You are on page 1of 9

816339

research-article2018
BBSXXX10.1177/2372732218816339Policy Insights From the Behavioral and Brain SciencesElleman and Oslund

Article

Policy Insights from the

Reading Comprehension Research:


Behavioral and Brain Sciences
2019, Vol. 6(1) 3­–11
© The Author(s) 2018
Implications for Practice and Policy Article reuse guidelines:
sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/2372732218816339
https://doi.org/10.1177/2372732218816339
journals.sagepub.com/home/bbs

Amy M. Elleman1 and Eric L. Oslund1

Abstract
Reading comprehension is one of the most complex cognitive activities in which humans engage, making it difficult to teach,
measure, and research. Despite decades of research in reading comprehension, international and national reading scores
indicate stagnant growth for U.S. adolescents. In this article, we review the theoretical and empirical research in reading
comprehension. We first explore different theoretical models for comprehension and then focus on components shown
to be important across models that represent potential targets for instruction. In the last part of the article, we consider
solutions for translating research to practice and policies for improving instruction. Improving reading scores will require
a concerted and collaborative effort by researchers, educators, and policy makers with a focus on long-term solutions. An
early and sustained focus on developing background knowledge, vocabulary, inference, and comprehension monitoring skills
across development will be necessary to improve comprehension.

Keywords
reading comprehension, instruction, theory, practice, policy

Tweet and National Assessment of Educational Progress (National


Assessment of Education Progress [NAEP]), students in the
To increase reading comprehension, educators will need to United States are unable to do relatively easy literacy tasks
provide early and sustained instruction in knowledge, vocab- such as locate relevant information to determine the main
ulary, inference generation, and comprehension monitoring. idea of a text or make simple inferences (Kastberg, Chan, &
Murray, 2016; National Center for Education Statistics,
Key Points 2017). According to the most recent PISA, U.S. adolescents
rank 15th in literacy skills. Results showed that 19% of the
•• Improving adolescent reading comprehension will 15-year-olds tested scored below a Level 2 (of 6) indicating
require a concerted effort from researchers, educators, they had difficulty with tasks such as locating explicitly
and policy makers to forgo short-term gains on mea- stated information, recognizing main ideas, and making low-
sures that tap low-level comprehension for long-term level inferences in a familiar topic. Only 10% of U.S. students
solutions that take years to develop. achieved a Level 5 indicating that they could organize several
•• An early and sustained focus on developing back- pieces of deeply embedded information, and engage in reflec-
ground knowledge, vocabulary, inference, and com- tive, evaluative, and interpretative tasks in unfamiliar topics.
prehension monitoring skills is necessary to improve Similarly, the NAEP scores showed that 64% of eighth grade
reading comprehension across grade levels. students read at or below a basic level. Unfortunately, these
•• Despite decades of reading comprehension research, a scores have remained relatively flat for many years and have
limited amount of time is spent using evidence-based led many educators, researchers, and policy makers to ques-
methods in classrooms. tion how well students are being prepared for a job market
•• Education leaders will need to strengthen teacher that increasingly requires self-learning, analytical skills, and
preparation programs and professional development transferable knowledge (e.g., Goldman & Pellegrino, 2015).
to ensure teachers are prepared to use evidence-based
practices to meet the literacy needs of their students.
1
Middle Tennessee State University, Murfreesboro, USA
Introduction Corresponding Author:
Amy M. Elleman, Middle Tennessee State University, P.O. Box 69,
According to national and international tests of literacy, such Murfreesboro, TN 37132, USA.
as the Program for International Student Assessment (PISA) Email: amy.elleman@mtsu.edu
4 Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6(1)

