You are on page 1of 11

Journal of Purchasing and Supply Management 25 (2019) 100560

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Purchasing and Supply Management


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/pursup

Attention-based view on achieving ambidexterity in purchasing and supply T


management
Harri Lorentza,∗, Sini Laaria, Janne Engbloma, Kari Tanskanenb
a
Turku School of Economics, University of Turku, P.O. Box 20014, Finland
b
Aalto University School of Science, P.O. Box 15500, 00076, Aalto, Finland

A R T I C LE I N FO A B S T R A C T

Keywords: The focus of purchasing and supply management (PSM) is no longer just on short-term cost savings, but is also on
Purchasing long-term goals such as innovation and renewal. Because of this new dual role PSM needs to balance the short-
Ambidexterity term goals referred to as exploitation, and the long-term strategic goals referred to as exploration. However,
Attention-based view developing in-depth knowledge of the supply markets in order to achieve PSM ambidexterity is challenging, as it
Structural equation modelling
requires much managerial attention that is limited in nature. This research draws on the attention-based view
(ABV) to study the antecedents of ambidexterity in PSM. Based on a survey of 154 Finnish manufacturing firms,
a set of hypotheses was tested using structural equation modeling. The results indicate that bottom-up supply
market attention has a stronger association than top-down supply market attention with all three PSM goals:
exploitation, exploration and ambidexterity. PSM status is positively associated with both bottom-up and top-
down supply market attention. Top management should support activities aimed at capturing supply market
opportunities by ensuring that the PSM staff is not overburdened and have enough time to focus on supply
market intelligence gathering and analyses. This research is among the first to study the role and enablers of
attentional mechanisms that underpin ambidexterity in PSM. A contribution to the ABV theory is made by
suggesting operationalizations of managerial attention constructs.

1. Introduction Pihlajamaa et al., 2017) means that PSM must be capable of carrying
out both exploration and exploitation tasks (March, 1991). Since these
Since the share and importance of purchased products and services tasks require very different management activities, an essential chal-
has grown during recent decennia, both managers and scholars in- lenge for any organization is to balance their efforts to both explore and
creasingly consider purchasing and supply management (PSM) to be a exploit, i.e. to achieve ambidexterity (O’Reilly and Tushman, 2004).
strategic function of the firm (Carr and Pearson, 2002; Chen et al., Although exploitation, exploration and ambidexterity have gained
2004; Van Weele and Van Raaij, 2014; Handfield et al., 2015). The substantial attention in organization theory literature (see e.g. Raisch
focus of PSM is no longer just on short-term cost savings, but is in- and Birkinshaw, 2008; Birkinshaw and Gupta, 2013), the accumulated
creasingly also on long-term goals such as innovation and renewal, and knowledge regarding ambidexterity is still somewhat sparse in the PSM
the product life-cycle cost implications of procurement decisions context, particularly regarding its antecedents (see e.g. Gualandris
(Ellram, 1995). Although the strategic nature of the PSM function has et al., 2018).
been widely recognized in the literature (Gadde and Håkansson, 1994; As PSM is essentially about the management of external resources,
Chen et al., 2004; Krause et al., 2009), it has been argued that there is managerial attention regarding such resources is crucial (Ocasio, 1997).
still a gap between the intended strategic role of PSM and the prevailing Therefore, it may be argued that supply market knowledge is indis-
reality (Knoppen and Sáenz, 2015). The gap may be explained by the pensable in order to increase the strategic role of the PSM function (Van
imbalance between PSM’s strategic importance and the resources and Weele and Van Raaij, 2014), as it supports both the short-term ex-
managerial capabilities devoted to it. Widening the PSM perspective ploitation goals such as continuous quality improvement (Kilpi et al.,
from price to total cost of ownership and even to value (Ellram, 1995; 2018), and the long term exploration goals such as innovation (Aslam
Ferrin and Plank, 2002), and from buying products and services to in- et al., 2018 Cousins et al., 2011; Aslam et al., 2018), and thus ambi-
volving suppliers in a firm’s innovation activities (Johnsen, 2009; dexterity. However, despite this logic, the attainment of ambidexterity


Corresponding author.
E-mail addresses: harri.lorentz@utu.fi (H. Lorentz), sini.laari@utu.fi (S. Laari), janne.engblom@utu.fi (J. Engblom), kari.tanskanen@aalto.fi (K. Tanskanen).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pursup.2019.100560
Received 26 February 2019; Received in revised form 28 June 2019; Accepted 16 August 2019
Available online 17 August 2019
1478-4092/ © 2019 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
H. Lorentz, et al. Journal of Purchasing and Supply Management 25 (2019) 100560

in PSM is challenging, as the managerial attention needed for gaining organizational adaptation (Ocasio, 2011), and thus, as a simplification,
in-depth knowledge of supply markets is limited in nature (Ocasio, it can be said that what decision makers do depends on what they focus
1997; Shepherd et al., 2017), especially as regards the often resource- on (Ocasio, 1997).
constrained PSM function (Quayle, 2002; Zheng et al., 2007). Much of This simple, yet evidence-based, statement has significant implica-
the potentially vast supply market may therefore lie at the periphery of tions for strategic management in general and PSM in particular. As the
managerial vision, i.e. where managers’ attention is not (Day and focus of attention, or its allocation, is governed by attentional structures
Schoemaker, 2004). This limits managers’ ability to notice and, cru- (Ocasio, 1997), or drivers such as the various social, economic, and
cially, to act on opportunities, and to mitigate risks (Hambrick, 1981) in cultural aspects of organizations, it results in considerable hetero-
an ambidextrous manner. Due to the constrained attention allocation, it geneity in how attention is focused, not only among firms but also
has been suggested that managers “consciously select and accurately among individuals inside firms (Barreto and Patient, 2013). Conse-
scan appropriate environmental sectors” in order to initiate effective quently, strategic action is not influenced by industry structure alone
actions to align supply with the various competitive priorities of the (e.g. Porter, 1985), but may perhaps be primarily driven by managerial
organization (cf. Garg et al., 2003), such as cost, quality, delivery, cognition (e.g. Nadkarni and Barr, 2008; Daft and Weick, 1984). In-
flexibility, innovation or sustainability (Krause et al., 2001; Pagell and deed, recent commentary implies that ABV defines a firm’s strategy as
Wu, 2009). In other words, attentional processing in PSM regarding the pattern of organizational attention (Ocasio et al., 2018). Further-
supply markets, i.e. supply market attention, produces key knowledge more, evidence suggests that attention-driven sensemaking regarding
inputs for exploitation-oriented improvement activities and explora- salient phenomena in the environment results in the emergence of new
tion-oriented innovation activities for PSM alignment (March, 1991; institutions in organizations (Nigam and Ocasio, 2010), i.e. rules, norms
Mom et al., 2007). and beliefs, and “explanations of what is and what is not, what can be
In this research, we seek to shed further light on the circumstances acted upon and what cannot” (Hoffman, 1999, 351).
in which the PSM function develops ambidexterity in order to improve According to ABV, there is variety in the ways in which individuals
both short and long-term PSM outcomes. In this effort, we adopt the and groups focus attention and process specific sets of stimuli (Ocasio,
attention-based view of the firm (ABV; Ocasio, 1997). Drawing on be- 2011). On one hand, top-down attentional processing “helps top man-
havioral theories in management (Simon, 1947; Cyert and March, agers achieve efficiency, predictability, and reliability, by directing
1963), the ABV conception of attention relates to the individual deci- their attention towards the aspects of the environment that are expected
sion processes of noticing and selecting aspects of situations to be at- to reveal potential opportunities and away from those aspects that are
tended to, or to be ignored (Ocasio, 1997). Whether the scope of at- not expected to be important” (Shepherd et al., 2017, 630). Experience,
tention is broad or laser-sharp (Day and Schoemaker, 2004) is heuristics and knowledge structures, or cognitive frameworks
suggested as depending on the nature of the context, both inside and (McNamara et al., 2002), serve as the “kind of mental template[s] that
outside the organization (Ben-Menahem et al., 2013; Bouquet and individuals impose on an information environment to give it form and
Birkinshaw, 2011). For example, the complexity of purchasing tasks (Liu meaning” (Walsh, 1995, 281). Top-down attentional processing is in
and Li, 2012) and the supply base (Bozarth et al., 2009) may burden essence goal or schema-driven (Ocasio, 2011).
managers and constrain the scope of attention. Furthermore, top On the other hand, by engaging in more bottom-up attentional
management may communicate priorities and commitments, such as processing, “top managers allow the environment to capture attention”,
regarding the status of PSM in the company, and thus foster and em- involving “the features and/or properties inherent in the situation that
power attention to notice, for example, disruptive technologies in draw attention to themselves even when individuals are not actively
supply markets (cf. Bouquet and Birkinshaw, 2011). Therefore, we can searching for them” (Shepherd et al., 2017, 631). It is based on ad hoc,
say that certain organizational and environmental factors may play a random, unpredictable and reciprocal interactions (Shepherd et al.,
role in the attention allocation patterns of organizations in general, and 2017), and is in essence data or stimulus-driven (Ocasio, 2011). In the
of PSM in particular. context of PSM, managers pay attention to the dynamic supply market
Based on the above, our research aims are the following: (1) to test in a top-down and/or bottom-up manner, in their efforts to align supply
the association of supply complexity and PSM status with supply market with the competitive priorities of the firm.
attention, and (2) to test the association of supply market attention with Essentially, ABV and the attentional processing construct provide a
exploitative and explorative orientation, as well as ambidexterity in useful lens for understanding why PSM differs across organizations, as
PSM. These aims allow us to examine the attentional antecedents of functions and managers are likely to vary in terms of how they foster
ambidexterity in PSM. Our approach of using ABV in this research on externally-oriented attention structures, and how they notice and re-
PSM is novel and “shifts the emphasis from what the firm does to how spond to external events such as discontinuous technological change
its managers act” in order to achieve their PSM-related objectives (cf. (Kammerlander and Ganter, 2015) and emerging opportunities in
Bouquet and Birkinshaw, 2011, 257). supply markets (cf. Ren and Guo, 2011). This attentional heterogeneity
ABV has seldom been applied in empirical research, especially in gives rise to differing institutions, and further, asymmetric value ex-
survey studies, due in part to practical measurement challenges pectations regarding external resources, which suggests that managerial
(Ocasio, 2011). Therefore, one of the contributions of this research is attention and focus may become the key determinants of PSM-driven
the operationalization of attentional processing in PSM. Increased competitive advantage (cf. Barney, 2012; Ramsay, 2001a; Ramsay,
awareness of the types of attentional processing and their drivers, al- 2001b).
lows managers to consciously make better attention allocation deci- The association of attention and PSM-based competitive advantage
sions, in order to develop supply market knowledge and adapt the PSM may, however, be more nuanced, as a mediating role may be proposed
organization in an ambidextrous manner. for ambidexterity between the constructs of attentional processing and
PSM performance. In terms of the outcome side of this chain of asso-
2. Theoretical grounding and hypothesis development ciations, it is noted that several studies seem to show the positive as-
sociation of organizational ambidexterity and firm performance out-
2.1. Defining and linking attentional processing and ambidexterity comes, particularly in uncertain environments (O’Reilly and Tushman,
2013). In the PSM context, Gualandris et al. (2018) show exploratory
ABV conceptualizes attention as a scarce resource used to notice and exploitative activities (ambidexterity) serving as the determinants
opportunities and threats, or events, in the environment (e.g. Ren and of supplier efficiency and product innovation, as well as buyer financial
Guo, 2011). As such, attentional focus determines both the speed of performance. Regarding the antecedent side, it is useful to consider the
strategic response to events (Nadkarni and Barr, 2008) and the different approaches to ambidexterity. The approaches can be divided

