You are on page 1of 7

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/26689731

Preference for consonant music over dissonant music by an infant


chimpanzee

Article  in  Primates · August 2009


DOI: 10.1007/s10329-009-0160-3 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

56 1,285

7 authors, including:

Hiromi Kobayashi Hideko Takeshita


Kyushu University Otemon Gakuin University
12 PUBLICATIONS   943 CITATIONS    29 PUBLICATIONS   530 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Tomoyasu Nakamura Kazuhide Hashiya


Kyushu University Kyushu University
22 PUBLICATIONS   172 CITATIONS    50 PUBLICATIONS   434 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Development of Intersubjective Recognition View project

A new screening test for autism spectrum disorders in toddlers using item response theory View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Hideko Takeshita on 31 May 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Primates (2010) 51:7–12
DOI 10.1007/s10329-009-0160-3

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Preference for consonant music over dissonant music


by an infant chimpanzee
Tasuku Sugimoto Æ Hiromi Kobayashi Æ Noritomo Nobuyoshi Æ Yasushi Kiriyama Æ
Hideko Takeshita Æ Tomoyasu Nakamura Æ Kazuhide Hashiya

Received: 8 December 2008 / Accepted: 22 June 2009 / Published online: 22 July 2009
 Japan Monkey Centre and Springer 2009

Abstract It has been shown that humans prefer conso- one major basis of musical appreciation has some evolu-
nant sounds from the early stages of development. From a tionary origins.
comparative psychological perspective, although previous
studies have shown that birds and monkeys can discrimi- Keywords Chimpanzee  Infant  Music  Consonance 
nate between consonant and dissonant sounds, it remains Dissonance
unclear whether nonhumans have a spontaneous preference
for consonant music over dissonant music as humans do.
We report here that a five-month-old human-raised chim- Introduction
panzee (Pan troglodytes) preferred consonant music. The
infant chimpanzee consistently preferred to produce, with The appreciation of music is a universal aspect of human
the aid of our computerized setup, consonant versions of nature, one that transcends cultural differences. However,
music for a longer duration than dissonant versions. This much controversy surrounds the origins of music. The
result suggests that the preference for consonance is not perception of consonance and dissonance is an enigmatic
unique to humans. Further, it supports the hypothesis that aspect of this issue. Whereas consonance is static and
evokes a pleasant feeling, dissonance is dynamic and
intense, and gives rise to an unpleasant feeling. Neuro-
physiological studies have shown distinct neural correlates
Electronic supplementary material The online version of this
article (doi:10.1007/s10329-009-0160-3) contains supplementary of the perception of consonant and dissonant music in
material, which is available to authorized users. human adults (Blood et al. 1999; Sammler et al. 2007);
further, they have also proposed that those neural correlates
T. Sugimoto (&)
are associated with pleasant and unpleasant emotional
Graduate School of Human-Environment Studies,
Kyushu University, 6-19-1, Hakozaki, states.
Higashi-ku, Fukuoka 812-8581, Japan Evidence shows that humans tend to prefer consonant
e-mail: tasukunzz@gmail.com sounds. This leads to the following question: Is such a
preference the result of cultural learning or does it have
H. Kobayashi  T. Nakamura  K. Hashiya (&)
Faculty of Human-Environment Studies, some basis that is independent of experience? Develop-
Kyushu University, 6-19-1, Hakozaki, mental psychologists have empirically demonstrated that
Higashi-ku, Fukuoka 812-8581, Japan preverbal human infants prefer consonant chords and music
e-mail: hashiya@mindless.com
to dissonant ones; in other words, they pay more attention
N. Nobuyoshi  Y. Kiriyama to consonant chords and music than to dissonant ones
Itozu-no-Mori Park, 4-1 Itozu, Kokurakita-ku, (Crouder et al. 1991; Zentner and Kagan 1996; Trainor and
Kitakyushu, Fukuoka 803-0845, Japan Heinmiller 1998; Trainor et al. 2002). Such a preference
can be observed even in two-day-old newborns with nor-
H. Takeshita
School of Human Cultures, The University of Shiga Prefecture, mal hearing born to deaf parents (Masataka 2006). This
2500 Hassakacho, Hikone, Shiga 522-8583, Japan suggests that the preference for consonant sounds is present