In this article, we briefly review the theoretical and empir- Snyder-Hogan, & Luciw-Dubas, 2010; Oslund, Clemens,
ical research in comprehension and consider the reasons for Simmons, & Simmons, 2018; Oslund, Clemens, Simmons,
stagnant scores in reading comprehension in the United Smith, & Simmons, 2016) have been fairly consistent in their
States. In the first part of the article, we explore different findings. These models indicate that vocabulary, both directly
theoretical models for comprehension and then focus on and indirectly, is consistently the strongest predictor of read-
malleable factors that have been shown to be important to ing comprehension for younger adolescents. Furthermore,
comprehension. We conclude with possible solutions for these models also demonstrated that, although not as strong
translating research to practice and policies for improving as vocabulary, inference-making and background knowledge
reading comprehension instruction. also had strong direct and indirect effects on comprehension.
As students get older, inference-making plays a stronger
direct role in comprehension than vocabulary (Cromley
The Complexity of Reading
et al., 2010, replicated by Ahmed et al., 2016). Across stud-
Comprehension ies, vocabulary, inference-making, and background knowl-
Reading comprehension is one of the most complex behav- edge all influence, both directly and indirectly, reading
iors in which humans engage. Reading theorists have grap- comprehension from adolescent to young adult readers.
pled with how to comprehensively and meaningfully portray Whereas models such as the SVR and DIME have
reading comprehension and many different theoretical mod- attempted to identify underlying components to reading
els have been proposed in recent decades (McNamara & comprehension, other theorists have examined the process of
Magliano, 2009; Perfetti & Stafura, 2014). These models reading comprehension. Most theories of comprehension
range from broad theoretical models depicting the relation- align with Kintsch’s (1988) construction integration (CI)
ships and interactions among comprehension subcompo- model. During the construction phase, information from the
nents to models of specific comprehension processes. We text and the readers’ related knowledge automatically acti-
review different frameworks and models that have signifi- vate. In the subsequent integration phase, activation spreads
cantly impacted theory development, reading comprehen- throughout the memory network, settling on concepts with
sion research, and instruction. greater activation and more links to other concepts while
One framework, the Simple View of Reading (SVR), pos- suppressing weakly linked concepts. This process occurs
its that reading comprehension is the product of word decod- iteratively as the reader processes ideas in a text. In this way,
ing and linguistic comprehension (Gough & Tunmer, 1986). readers integrate information from the text with their back-
Across many languages, research has shown that reading ground knowledge to form an overall mental representation.
comprehension can be explained by individual differences in Similarly, the RAND reading model, another influential
these two components, though the relative relationship of the reading framework for research and practice, defined reading
components changes over time (Catts, 2018). Early in devel- comprehension as the process of “extracting and construct-
opment, decoding is more closely associated with reading ing meaning through interaction and involvement with writ-
comprehension than linguistic competence, but once decod- ten language” (RAND Reading Study Group, 2002, p. 11).
ing is mastered, linguistic comprehension becomes a better Specifically, reading comprehension is the interaction
predictor of reading comprehension (e.g., Catts, Adlof, & between reader, text, and task characteristics within a socio-
Weismer, 2005). The SVR has been useful to researchers and cultural context. This model highlights the context-depen-
practitioners by providing a framework for understanding dent nature of comprehension. A person may achieve a clear
different profiles of struggling readers including students understanding of a text when the text is easy and the task
who struggle primarily due to word-level problems (i.e., dys- simple (e.g., answering multiple choice questions), but the
lexic), comprehension issues (i.e., poor comprehender), or same reader may struggle when encountering complex text
both (i.e., garden variety poor reader). As useful as the model on an unfamiliar topic.
has been, it does not explicate the subcomponents of lan-
guage or cognitive processes that underlie reading compre- Effective Reading Comprehension
hension (Catts, 2018).
Reading comprehension requires the coordination of mul-
Instruction
tiple linguistic and cognitive processes including, but not Many linguistic abilities, cognitive processes, and knowl-
limited to, word reading ability, working memory, inference edge sources undergird comprehension ability complicating
generation, comprehension monitoring, vocabulary, and which comprehension components instruction should target
prior knowledge (Perfetti, Landi, & Oakhill, 2005). The mul- (Perfetti & Adlof, 2012). To identify which components are
tifaceted nature of reading comprehension is reflected in worth measuring, some components serve as “pressure
component models that consider subcomponents of compre- points” that, if changed, would significantly impact students’
hension. Component models such as the direct and inferen- comprehension ability. Such components should be integral
tial mediation model (DIME; Ahmed et al., 2016; Cromley, to reading comprehension, vary across individuals, and
Elleman and Oslund 5

