You are on page 1of 4

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/260431703

Anthropology. Out of Beringia?

Article  in  Science · February 2014


DOI: 10.1126/science.1250768 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS
25 966

3 authors:

John F Hoffecker Scott A. Elias


University of Colorado Boulder Royal Holloway, University of London
106 PUBLICATIONS   1,665 CITATIONS    198 PUBLICATIONS   4,094 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Dennis O'Rourke
University of Kansas
97 PUBLICATIONS   2,691 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

The early Lateglacial Interstadial environment in Britain: evidence of alder (Alnus glutinosa [L. ] Gaertn.) in the Vale of Mowbray, North Yorkshire, UK View project

Late glacial climate change View project

All content following this page was uploaded by John F Hoffecker on 14 June 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Out of Beringia?
John F. Hoffecker et al.
Science 343, 979 (2014);
DOI: 10.1126/science.1250768

This copy is for your personal, non-commercial use only.

If you wish to distribute this article to others, you can order high-quality copies for your
colleagues, clients, or customers by clicking here.

Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on February 28, 2014


Permission to republish or repurpose articles or portions of articles can be obtained by
following the guidelines here.

The following resources related to this article are available online at


www.sciencemag.org (this information is current as of February 28, 2014 ):

Updated information and services, including high-resolution figures, can be found in the online
version of this article at:
http://www.sciencemag.org/content/343/6174/979.full.html
A list of selected additional articles on the Science Web sites related to this article can be
found at:
http://www.sciencemag.org/content/343/6174/979.full.html#related
This article cites 10 articles, 3 of which can be accessed free:
http://www.sciencemag.org/content/343/6174/979.full.html#ref-list-1
This article appears in the following subject collections:
Paleontology
http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/collection/paleo

Science (print ISSN 0036-8075; online ISSN 1095-9203) is published weekly, except the last week in December, by the
American Association for the Advancement of Science, 1200 New York Avenue NW, Washington, DC 20005. Copyright
2014 by the American Association for the Advancement of Science; all rights reserved. The title Science is a
registered trademark of AAAS.
PERSPECTIVES

in a multitude of contexts nonetheless activate higher aspects of linguistic communication 5. L. Fadiga et al., in Broca’s Region, Y. Grodzinsky,
the same brain regions. Moreover, invariance also exists, at least to some extent (12, 13). K. Amunts, Eds. (Oxford Univ. Press, New York, 2006).
6. N. Chomsky, M. Halle, The Sound Pattern of English
seems to be governed by articulatory distinc- These results may suggest a shift in view on (Harper & Row, New York, 1969).
tive features, thereby supporting the 80-year- brain-language relations: from earlier modal- 7. R. Jakobson, Kindersprache, Aphasie und allgemeine
Lautgesetze (1941) [English transl.: Child Language,
old theory of Jakobson and Trubetzkoy. Inter- ity-based models (14), we moved to attempts Aphasia, and Phonological Universals (Mouton, The
estingly, features do not have equal neural to identify the neural code for specific linguis- Hague, 1968)].
representation, and those that induce strong tic units and concatenating operations. This 8. E. D. Young, M. B. Sachs, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 66, 1381
(1979).
neural invariance have strong acoustic cor- move carries the hope that someday, the com- 9. M. Steinschneider, I. O. Volkov, M. D. Noh, P. C. Garell,
relates. Speech representation in the auditory plete neural code for language will be identi- M. A. Howard 3rd, J. Neurophysiol. 82, 2346 (1999).
cortex, in other words, is governed by acous- fied, thereby making good on the promise that 10. Y. Grodzinsky, A. Santi, Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 474 (2008).
11. S. Heim et al., Front. Evol. Neurosci. 4, 4 (2012).
tic features, but not by just any acoustic fea- linguistics be “part of psychology, ultimately 12. M. Makuuchi et al., Cereb. Cortex 23, 694 (2013).
ture—the features that dominate speech rep- biology” (15). 13. A. D. Friederici et al., Cereb. Cortex 16, 1709 (2006).
14. N. Geschwind, Science 170, 940 (1970).
resentation are precisely those that are asso- 15. N. Chomsky, Knowledge of Language, Its Nature, Origin,
References and Notes
ciated with abstract, linguistically defined 1. N. Mesgarani et al., Science 343, 1006 (2014); and Use (Praeger, New York, 1986).
distinctive features. Mesgarani et al., who 10.1126/science.1245994.
2. A. M. Liberman, F. S. Cooper, D. P. Shankweiler, M. Acknowledgments: Support by an Insight grant from the
base their investigation on linguistic distinc- Canada Social Sciences and Humanities Council (Y.G.) and by the
Studdert-Kennedy, Psychol. Rev. 74, 431 (1967).
tions (6), further demonstrate that features are 3. N. Trubetzkoy, Principles of Phonology (Univ. of Califor- Canada Research Chairs (Y.G.) and by the Israel Science Founda-
distinguishable by the degree of the neural nia Press, Berkeley, 1969). tion (I.N.).
invariance they evoke, forming an order that 4. A. M. Liberman, I. G. Mattingly, Cognition 21, 1 (1985). 10.1126/science.1251495
is remarkably in keeping with old linguistic
observations: Manner of articulation (mani-
festing early in developing children) pro- ANTHROPOLOGY
duces a neural invariance that is more promi-
nent than that related to place of articulation
(manifesting late in children). A hierarchy Out of Beringia?
noted in 1941 for language acquisition is now
resurfacing as part of the neural sensitivity to John F. Hoffecker,1 Scott A. Elias,2 Dennis H. O’Rourke3
speech sounds (7).
But linguistic communication is based on A shrub tundra refugium on the Bering land bridge may have played a pivotal role in the peopling
larger pieces than the basic building blocks of of the Americas.
speech. It also requires rules that create com-

