. Observations on individual SAGAT which measure(s) to use is important. Considerations
queries were used as the response measure in a must be given to each measurement approach in terms
regression model in order to establish whether any of the of the following criteria:
SABARS items were significantly related to SAGAT
performance. Results showed that some of the SABARS • Validity - Is it measuring what it is intended to
factors accounted for significant portions of variance in measure?
some SAGAT questions. Results of the analysis also • Reliability - Does the measure remain consistent?
showed the SABARS to be sensitive to the experience With what else does it correlate?
levels of soldiers. • Sensitivity - Will the measure detect differences in
SA? What is a good SA score?
MODELING SA - Rather than simply measure SA, the • Operational Constraints – What are the task’s
goal of SA modeling approaches is to objectively predict dynamic priorities? Where will measurement be
SA based on readily observable verbal and non-verbal located? What is the cost to implement? How much
communications. Specifically, team communications time is required to analyze the data?
(particularly verbal communications) support the
knowledge building and information processing that In general, objective measures of SA are the most
leads to SA construction [54]. Thus, since SA may be sensitive and diagnostic. Subjective measures of SA are
distributed via communication, computational linguistics more related to confidence in one’s SA abilities and
and machine learning techniques can be combined with performance measures require SA to be inferred, rather
natural language analytical techniques (e.g., Latent than actually providing direct measures of SA.
Semantic Analysis) to create models that draw on the
verbal expressions of the team to predict SA and task In addition, the choice of which measurement approach
performance [55, 56]. For example, the Automated to utilize greatly depends on the research question that
Communication and Situation Awareness (ACASA) tool is being addressed. Process indices (e.g., eye tracking)
offers real-time, non-intrusive, quantitative assessment can be used to examine the ability of drivers to look at
of SA by analyzing communication exchanges among the information needed for SA development. Direct,
team members [57]. Since the communication data is objective state-of-knowledge metrics (e.g., SAGAT, real-
collected using either automatic speech recognition time probes) are useful for assessing continuous levels
(ASR) software or transcriptions of speech recordings, of SA during driving; for example, SAGAT has been
this methodology does not interrupt activities or affect used successfully to examine age-related differences in
task performance. Thus, SA modeling approaches, such driving performance [44]. Alternately, direct, subjective
as the ACASA tool, are appropriate for use in both measures (e.g., MARS) are more suitable for soliciting
simulations and real world environments. Further, this self-reports of drivers’ confidence in their SA abilities.
methodology can provide diagnostic information Performance and behavioral-based measures may be
regarding current SA. For example, when coupled with preferred when the focus is on drivers’ observable
ASR software, the ACASA tool can be used to quickly behaviors or a desired outcome. Finally, SA modeling
identify if immediate action needs to be taken to address approaches are especially useful for assessing the SA
poor SA among team members. among two or more team members.
CONCLUSION performance on a secondary task declines due to high
workload, the assumption is that SA declines as well.
The measurement of situation awareness has great While this is a valid hypothesis, it may not always be
potential for use by scientists and automotive system true. While it is possible that high workload will be
engineers to assess and design systems for drivers and associated with low SA, it also possible to have high
their passengers. Understanding how SA changes with workload and still have high SA. Indeed, several studies