2
H. Lorentz, et al. Journal of Purchasing and Supply Management 25 (2019) 100560

to two types: those that are based on separating exploration and ex- To further emphasize the possibly important implications of this
ploitation activities organizationally or by varying emphasis over time, dichotomous theoretical concept, we suggest a link between attentional
and those that aim for coexistence of exploitation and exploration in the processing and the concept of ambidexterity (March, 1991), or, more
same organizational unit (contextual ambidexterity) (O’Reilly and precisely, the components of ambidexterity, as suggested and supported
Tushman, 2013; Raisch and Birkinshaw, 2008; March, 1991). Studies by the above discussion. While top-down supply market attention is
focusing on ambidexterity of the PSM or supply chain functions (e.g. likely to produce useful insights for both exploitation and exploration,
Gualandris et al., 2018; Kristal et al., 2010), or of the suppliers it may indeed be hypothesized that top-down processing is more sali-
(Azadegan and Dooley, 2010), could be classified as taking the con- ently associated with exploitation-oriented learning and development
textual approach, which suggests that business units or functions may (Mom et al., 2007). Furthermore, while bottom-up supply market at-
achieve ambidexterity without temporal or organizational separation of tention may result in discoveries that contribute to both incremental
its conflicting underlying components. Importantly, this may be process improvement and more disruptive management innovations,
achieved by the means of behavioral capacity of its decision makers for this type of attentional processing would be more saliently associated
simultaneous pursuance of exploitation and exploration, supported by with explorative development activities, as is suggested by Mom et al.
managements systems or culture (Gibson and Birkinshaw, 2004; (2007; see also Laureiro-Martínez et al., 2015; Day and Schoemaker,
O’Reilly and Tushman, 2013). Aoki and Wilhelm (2016) appear to show 2004). As it may be proposed that these theorized associations apply
such culture-enabled behavioral capacity in Toyota, where experience- also in the context of developing supply and various procurement
based knowledge accumulation regarding internal production processes processes and practices, we state the following set of hypotheses:
is suggested to underlie ambidextrous supplier management activities.
H1a. Top-down supply market attention is positively associated with
Kauppila (2010) seems to suggest that matching types of knowledge
exploitative PSM orientation.
is required for exploitation and exploration in order to achieve con-
textual ambidexterity. In the PSM context, similar stance has been H1b. Top-down supply market attention is positively associated with
adopted by Kilpi et al. (2018), who show that knowledge acquisition, explorative PSM orientation.
regarding the firm’s existing supply base and broader supply markets,
H1c. The association between top-down supply market attention and
supports the exploitative and explorative orientations in PSM respec-
exploitative PSM orientation is stronger than the association between
tively. Based on these studies it may therefore be proposed that beha-
top-down supply market attention and explorative PSM orientation.
vioural capacities that support the acquisition and accumulation of
knowledge relating to external resources therefore seems to serve as a H1d. Bottom-up supply market attention is positively associated with
key antecedent of ambidexterity in PSM, and that such associations may explorative PSM orientation.
have a favorable impact on firm performance. As we focus on the
H1e. Bottom-up supply market attention is positively associated with
antecedent side in our study, it is further proposed that attentional
exploitative PSM orientation.
processing as a behavioural capacity, regarding the supply environ-
ment, essentially produces the key knowledge inputs for exploitation- H1f. The association between bottom-up supply market attention and
oriented improvement activities and exploration-oriented innovation explorative PSM orientation is stronger than the association between
activities in PSM (March, 1991; Mom et al., 2007). Thus it is clear that bottom-up supply market attention and exploitative PSM orientation.
the linkage between attentional processing and ambidexterity seems to
The exploitation and exploration development orientations together
hold significance for PSM, and understanding this association would
contribute to organizational ambidexterity (March, 1991), which im-
indeed be the next logical step in advancing the literature on ambi-
plies higher performance (e.g. Bierly and Daly, 2007). However, the
dexterity in PSM. For the benefit of the following hypothesis develop-
literature suggests tensions between the components (Lavie et al.,
ment, we define ambidextrous PSM orientation to be the simultaneous
2010), and a lack of an explicit trade-off point in terms of balancing and
coexistence of exploitative and explorative orientations within the
prioritization (Raisch et al., 2009; Tushman and O’Reilly, 1996). Allo-
function, the former relating to efficiency, alignment with and im-
cation of resources for exploitative and explorative activities requires
provement of existing practices, and their standardization, and the
analysis and assessment (March, 1991), and resource-constrained firms
latter to adaptation, experimentation with new practices, challenging of
may indeed have to be selective (Voss and Voss, 2013). It may be as-
existing practices and to the search for new solutions and practices (cf.
sumed that, typically, firms are more naturally exploitative, as most
Kilpi et al., 2018).
organizations lack the extra resources for experimenting with novel and
risky concepts (Lavie et al., 2010) and behavioral routines are self-re-
2.2. Hypotheses and research models
inforcing (Birkinshaw and Gupta, 2013). Ambidexterity is therefore
difficult to achieve and exceptional among organizations (Birkinshaw
ABV suggests that top-down attentional processing is more likely to
and Gupta, 2013). Drawing on the earlier discussion, bottom-up at-
result in noting incremental changes in the environment and current
tentional processing may be considered to be associated relatively more
trajectories, and thus would be oriented towards, or associated with
with a balance between exploitation and exploration (Birkinshaw and
similar kinds of development opportunities or actions. In contrast,
Gupta, 2013), as it is the primary driver of exploration (Mom et al.,
bottom-up attentional processing is likely to be associated with noting
2007; Laureiro-Martínez et al., 2015; Day and Schoemaker, 2004), and
discontinuous changes and disruptions inconsistent with the current
thus balances naturally-occurring exploitative tendencies. We empha-
trajectories, and thus may lead to more radical development opportu-
size that here the definition and operationalization both matter, and
nities and actions (Shepherd et al., 2017; cf. Day and Schoemaker,
that balanced ambidexterity essentially means that equal levels in both
2004). Furthermore, too much focus on either may result in missing
components is the key to improved performance. We propose an al-
unexpected signals in the environment, or risking a reinvention of the
ternative set of hypotheses for the outcome side as follows:
wheel and being burdened with complexity, and thus in underscoring
the importance of appropriately balanced attention allocation, i.e. be- H2a. Top-down supply market attention is positively associated with
tween the domains of top-down and bottom-up (Shepherd et al., 2017). ambidextrous (balanced) PSM orientation.
At the same time, it should be noted that managerial attention is a
H2b. Bottom-up supply market attention is negatively associated with
scarce resource (Ocasio, 1997; Simon, 1947). Therefore, there are costs
ambidextrous (balanced) PSM orientation.
and an eventual burden associated with absorbing information inputs
from increasing the level of resources dedicated to peripheral vision, for Having developed alternative sets of hypotheses for the outcome
example (Day and Schoemaker, 2004). side of supply market attention, we turn to the antecedents or