123
8 Primates (2010) 51:7–12

from birth and is independent of specific prenatal or early Methods


postnatal ‘‘cultural’’ experiences. Thus, we may infer that
human infants possess a biological preparedness that makes The study was conducted in accordance with the 2002
consonance perceptually more attractive. version (2nd edn) of the Guide for Care and Use of Lab-
The next question with regard to the origin of music oratory Primates published by the Primate Research
pertains to its evolution and whether the preference for Institute of Kyoto University. The chimpanzee was not
consonant sounds is unique to humans. In this regard, deprived of food or water at any point during research.
researchers have asked whether abilities of nonhuman
animals are comparable with those of humans. In fact, Subject
studies have demonstrated that Java sparrows (Padda
oryzivora) (Watanabe et al. 2005), European starlings One infant female chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) (named
(Sturnus vulgaris) (Hulse et al. 1995), and Japanese mon- Sakura) served as the subject from 17 to 23 weeks of age.
keys (Macaca fuscata) (Izumi 2000) can differentiate Since birth she had been reared by humans, mainly the
between consonant and dissonant sounds. Further, the neural third and fourth authors, at Itozu-no-Mori Park, Kitakyu-
correlates for consonance and dissonance in the auditory shu-Fukuoka, Japan; this was because her biological
cortex of macaque monkeys (Macaca fascicularis) and mother failed to rear her. We confirmed that she had not
humans have also been clarified (Fishman et al. 2001). been exposed to any particular music source such as a
However, most of these studies have focused on the animals’ radio, TV, or CD player throughout her development, apart
ability to discriminate between the consonant/dissonant from that required for the purpose of this experiment.
dichotomy; it remains unclear whether animals evaluate
this dichotomy in the same manner as humans do.
Stimuli
As an exception to these studies, one study examined
spontaneous sound preference in cotton-top tamarins
Six stimulus sets—three melody patterns (A/B/C) 9 two
(Saguinus oedipus), a nonhuman primate species. The
timbres (piano/marimba)—were prepared using Midi
results differed from findings of studies on human infants;
Orchestrator software (Internet Singer Song Writer 8.0).
tamarins showed no preference for consonant chords over
The three music patterns were selected from simple min-
dissonant ones (McDermott and Hauser 2004). The
uets in the etudes of recorders duets (Giesbert 1978). Each
researchers concluded that the preference of tamarins in
stimulus set consisted of two versions of a music pattern–
this regard might be qualitatively different from that of
timbre combination referred to in a previous study (Trainor
humans. However, as the authors pointed out, the tamarins’
and Heinmiller 1998); the CV was played as written by the
lack of preference for consonant or dissonant chords cannot
original composer, whereas for the DV some tones were
be regarded as adequate evidence of the preference for
changed to create dissonant intervals. Other musical factors
consonance as unique to humans. Other primate species
potentially related to musical preferences, for example
that are phylogenetically closer to humans, e.g., chimpan-
tempos (as shown by McDermott and Hauser 2007),
zees, might have more similar acoustic preferences
intensities, and articulations of the stimuli were the same
(McDermott and Hauser 2004), which could be important
across the CV/DV in a stimulus set. The tempos of the
for shedding light on the evolutionary origin of the per-
stimuli were 90–140 quarter notes per minute, and the
ception of music.
intensities were approximately 60 dBs SPL (Fig. 1).
Our study examined the spontaneous preference for
A detailed description of the stimuli is provided below.
consonant music over dissonant music of a five-month-old
(age at the beginning of the experiment) female chimpan- Melody pattern A: A 38-second minuet from Duette
zee (Pan troglodytes). We used a computerized apparatus Englischer Meister in F major. In the DV, all the Gs
that made it possible to produce consonant version (CV) or were changed to GE and all the Cs were changed to CE.
dissonant version (DV) music when the subject pulled a It comprised a total of 82 intervals. Whereas the CV had
string tied to her arm. We chose this operant-based pro- no dissonant intervals the DV had 32 (three augmented
cedure in an attempt to collect data which were as stable as fourths and 29 major seconds).
possible within the limitation that only one subject was Melody pattern B: A 38-second minuet from 1720
available. We predicted that if the established human German handwritten music in C major. In the DV, all the
preference for the consonance over dissonance has evolu- Ds were changed to DE, all the Gs were changed to GE,
tionary origins shared with their phylogenetically close and all the As were changed to AE. It comprised a total
species, then the chimpanzee infant would show the same of 84 intervals. The CV had 16 dissonant intervals (eight
bias and respond to produce CV music more frequently major seconds, seven minor sevenths, and one aug-
than DV music. mented fourth) and the DV had 23 dissonant intervals

123
Primates (2010) 51:7–12 9

Fig. 1 Part of the stimuli


(melody pattern A). The CV
was played as written by the
original composer; in the DV
some tones were changed (all
the Gs were changed to GE and
all the Cs were changed to CE)
to create dissonant intervals

(six major seconds, ten minor sevenths, two augmented


fourths, and five sharp dissonant intervals—three minor
seconds and two major sevenths).
Melody pattern C: A 63-second minuet from Barocke
Spielstucke in C major. In the DV, all the Ds were
changed to DE and all the Gs were changed to GE. It
comprised a total of 126 intervals. The CV had no
dissonant intervals whereas the DV has 40 (two
augmented fourths, two minor sevenths, and 36 major
seconds).