represent malleable instructional targets (Perfetti & Adlof, supports relevant comprehension and learning (Barnes,
2012). With this in mind, we briefly review four components Dennis, & Haefele-Kalvaitis, 1996). Readers with more prior
of reading comprehension (i.e., inference, knowledge, knowledge consistently outperform readers with less, indi-
vocabulary, comprehension monitoring) that play prominent cating that increased background knowledge in an area may
roles across theories of reading comprehension, are integral help less skilled readers compensate for a general compre-
for understanding text, and represent potentially malleable hension deficit (Schneider, Körkel, & Weinert, 1989). Prior
targets for instruction. This review focuses on higher order knowledge of a domain predicts text recall for students
comprehension skills but acknowledges the foundational across development and supports the ability to make infer-
role that efficient word recognition plays in reading compre- ences (Recht & Leslie, 1988) and learn new words (Kaefer,
hension (see Perfetti & Stafura, 2014). Neuman, & Pinkham, 2015).
Although educators acknowledge the important role of
knowledge in comprehension, very little time in early ele-
Inference Generation
mentary school is focused on informational text (Duke,
Inference generation, the ability to integrate information 2000). According to survey research reported in the National
within or across texts using background knowledge to fill in Survey of Science and Mathematics Education, elementary
information not explicitly stated, is an essential component teachers spend over 80 min on language arts instruction a
of language comprehension (Kendeou, McMaster, & Christ, day compared to an average of 21 min in science and 18 min
2016; Kintsch, 1988). Inference generation is a general skill in social studies (Banilower et al., 2013). This is compounded
important for communication and learning at all stages of for less skilled readers who have knowledge deficits
development. When prompted, even preschool age children (Compton, Miller, Gilbert, & Steacy, 2013), difficulties
can generate causal inferences about events (van den Broek, understanding expository text (Saenz & Fuchs, 2002), and
Lorch, & Thurlow, 1996). Inference ability has been shown are often pulled from content area classes for additional read-
to be a unique predictor of reading comprehension across ing instruction (Banilower et al., 2013).
developmental stages (Barth, Barnes, Francis, Vaughn, & To alleviate these issues, well-designed programs can
York, 2015). Comparing good and poor comprehenders integrate domain knowledge acquisition and comprehension
(matched on decoding and vocabulary) shows differences on instruction. A recent meta-analysis of integrated science and
inferential tasks at the word, sentence, and passage level reading programs found a moderate effect for both science
(Cain & Oakhill, 1999). Compared to skilled comprehenders, and reading outcomes (Talbert, Parrish, & Elleman, 2016).
poor comprehenders demonstrate difficulties with generating Programs focusing on social studies content have also
topic-related inferences, integrating words into context, shown success at promoting knowledge acquisition and
resolving contextual references, and answering inference reading comprehension (e.g., Guthrie & Klauda, 2014;
questions in a logical manner (Long, Oppy, & Seely, 1994; Vaughn et al., 2013).
Perfetti & Stafura, 2014).
Not only does inference ability predict reading compre-
hension, it is also malleable through instruction. In a meta-
Vocabulary
analysis of inference intervention studies, teaching inferences Vocabulary is a robust predictor of reading comprehension
improved general comprehension as well as inferential and across development. Children acquire vocabulary at an
literal comprehension skills (Elleman, 2017). Most interven- astounding rate, on average 2 to 8 root words per day
tions included fewer than 10 hr of instruction, implying that (Biemiller & Slonim, 2001). Once children start indepen-
teaching inference strategies is useful, and extended practice dently reading, vocabulary acquisition becomes dependent
as a context-independent skill may not be necessary on exposure to print, not oral language or instruction.
(Willingham, 2017). Effective inference instructional tech- Compared to written language, oral language experiences do
niques include teaching students to use their background not provide enough unknown words to foster substantial
knowledge and integrate it with the information in the text, vocabulary growth (Hayes & Ahrens, 1988). Explicit instruc-
self-generated elaborations, graphic organizers that connect tion has also been ruled out as a significant factor in vocabu-
concepts to one another, and text clues (e.g., Elbro & Buch- lary acquisition because children are instructed on only a
Iversen, 2013; Kendeou et al., 2016). fraction of the words it would take to accrue the 40,000
words estimated to be known by the average high school stu-
dent (Stahl & Nagy, 2006). Most words are learned implic-
Background Knowledge itly through repeated exposures in multiple contexts over
A reader’s background knowledge is necessary in building a time (Landauer & Dumais, 1997).
coherent representation of a text. Well-connected memory Unfortunately, large individual differences in vocabulary
storage facilitates quicker retrieval and use of relevant infor- size exist in early readers and persist through elementary
mation (Kintsch & Rawson, 2005). Prior content knowledge school (Biemiller & Slonim, 2001). By the end of second
6 Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6(1)

grade, disadvantaged students can lag 2 years behind their struggling readers (e.g., Gersten, Fuchs, Williams, & Baker,
average peers and 4 years behind those in the upper quartile 2001). Comprehension strategy instruction is beneficial
on vocabulary knowledge. An intense focus on early and con- when multiple strategies are taught, strategies are explicitly
tinued vocabulary learning makes sense to ameliorate these modeled, and students gradually assume more responsibility
differences, but it is unclear if enough vocabulary can be in using strategies independently (Pearson & Dole, 1987).
explicitly taught to impact students’ general comprehension.
Two reviews examining the impact of vocabulary instruction
Implications for Policy
on comprehension found gains on comprehension measures
that contained the taught words, but not for general compre- Having covered some contemporary theories and research in
hension measures (Elleman, Lindo, Morphy, & Compton, reading comprehension, we consider why reading scores for
2009; Wright & Cervetti, 2017). Considering only researcher U.S. adolescents have stagnated. We then discuss policy
designed comprehension measures, while all readers implications and suggest recommendations to improve read-
benefited from vocabulary instruction, less skilled readers ing comprehension.
benefited more than typical readers, highlighting the impor-
tance of vocabulary for these students (Elleman et al., 2009).
Interactive approaches to word learning appear more
Curriculum and Instruction
effective than ones that rely on definitional types of instruc- Knowledge and vocabulary development.  Comprehension strat-
tion (Wright & Cervetti, 2017). However, some studies show egy instruction is one of the most highly recommended
gains with very little instructional time spent per word (in instructional methods for improving comprehension (e.g.,
some cases less than a minute per word), suggesting that at NICHD, 2000). Critics, however, contend that an overem-
least some type of vocabulary instruction is better than none. phasis on strategy instruction at the expense of knowledge
Thus, if the instructional goal in a lesson is to improve com- building has led to the stagnation of scores (Willingham,
prehension, providing brief explanations of words before or 2006). Also, strategy instruction may result in a shallow rep-
during reading may be an efficient way to promote word resentation of a text and interfere with deeper processing of
learning and increase text comprehension. Other generative the content (Compton, Miller, Elleman, & Steacy, 2014).
word learning strategies that go beyond learning individual Often, the focus of instruction becomes the strategy itself
words—such as learning to use context to derive word mean- (e.g., learning how to make main idea statements in different
ings (Fukkink & deGlopper, 1998) and morphological analy- texts), instead of flexibly using the strategy to build a coher-
sis (Goodwin & Ahn, 2013)—also improve word learning. In ent representation of the texts being read. Early reviews of
addition, leveraging knowledge networks may be another strategy instruction revealed that no matter which strategies
way to more efficiently teach vocabulary and knowledge were combined in a program, effects were the same (Rosen-
simultaneously (Neuman & Wright, 2014). shine & Meister, 1994), suggesting that the strategies them-
selves might not be the causal mechanism for increasing
comprehension, but instead a third variable such as increased
Comprehension Monitoring and Strategy engagement and comprehension monitoring might be
Instruction responsible (Wilkinson & Son, 2011). As noted, knowledge
Comprehension monitoring is a metacognitive skill that is a key factor in comprehension, and young students get
refers to readers’ ability to reflect on their understanding of a little content exposure in the elementary grades. Some critics
written text (Language and Reading Research Consortium & cite the pressures from No Child Left Behind (NCLB) legis-
Yeomans-Maldonado, 2017). Strategic monitoring of text lation, which they believe unintentionally narrowed the cur-
understanding matters (Oakhill, Hartt, & Samols, 2005). The riculum and made school seek short-term gains focusing on
ability to monitor comprehension correlates with reading skills and strategies to increase standardized test scores
comprehension and increases over development (Language (Jones & Workman, 2016).
and Reading Research Consortium & Yeomans-Maldonado, Knowledge and vocabulary acquisition take time to
2017). Young readers and less skilled readers demonstrate develop. Given this, an early and sustained focus on knowl-
weaknesses in detecting inconsistencies within a text edge and vocabulary acquisition is necessary across all grade
(Oakhill et al., 2005). Readers must be able to monitor and levels. The Common Core State Standards in English
use fix-up strategies when comprehension breaks down. Language Arts (CCSS/ELA), adopted by most U.S. states,
The intent of comprehension strategy instruction is to reflect this focus on knowledge acquisition by recommend-
teach students to actively monitor their comprehension and ing an increase in informational text at all grade levels with
employ an appropriate strategy to make sense of the text. an increasing emphasis on informational text as students get
Comprehension strategy instruction improves comprehen- older (Cervetti & Hiebert, 2015). Yet, standards do not guide
sion for typically developing (e.g., National Institute of Child curricular decisions about content. Domain knowledge must
Health and Human Development [NICHD], 2000) and be thoughtfully presented as concepts build on one another
Elleman and Oslund 7