3
H. Lorentz, et al. Journal of Purchasing and Supply Management 25 (2019) 100560

Fig. 1. Two alternative research models.

circumstances of attentional processing in PSM. In essence, we are in- development in PSM; however, explorative orientation may depend
terested in what may constrain or stimulate attentional processing in more on status (Kilpi et al., 2018), as status drives empowerment
general, and whether there are differences in terms of how the two (Seibert et al., 2004), the availability of slack resources and favorable
dimensions of attention are affected. Here, the important assumptions conditions for experimenting with novel and risky concepts (Lavie
are that managerial attention is limited (Ocasio, 1997; Simon, 1947; et al., 2010; Levinthal and March, 1993). Status may also drive atten-
Day and Schoemaker, 2004), and that the external environment tional coherence across the various levels, units and functions of the
changes dynamically with enough diversity such that phenomena may organization, allowing attentional triangulation and spotting weak
take place at the periphery of focus (Shepherd et al., 2017). Therefore, signals at the periphery of vision (Rerup, 2009). Furthermore, by de-
the allocation of attention between the dichotomous dimensions of at- finition, status involves the integration of PSM with other functions, the
tentional processing, and the causes of such allocation, are important cross-functional category teams potentially serving as embodiments of
issues to be considered. contexts in which established attentional perspectives are challenged
In seeking to meet the performance goals defined by organizations, due to variation in the knowledge structures and aims of the partici-
managers face task challenges that determine the level of task or job pating individuals. Such attentional engagement allows organizations
demands, the latter defined as “the degree to which a given executive to identify new issues, generate new action alternatives and make sense
experiences his or her job as difficult or challenging” (Hambrick et al., of causes and consequences (Ocasio, 2011). The logic behind this ar-
2005). It has been suggested that a key driver of task demands is the gumentation is often based on Ashby’s (1958) law of requisite variety,
task complexity arising from numerous components and dynamism (Liu which holds that “an organization can only detect variety in the en-
and Li, 2012), as this burdens the information-processing capacity of vironment, such as weak cues, non-events, or emerging problems, to the
managers, and thus also implies challenges for supply management extent that it has sufficient internal attentional variety to represent it”
(e.g. Bozarth et al., 2009). While the uncertainty and dynamism aspects (Rerup, 2009, p. 888). It could therefore be hypothesized that PSM
of complexity may cause significant challenges in supply management status, through its empowering and integrative nature, is a relatively
(Bozarth et al., 2009; Milgate, 2001), detail complexity in the form of more important driver of such attentional processing that is bottom-up
large supplier networks prominently drives poor performance (Bode in nature. We therefore state the following hypotheses.
and Wagner, 2015; Lorentz et al., 2012; Choi and Krause, 2006; Vachon
H4a. PSM status is positively associated with bottom-up supply market
and Klassen, 2002). Due to the need to make frequent decisions in terms
attention.
of numerous sourced items, suppliers, orders and source country con-
texts, i.e. supply complexity, supply managers are likely to be under a H4b. PSM status is positively associated with top-down supply market
high task challenge, and thus experience significant and competing task attention.
demands (Hambrick et al., 2005). With such salient items demanding
H4c. The association between PSM status and bottom-up supply market
the focus and attention of managers, and with these challenges ad-
attention is stronger than the association between PSM status and top-
dressed naturally through the existing knowledge structures in the
down supply market attention.
manner of top-down attentional processing (e.g. Birkinshaw and Gupta,
2013; Peeters et al., 2015; Turner et al., 2018), the periphery of vision The previously stated hypotheses are summarized as the research
may be starved of bottom-up attentional processing of emergent weak models in Fig. 1 (the main model and the alternative outcome side
signals. Indeed, it has been suggested that the higher the level of model), which allows us to examine the antecedents and outcomes of
competing task demands, the higher the reliance on top-down atten- attentional processing in PSM, as well as ambidexterity in PSM.
tional processing by managers (Shepherd et al., 2017). Therefore we
state the following hypotheses. 3. Methods
H3a. Supply complexity is positively associated with top-down supply
market attention. 3.1. Measurement

H3b. Supply complexity is negatively associated with bottom-up supply Based on the above-discussed conceptual foundations and defini-
market attention. tions of the two dimensions in attentional processing, and due to a lack
The role and status of the PSM function is related to visibility and of existing operationalizations, we engaged in developing novel PSM-
participation in strategic decision-making, equality among functions, specific statements for these concepts. These statements essentially
top-management support and a prominent role in the company (Carr describe the PSM function’s way of operating in terms of supply market
and Smeltzer, 1997; Cousins and Menguc, 2006). These characteristics attention. For the top-down attentional processing related items, we
have been shown to enable both exploitative and explorative adopted the idea that there are certain standard operating procedures,
goals, requirements and experience that direct and define the way PSM