the addition of a new system or how SA varies among that compared SA and workload metrics revealed a
different designs and how this is linked to performance disassociation between these two attributes [59, 60].
provides the researcher with a very valuable tool. The Endsley [59] states that the relationship between SA and
diagnosticity of SA measurement is of particular workload are “flip sides of the same coin.” Essentially,
importance. Knowing that drivers feel they have better SA is a precursor to performance and workload is
SA with a new device is just as important as measuring expended to achieve and maintain SA, but the
a real improvement in task performance. This is relationship is not always clear. In general, one would
especially critical in today’s market as science and expect that a reduction in SA would be associated with a
technology are changing at an extraordinarily fast pace. reduction in driving performance — a direct
In particular, the use of in-vehicle devices (both correspondence. This is not strictly so, however. The
integrated with the vehicle and carried-in) and advanced loss of SA merely puts the driver at an increased risk of
automation technologies is expanding. These devices a performance error. Prior studies have shown that good
represent a critical mediating factor in linkages of SA to SA is necessary, but not sufficient for good performance
specific driving behaviors and performance, with the [52, 61]. Thus, metrics that address workload and
potential for both positive and negative effects [4]. Future performance should be coupled with valid SA metrics to
automotive environments pose interesting and unique determine how best to support SA while lowering the
challenges for SA as drivers are tempted with many new workload of the driver.
technologies that will vie for their attention and can
degrade awareness of road conditions and other drivers. Finally, one important issue that still remains is to
Conversely, these same technologies offer potential determine what actually constitutes good SA. The
improvements in SA over the present day by making measurement of SA provides a powerful tool to
some tasks easier or by augmenting human sensory researchers and vehicle system designers. However,
capacity. Accordingly, the development of successful what is considered a good level of SA for the driving task
and appropriate new technologies for the automobile is has not yet been established. For example, if for a
dependent on consideration of factors that impact driver sample of drivers, SA and performance is being
SA as well as careful measurement of the both the measured while the drivers each are using a vehicle
positive and negative effects of these factors. equipped with a new device and the drivers have fairly
high performance scores, high subjective SA scores, but
Furthermore, researchers must consider if these new only moderate objective SA (SAGAT) scores (~50%), is
technologies improve performance across the entire age the new device considered a success? Since SAGAT is
range of product users – the drivers. Unlike military highly dependent upon the queries being asked, it may
applications, in which the population is relatively small be that the queries selected are too difficult for the
and homogeneous, automobile drivers and passengers drivers to answer using the available systems.
are quite varied and the age range can be very broad. Additionally, since subjective SA is high and
Research has shown that older adults have less SA than performance is high, objective SA may not be
younger adults [44], and that older adults usually perform considered as essential. Thus, researchers must
less well with new technology [58]. Thus, the integration consider the hypotheses for their experiments and adopt
of new technology into the automobile should take this the criterion that best fits their research questions. In
research issue into consideration and measure SA and sum, there is no clear right or wrong approach to
performance to ensure that a system which improves SA measuring SA as, ultimately, the most appropriate level
for younger drivers does not impede SA for older drivers. of SA is context-specific, varying from person to person