over time. Similarly, vocabulary instruction must be broad therefore, ready to understand texts when they enter college
and include systematic instruction in academic vocabulary or the workforce (Hiebert & Mesmer, 2013).
and word learning strategies. Rich instruction in knowledge However, some questions about text complexity remain
acquisition and vocabulary development must start as early unresolved. Some researchers are concerned about the poten-
as possible and continue throughout children’s school career. tial negative impact of increasing text difficulty at younger
ages or with less skilled readers (Hiebert & Mesmer, 2013).
Strategy instruction.  Although it would be easy to blame strat- Most states allow different types of text complexity measure-
egy instruction as the reason for stagnant scores, strategy ment systems to ensure students are exposed to increasingly
instruction does effectively increase comprehension. Also, complex texts over time. We know little about the impact of
students with poor comprehension are passive in applying increasing text complexity on comprehension, nor do we
their knowledge and demonstrate difficulties making infer- know the impact of using different measurement systems.
ences, even when they have the necessary background Although some methods help to identify increases in text
knowledge to do so (Barnes et al., 1996). So, increasing complexity features like sentence length and word frequency,
background knowledge and vocabulary is only part of the they do little to help teachers identify sets of texts that are
answer to improving reading comprehension. Students also topically relevant and cohesive. Text complexity measure-
need to become strategic readers who can independently ment systems that help teachers choose challenging texts and
learn from text. What is unclear from research is how to do provide information about conceptual overlap among texts
this most effectively. We know very little about when to would be useful to ensure students are exposed to complex
teach certain strategies in development. As readers develop, text that supports efficient and effective knowledge and
they require more specialized strategies for engaging in text. vocabulary acquisition (see Compton et al., 2014).
Content such as history, science, and mathematics require
different types of text analysis skills (Shanahan & Shanahan,
2008). It is unclear when it is most beneficial to start teach-
Assessment
ing advanced disciplinary strategies. This review focuses on comprehension theory and instruc-
Another question to resolve is how much time to spend on tion, but measurement is crucial in comprehension research
strategies. As noted, just a few hours of inference instruction and practice. The complexity of comprehension makes it dif-
can be effective (Elleman, 2017), suggesting extended prac- ficult to capture. Indeed, comprehension tests are not highly
tice may be unnecessary (Willingham, 2017). Research correlated with one another, differentially measure underly-
should examine which strategies are most generally effec- ing skills, and cannot reliably identify children with compre-
tive, which specific strategies at what ages, and how long hension difficulties across different tests (Cutting &
instruction should last. Awaiting more information, practitio- Scarborough, 2006; Keenan & Meenan, 2014). Therefore,
ners should teach inference generation and comprehension research and educational decision making must use multiple
monitoring across development, while simultaneously teach- tests and interpret their scores based on the specific test’s
ing vocabulary and domain knowledge. characteristics.
To better understand the contextualized nature of compre-
Text complexity.  Another explanation for the lack of growth hension and provide more information on existing compre-
in adolescent reading comprehension is that students are hension measures, researchers have started modeling the
being exposed to less complex text than in the past (Adams, complex interactions between readers, text, and tasks on dif-
2011). A 2-year gap separates the complexity of 12th grade ferent comprehension measures using the RAND framework
high school student texts and texts for college freshman. (Kulesz et al., 2016; Miller et al., 2014). For instance, in a
What distinguishes students on some standardized tests is the study examining reader, text, and test characteristics (Kulesz
ability to understand more difficult passages (ACT, 2006; et al., 2016) adolescent readers with better vocabulary and
Kulesz, Francis, Barnes, & Fletcher, 2016). Variability in background knowledge outperformed others on a standard-
text difficulty impacts reading comprehension (McNamara, ized reading comprehension measure. Also, the types of
Graesser, & Louwerse, 2012). Certain linguistic text features questions asked on the test (e.g., literal and inferential) did
make a text more difficult, such as use of rarer words and not predict item difficulty, but the type of text did matter
longer sentences (Williamson, Fitzgerald, & Stenner, 2013). (expository passages were more difficult than narrative pas-
Other factors—such as referential cohesion (i.e., the overlap sages), underscoring the need to consider the contextual
of concepts across a text), deep cohesion (i.e., words that dependencies at play when measuring comprehension.
connect the relationship between ideas in the text), and text Most researchers and practitioners would agree that most
type (e.g., narrative, expository)—also affect comprehension standardized reading comprehension measures fall short of
(McNamara et al., 2012). Consequently, the CCSS dedicates fully capturing the process of comprehension (Fuchs et al.,
an entire standard to text complexity, to ensure students are 2018). In addition to analyzing current measures, researchers
exposed to increasingly difficult text across grades and are, and education leaders should design measures that go beyond
8 Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6(1)