Apparatus

The experiment was conducted in the park keepers’ room.


The subject was made to lie on her back on a bed and was
loosely fastened to the bed with a soft belt. A pair of
loudspeakers (Time Domain Mini) was located one on each
side of the bed and every stimulus was presented through Fig. 2 Schematic illustration of the experimental setup. The
both. A woolen string was attached to the subject’s right subject lay on her back on a bed that was enclosed within three
cloth screens; she was loosely fastened to the bed with a soft belt.
arm; her pulling responses were detected by a digital A pair of loudspeakers were located one on each side of the bed
switch (AbleNet String Switch 1-SS) and a USB input (behind the curtains). A woolen string was attached to the subject’s
device (Technotool Corporation TSWNA-A01 ver.1) con- right arm, and the subject’s pulling response was recorded by
nected to the PC (NEC PC-LR900ED). The PC controlled means of a PC
the stimulus presentations and recorded the frequency and
time course of the subject’s string-pulling responses. The
pulling response occurred frequently as a part of the sub-
experimenters observed the subject’s responses from a
ject’s spontaneous repertoire and that it was more frequent
distance (about 3 m) by means of a video camera, ensuring
when it resulted in auditory feedback.
that the subject’s natural responses were not affected
The main experiment lasted six weeks and was con-
(Fig. 2).
ducted in weekly sessions. Each session comprised one
test. One of the six stimulus sets was used in a single test
Procedure
session. The order of the test session was:
• Melody pattern A-piano,
Before the actual experiment began, four preliminary
• Melody pattern B-marimba,
training sessions (15–20 min per session) were conducted
• Melody pattern B-piano,
weekly to establish the procedure and sequence of the
• Melody pattern C-piano,
experiment. We verified whether the subject’s pulling
• Melody pattern A-marimba, and
response depended on the music being played; we also
• Melody pattern C-marimba.
decided the duration of the musical presentation and
musical instruments. For the preliminary training we used Upon confirming the subject was awake and calm, we
different musical pieces from those in the main experiment. began the test and completed it in 15–20 min, depending
Although statistical analyses were not feasible as the on the subject’s state. In three of the six sessions, testing
number of trials was small and the experimental conditions began with the CV presentation, and in the remaining three,
varied across training sessions, we observed that the with the DV presentation. The procedure basically

123
10 Primates (2010) 51:7–12

Stimulus CV Resp. bout DV Resp. bout CV Resp. bout


presentation
CV CV DV CV
Pull
IPI
string
Time
7 sec 14 sec 14 sec

Fig. 3 Schematic sequence of the experiment. An IPI of longer than


14 s or a stimulus presentation of more than 120 s resulted in the
completion of one CV/DV response bout and the beginning of the
pulling response of the next DV/CV response bout