and situation to situation.
Researchers also need to be cognizant that many
metrics of performance and workload, which are thought Nonetheless, in looking at the literature on the use of
to measure SA, are in fact measuring something surrogate methods to assess the effects of new devices
different. This is not to say that these are not useful and tasks on driver performance and behavior, it would
metrics, but rather they are assessing an inter-related appear that the theoretical framework of SA, as well as
construct and thus, should be used in combination with a the practical measurement tools that are available within
validated SA metric to provide a more diagnostic its umbrella, could complement the research already
interpretation of the results. For example, several of the underway in this area. A review of the literature suggests
authors in this book (see chapters by Angell and van der that a great deal of research and measurement has
Horst) have utilized a secondary detection task as a been directed to what would be characterized as
means of assessing workload. They state that when assessing general SA, while more validated metrics
provide an assessment of the levels of SA. This will and Driving. Transportation Research Record, 1779,
provide more diagnostic feedback in terms of how 26-32.
drivers are reacting and what can be done to improve 12. Gugerty, L.J., & Tirre, W.C. (2000). Individual
their performance. Perhaps it is in this domain that Differences in Situation Awareness. In M.R. Endsley,
situation awareness methods and theory will offer new & D.J. Garland (Eds.), Situation Awareness Analysis
insights as future research is initiated. and Measurement, 249-276. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
13. Gugerty, L.J. (1997). Situation Awareness during
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Driving: Explicit and Implicit Knowledge in dynamic
spatial memory. Journal of Experimental
Portions of this paper were presented at an invited Psychology: Applied, 3(1), 42-66.
lecture at the SAE Driver Metrics Workshop, San 14. Treat, J.R., Tumbas, N.S., McDonald, S.T., Shinar,
Antonio, TX, 2-3 June 2008. The authors wish to D., Hume, R.D., Mayer, R.E., Stansifer, R. L.,
express appreciation to Mr. Anthony M. Costello for his &Catellan, N.J. (1979). Tri-level Study of the Causes
assistance in finalizing this chapter. The authors also of Traffic Accidents: Final Report Volume I: Causal
wish to thank the reviewers for their comments which Factor Tabulations and Assessments. Institute for
greatly enhanced the contribution of this chapter. Research in Public Safety (DOT Publication No.
DOT HS-805 085). Bloomington, IN: Indiana
REFERENCES University.
15. Shinar, D. (1978). Psychology on the Road: The
1. Endsley, M.R. (1997). The Role of Situation Human Factor in Traffic Safety. New York: Wiley.
Awareness in Naturalistic Decision Making. In C.E. 16. Sheridan, T.B. (2002). Human and Automation:
Zsambok & G. Klein (Eds.), Naturalistic Decision System Design and Research Issues. New York:
Making, 269-283. Mahwah, NJ: LEA. Wiley.
2. Endsley, M.R. (1995). Toward a Theory of Situation 17. Kaber, D.B. & Endsley, M.R. (2003). The Effects of
Awareness in Dynamic Systems. Human Factors Level of Automation and Adaptive Automation on
37(1), 32-64. Human Performance, Situation Awareness and
3. Endsley, M.R. & Kiris, E.O. (1995). The Out-of-the- Workload in a Dynamic Control Task. Theoretical
Loop Performance Problem and Level of Control in Issues in Ergonomic Science, 5(2), 113-153.
Automation. Human Factors, 37(2), 381-394. 18. Rudin-Brown, C.M., Parker, H.A., & Malisia, A.R.
4. Ma, R., & Kaber, D.B. (2005). Situation Awareness (2003). Behavioral Adaptation to Adaptive Cruise
and Workload in Driving while Using Adaptive Cruise Control. Proceedings of the 47th Annual Meeting of
Control and a Cell Phone. International Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society. Santa
Industrial Ergonomics, 35, 939-953. Monica, CA: Human Factors and Ergonomics
5. Adams, M., Tenney, Y., & Pew, R. (1995). Situation Society.