4
H. Lorentz, et al. Journal of Purchasing and Supply Management 25 (2019) 100560

professionals make decisions, engage in discussions with suppliers, invitation to participate in the survey was sent to a convenience sample
observe and research supply markets, and examine supply risks (cf. of 51 PSM professionals in the authors’ professional networks. In total,
Shepherd et al., 2017; Ocasio, 2011). Appendix A presents the oper- 25 useable responses were collected.
ationalizations for top-down attentional processing. Next, an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was conducted, using
In Appendix A we also define statements for bottom-up attention principal component analysis with Varimax rotation. Based on the item
allocation in PSM. The design of these statements was guided by con- loadings, two factors were extracted and labeled as Bottom-up supply
cepts such as informality, emergence, as well as reciprocal and ad-hoc market attention and Top-down supply market attention. The final
exchange and identification of ideas with existing and new suppliers, scales account for 68.652% of the cumulative variance extracted. The
other functions and colleagues about markets, products and technolo- scale reliability of the instrument was inspected for both scales.
gies. The statements essentially describe a way of operating that ex- Cronbach’s alpha for bottom-up and top-down supply market attention
poses PSM to external stimuli (cf. Shepherd et al., 2017; Ocasio, 2011). turned out to be 0.865 and 0.789 respectively, which is above the
It is important to note that the statements regarding attentional pro- threshold value of 0.7 (Nunnally, 1978).
cessing must be defined so that they allow respondents to agree or
disagree with both the top-down and the bottom-up statements si- 3.3. Data collection
multaneously, for example. This is in line with the earlier considered
hypothesis regarding a balanced attention allocation, in comparison to The sample consists of Finnish firms in the manufacturing sector. A
a biased allocation, where, for example, only the top-down attention commercial provider of company contact details was used to establish a
allocation would be practiced. sampling frame. The sampling was limited to large, medium and small
On the outcome side, we used most of the items measuring ex- manufacturing firms, essentially excluding micro-sized firms, i.e. those
ploitative and explorative PSM orientation as suggested by Kilpi et al. with an annual net turnover of under EUR 2 million. The final sampling
(2018). The deleted items d and h were replaced by the following frame was determined by the existence of contact details (email ad-
statements which are aligned with Mom et al. (2007): (1) We con- dresses) of company procurement personnel and chief executive officers
tinuously assess new suppliers and supply markets in order to replace (CEOs), resulting in a list of 1853 firms from a population size of 2830
existing supply arrangements; (2) We mostly develop procurement firms. The coverage of the population by the sampling frame can thus
within the framework of existing practices and policies (see Appendix be considered reasonably adequate. The final questionnaire was ad-
for full set of measures). We note that the operationalization of ambi- ministered online. The number of responses was 154 and the response
dexterity varies greatly, and our approach can be considered as mea- rate was 8.3%. According to Gu et al. (2017) and Mora-Monge et al.
surement of the propensity to do something, and measurement of ba- (2019) such a response rate is typical when target respondents hold
lanced ambidexterity as follows: |Exploitative PSM orientation – executive positions in a firm. De Beuckelaer and Wagner (2012) point
Explorative PSM orientation| (the lower the better), instead of a pro- out that small sample surveys are not deficient per se. Instead, if the
duct or sum (Exploitative PSM orientation x Explorative PSM orienta- methodological issues are understood and good practices are followed,
tion, Exploitative PSM orientation + Explorative PSM orientation, small sample surveys are able to provide valuable and valid contribu-
suggesting higher the better) which focuses on the trade-off (see dis- tions.
cussion in Birkinshaw and Gupta, 2013). However, lower response rates require testing for non-response bias.
On the antecedent side, the PSM status measurement was based on Early and late respondents were compared with an independent sam-
Kilpi et al. (2018). Their measures are based on the work of Carr and ples t-test to evaluate possible non-response bias (Armstrong and
Smeltzer (1997) and Cousins and Menguc (2006), and are associated Overton, 1977). The early and late respondents were compared across
with participation in decision-making, equality among functions and theoretical constructs and demographic variables. Furthermore, we
relevance from the competitiveness point of view (see Appendix). compared our final sample to 400 randomly selected non-respondents
Furthermore, three objective single item measures were used to using demographic variables such as annual turnover and profit. No
assess the extent of supply complexity: (1) the number of active items significant differences (p < 0.05) were found, which indicates that non-
handled by the procurement function, (2) the number of orders handled response bias does not influence the results of our study (Anderson and
by the procurement function, (3) the number of suppliers handled by Gerbing, 1988).
the procurement function. These more objective indicators were di- To reduce the likelihood of consistency motive bias, the theoretical
vided by the number of procurement personnel, in order to make the constructs were separated and placed in different phases of the survey.
complexity measures comparable across firms and PSM functions of To avoid social desirability bias, the respondents were assured con-
different size. Conceptually, all the complexity measures contribute to fidentiality and could choose either to complete the survey anon-
task complexity due to the number of elements to be managed (e.g. Liu ymously or to reveal the name of the firm and an email address in order
and Li, 2012), whereas the third may also be considered as an indicator to receive a benchmarking report.
of horizontal supply chain complexity (Bode and Wagner, 2015). 61 (40%) of the firms were small (turnover EUR 2–10 million), 66
The survey questions inquired about current states and practices, (43%) were medium-sized (turnover EUR 10–50 million), while 27
and the appropriate averages during the last 12 months. The subjective (17%) were large (turnover in excess of EUR 50 million). The sample
measures used a 7-point Likert scale, consisting of the following points: covers a wide spectrum of industries, the largest groups being the
1) completely disagree, 2) mostly disagree, 3) somewhat disagree, 4) manufacture of fabricated metal products (35) and the manufacture of
neither agree not disagree, 5) somewhat agree, 6) mostly agree, and 7) machinery and equipment (30).
completely agree.
3.4. Common method variance
3.2. Pretest procedure for supply market attention scales
Common method variance (CMV) was addressed through proce-
The initial versions of the top-down and bottom-up supply market dural and methodological approaches. We took pre-emptive procedural
attention scales were presented separately to several industry experts remedies to avoid CMV as suggested by Podsakoff et al. (2003) and
who were asked to critically review each construct and item. The placed the measures in different phases of the questionnaire. To test the
constructs and survey items were then refined for clarity using this existence of CMV we first performed the Harman’s single-factor test.
feedback. Next, an online questionnaire was created to pilot test the The single factor accounted for 30.4% of cumulative variance. Next, we
items, in order to identify potential problems and to make adjustments chose item “What is the share of suppliers that possess a relevant cer-
before the full-scale launch of the survey in late spring 2017. The tificate (related to quality or CSR, for example)?” as a marker variable

5
H. Lorentz, et al. Journal of Purchasing and Supply Management 25 (2019) 100560

Table 1
Zero-order correlations and adjusted correlations.
Status Complexity Top-down Bottom-up Explore Exploit

Status 1 0.079 −0.045 −0.048 −0.020 0.237**


Complexity 0.067 1 0.193* 0.192* 0.096 0.069
Top-down −0.039 0.198* 1 0.904** 0.319** 0.474**
Bottom-up −0.042 0.197* 0.905** 1 0.303** 0.369**
Explore −0.014 0.101 0.323** 0.307** 1 0.514**
Exploit 0.242** 0.075 0.477** 0.373** 0.517** 1
Marker variable 0.085 0.027 −0.006 0.058 −0.125 −0.025

Zero-order correlations are below the diagonal; adjusted correlations are above the diagonal.
*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01.

that was theoretically unrelated to other substantive variables. We used Table 2


the smallest correlation between the marker variable and other vari- Results of the confirmatory factor analysis.
ables as an estimate for method bias (Lindell and Whitney, 2001; Latent variables Unstandardised factor Completely standardised t-value
Podsakoff et al., 2012). We then applied the partial correlation proce- loading factor loading
dure to adjust the correlation coefficients and tested their significance.
All CMV-controlled correlations remained significant after the adjust- PSM status (α = 0.825; CR = 0.972; AVE = 0.920)
STATUS1 1.000 0.769 -a
ment (Table 1), which suggests that the results cannot be accounted for STATUS2 1.539 0.860 6.783
by CMV. STATUS4 1.205 0.743 5.815
Finally, Fuller et al., (2016) demonstrated with a simulation study Supply complexity (α = 0.663; CR = 0.932; AVE = 0.823)
that CMV can be present at relatively high levels (70% or more) before COMP2 1.000 0.573 -a
COMP3 0.939 0.754 3.932
it induces significant biases. Taken together, CMV is hence unlikely to
COMP4 0.750 0.703 3.663
be a threat to the validity of the results. Top-down supply market attention (α = 0.780; CR = 0.960; AVE = 0.890)
TOP2 1.000 0.758 -a
TOP3 0.957 0.705 7.155
4. Data analysis and results TOP4 1.115 0.774 5.890
Bottom-up supply market attention (α = 0.709; CR = 0.784; AVE = 0.548)
4.1. Measurement model BOTTOM1 1.000 0.682 -a
BOTTOM3 1.060 0.690 3.415
BOTTOM4 1.016 0.634 3.438
The first step to evaluate the validity of the measurement instru- Explorative PSM orientation (α = 0.755; CR = 0.958; AVE = 0.887)
ment was an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) of principal components. EXPLORE1 1.000 0.749 -a
First, we excluded items with small loadings. After defining the mea- EXPLORE2 1.184 0.849 11.933
EXPLORE4 0.775 0.599 4.254
surement instruments, another factor analysis was performed. There
Exploitative PSM prientation (α = 0.745; CR = 0.0.953; AVE = 0.874)
were six factors explaining 71% of total variance. EXPLOIT1 1.000 0.601 -a
Before the confirmatory factor analysis we conducted Little’s MCAR EXPLOIT2 1.685 0.873 4.247
test and concluded that the missing values in the data are missing EXPLOIT3 1.152 0.631 4.334
completely at random (Χ2 = 457.948, df = 429, p = 0.161). In the
at-statistics fixed for scaling.
subsequent stages we thus proceeded by using the full information
Χ2/df = 1.539; RMSEA = 0.059; CFI = 0.919; TLI = 0.896.
maximum likelihood (FIML) method which avoids listwise deletion and
uses all available data. This yields smaller errors in parameter estimates
Table 3
and provides appropriate standard errors of estimation (Newman,
Discriminant validity test with Fornell-Larcker criterion.
2003).
To assess the reliability and validity of the measurement model a Status Complexity Top-down Bottom-up Explore Exploit
confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was conducted (see Table 2). The
Status 0.959
hypothesized model fits the data appropriately (Χ2/df = 1.539, Complexity −0.165 0.907
CFI = 0.919, TLI = 0.896, RMSEA = 0.059, p-value < 0.01) (Hu and Top-down 0.310** −0.072 0.944
Bentler, 1999). All items were loaded on the intended constructs. The Bottom-up 0.412** −0.130 0.507** 0.740
standardized factor loadings were significant and ranged from 0.573 to Explore 0.329** 0.230 0.652** 0.652** 0.942
Exploit 0.211** 0.011 0.225** 0.287** 0.449** 0.935
0.873, exceeding the recommended threshold of 0.5 (Hazen et al.,
2015). The average variance extracted (AVE) values for all the con- *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01, the square root of AVE appears on the diagonal in bold.
structs are higher than 0.50, which indicates convergent validity
(Fornell and Larcker, 1981). The composite reliabilities ranged from Table 4
0.784 to 0.972, demonstrating acceptable reliability and internal con- Discriminant validity test with Heterotrait-monotrait (HTMT) ratios.
sistency of the scales (Garver and Mentzer, 1999).
Status Complexity Top-down Bottom-up Explore Exploit
We followed the guidelines of Voorhees et al. (2016) to test the
discriminant validity of the constructs. First, we compared the square Status
root of AVE to the correlations between each construct (Fornell and Complexity 0.150
Larcker, 1981). Given that the square root of AVE (displayed on the Top-down 0.615 0.110
Bottom-up 0.624 0.140 0.693
diagonal of Table 3) is higher than the correlation coefficient for all
Explore 0.443 0.175 0.583 0.767
constructs, the presence of discriminant validity was achieved. Exploit 0.444 0.040 0.499 0.552 0.803
Secondly, we used the heterotrait-monotrait (HTMT) test (Henseler
et al., 2015). The HTMT is a relatively new criterion to test discriminant
validity but has been demonstrated to perform better than the con-
ventional constrained Phi approach (Voorhees et al., 2016). All HTMT