the low-level skills tapped by multiple choice questions and and widely disseminated using this research framework
closed-ended items to measures that provide information (Snow, 2015). These types of on-going partnerships between
about students’ deeper processing of text. As part of the practitioners and researchers may help shrink the research-
Reading for Understanding (RFU) initiative funded by the to-practice gap in literacy by producing effective interven-
Institutes of Education Sciences (IES), one research team has tions that practitioners want to use.
designed a measure intended to capture deeper comprehen- In addition to developing and disseminating effective
sion than current multiple choice tests (Sabatini, O’Reilly, research practices and programs, teacher preparation and
Halderman, & Bruce, 2014). The Global Integrated Scenario- professional development will need to be strengthened to
based Assessments (GISA) require students to integrate improve reading comprehension instruction. One first step is
information across multiple materials to complete a literacy- for all states to require that teachers are knowledgeable about
based task (e.g., designing a website) and defend a position evidence-based literacy practices and have expertise in the
based on what they learn. ESSA (2015) requires that states content they plan to teach. Ensuring that all teachers meet
use assessments that measure higher order thinking. these standards will require collaboration among policy
Besides globally measuring deep learning, few diagnostic makers, state and district leaders, teacher preparation pro-
tests pinpoint specific difficulties children encounter in com- grams, and accrediting agencies.
prehension (Francis et al., 2006). These tests are necessary,
so teachers can identify areas of needed support, provide
appropriate interventions, and monitor students’ progress
Concluding Remarks
(Kendeou et al., 2016). Developing and testing these types of Reading comprehension is complex and multifaceted, mak-
measures across grades and types of learners is necessary to ing it difficult to improve. Stagnant scores of adolescents are
move comprehension instruction forward. likely due to multiple reasons including the ones outlined in
this article. Improving adolescent reading comprehension
will require a concerted effort from researchers, educators,
Translating Comprehension Research to Practice and policymakers to forgo short-term gains on measures that
The inadequate translation of research to practice may also tap low-level comprehension for long-term solutions that
contribute to stagnant adolescent reading scores. Despite take years to develop. An early and sustained focus on devel-
decades of experimental research in reading comprehension oping background knowledge, vocabulary, inference, and
(Scammacca et al., 2016), little classroom time is used to comprehension monitoring skills is necessary to improve
teach evidence-based comprehension practices (Swanson reading comprehension across grade levels.
et al., 2016). Current policies advocate evidence-based
instructional policies. ESSA emphasizes evidence-based Declaration of Conflicting Interests
instructional methods, as first proposed by the NCLB. The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect
However, these methods are not making their way into prac- to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
titioners’ hands. One federal agency, IES, has taken an active
role in the promotion of evidence-based practices through Funding
the evaluation of programs (e.g., What Works Clearinghouse)
The author(s) received no financial support for the research, author-
and the dissemination of findings (e.g., practitioner-friendly ship, and/or publication of this article.
practice guides containing practices and strategies vetted
through science). IES has also supported important research ORCID iD
initiatives in reading comprehension such as the Reading for
Amy M. Elleman https://orcid.org/0000-0001-6686-589X
Understanding initiatives in which interdisciplinary research
teams examined different aspects of development in lan-
guage and reading development. The research findings from References
these studies are just now being published, but the question ACT. (2006). Reading between the lines: What the ACT reveals
remains on whether the findings will be successfully trans- about college readiness in reading. Iowa City, IA: Author.
lated to regular classroom practice. Adams, M. J. (2011). Advancing our students’ language and lit-
Translating research to practice is complex and will eracy: The challenge of complex texts. American Educator,
34(4), 3-11.
require a multipronged approach. One area of growing inter-
Ahmed, Y., Francis, D. J., York, M., Fletcher, J. M., Barnes, M.,
est is the use of practice-embedded educational research & Kulesz, P. (2016). Validation of the direct and inferen-
(Snow, 2015) in which researchers and practitioners partner tial mediation (DIME) model of reading comprehension in
to identify solutions for real problems in practice settings grades 7 through 12. Contemporary Educational Psychology,
using rigorous research methods. Evidence-based programs 44,68-82.
for increasing vocabulary and comprehension (e.g., Word Banilower, E. R., Smith, P. S., Weiss, I. R., Malzahn, K. A.,
Generation and STARI) have been successfully developed Campbell, K. M., & Weis, A. M. (2013). Report of the 2012
Elleman and Oslund 9