B
plex combinations from basic units. Linguis- ased on the distribution of tundra plant macrofossils, and insect remains from
tic combinatorics is therefore an essential part plants around the Bering Strait region, dated sediments extracted from the floor of
of verbal communication, allowing it to be Eric Hultén proposed in the 1930s the Bering Sea indicate a mesic tundra habitat
flexible and efficient. Here, too, Mesgarani et that the now-submerged plain between Chu- during the LGM (2, 3). Although pollen data
al. offer some clues. They show that sequenc- kotka and Alaska—the Bering land bridge— from islands in the Bering Sea suggest more
ing processes, particularly those that deter- became a refugium for shrub tundra vegeta- steppic vegetation (or “steppe-tundra”), these
mine voice onset time, tend to be more dis- tion during cold periods (1), which include islands represent former upland areas on the
tributed in neural tissue than the rather local- the last glacial maximum (LGM) between now-submerged land bridge (see the figure).
ized distinctive features (8, 9). This suggests ~28,000 and 18,000 cal BP (calibrated radio- Several tree species, including spruce, birch,
that combinatorial rules that concatenate carbon years before the present). Adjoining and alder, also probably survived locally dur-
basic elements into bigger units might depend areas to the west and east supported drier ing the LGM (3, 4). Fossil insect remains from
on larger, perhaps somewhat more widely dis- plant communities with a higher percentage both sides of the Bering Strait suggest sur-
tributed, neural chunks, than the stored rep- of grasses during glacial periods. According prisingly mild temperatures during the cold-
resentations of basic building blocks. How to Hultén, when warmer and wetter condi- est phases of the LGM, despite the high lati-
distributed (and speech-specific) such pro- tions returned to these areas, the land bridge, tude. All of these data presumably reflect the
cesses are is not revealed by the Mesgarani et which he named Beringia, became a center impact of the North Pacific circulation, which
al. study, but evidence about the neural speci- of dispersal for tundra plants. Now it appears brought comparatively moist and warm air
ficity of language combinatorics at other lev- that it also may have been a glacial refugium to southern Beringia during the LGM (4). In
els of analysis does exist: Operations involved and postglacial center of dispersal for the peo- fact, the latest study of Beringian vegetation
in building complex expressions—sentences ple who first settled the Americas. indicates that grasses were less dominant in
with rich syntax and semantics—are rela- Since 1960, much evidence has accumu- areas outside the land bridge than previously
tively localized in parts of the left cerebral lated to support the shrub tundra refugium thought (5).
hemisphere (and distinct from other combi- thesis, including data collected from the for- The shrub tundra refugium in Beringia
natorial processes such as arithmetic), even if mer surface of the Bering land bridge. Pollen, may also have played a pivotal role in the
the neural chunks that support them may be peopling of the Americas. Genetic evidence
as large as several cubic centimeters (10, 11). 1
Institute of Arctic and Alpine Research, University of suggests that most Native Americans are
Although the study of Mesgarani et al. Colorado, Boulder, CO 80309, USA. 2Department of Geog- descended from a population that was iso-
was carried out in English, the findings have raphy, Royal Holloway University of London, Egham TW20 lated somewhere between northeast Asia and
0EX, UK. 3Department of Anthropology, University of Utah,
universal implications. Cross-linguistic evi- Salt Lake City, UT 84112, USA. E-mail: john.hoffecker@ Alaska during the LGM (6). According to the
dence for universal neural representation of colorado.edu Beringian standstill hypothesis, this popula-