6
H. Lorentz, et al. Journal of Purchasing and Supply Management 25 (2019) 100560

ratios of correlations were lower than the threshold value of 0.85 for whereas PSM status is connected to both top-down and bottom-up
every pair of constructs (Table 4), which indicates discriminant validity supply market attention. As expected, the results reveal a statistically
(Henseler et al., 2015). significant (p < 0.1) negative relationship between bottom-up supply
market attention and ambidextrous PSM orientation (H2b). However,
contrary to the hypothesis, top-down supply market attention is not
4.2. Structural equation modelling
associated with ambidextrous PSM orientation (H2a). Table 5 sum-
marizes the results of the hypothesis testing.
We used structural equation modeling to test the hypothesized re-
lationships simultaneously. First, we tested the set of hypotheses in
which the exploitative and explorative PSM orientations are measured 5. Discussion and conclusions
as two separate constructs. The model fit was found to be adequate (Χ2/
df = 1.593, CFI = 0.907, TLI = 0.886, RMSEA = 0.062, p-value < 5.1. Theoretical implications
0.01). The model explains 0.231% of the variance of top-down supply
market attention and 0.334% of the variance of bottom-up supply The results suggest a salient role for PSM status in supporting both
market attention. types of supply market attention (H4a & H4b). As the difference be-
The results reveal that bottom-up supply market attention is posi- tween effect sizes is not statistically significant, it cannot be confirmed
tively associated with explorative (H1d) and exploitative PSM or- that status would be more beneficial in supporting bottom-up proces-
ientation (H1e). While there is no statistically significant association sing, as was hypothesized (H4c). The results from our sample simply
between top-down supply market attention and exploitative or ex- suggest that an elevated status for the PSM function may empower
plorative PSM orientation (H1a-b), the result is aligned with the hy- teams and individuals for strategically aligned supply market mon-
pothesis. Although the association between top-down supply market itoring and supplier interaction, and may also endow the function with
attention and exploitative PSM orientation is stronger than the asso- slack resources (e.g. Lavie et al., 2010), which are needed to focus more
ciation between top-down supply market attention and explorative PSM attention on emerging issues, even at the periphery of vision. With
orientation, the difference between the coefficients is not statistically elevated status, the PSM staff will have greater incentive to conduct
significant. Hence, we cannot confirm H1c. However, we found statis- targeted searches and analyses and to spot emerging trends in the
tically significant support (p < 0.01) for H1f which suggests that the supply markets, as status enables ideas to be promoted and accepted
association between bottom-up supply market attention and explorative within the organization (Pemer and Skjølsvik, 2016).
PSM orientation is stronger than the association between bottom-up Secondly, it is interesting to note the absence of associations be-
supply market attention and exploitative PSM orientation. No sig- tween supply complexity and the supply market attention constructs in
nificant association was found between supply complexity and atten- our study (H3a & H3b). Although the literature emphasizes the role of
tional processing (H3a-b). PSM status, in turn, is positively associated high supply complexity in poor performance outcomes (e.g. Choi and
with both top-down (H4a) and bottom-up (H4b) supply market atten- Krause, 2006), and practitioners often lament the relatively high task
tion. However, the difference is not statistically significant (H4c). The challenge in PSM, such negative effects on supply market attention
results are depicted in Fig. 2. were not found in our sample. A possible explanation could be the in-
Following the recommendation of Riedl et al. (2014), we calculated creased use of e-procurement solutions and other forms of digitalization
the statistical power of the structural model. The resulting value of 0.93 for supporting, simplifying and automating work flows, resulting in
(ε0 = 0.05 and εa = 0.08) in a test of close fit falls within the range burden reduction for PSM staff (Srai and Lorentz, 2019; Nissen and
from 0.8 to 1.0, which implies that the likelihood of failing to reject an Sengupta, 2006). The key question here could be whether supply
inappropriately conceptualized model is very low (MacCallum et al., complexity is becoming irrelevant, from the task complexity perspec-
1996; McQuitty, 2004). Hence, the reliability and validity of the results tive, due to digitalization trends that are allowing managers to pay
are strongly supported. more attention to the supply markets.
Next, in order to test the alternative set of hypotheses (i.e. the Thirdly, only bottom-up supply market attention appears to be as-
ambidextrous PSM orientation), we formed the construct of ambi- sociated with exploitative and explorative PSM orientations (H1a-e).
dexterity by calculating the arithmetic means of exploitative and ex- Although this, to some degree, runs counter to the results of Shepherd
plorative PSM orientation and calculated their absolute difference. The et al. (2017), it is noted that the association of bottom-up supply market
error variance of the ambidexterity factor was set to the product of the attention with explorative PSM attention is relatively stronger. This
variable’s variance and one minus reliability (assumed 0.7) (Jöreskog supports the hypothesis that bottom-up attentional processing matches
and Sörbom, 1993). better with aims and actions promoting novel or even radical devel-
Fit indices imply a good fit (Χ2/df = 1.142, CFI = 0.980, opment which could be characterized as exploratory in nature (H1f).
TLI = 0.974, RMSEA = 0.030, p-value = 0.213). The results regarding From the perspective of ambidexterity in PSM, the findings indeed
supply complexity and PSM status are similar to those in the previous suggest that increased levels of bottom-up supply market attention
analysis: supply complexity has no statistically significant relationships, balance the hypothesized default tendencies for exploitative PSM

Fig. 2. SEM results, first set of hypotheses.

7
H. Lorentz, et al. Journal of Purchasing and Supply Management 25 (2019) 100560

Fig. 3. SEM results, second set of hypotheses.

Table 5
Results of the hypothesis testing.
Hypothesis Outcome

H1a: Top-down supply market attention is positively associated with exploitative PSM orientation. Not supported
H1b: Top-down supply market attention is positively associated with explorative PSM orientation. Not supported
H1c: The association between top-down supply market attention and exploitative PSM orientation is stronger than the association between top-down supply Not supported
market attention and explorative PSM orientation.
H1d: Bottom-up supply market attention is positively associated with explorative PSM orientation. Supported
H1e: Bottom-up supply market attention is positively associated with exploitative PSM orientation. Supported
H1f: The association between bottom-up supply market attention and explorative PSM orientation is stronger than the association between bottom-up supply Supported
market attention and exploitative PSM orientation.
H2a: Top-down supply market attention is positively associated with ambidextrous (balanced) PSM orientation. Not supported
H2b: Bottom-up supply market attention is negatively associated with ambidextrous (balanced) PSM orientation. Supported
H3a: Supply complexity is positively associated with top-down supply market attention. Not supported
H3b: Supply complexity is negatively associated with bottom-up supply market attention. Not supported
H4a: PSM status is positively associated with bottom-up supply market attention. Supported
H4b: PSM status is positively associated with top-down supply market attention. Supported
H4c: The association between PSM status and bottom-up supply market attention is stronger than the association between PSM status and top-down supply Not supported
market attention.