national survey of science and mathematics education. Chapel and less skilled readers: A meta-analytic review. Journal of
Hill, NC: Horizon Research. Educational Psychology, 109, 761-781.
Barnes, M. A., Dennis, M., & Haefele-Kalvaitis, J. (1996). The Elleman, A. M., Lindo, E. J., Morphy, P., & Compton, D. L. (2009).
effects of knowledge availability and knowledge accessibil- The impact of vocabulary instruction on passage-level com-
ity on coherence and elaborative inferencing in children from prehension of school-age children: A meta-analysis. Journal of
six to fifteen years of age. Journal of Experimental Child Research on Educational Effectiveness, 2, 1-44.
Psychology, 61, 216-241. ESSA. (2015). Every Student Succeeds Act of 2015, Pub. L. No.
Barth, A. E., Barnes, M., Francis, D., Vaughn, S., & York, M. (2015). 114-95 § 114 Stat. 1177.
Inferential processing among adequate and struggling adolescent Francis, D. J., Snow, C. E., August, D., Carlson, C. D., Miller, J.,
comprehenders and relations to reading comprehension. Reading & Iglesias, A. (2006). Measures of reading comprehension: A
and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 28, 587-609. latent variable analysis of the diagnostic assessment of reading
Biemiller, A., & Slonim, N. (2001). Estimating root word vocabu- comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10, 301-322.
lary growth in normative and advantaged populations: Evidence Fuchs, D., Hendricks, E., Walsh, M. E., Fuchs, L. S., Gilbert, J.
for a common sequence of vocabulary acquisition. Journal of K., Zhang Tracy, W., . . . Peng, P. (2018). Evaluating a mul-
Educational Psychology, 93, 498-520. tidimensional reading comprehension program and reconsid-
Cain, K., & Oakhill, J. V. (1999). Inference making ability and its ering the lowly reputation of tests of near-transfer. Learning
relation to comprehension failure in young children. Reading Disabilities Research & Practice, 33, 11-23.
and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 11, 489-503. Fukkink, R. G., & deGlopper, K. (1998). Effects of instruction in
doi:10.1023/A:1008084120205 deriving word meaning from context: A meta-analysis. Review
Catts, H. W. (2018). The simple view of reading: Advancements of Educational Research, 68, 450-469.
and false impressions. Remedial and Special Education, 39, Gersten, R., Fuchs, L. S., Williams, J. P., & Baker, S. (2001).
317-323. Teaching reading comprehension strategies to students
Catts, H. W., Adlof, S. M., & Weismer, S. E. (2005). Language with learning disabilities: A review of research. Review of
deficits in poor comprehenders: A case for the simple view of Educational Research, 71, 279-320.
reading. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, Goldman, S. R., & Pellegrino, J. W. (2015). Research on learning
49, 278-293. and instruction: Implications for curriculum, instruction, and
Cervetti, G. N., & Hiebert, E. H. (2015). The sixth pillar of reading assessment. Policy Insights From the Behavioral and Brain
instruction: Knowledge development. The Reading Teacher, Sciences, 2, 33-41.
68, 548-551. Goodwin, A. P., & Ahn, S. (2013). A meta-analysis of morphologi-
Compton, D. L., Miller, A. C., Elleman, A. M., & Steacy, L. M. cal interventions in English: Effects on literacy outcomes for
(2014). Have we forsaken reading theory in the name of school-age children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17, 257-285.
“quick fix” interventions for children with reading disability? Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and
Scientific Studies of Reading, 18, 55-73. reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7(1), 6-10.
Compton, D. L., Miller, A. C., Gilbert, J. K., & Steacy, L. M. Guthrie, J. T., & Klauda, S. L. (2014). Effects of classroom prac-
(2013). What can be learned about the reading comprehension tices on reading comprehension, engagement, and motivations
of poor readers through the use of advanced statistical model- for adolescents. Reading Research Quarterly, 49, 387-416.
ing techniques. In B. Miller, L. Cutting, & P. McCardle (Eds.), Hayes, D. P., & Ahrens, M. G. (1988). Vocabulary simplifications
Unraveling the behavioral, neurobiological, & genetic com- for children: A special case of “motherese”? Journal of Child
ponents of reading comprehension (pp. 135-147). Baltimore, Language, 15, 395-410.
MD: Paul H. Brookes. Hiebert, E. H., & Mesmer, H. A. E. (2013). Upping the ante of text
Cromley, J. G., Snyder-Hogan, L. E., & Luciw-Dubas, U. A. complexity in the Common Core State Standards: Examining
(2010). Reading comprehension of scientific text: A domain- its potential impact on young readers. Educational Researcher,
specific test of the direct and inferential mediation model of 42, 44-51.
reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, Jones, S. D., & Workman, E. (2016). ESSA’s well-rounded edu-
102, 687-700. cation. Retrieved from http://knowledgematterscampaign.org
Cutting, L. E., & Scarborough, H. S. (2006). Prediction of reading /seize-the-day/
comprehension: Relative contributions of word recognition, Kaefer, T., Neuman, S. B., & Pinkham, A. M. (2015). Pre-existing
language proficiency, and other cognitive skills can depend on background knowledge influences socioeconomic differences
how comprehension is measured. Scientific Studies of Reading, in preschoolers’ word learning and comprehension. Reading
10, 277-299. Psychology, 36, 203-231.
Duke, N. K. (2000). 3.6 minutes per day: The scarcity of infor- Kastberg, D., Chan, J. Y., & Murray, G. (2016). Performance of
mational texts in first grade. Reading Research Quarterly, 35, U.S. 15-year-old students in science, reading, and mathemat-
202-224. ics literacy in an international context: First look at PISA
Elbro, C., & Buch-Iversen, I. (2013). Activation of background 2015 (NCES 2017-048). Washington, DC: National Center for
knowledge for inference making: Effects on reading compre- Education Statistics, U.S. Department of Education. Retrieved
hension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17, 435-452. doi:10.108 from http://nces.ed.gov/pubsearch
0/10888438.2013.774005 Keenan, J. M., & Meenan, C. E. (2014). Test differences in diag-
Elleman, A. M. (2017). Examining the impact of inference instruc- nosing reading comprehension deficits. Journal of Learning
tion on the literal and inferential comprehension of skilled Disabilities, 47, 125-135.
10 Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6(1)