www.sciencemag.org SCIENCE VOL 343 28 FEBRUARY 2014 979


Published by AAAS
PERSPECTIVES

A possible human refugium. Shrub tundra 170°E 180° 170°W 160°W 150°W At the same time, dated archaeo-
is likely to have covered much of the now- logical and human remains indicate
70°N
submerged plain that lies between Chukotka that settlement of the Western Hemi-
and Alaska during the last glacial maximum sphere probably took place after the
(LGM), reflecting the effect of moist air from LGM (13). At this point, the major
the North Pacific Ocean. Shrub tundra com-
outstanding questions are where the
munities also probably extended into parts
of western Alaska and Chukotka, whereas standstill population was located dur-
drier steppe-tundra covered much of the ing the LGM, and how it was isolated
unglaciated landscape that lay outside cen- Chukotka genetically from its Asian source.
65°N
tral Beringia. Pollen studies indicate that The shrub tundra zone in central
some trees also probably survived in parts of Alaska Beringia represents the most plau-
central Beringia during the LGM. These con- sible home for the isolated standstill
clusions are based in part on the analysis population. Although high-latitude
of sediment cores extracted from the sites archaeological sites of LGM age are
shown on this map (2, 3). The shrub tundra unknown, postglacial submergence of
Bering land bridge
zone in central Beringia represents the most
60°N
the Bering land bridge would explain
plausible home for the people who were
genetically isolated from their Asian parent the absence of traces of people con-
groups during the LGM and later dispersed centrated in central Beringia. On the
throughout the Americas. other hand, occupation of western
Beringia before the LGM is well doc-
umented (14). The post-LGM archae-
Sites indicating steppe-tundra
tion occupied northeast Asia before Sites indicating shrub tundra
ological record contains two sets of
the LGM, became genetically isolated Sites indicating a mixture of remains, one of which represents a
55°N
from its Asian source during the latter, steppe-tundra and shrub tundra movement of people from central
and—following a protracted sojourn Siberia into Beringia about 15,000
in Beringia—dispersed throughout the West- can mtDNA haplogroups had diverged more cal BP (and may be an archaeological proxy
ern Hemisphere, when retreating glaciers than 25,000 years ago but that the latter dis- for mtDNA subclade D2a) (6,15). The other
opened access to coastal and interior corri- persed in the New World less than 15,000 lacks obvious antecedents in Asia and might
dors in northwestern North America (6). years ago (6). represent the post-LGM standstill popula-
Hultén’s mesic tundra refugium offers Although the Beringian standstill hypoth- tion, expanding out of central Beringia with
not only a credible home for the Beringians esis is based mainly on mtDNA, analyses of the shrub tundra (15). To confirm the hypoth-
but also a mechanism for their genetic isola- nuclear (including Y chromosome) DNA pro- esis, archaeological sites of LGM age must be
tion, because other high-latitude regions were vide some support for it. Similarly, an allele at documented in Beringia. Although most such
apparently abandoned by human populations a microsatellite locus on chromosome 9 in the sites presumably would lie underwater, some
during the LGM. Wood fuel was probably the nuclear genome (the D9S1120 locus) is found might be found in areas of the LGM shrub
key variable in determining which regions at appreciable frequency in all Native Amer- tundra refugium that remain above sea level,