orientation, and thus allow the PSM function to achieve a beneficial In broader terms, our study answers the call for research on how
situation in which both exploitation and exploration co-exist for am- information is acquired and acted on in the supply chain (Schorsch
bidexterity (Birkinshaw and Gupta, 2013). et al., 2017), by examining how attentional engagement influences the
Fourthly, whereas our results emphasize the importance of bottom- strategic agenda of the PSM function, and thereby contributes to the
up attentional processing in PSM, it is noted that the evidence suggests emerging field of behavioral supply chain management. A contribution
that PSM benefits from aligned purchasing strategy and practices (Baier to the ABV theory is made by suggesting operationalizations of man-
et al., 2008; also Hesping and Schiele, 2015). This perspective ad- agerial attention constructs. Despite the abundance of studies adopting
vocates a top-down alignment in PSM, and confirms that goal-driven, or the ABV framework, there are few studies using surveys to test the
top-down attentional processing for external resources is needed, and relationship of attentional processing to its antecedents and outcomes.
that it often serves perhaps as the default attentional-DNA in PSM teams
and organizations. However, if goal-driven and schema-guided ten- 5.2. Managerial implications
dencies related to supply base management and supply market analyses
are too strong, the function – which is in charge of spotting opportu- The findings also provide relevant insight for executives. First, we
nities in the supply markets – will be in danger of missing key events, encourage that top management should formally and informally re-
emerging technologies and evolving risks at the periphery of attention. cognize the role and status of the PSM function and dedicate resources,
Our results suggest that, for ambidextrous PSM orientation, i.e. for such as money, personnel and time, to encourage purchasing personnel
improving and exploiting existing resources while simultaneously ex- to search for novel ideas and solutions. A resource-constrained PSM
ploring novel concepts and ideas from the markets, a determined effort function is prone to sticking to existing routines and searching ways of
will be needed to offset any default bias towards top-down attention by exploiting existing supply arrangements. In order to tackle challenges
emphasizing bottom-up supply market attention (H2a & H2b). With ranging from innovation to climate change, PSM needs to be empow-
reference to the famous metaphor by O’Reilly and Tushman (2004), we ered to discover critical ideas, knowledge and solutions hidden within
deviate slightly by suggesting that, similarly to the Roman god Janus, supply networks (Gualandris et al., 2018).
PSM indeed needs two sets of open eyes: one set for top-down attention Second, while the goal-driven and schema-guided top-down ap-
and, as emphasized by our results, another set for bottom-up attention, proach may be necessary to achieve predictability, efficiency, and re-
in order to enable the two equally strong hands of exploitation and liability by directing attention on features that are expected to be im-
exploration to work and develop the PSM processes. The key enablers in portant (Shepherd et al., 2017), a more bottom-up approach can allow
this balanced effort will be the empowerment, the early involvement in purchasing professionals to notice and interpret strategically relevant
planning and decision-making, and the slack resources that come with weak signals and discontinuous change in the supply market. Im-
high PSM status within the organization. Herein lies the theoretical portantly, the results emphasize that there needs to be a right balance
contribution of our research: understanding the role and enablers of between top-down and bottom-up supply market attention. A balanced
attentional mechanisms that underpin ambidextrous orientation in approach implies that PSM staff will not need to choose between ex-
PSM. ploitation and exploration. It should also be noted that this attentional

8
H. Lorentz, et al. Journal of Purchasing and Supply Management 25 (2019) 100560

balance is dynamic and needs to be adapted over time (Walrave et al., While this study emphasized that empowerment of the PSM func-
2017). tion supports both top-down and bottom-up attentional processing, an
additional research opportunity lies in an individual-level examination
5.3. Limitations and future research of managerial attention and decision-making (Kaufmann et al., 2017).
By discovering the factors within individuals (e.g. their values and
The above conclusions need to be considered in the light of some beliefs) and within organizations (e.g. their organizational culture and
limitations of our research. Our sample is somewhat small and geo- routines) that affect attention allocation, we could shed further light on
graphically limited, inhibiting the generalizability of the results. Data how to achieve ambidextrous PSM. For example, Pemer and Skjølsvik
collection is cross-sectional in nature, and thus conclusions regarding (2016) found that purchasing professionals were guided by market
causality between constructs have to be made with caution. logic, such as creating effective markets, increasing transaction effi-
Nevertheless, the results suggest relatively strong associations, which ciency and using quantifiable selection criteria, when purchasing
are in line with the theory-based hypotheses, increasing the level of knowledge-intensive services. On the other hand, excessive focus on
confidence in the results. In addition to suggesting operationalizations these specific values and guidelines could blind them from seeing al-
of the attentional processing constructs for survey research, this re- ternative courses of action.
search provides novel insights into ambidexterity in PSM, and essen-
tially suggests that how purchasing and supply managers focus their Acknowledgements
attention reflects what they know, and further, what they know reflects
how they manage external resources. This makes the ABV an exciting This work was supported by the Finnish Foundation for Economic
new perspective for understanding strategic PSM. Education (Liikesivistysrahasto; grant no. 160171)

Appendix.A

Subjective statements and items used in the research

Top-down supply market attention


a. Defined ways of working guide our procurement decisions in a significant manner (e.g. supplier selection or collaboration).
b. Defined goals guide our discussions with suppliers in a significant manner.
c. Defined procurement requirements guide the monitoring of major supply markets in a significant manner.
d. Proven ways of working guide the identification of threats to our supply in a significant manner.
Bottom-up supply market attention a. We continuously engage in informal discussions with suppliers on markets, products or technologies, for example.
b. We continuously engage in informal discussions with the other functions of the organization (e.g. research and development).
c. We continuously monitor the development of major supply markets by, for example, visiting exhibitions and following news feeds and professional publications.
d. We actively communicate our sourcing requirements to potential suppliers.
e. We regularly participate in seminars and professional networks dedicated to purchasing and supply management.
Explorative PSM orientation (see Kilpi et al., 2018) a. We often try new tools and practices.
b. We continuously challenge current practices.
c. We continuously search for significant new solutions, such as products and technologies for the firm.
d. We continuously assess new suppliers and supply markets in order to replace existing supply arrangements.
Exploitative PSM orientation (see Kilpi et al. 2018) a. We focus on increasing efficiency in operations.
b. We determinedly focus on improving current practices.
c. We seek to determinedly standardize practices and processes.
d. We mostly develop procurement within the framework of existing practices and policies.
PSM status (see Kilpi et al., 2018) a. Purchasing is perceived as an important contributor for the competitiveness of the company
b. Purchasing is involved early on in the preparation of important decisions regarding the company
c. Purchasing functions’ views on purchasing policies are taken into consideration in other functions of the company.
d. Purchasing function’s suggestions for policy changes are considered as important in other functions suggestions within the company.

References desirability and feasibility factors. Strateg. Manag. J. 34 (6), 687–703.