followed the contingency procedure previously adopted in


human infant studies (e.g., DeCasper and Fifer 1980; Sul- Fig. 4 Results of the six sessions in the order of being tested from
left to right. The mean duration of the CV or DV presentation per
livan and Lewis 2003). If the subject pulled the string that
response bout is represented in each bar. The error bars show the
was attached to her right arm, it activated a digital switch standard errors of the mean
connected to a PC; thereafter, CV or DV music was pre-
sented through the loudspeakers for 7 s. If the subject By treating each response bout as a sample, we con-
pulled the string again within an interval (inter-pulling ducted ANOVA with the three melody patterns, two types
interval: IPI) of less than 7 s, the same music was presented of timbres, and the CV/DV dichotomy as within-subject
continuously for up to 120 s. An IPI of between 7 and 14 s factors. A significant main effect was revealed for the CV/
also resulted in the continuation of the same music, but DV dichotomy (F (1, 8) = 7.50, P = 0.03, g2p = 0.48), but
following a pause of 0 to 7 s. An IPI of more than 14 s or not for the melody patterns (F (2, 16) = 0.66, P = 0.53,
the presentation of a particular version of music for more g2p = 0.08) or timbres (F (1, 8) = 1.21, P = 0.30,
than 120 s terminated that particular trial and resulted in g2p = 0.13). No significant interaction was found among the
the music being changed from CV to DV or vice versa. A three factors. We also conducted ANOVA with six sessions
series of pulling responses between the onset and the end of and the CD/DV dichotomy, which again revealed only the
one version without interruption by another version was significant main effect of the CV/DV dichotomy, with no
defined as a response bout (Fig. 3 and supplementary significant main effect of session (F (5, 40) = 0.89,
video). P = 0.50, g2p = 0.10) or significant interaction (F (5,
40) = 0.28, P = 0.92, g2p = 0.03). This indicated that the
subject’s performance was consistent across the six ses-
Results sions and any effect of learning or experience through the
test was estimated as rather small. These results suggested
The number of response bouts included in a single session that the infant chimpanzee consistently acted to produce
varied among the six sessions (from 22 to 28) because the CV music of longer duration than DV music, regardless of
termination of each response bout depended on the subject. the melody pattern or timbre.
However, one session included at least 11 response bouts
for each of the CV and DV presentations. To equalize the
number of data points among sessions, response bouts over Discussion
the 22nd were excluded from the main analysis. We also
eliminated the first four response bouts (two CV and two The preference for consonant music over dissonant music
DV presentations) of each session, because the subject in an infant chimpanzee has implications for the debate
tended to respond rather feverishly to the onset of the novel surrounding human uniqueness in the capacity for music
stimulus set at the beginning of the session. Therefore, 18 appreciation. Specifically, the results suggest that one of
response bouts (between the 5th and 22nd; nine CV and the major factors that constitute musical appreciation might
nine DV presentations) served as samples for the analysis. not be unique to humans; instead it might be something that
Figure 4 shows the mean durations of the stimulus pre- we share with our phylogenetically closest relatives.
sentation per response bout for the CV and DV of each Although based on a single-case study with only six
melody pattern and timbre combination. The overall mean experimental sessions, our results showed a robust prefer-
durations across the sessions were 24.58 s (SE = 1.78) for ence for consonant music across the sessions. This bias is
CV presentations, and 15.89 s (SE = 0.95) for DV consistent with that previously obtained in human infants
presentations. under different experimental paradigms (Crouder et al.