Awareness and the Cognitive Management of 19. Kiefer, R. & Salinger, J. (2006). Automotive Collision
Complex Systems. Human Factors, 31(1), 85-104. Avoidance System Field Operational Test Program.
6. Gaba, D.M., Howard, S.K., & Small, S.D. (1995). Presented at the US DOT Public Meeting on the
Situation Awareness in Anesthesiology. Human Benefits of Advanced Crash Avoidance Systems.
Factors, 37, 20-31. Ypsilanti, Michigan.
7. Salas, E., Prince, C., Baker, D.P., & Shrestha, L. http://www.itsa.org/itsa/files/pdf/ACAS GM_UMTRI
(1995). Situation Awareness in Team Performance: FINAL.pdf.
Implications for Measurement and Training. Human 20. Michon, J.A. (1985). A critical view of driver behavior
Factors, 37(1), 123-136. models: What do we know, what should we do? In L.
8. Endsley, M.R. (1988). Situation Awareness Global Evans & R. C. Schwing (Eds.), Human Behavior and
Assessment Technique (SAGAT). Proceedings of Traffic Safety. New York: Plenum Press.
the National Aerospace and Electronics Conference 21. Ma, R., & Kaber, D.B. (2007). Situation Awareness
(NAECON), 789-795. and Driving Performance in a Simulated Navigation
9. Endsley, M.R. (1993). Situation Awareness: The Task. Ergonomics, 50(8), 1351-1364.
Development and Application of a Theoretical 22. Dingus, T., McGehee, D., Hulse, M., Jahns, S.,
Framework. Proceedings of the Human Factors Mannakkul, N., Mollenhauer, M., &Fleischman, R.
Society 37th Annual Meeting, 39-40. Santa Monica, (1995) Travtek Evaluation Task C3 – Camera Car
CA: Human Factors and Ergonomics Society. Study. FHWA-RD-94-076. McLean, VA: Federal
10. Ward, N.J. (2000). Automation of Task Processed: Highway Administration.
an Example of Intelligent Transportation Systems. http://www.itsdocs.fhwa.dot.gov/JPODOCS/REPTS_
Human Factors and Ergonomics in Manufacturing, TE/5303.pdf.
10(4), 395-408. 23. Redelmeier, D.A., & Tibshirani, R.J. (1997).
11. Matthews, M.L., Bryant, D.J., Webb, R.D., & Association Between Cellular-telephone Calls and
Harbluk, J.L. (2001). Model of Situation Awareness Motor Vehicle Collisions. New England Journal of
Medicine, 336(7), 453-458.
24. Violanti, J.M. (1997). Cellular Phones and Traffic nrd.nhtsa.dot.gov/pdf/nrd-
Accidents. Public Health, 111(6), 423-428. 2/Driver%20Workload%20Metrics%Final%20Report.
25. Young, R.A. (2001).Association between embedded pdf&http://www.nhtsa.dot.gov/staticfiles/DOT/NHTS
cellular phone calls and vehicle crashes involving A/NRD/Multimedia/PDFs/Crash%20Avoidance/Drive
airbag deployment. Proceedings of Driving r%20Distraction/Driver%20Workload%20Metrics_ap
Assessment 2001: The First International Driving pendices.pdf.
Symposium on Human Factors in Driver 34. Endsley, M.R., Bolte, B. & Jones, D.G. (2003).
Assessment, Training and Vehicle Design, 1, pp. Designing for Situation Awareness: An Approach to
390–400. http://ppc.uiowa.edu/Driving- User-Centered Design. New York: Taylor & Francis.
Assessment/2001/Summaries/Driving%20Assessme 35. Parasuraman, P.R. & Riley, V. (1997). Humans and
nt%20Papers/81_young-richard.htm Automation: Use, Misuse, Disuse, Abuse. Human
26. Young, R.A. & Schreiner, C.S. (2008), “Personal Factors, 39(2), 230-253.
conversations on a hands-free embedded wireless 36. Llaneras, R. E., Green, C.A., Kiefer, R. J., Chundrlik,
device while driving: 1.Real-world airbag deployment W., Altan, O.D., & Singer, J. P. (2005). Design and
crash risk,” Risk Analysis. DOI: 10.1111/j.1539- Evaluation of a Prototype Rear obstacle Detection
6924.2008.01146.x. and Driver Warning System, Human Factors, 47(1),
http://www3.interscience.wiley.com/journal/1214949 199-215.
51/abstract 37. McLaughlin, S.B., Hankey, J. M., Green, C. A., &
27. Hahn, R. W. & Prieger, J. E. (2006). The Impact of Kiefer, R. J. (2003). Driver Performance Evaluation
th
Driver Cell Phone Use On Accidents. Advances in of Two Rear Parking Aids. Proceedings of the 18
Economic Analysis & Policy, Volume 6, Issue 1, Enhanced Safety Vehicle Conference.
Article 9. 38. Graham, S.E. & Matthews, M.D, (2000). Modeling
28. Hancock, P.A., Lesch, M., & Simmons, L. (2002). and Measuring Situation Awareness. In J.H. Hiller &
The Distraction Effects of Phone use During a R.L. Wampler (Eds.), Workshop on Assessing and