Ben-Menahem, S.M., Kwee, Z., Volberda, H.W., Van Den Bosch, F.A., 2013. Strategic
renewal over time: the enabling role of potential absorptive capacity in aligning in-
Laureiro-Martínez, D., Brusoni, S., Canessa, N., Zollo, M., 2015. “Understanding the ex- ternal and external rates of change. Long. Range Plan. 46 (3), 216–235.
ploration–exploitation dilemma: an fMRI study of attention control and decision‐- Bierly III, P.E., Daly, P.S., 2007. Alternative knowledge strategies, competitive environ-
making performance”. Strateg. Manag. J. 36 (3), 319–338. ment, and organizational performance in small manufacturing firms. Entrep. Theory
Anderson, J.C., Gerbing, D.W., 1988. Structural equation modeling in practice: a review Pract. 31 (4), 493–516.
and recommended two-step approach. Psychol. Bull. 103 (3), 411. Birkinshaw, J., Gupta, K., 2013. Clarifying the distinctive contribution of ambidexterity to
Aoki, K., Wilhelm, M., 2016. Managing the productivity dilemma: how Toyota develops a the field of organization studies. Acad. Manag. Perspect. 27 (4), 287–298.
context for ambidexterity with suppliers. Acad. Manag. Proc. 2016 (1), 12780. Bode, C., Wagner, S.M., 2015. Structural drivers of upstream supply chain complexity and
Armstrong, J.S., Overton, T.S., 1977. Estimating nonresponse bias in mail surveys. J. the frequency of supply chain disruptions. J. Oper. Manag. 36, 215–228.
Mark. Res. 14 (3), 396–402. Bouquet, C., Birkinshaw, J., 2011. How global strategies emerge: an attention perspec-
Ashby W., R., 1958. Requisite variety and its implications for the control of complex tive. Global Strategy Journal 1 (3–4), 243–262.
systems. Cybernetica 1, 83–99. Bozarth, C.C., Warsing, D.P., Flynn, B.B., Flynn, E.J., 2009. The impact of supply chain
Aslam, H., Blome, C., Roscoe, S., Azhar, T.M., 2018. Dynamic supply chain capabilities: complexity on manufacturing plant performance. J. Oper. Manag. 27 (1), 78–93.
how market sensing, supply chain agility and adaptability affect supply chain am- Carr, A.S., Pearson, J.N., 2002. “The impact of purchasing and supplier involvement on
bidexterity. Int. J. Oper. Prod. Manag. 38 (12), 2266–2285. strategic purchasing and its impact on firm's performance”. Int. J. Oper. Prod. Manag.
Azadegan, A., Dooley, K.J., 2010. Supplier innovativeness, organizational learning styles 22 (9), 1032–1053.
and manufacturer performance: an empirical assessment. J. Oper. Manag. 28 (6), Carr, A.S., Smeltzer, L.R., 1997. An empirically based operational definition of strategic
488–505. purchasing. Eur. J. Purch. Supply Manag. 3 (4), 199–207.
Baier, C., Hartmann, E., Moser, R., 2008. Strategic alignment and purchasing efficacy: an Chen, I.J., Paulraj, A., Lado, A.A., 2004. Strategic purchasing, supply management, and
exploratory analysis of their impact on financial performance. J. Supply Chain firm performance. J. Oper. Manag. 22 (5), 505–523.
Manag. 44 (4), 36–52. Choi, T.Y., Krause, D.R., 2006. The supply base and its complexity: implications for
Barney, J.B., 2012. Purchasing, supply chain management and sustained competitive transaction costs, risks, responsiveness, and innovation. J. Oper. Manag. 24 (5),
advantage: the relevance of resource-based theory. J. Supply Chain Manag. 48 637–652.
(2), 3–6. Cousins, P.D., Menguc, B., 2006. The implications of socialization and integration in
Barreto, I., Patient, D.L., 2013. Toward a theory of intraorganizational attention based on supply chain management. J. Oper. Manag. 24 (5), 604–620.