123
Primates (2010) 51:7–12 11

1991; Zentner and Kagan 1996; Trainor and Heinmiller Although replication of these results with other chim-
1998; Trainor et al. 2002; Masataka 2006). panzee infants and more strict control of the early auditory
The infant chimpanzee’s limited exposure to music environment are desirable, this study suggests that one
before the experiment (including preliminary training ses- basic characteristic of human musical appreciation appears
sions), which clearly contrasts with the typical case of not to be species-specific; it might be shared with chim-
human infants, suggests that musical experience itself panzees at least in the earlier stages of ontogeny. It would
might not be necessary for the emergence of a preference be of interest to further address patterns of convergence
for consonance (Masataka 2006). It would also support the and divergence of musical capacities of other animals, to
view that music perception draws upon a capacity that has construct a clearer picture of what aspects of the music
evolved for a more general auditory analysis (Hauser and faculty might be unique to humans, along with the building
McDermott 2003). According to Helmholtz’s psychoa- blocks that provide the foundation for its evolution.
coustic theory, the perception of dissonance with regard to
the sensation of ‘‘beats’’ and ‘‘roughness’’ is caused by Acknowledgments We would like to thank Sumiharu Nagumo for
his help in programming and for his technical assistance. We are also
interactions in the auditory periphery between adjacent grateful to the staff of Itozu-no-Mori Park for their help. Thanks are
partials of complex tones comprising a musical chord also due to the referees of the article for their constructive criticism
(Helmholtz 1954). Based on its explanation regarding the and warm support. This study was supported by Grants-in-Aid for
lack of preference for dissonance and the consequent Scientific Research from the Japan Society for the Promotion of
Science (JSPS), #16203034 to H.T. and #39047364 to K.H. This
(relative) appreciation of consonance, this theory leads to article is in memory of Sakura, our chimpanzee participant, who
the prediction that animal species sharing similar auditory passed away in an accident on 26th Nov. 2008.
systems and neural mechanisms will also dislike disso-
nance and prefer consonance.
One possibility, which we did not test, is that somehow
the dissonant music was more relaxing for the chimpanzee, References
causing her to be less active. Although we feel that this
possibility seems unlikely, it is desirable for future studies Blood AJ, Zatorre RJ, Bermudez P, Evans AC (1999) Emotional
responses to pleasant and unpleasant music correlate with
to develop a procedure that would enable determination of
activity paralimbic brain regions. Nat Neurosci 2:382–387
possible mechanisms underlying preferences more clearly. Crouder RG, Reznick JS, Rosenkrantz SL (1991) Perception of major/
The lack of preference for consonance in tamarins minor distinction: V. Preferences among infants. Psychon Bull
(McDermott and Hauser 2004), which seems to contradict Rev 29:187–188
DeCasper AJ, Fifer WP (1980) Of human bonding: newborns prefer
the above-mentioned theory, might be explained by the
their mothers’ voice. Science 208:1174–1176
different vocal repertoires, including a higher frequency Fishman YI, Volkov IO, Noh MD, Garell PC, Bakken H, Arezzo JC,
range of vocalizations in tamarins, as compared with Howard MA, Steinschneider M (2001) Consonance and disso-
humans and chimpanzees. One good method for further nance of musical chords: neural correlates in auditory cortex of
monkeys and humans. J Neurophysiol 86:2761–2788
exploration of a possible preference for consonance in
Giesbert FJ (1978) Method for the treble recorder. B. Schott’s Söhne,
tamarins would be to use a higher pitch than those used in Mainz
the previous study (McDermott and Hauser 2004). More- Hannon EE, Trehub SE (2005) Metrical categories in infancy and
over, another major difference between the study on tam- adulthood. Psychol Sci 16:48–55
Hauser M, McDermott J (2003) The evolution of the music faculty: a
arins and our study is that the latter tested an infant
comparative perspective. Nat Neurosci 6:663–668
chimpanzee whereas the former used adult tamarins. In the Helmholtz HV (1954) On the sensations of tone as a physiological
course of development, specialization with regard to bio- basis for the theory of music. Dover, New York
logically important sounds, for example conspecific Hulse SH, Bernard DJ, Braaten RF (1995) Auditory discrimination of
chord-based spectral structures by European starlings. J Exp
vocalizations might occur, resulting in the loss of sensi-
Psychol Gen 124:409–423
tivity to acoustic properties that are not relevant to the Iverson P, Kuhl PK, Akahane-Yamada R, Diesch E, Tohkura Y,
individual’s environment. This recalls what happens in the Kettermann A, Siebert C (2003) A perceptual interference
development of human speech perception, wherein the account of acquisition difficulties for non-native phonemes.
Cognition 87:B47–B57
individual adapts to his/her native language (Kuhl et al.
Izumi A (2000) Japanese monkeys perceive sensory consonance of
1992; Iverson et al. 2003) and the case of rhythmic change chords. J Acoust Soc Am 108:3073–3078
perception (Hannon and Trehub 2005) that might reflect Kuhl PK, Williams KA, Lacerda F, Stevems KN, Lindblom B (1992)
the development of specific perceptual and neural sensi- Linguistic experience alters phonetic perception in infants by
6 months of age. Science 255:606–608
tivities (Stewart and Walsh 2005). In this context, studies
Masataka N (2006) Preference for consonance over dissonance by
of adult chimpanzees might be of interest to determine the hearing newborns of deaf parents and of hearing parents. Dev Sci
origin of the appreciation of consonance. 9:46–50

123
12 Primates (2010) 51:7–12

McDermott J, Hauser M (2004) Are consonant intervals music to their Trainor JJ, Heinmiller BM (1998) The development of evaluative
ears? Spontaneous acoustic preferences in nonhuman primates. responses to music. Inf Behav Dev 21:77–88
Cognition 94:B11–B21 Trainor LJ, Tsang CD, Cheung VHW (2002) Preference for sensory
McDermott J, Hauser M (2007) Nonhuman primates prefer slow consonance in 2- and 4-month-old infants. Music Percep
tempos but dislike music overall. Cognition 104:654–668 20:187–194
Sammler D, Grigutsch M, Fritz T, Koelsch S (2007) Music and Watanabe S, Uozumi M, Tanaka N (2005) Discrimination of
emotion: electrophysiological correlates of the processing of consonance and dissonance in Java sparrows. Behav Proc
pleasant and unpleasant music. Psychophysiology 44:293–304 70:203–208
Stewart L, Walsh V (2005) Infant learning: music and the baby brain. Zentner MR, Kagan J (1996) Perception of music by infants. Nature
Curr Biol 15:R882–R884 383:29
Sullivan MW, Lewis M (2003) Contextual determinants of anger and
other negative expressions in young infants. Dev Psychol
39:693–705

123

View publication stats

You might also like