Kendeou, P., McMaster, K. L., & Christ, T. J. (2016). Reading com- middle-school students from low socioeconomic backgrounds
prehension core components and processes. Policy Insights influences comprehension processes and outcomes. Learning
From the Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3, 62-69. and Individual Differences, 45, 159-165.
Kintsch, W. (1988). The role of knowledge in discourse compre- Pearson, P. D., & Dole, J. A. (1987). Explicit comprehension
hension: A construction-integration model. Psychological instruction: A review of research and a new conceptualization
Review, 95, 163-182. of instruction. The Elementary School Journal, 88, 151-165.
Kintsch, W., & Rawson, K. A. (2005). Comprehension. In M. J. doi:10.1086/461530
Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A hand- Perfetti, C. A., & Adlof, S. M. (2012). Reading comprehension:
book (pp. 209-226). Oxford, UK: Blackwell. A conceptual framework from word meaning to text meaning.
Kulesz, P. A., Francis, D. J., Barnes, M. A., & Fletcher, J. M. In J. Sabatini, E. Albro, & T. O’Reilly (Eds.), Measuring up:
(2016). The influence of properties of the test and their inter- Advances in how to assess reading ability (pp. 3-20). Lanham,
actions with reader characteristics on reading comprehension: MD: Rowman & Littlefield Education.
An explanatory item response study. Journal of Educational Perfetti, C. A., Landi, N., & Oakhill, J. V. (2005). The acquisition
Psychology, 108, 1078-1097. of reading comprehension skill. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme
Landauer, T. K., & Dumais, S. T. (1997). A solution to Plato’s (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 227-247).
problem: The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, Oxford, UK: Blackwell. doi:10.1002/9780470757642.ch13
induction, and representation of knowledge. Psychological Perfetti, C. A., & Stafura, J. (2014). Word knowledge in a theory of
Review, 104, 211-240. reading comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18, 22-37.
Language and Reading Research Consortium, Yeomans-Maldonado, RAND Reading Study Group. (2002). Reading for understand-
G. (2017). Development of comprehension monitoring in begin- ing: Toward an R&D program in reading comprehension.
ner readers. Reading and Writing, 30, 2039-2067. Washington, DC: RAND Education.
Long, D. L., Oppy, B. J., & Seely, M. R. (1994). Individual differ- Recht, D. R., & Leslie, L. (1988). Effect of prior knowledge on
ences in the time course of inferential processing. Journal of good and poor readers’ memory of text. Journal of Educational
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, Psychology, 80, 16-20. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.80.1.16
20, 1456-1470. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.20.6.1456 Rosenshine, B., & Meister, C. (1994). Reciprocal teaching: A review
McNamara, D. S., Graesser, A. C., & Louwerse, M. M. (2012). of the research. Review of Educational Research, 64, 479-530.
Sources of text difficulty: Across genres and grades. In J. Sabatini, J. P., O’Reilly, T., Halderman, L. K., & Bruce, K. (2014).
Sabatini, E. Albro, & T. O’Reilly (Eds.), Measuring up: Integrating scenario-based and component reading skill mea-
Advances in how to assess reading ability (pp. 3-20). Lanham, sures to understand the reading behavior of struggling readers.
MD: Rowman & Littlefield Education. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 29, 36-43.
McNamara, D. S., & Magliano, J. (2009). Toward a comprehen- Saenz, L. M., & Fuchs, L. S. (2002). Examining the reading difficulty
sive model of comprehension. Psychology of Learning and of secondary students with learning disabilities expository ver-
Motivation, 51, 297-384. doi:10.1016/S00797421(09)51009-2 sus narrative text. Remedial and Special Education, 23, 31-41.
Miller, A. C., Davis, N., Gilbert, J. K., Cho, S. J., Toste, J. R., Street, Scammacca, N. K., Roberts, G. J., Cho, E., Williams, K. J., Roberts,
J., & Cutting, L. E. (2014). Novel approaches to examine pas- G., Vaughn, S. R., & Carroll, M. (2016). A century of progress:
sage, student, and question effects on reading comprehension. Reading interventions for students in grades 4–12, 1914–2014.
Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 29, 25-35. Review of Educational Research, 86, 756-800.
National Center for Education Statistics. (2017). The nation’s Schneider, W., Körkel, J., & Weinert, F. E. (1989). Domain-specific
report card: Reading 2017. Washington, DC: National Center knowledge and memory performance: A comparison of high-
for Education Statistics, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. and low-aptitude children. Journal of Educational Psychology,
Department of Education. 81, 306-312.
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. Shanahan, T., & Shanahan, C. (2008). Teaching disciplinary liter-
(2000). Report of the national reading panel (NIH Publication acy to adolescents: Rethinking content-area literacy. Harvard
No. 00-4754). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health Educational Review, 78, 40-59.
and Human Services. Snow, C. E. (2015). 2014 Wallace Foundation Distinguished
Neuman, S. B., & Wright, T. S. (2014). The magic of words: Lecture: Rigor and realism: Doing educational science in the
Teaching vocabulary in the early childhood classroom. real world. Educational Researcher, 44, 460-466.
American Educator, 38(2), 4-13. Stahl, S. A., & Nagy, W. E. (2006). Teaching word meanings.
Oakhill, J., Hartt, J., & Samols, D. (2005). Levels of comprehension Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.
monitoring and working memory in good and poor compre- Swanson, E., Wanzek, J., McCulley, L., Stillman-Spisak, S.,
henders. Reading and Writing, 18, 657-686. Vaughn, S., Simmons, D., . . . Hairrell, A. (2016). Literacy
Oslund, E. L., Clemens, N. H., Simmons, D. C., & Simmons, and text reading in middle and high school social studies
L. E. (2018). The direct and indirect effects of word read- and English language arts classrooms. Reading & Writing
ing and vocabulary on adolescents’ reading comprehension: Quarterly, 32, 199-222.
Comparing struggling and adequate comprehenders. Reading Talbert, S., Parrish, J. L., & Elleman, A. M. (2016, July). Integrated
and Writing, 31, 355-379. science and literacy K-12 instruction: A meta-analysis. Poster
Oslund, E. L., Clemens, N. H., Simmons, D. C., Smith, S. L., presented at the Twenty-Third Annual Meeting of the Society
& Simmons, L. E. (2016). How vocabulary knowledge of for the Scientific Study of Reading, Porto, Portugal.
Elleman and Oslund 11