Crucial Driving Maneuver. Accident Analysis and Measuring Training Performance Effectiveness
Prevention, 1-14. (Tech. Rep. 1116) (pp. 14-24). Alexandria, VA: U.S.
29. Hancock, P.A., Simmons, L., Hashemi, L., Howarth, Army Research Institute for the Behavioral and
H., & Ranney, T. (1999). The Effects of In-Vehicle Social Sciences.
Distraction on Driver Response During a Critical 39. Wilson, G.F. (2000). Strategies for
Driving Maneuver. Transportation Human Factors, Psychophysiological Assessment of Situation
1(4), 295-309. Awareness. In M.R. Endsley & D.J. Garland (Eds.),
30. Lee, J.D., Caven, B., Haake, S., & Brown, T.L. Situation Awareness Analysis and Measurement,
(2000). Speech-Based Interaction with In-Vehicle 175-188, Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Computers: The effect of Speech-Based E-mail on Associates.
Drivers’ Attention to the Roadway, presented at the 40. Poe, G.R., Suyenobu, B.Y. et al. (1991). EEG
Driver Distraction Internet Forum. Retrieved Correlated of Critical Decision Making in Computer
December 22, 2008, from Simulated Combat. Sixth International Symposium
http://www.engineering.uiowa.edu/~csl/publications/i on Aviation Psychology, Columbus, OH, Department
ndex.html#00. of Aviation, The Ohio State University.
31. Bowyer, S.M., Hsieh, L., Moran, J.E., Young, R.A., 41. Vidulich, M.A., Stratton, M., Crabtree, M., & Wilson
Manoharan, A., Liao, C.-C., Malladi, K., Yu, Y.-J., G. (1994). Performance-Based and Physiological
Chiang, Y.-R., & Tepley, N. (2009). Conversation Measures of Situational Awareness. Aviation, Space
effects on neural mechanisms underlying reaction and Environmental Medicine, 65 (5 Suppl.), A7-A12.
time to visual events while viewing a driving scene 42. Endsley, M.R. (1995). Measurement of Situation
using MEG. Brain Research, 1251, 151-161. Awareness in Dynamic Systems. Human Factors,
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2008.10.001 37(1), 65-84.
32. Hsieh, L., Young, R., Bowyer, S.M., Moran, J.E., 43. Bolstad, C.A. (2001) Age-Related Factors Affecting
Genik, R.J. II, Green, C.C., Chiang, Y.R., Yu, Y.J., the Perception of Essential Information during
Liao, C.C., &Seaman, S. (2009). Conversation Complex Driving Situations. Unpublished
effects on neural mechanisms underlying reaction Dissertation, North Carolina State University,
time to visual events while viewing a driving scene: Raleigh.
fMRI analysis and asynchrony model. Brain 44. Bolstad, C.A. (2001). Situation Awareness: Does It
Research 1251, 162–175. Change with Age? Proceedings of the Human
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2008.10.002 Factors and Ergonomics Society 45th Annual
33. Angell, L., Auflick, J., Austria, P. A., Kocchar, D., Meeting, 272-276. Santa Monica, CA: Human
Tijerina, L., Biever, W., Diptiman, T., Hogsett, J. & Factors and Ergonomics Society.
Kiger, S. (2006). Driver Workload Metrics Task 2 45. Bolstad, C.A. & Endsley, M.R. (in press). Measuring
Final Report & Appendices. Final Research Report: Shared and Team Situation Awareness in the U.S.
DOT HS 810 635. NHTSA/USDOT, Washington, Army’s Future Objective Force. Military Psychology.
D.C. Available at http://www-
46. Jones, D.G. & Endsley, M.R. (2000). Examining the 57. Foltz, P.W., Bolstad, C.A., Cuevas, H.M., Franzke,
Validity of Real-Time Probes as a Metric of Situation M., Rosenstein, M., & Costello, A.M. (2008).
Awareness. Proceedings of the 14th Triennial Measuring Situation Awareness through Automated
Congress of the International Ergonomics Communication Analysis. In M. Letsky, N. Warner,
Association and the 44th Annual Meeting of the S. M. Fiore, & C. Smith (Eds.), Macrocognition in
Human Factors and Ergonomics Society. Santa Teams. Aldershot, England: Ashgate.
Monica, CA: Human Factors and Ergonomics 58. Xie, B. (2007). Information Technology Education for
Society. Older Adults as a Continuing Peer-Learning
47. Bolstad, C.A., Cuevas, H.M., Costello, A.M., & Process: A Chinese Case Study. Educational
Rousey, J. (2005). Improving Situation Awareness Gerontology, 33(5), 429-450.