remained occupied. People in interior tree- ican populations examined, as well as two such as low-lying portions of southwestern
less settings relied heavily on fresh bone, but Northeast Asian populations, but is absent in Alaska and eastern Chukotka.
experimental studies have shown that at least the rest of the world (8). These observations
a modest quantity of wood is necessary to strongly suggest an origin from a single source References and Notes
1. E. Hultén, Outline of the History of Arctic and Boreal
render bone practical as a fuel (7). The woody population for most Native American popula- Biota During the Quaternary Period (Lehre J. Cramer,
shrubs and occasional trees of the Beringian tions, consistent with the Beringian standstill New York, 1937).
shrub tundra zone may have been the only hypothesis. A large survey of nuclear genetic 2. S. A. Elias, B. Crocker, Quat. Sci. Rev. 27, 2473 (2008).
substantive source of wood fuel at higher lati- variation concluded that initial settlement 3. R. Westbrook et al., GSA Annual Meeting, 180-10
(Charlotte, NC, 4–7 November 2012).
tudes during the LGM. of the Western Hemisphere was followed 4. L. B. Brubaker et al., J. Biogeogr. 32, 833 (2005).
The Beringian standstill hypothesis was by at least two separate and later population 5. E. Willerslev et al., Nature 506, 47 (2014).
first fully articulated in 2007 by Tamm and movements from Asia (9). Recent studies of 6. E. Tamm et al., PLOS ONE 2, e829 (2007).
7. I. Théry-Parisot et al., in The Zooarchaeology of Milk and
colleagues, who worked with a large sam- Y-DNA in both North and South American Fats (Oxbow Books, Oxford, 2005), pp. 50–59.
ple of mitochondrial DNA (mtDNA) from indigenous populations found greater diver- 8. K. B. Schroeder et al., Biol. Lett. 3, 218 (2007).
living Native Americans. They identified a sity than previously assumed among the ini- 9. D. Reich et al., Nature 488, 370 (2012).
set of mutations that accumulated after the tial population, reduced diversity as a func- 10. M. C. Dulik et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 8471
(2012).
divergence of the major haplogroups (A, B, tion of distance from Beringia, and differen- 11. V. Battaglia et al., PLOS ONE 8, e71390 (2013).
C, D, and X) from their Asian parents but tiation of haplogroup Q in Beringia (10, 11). 12. M. Raghavan et al., Nature 505, 87 (2014).
before the dispersal throughout the West- To what extent is the Beringian stand- 13. T. Goebel et al., Science 319, 1497 (2008).
14. V. Pitulko et al., in Paleoamerican Odyssey, K. E. Grant
ern Hemisphere. Tamm et al. concluded still hypothesis supported by archaeological et al., Eds. (Texas A&M University Press, College Station,
that ancestral Native Americans were iso- data and dated human remains? The analy- 2013), pp. 13–44.
lated genetically from other populations sis of ancient DNA from human skeletal 15. J. F. Hoffecker, S. A. Elias, Human Ecology of Beringia
(Columbia Univ. Press, New York, 2007).
for a least a few thousand years before the remains dating to 24,000 cal BP from Mal’ta
dispersal, probably in Beringia. Applying a in southern Siberia appears to confirm the Acknowledgments: We thank N. H. Bigelow, O. K. Mason,
mutation rate of 3.5 × 10−8 per year per posi- pre-LGM divergence of Native Americans G. R. Scott, and two anonymous reviewers who commented on
tion, they estimated that Asian and Ameri- from their Asian parent haplogroups (12). earlier drafts.
10.1126/science.1250768

980 28 FEBRUARY 2014 VOL 343 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org


View publication stats Published by AAAS

You might also like