van den Broek, P., Lorch, E. P., & Thurlow, R. (1996). Children’s Williamson, G. L., Fitzgerald, J., & Stenner, A. J. (2013). The
and adults’ memory for television stories: The role of causal Common Core State Standards’ quantitative text complex-
factors, story-grammar categories, and hierarchical level. Child ity trajectory: Figuring out how much complexity is enough.
Development, 67, 3010-3028. Educational Researcher, 42, 59-69.
Vaughn, S., Swanson, E. A., Roberts, G., Wanzek, J., Stillman- Willingham, D. T. (2006). The usefulness of brief instruction in read-
Spisak, S. J., Solis, M., & Simmons, D. (2013). Improving ing comprehension strategies. American Educator, 30(4), 39-50.
reading comprehension and social studies knowledge in middle Willingham, D. T. (2017, March 27). What happens when you
school. Reading Research Quarterly, 48, 77-93. teach children to make inferences while reading? [Blog post].
Wilkinson, I. A. G., & Son, E. H. (2011). A dialogic turn in Retrieved from http://www.danielwillingham.com/daniel-will-
research on learning and teaching to comprehend. In M. ingham-science-and-education-blog/
L. Kamil, P. D. Pearson, E. Moje, & P. Afflerbach (Eds.), Wright, T. S., & Cervetti, G. N. (2017). A systematic review of the
Handbook of reading research (Vol. IV, pp. 359-387). New research on vocabulary instruction that impacts text compre-
York, NY: Routledge. hension. Reading Research Quarterly, 52, 203-226.

You might also like