through Cross-Training. Proceedings of the 49th 59. Endsley, M. R. (1993). Situation Awareness and
Annual Meeting of the Human Factors and Workload: Flip Sides of the Same Coin. Seventh
Ergonomics Society, 2159-2163. Santa Monica, CA: International Symposium on Aviation Psychology.
Human Factors and Ergonomics Society. Columbus, OH: Department of Aviation, The Ohio
48. Jones, D.G. & Endsley M.R. (1996). Sources of State University.
Situation Awareness Errors in Aviation. Aviation, 60. Fracker, M. L., & Davis, S. A. (1990). Measuring
Space and Environmental Medicine 67(6), 507-512. Operator Situation Awareness and Mental Workload.
49. Rodgers, M.D., Mogford, R.H. & Strauch, B. (2000). Paper presented at the Fifth Mid-Central
Post-Hoc Assessment of Situation Awareness in Air Ergonomics/Human Factors Conference, Dayton,
Traffic Control Incidents and Major Aircraft OH.
Accidents. In M.R. Endsley & D.J. Garland (Eds.), 61. Venturino, M., Hamilton, W. L., & Dvorchak, S. R.
Situation Awareness Analysis and Measurement, (1989). Performance-Based Measures of Merit for
73-112. Mahwah, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum. Tactical Situation Awareness. Situation Awareness
50. Jones, D.G. (2000). Subjective Measures of in AERO 4/5. Copenhagen, Denmark: NATO –
Situation Awareness. In M.R. Endsley & D.J. AGARD.
Garland (Eds.), Situation awareness analysis and
measurement, 113-128. Mahwah, NJ, Lawrence
Erlbaum.
51. Matthews, M. D., Beal, S. A., & Pleban, R. J. (2002).
Situation Awareness in a Virtual Environment:
Description of a Subjective Measure. (Research
Report 1786). Alexandria, VA: U.S. Army Research
Institute for the Behavioral and Social Sciences.
52. Endsley, M.R. (1990). Predictive Utility of an
Objective Measure of Situation Awareness.
Proceedings of the Human Factors Society 34th
Annual Meeting, 41-45. Santa Monica, CA: Human
Factors and Ergonomics Society.
53. Strater, L.D., Endsley, M.R., Pleban, R.J., &
Matthews, M.D. (2001). Measures of Platoon Leader
Situation Awareness in Virtual Decision Making
Exercises (No. Research Report 1770). Alexandria,
VA: Army Research Institute.
54. Endsley, M.R. & Jones, W.M. (1997). Situation
Awareness, Information Dominance, and Information
Warfare (No. AL/CF-TR-1997-0156). Wright-
Patterson AFB, OH: United States Air Force
Armstrong Laboratory.
55. Bolstad, C.A., Cuevas H.M., Gonzalez, C., &
Schneider, M. (2005). Modeling Shared Situation
Awareness. Proceedings of the 14th Conference on
Behavior Representation in Modeling and Simulation
(BRIMS), Los Angeles, CA.
56. Bolstad, C.A., Foltz, P., Franzke, M., Cuevas, H.M.,
Rosenstein, M., & Costello, A.M. (2007). Predicting
Situation Awareness from Team Communications.
Proceedings of the 51st Annual Meeting of the
Human Factors and Ergonomics Society. Santa
Monica, CA: Human Factors and Ergonomics
Society.
CONTACT

Cheryl A. Bolstad is a Senior Research Associate with


SA Technologies. She has a bachelor's degree in
Computer Applications in Psychology and a Master's
degree in Cognitive Psychology. She completed her
doctoral degree in Psychology at North Carolina State
University, specializing in cognition and aging. Dr.
Bolstad has over 15 years of experience as a human
factors engineer. Her recent research has focused on
developing methods for supporting team situation
awareness in distributed systems and developing
training systems for supporting situation awareness.
Address correspondence to: Cheryl A. Bolstad, Ph.D.,
SA Technologies, 76 Lillian Court, Forest Hill, MD
21050, email: cheryl@satechnologies.com.

DEFINITIONS

Situation awareness: Perception of the elements in the


environment within a volume of time and space, the
comprehension of their meaning and the projection of
their status in the near future [2].

ACRONYMS AND ABBREVIATIONS

ACASA: Automated Communication and Situation


Awareness

ACC: adaptive cruise control

ASR: automatic speech recognition

GDTA: goal directed task analysis

GPS: Global Positioning System

PSAQ: Post-Trial Participant Subjective Situation


Awareness Questionnaire

SA: situation awareness

SAGAT: Situation Awareness Global Assessment


Technique

View publication stats

You might also like