9
H. Lorentz, et al. Journal of Purchasing and Supply Management 25 (2019) 100560

Cousins, P.D., Lawson, B., Petersen, K.J., Handfield, R.B., 2011. Breakthrough scanning, Lorentz, H., Töyli, J., Solakivi, T., Hälinen, H.M., Ojala, L., 2012. Effects of geographic
supplier knowledge exchange, and new product development performance. J. Prod. dispersion on intra-firm supply chain performance. Supply Chain Manag.: Int. J. 17
Innov. Manag. 28 (6), 930–942. (6), 611–626.
Cyert, R.M., March, J.G., 1963. A Behavioral Theory of the Firm, vol. 2. pp. 169–187 MacCallum, R.C., Browne, M.W., Sugawara, H.M., 1996. Power analysis and determi-
Englewood Cliffs, NJ. nation of sample size for covariance structure modelling. Psychol. Methods 1 (2),
Daft, R.L., Weick, K.E., 1984. Toward a model of organizations as interpretation systems. 130.
Acad. Manag. Rev. 9 (2), 284–295. March, J.G., 1991. Exploration and exploitation in organizational learning. Organ. Sci. 2
Day, G.S., Schoemaker, P.J., 2004. Driving through the fog: managing at the edge. Long. (1), 71–87.
Range Plan. 37 (2), 127–142. McNamara, G.M., Luce, R.A., Tompson, G.H., 2002. Examining the effect of complexity in
De Beuckelaer, A., Wagner, M., 2012. Small sample surveys: increasing rigor in supply strategic group knowledge structures on firm performance”. Strateg. Manag. J. 23
chain management research”. Int. J. Phys. Distrib. Logist. Manag. 42 (7), 615–639. (2), 153–170.
Ellram, L.M., 1995. Total cost of ownership: an analysis approach for purchasing. Int. J. McQuitty, S., 2004. Statistical power and structural equation models in business research.
Phys. Distrib. Logist. Manag. 25 (8), 4–23. J. Bus. Res. 57 (2), 175–183.
Ferrin, B.G., Plank, R.E., 2002. Total cost of ownership models: an exploratory study. J. Milgate, M., 2001. Supply chain complexity and delivery performance: an international
Supply Chain Manag. 38 (2), 18–29. exploratory study. Supply Chain Manag.: Int. J. 6 (3), 106–118.
Fornell, C., Larcker, D.F., 1981. Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable Mom, T.J., Van Den Bosch, F.A., Volberda, H.W., 2007. “Investigating managers' ex-
variables and measurement error. J. Mark. Res. 18 (1), 39–50. ploration and exploitation activities: the influence of top‐down, bottom‐up, and
Fuller, C.M., Simmering, M.J., Atinc, G., Atinc, Y., Babin, B.J., 2016. Common methods horizontal knowledge inflows”. J. Manag. Stud. 44 (6), 910–931.
variance detection in business research. J. Bus. Res. 69 (8), 3192–3198. Mora-Monge, C., Quesada, G., Gonzalez, M.E., Davis, J.M., 2019. Trust, power and supply
Gadde, L.E., Håkansson, H., 1994. The changing role of purchasing: reconsidering three chain integration in Web-enabled supply chains. Supply Chain Manag.: Int. J. 24 (4),
strategic issues. Eur. J. Purch. Supply Manag. 1 (1), 27–35. 524–539.
Garg, V.K., Walters, B.A., Priem, R.L., 2003. Chief executive scanning emphases, en- Nadkarni, S., Barr, P.S., 2008. Environmental context, managerial cognition, and strategic
vironmental dynamism, and manufacturing firm performance. Strateg. Manag. J. 24 action: an integrated view. Strateg. Manag. J. 29 (13), 1395–1427.
(8), 725–744. Newman, D.A., 2003. Longitudinal modeling with randomly and systematically missing
Garver, M.S., Mentzer, J.T., 1999. Logistics research methods: employing structural data: a simulation of ad hoc, maximum likelihood, and multiple imputation techni-
equation modeling to test for construct validity. J. Bus. Logist. 20 (1), 33–57. ques. Organ. Res. Methods 6 (3), 328–362.
Gibson, C.B., Birkinshaw, J., 2004. The antecedents, consequences, and mediating role of Nigam, A., Ocasio, W., 2010. Event attention, environmental sensemaking, and change in
organizational ambidexterity. Acad. Manag. J. 47 (2), 209–226. institutional logics: an inductive analysis of the effects of public attention to Clinton's
Gu, Q., Jitpaipoon, T., Yang, J., 2017. The impact of information integration on financial health care reform initiative. Organ. Sci. 21 (4), 823–841.
performance: a knowledge-based view. Int. J. Prod. Econ. 191, 221–232. Nissen, M.E., Sengupta, K., 2006. Incorporating Software Agents into Supply Chains:
Gualandris, J., Legenvre, H., Kalchschmidt, M., 2018. Exploration and exploitation within Experimental Investigation with a Procurement Task. MIS Quarterly, pp. 145–166.
supply networks: examining purchasing ambidexterity and its multiple performance Nunnally, J.C., 1978. Psychometric Theory, 2d edition. McGraw-Hill, New York, NY.
implications. Int. J. Oper. Prod. Manag. 38 (3), 667–689. Ocasio, W., Laamanen, T., Vaara, E., 2018. Communication and attention dynamics: an
Hambrick, D.C., 1981. Strategic awareness within top management teams. Strateg. attention-based view of strategic change. Strateg. Manag. J. 39 (1), 155–167.
Manag. J. 2 (3), 263–279. O'Reilly III, C.A., Tushman, M.L., 2004. The ambidextrous organization. Harv. Bus. Rev.
Hambrick, D.C., Finkelstein, S., Mooney, A.C., 2005. Executive job demands: new insights 82 (4), 74.
for explaining strategic decisions and leader behaviors. Acad. Manag. Rev. 30 (3), O'Reilly III, C.A., Tushman, M.L., 2013. Organizational ambidexterity: past, present, and
472–491. future. Acad. Manag. Perspect. 27 (4), 324–338.
Handfield, R.B., Cousins, P.D., Lawson, B., Petersen, K.J., 2015. “How can supply man- Ocasio, W., 1997. Towards an attention-based view of the firm. Strateg. Manag. J. 18
agement really improve performance? A knowledge‐based model of alignment cap- (Summer), 187–206.
abilities”. J. Supply Chain Manag. 51 (3), 3–17. Ocasio, W., 2011. Attention to attention. Organ. Sci. 22 (5), 1286–1296.
Hazen, B.T., Overstreet, R.E., Boone, C.A., 2015. Suggested reporting guidelines for Pagell, M., Wu, Z., 2009. Building a more complete theory of sustainable supply chain
structural equation modeling in supply chain management research. Int. J. Logist. management using case studies of 10 exemplars. J. Supply Chain Manag. 45 (2),
Manag. 26 (3), 627–641. 37–56.
Henseler, J., Ringle, C.M., Sarstedt, M., 2015. A new criterion for assessing discriminant Peeters, C., Dehon, C., Garcia-Prieto, P., 2015. The attention stimulus of cultural differ-
validity in variance-based structural equation modeling. J. Acad. Mark. Sci. 43 (1), ences in global services sourcing. J. Int. Bus. Stud. 46 (2), 241–251.
115–135. Pemer, F., Skjølsvik, T., 2016. Purchasing policy or purchasing police? The influence of
Hesping, F.H., Schiele, H., 2015. Purchasing strategy development: a multi-level review. institutional logics and power on responses to purchasing formalization. J. Supply
J. Purch. Supply Manag. 21 (2), 138–150. Chain Manag. 52 (4), 5–21.
Hoffman, A.J., 1999. Institutional evolution and change: environmentalism and the US Pihlajamaa, M., Kaipia, R., Säilä, J., Tanskanen, K., 2017. Can supplier innovations
chemical industry. Acad. Manag. J. 42 (4), 351–371. substitute for internal R&D? A multiple case study from an absorptive capacity per-
Hu, L., Bentler, P.M., 1999. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: spective”. J. Purch. Supply Manag. 23 (4), 242–255.
conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model.: a Multidisciplinary Podsakoff, P.M., Mackenzie, S.B., Lee, J.Y., Podsakoff, N.P., 2003. Common method
Journal 6 (1), 1–55. biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended
Johnsen, T.E., 2009. Supplier involvement in new product development and innovation: remedies. J. Appl. Psychol. 88 (5), 879–903.
taking stock and looking to the future. J. Purch. Supply Manag. 15 (3), 187–197. Podsakoff, P.M., MacKenzie, S.B., Podsakoff, N.P., 2012. Sources of method bias in social
Jöreskog, K.G., Sörbom, D., 1993. LISREL 8 User's Reference Guide. SPSS, Chicago. science research and recommendations on how to control it. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 63,
Kammerlander, N., Ganter, M., 2015. “An attention‐based view of family firm adaptation 539–569.
to discontinuous technological change: exploring the role of family CEOs' none- Porter, M.E., 1985. Competitive Advantage. Free Press, New York.
conomic goals”. J. Prod. Innov. Manag. 32 (3), 361–383. Quayle, M., 2002. Purchasing in small firms. Eur. J. Purch. Supply Manag. 8 (3), 151–159.
Kaufmann, L., Wagner, C.M., Carter, C.R., 2017. Individual modes and patterns of rational Raisch, S., Birkinshaw, J., 2008. Organizational ambidexterity: antecedents, outcomes,
and intuitive decision-making by purchasing managers. J. Purch. Supply Manag. 23 and moderators. J. Manag. 34 (3), 375–409.
(2), 82–93. Raisch, S., Birkinshaw, J., Probst, G., Tushman, M.L., 2009. Organizational ambidexterity:
Kauppila, O.P., 2010. Creating ambidexterity by integrating and balancing structurally balancing exploitation and exploration for sustained performance. Organ. Sci. 20 (4),
separate interorganizational partnerships. Strateg. Organ. 8 (4), 283–312. 685–695.
Kilpi, V., Lorentz, H., Solakivi, T., Malmsten, J., 2018. The effect of external supply Ramsay, J., 2001a. Purchasing's strategic irrelevance. Eur. J. Purch. Supply Manag. 7 (4),
knowledge acquisition, development activities and organizational status on the 257–263.
supply performance of SMEs”. J. Purch. Supply Manag. 24 (3), 247–259. Ramsay, J., 2001b. The resource based perspective, rents, and purchasing's contribution
Knoppen, D., Sáenz, M.J., 2015. Purchasing: can we bridge the gap between strategy and to sustainable competitive advantage. J. Supply Chain Manag. 37 (2), 38–47.
daily reality? Bus. Horiz. 58 (1), 123–133. Ren, C.R., Guo, C., 2011. “Middle managers' strategic role in the corporate en-
Krause, D.R., Pagell, M., Curkovic, S., 2001. Toward a measure of competitive priorities trepreneurial process: attention-based effects”. J. Manag. 37 (6), 1586–1610.
for purchasing. J. Oper. Manag. 19 (4), 497–512. Rerup, C., 2009. Attentional triangulation: learning from unexpected rare crises. Organ.
Krause, D.R., Vachon, S., Klassen, R.D., 2009. Special topic forum on sustainable supply Sci. 20 (5), 876–893.
chain management: introduction and reflections on the role of purchasing manage- Riedl, D.F., Kaufmann, L., Gaeckler, J., 2014. Statistical power of structural equation
ment. J. Supply Chain Manag. 45 (4), 18–25. models in SCM research. J. Purch. Supply Manag. 20 (3), 208–212.
Kristal, M.M., Huang, X., Roth, A.V., 2010. The effect of an ambidextrous supply chain Schorsch, T., Wallenburg, C.M., Wieland, A., 2017. The human factor in SCM: introducing
strategy on combinative competitive capabilities and business performance. J. Oper. a meta-theory of behavioral supply chain management. Int. J. Phys. Distrib. Logist.
Manag. 28 (5), 415–429. Manag. 47 (4), 238–262.
Lavie, D., Stettner, U., Tushman, M.L., 2010. Exploration and exploitation within and Seibert, S.E., Silver, S.R., Randolph, W.A., 2004. Taking empowerment to the next level: a
across organizations. Acad. Manag. Ann. 4 (1), 109–155. multiple-level model of empowerment, performance, and satisfaction. Acad. Manag.
Levinthal, D.A., March, J.G., 1993. The myopia of learning. Strateg. Manag. J. 14 (S2), J. 47 (3), 332–349.
95–112. Shepherd, D.A., Mcmullen, J.S., Ocasio, W., 2017. Is that an opportunity? An attention
Lindell, M.K., Whitney, D.J., 2001. Accounting for common method variance in cross- model of top managers' opportunity beliefs for strategic action. Strateg. Manag. J. 38
sectional research designs. J. Appl. Psychol. 86 (1), 114–121. (3), 626–644.
Liu, P., Li, Z., 2012. Task complexity: a review and conceptualization framework. Int. J. Simon, H.A., 1947. Administrative Behavior. The Macmillan Company, New York, NY.
Ind. Ergon. 42 (6), 553–568. Srai, J.S., Lorentz, H., 2019. Developing design principles for the digitalisation of

10
H. Lorentz, et al. Journal of Purchasing and Supply Management 25 (2019) 100560

purchasing and supply management. J. Purch. Supply Manag. 25 (1), 78–98. Voss, G.B., Voss, Z.G., 2013. Strategic ambidexterity in small and medium-sized en-
Turner, N., Aitken, J., Bozarth, C., 2018. A framework for understanding managerial terprises: implementing exploration and exploitation in product and market domains.
responses to supply chain complexity. Int. J. Oper. Prod. Manag. 38 (6), 1433–1466. Organ. Sci. 24 (5), 1459–1477.
Tushman, M.L., O'Reilly III, C.A., 1996. Ambidextrous organizations: managing evolu- Walrave, B., Romme, A.G.L., van Oorschot, K.E., Langerak, F., 2017. Managerial attention
tionary and revolutionary change. Calif. Manag. Rev. 38 (4), 8–29. to exploitation versus exploration: toward a dynamic perspective on ambidexterity.
Vachon, S., Klassen, R.D., 2002. An exploratory investigation of the effects of supply Ind. Corp. Chang. 26 (6), 1145–1160.
chain complexity on delivery performance. IEEE Trans. Eng. Manag. 49 (3), 218–230. Walsh, J.P., 1995. Managerial and organizational cognition: notes from a trip down
Van Weele, A.J., Van Raaij, E.M., 2014. The future of purchasing and supply management memory lane. Organ. Sci. 6 (3), 280–321.
research: about relevance and rigor. J. Supply Chain Manag. 50 (1), 56–72. Zheng, J., Knight, L., Harland, C., Humby, S., James, K., 2007. An analysis of research into
Voorhees, C.M., Brady, M.K., Calantone, R., Ramirez, E., 2016. Discriminant validity the future of purchasing and supply management. J. Purch. Supply Manag. 13 (1),
testing in marketing: an analysis, causes for concern, and proposed remedies. J. Acad. 69–83.
Mark. Sci. 44 (1), 119–134.

11

You might also like