You are on page 1of 11

Journal of American College Health

ISSN: 0744-8481 (Print) 1940-3208 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/vach20

Narcissistic grandiosity and risky health behaviors


in college students

Sulamunn R. M. Coleman, Michael J. Bernstein, Jacob A. Benfield & Joshua M.


Smyth

To cite this article: Sulamunn R. M. Coleman, Michael J. Bernstein, Jacob A. Benfield & Joshua
M. Smyth (2020): Narcissistic grandiosity and risky health behaviors in college students, Journal of
American College Health, DOI: 10.1080/07448481.2020.1762606

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/07448481.2020.1762606

Published online: 14 May 2020.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 2

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=vach20
JOURNAL OF AMERICAN COLLEGE HEALTH
https://doi.org/10.1080/07448481.2020.1762606

MAJOR ARTICLE

Narcissistic grandiosity and risky health behaviors in college students


Sulamunn R. M. Coleman, PhDa, Michael J. Bernstein, PhDb, Jacob A. Benfield, PhDb, and Joshua M. Smyth, PhDc
a
Vermont Center on Behavior and Health, Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont, Burlington, Vermont, USA; bDepartment of
Psychological and Social Sciences, The Pennsylvania State University, Abington, Pennsylvania, USA; cDepartment of Biobehavioral Health,
The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, Pennsylvania, USA

ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY


Objectives: This study explored associations between narcissistic grandiosity (including its compo- Received 20 August 2019
nent traits) and reported risky health behaviors in college students. Participants: College male Revised 7 March 2020
and women (N ¼ 122) participated between December 2015 and January 2016. Methods: Accepted 26 April 2020
Participants completed self-report measures of grandiosity, alcohol consumption, number of sex
KEYWORDS
partners, and condom use. Results: Grandiosity was positively associated with alcohol consump- Alcohol; college students;
tion. Gender interacted with grandiose traits, driving associations with reported sexual behaviors. gender differences;
College male high in entitlement/exploitativeness reported more sex partners. Grandiose exhib- grandiosity; narcissism;
itionism was associated with condom use among women but not male, such that college women risky sex
higher in grandiose exhibitionism were more likely to report not using a condom with their most
recent partner. Conclusions: Grandiosity (and grandiose traits) may influence health behavior and/
or the reporting of health behavior in college students, but associations may differ for male
and women.

Narcissism is a complex aspect of personality characterized (i.e., a preoccupation with success, social status, and drive
in part by “grandiosity”—an overriding need for recognition for associated recognition and admiration), “grandiose exhi-
and admiration to maintain and enhance an inflated sense bitionism,” (i.e., a tendency to show off and to like being
of self.1,2 In a relatively small but growing body of literature the center of attention), and “entitlement/exploitativeness,”
examining associations between narcissism and health,3,4 (i.e., manipulative tendencies and a general willingness to
grandiosity appears to be related, albeit inconsistently, to take advantage of others to meet entitled expectations).14–16
risky substance use and sexual behavior.5–9 College students A 2015 meta-analysis demonstrated that male tended to be
in particular tend to engage in risky behaviors such as prob- higher than women in grandiosity, particularly in leader-
lematic alcohol consumption, having multiple sex partners ship/authority and entitlement/exploitativeness.16 As
and unprotected sex at high rates.10–13 Nonetheless, engage- research suggests college male and women tend to engage in
ment in such behaviors may be unevenly distributed risky health behaviors at different rates17–20 it is important
between college male and women.12,13 Therefore, gender dif- to examine whether grandiosity differentially associates with
health behaviors for male and women to potentially inform
ferences may help explain apparent discrepancies in associa-
prevention and treatment efforts.
tions between grandiosity and risky health behaviors in
Importantly, measures of grandiosity must be distin-
college students; however, existing studies have not consist-
guished from measures of “narcissistic vulnerability,” the lat-
ently examined gender as a potential moderator. In this
ter referring to deficits in regulating emotion and behavior
study, we examine associations between grandiosity (includ-
when grandiosity is threatened and which constitutes the
ing overall grandiosity as well as theoretically distinct traits), second primary dimension of narcissism.1,2 This distinction
gender, and risky health behaviors in college students, is particularly important for researchers and health profes-
including alcohol consumption, number of sex partners over sionals who may be interested in examining whether grandi-
the past 3 months, and condom use with most recent osity and vulnerability differentially influence health
sex partner. behavior. In the present study, which is a secondary analysis
of data collected as part of a larger study examining associa-
Grandiosity tions between individual differences and social health behav-
iors in college students, the available measure of narcissism
Grandiosity is a primary dimension of narcissism that is the Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI)-13.15 The
encompasses related but theoretically distinct traits. NPI-13 is a widely accepted measure of grandiosity16; thus,
Examples of grandiose traits include “leadership/authority” this study focused on conducting a thorough examination of

CONTACT Sulamunn R. M. Coleman sulamunn.coleman@uvm.edu Vermont Center on Behavior and Health, Department of Psychiatry, University of
Vermont, 1 South Prospect Street, Burlington, VT 05401.
ß 2020 Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
2 S. R. M. COLEMAN ET AL.

how grandiosity and specific grandiose traits associate with differences such as grandiosity associate with both alcohol
reported risky health behaviors among college students. consumption and risky sexual behaviors.

Risky health behaviors among college students Grandiosity and risky health behaviors
College is a period of transition from adolescence to adult- Two theories, in particular, may help contextualize potential
hood that may involve alcohol experimentation for many associations between grandiosity, gender, and risky health
students. Such experimentation may include binge drinking behaviors. These theories include the Dynamic Self-
(i.e., consuming four to five or more alcoholic beverages in Regulatory Processing Model of Narcissism43 and
a 2-hour window) and heavy alcohol use (i.e., binge drink- Objectification Theory.44 According to the Dynamic Self-
ing on five or more days over a period of one month),21 Regulatory Processing Model, grandiosity is vulnerable to
both of which are widespread among college students.10,22–24 disconfirmation for both genders, and this vulnerability
Every year, as many as 600,000 students are involved in results in an ongoing pursuit of validation from others.
alcohol-related accidents resulting in injury or death.25 However, gender-role socializations may lead to differences
Furthermore, individuals who begin consuming alcohol at in the behaviors one engages in to obtain such validation.
an earlier age are at greater risk for developing alcohol For example, male more than women may display competi-
dependence,26 which is known to contribute to serious tiveness and dominance (e.g., drinking the most alcohol,
health consequences such as liver disease.27 Although some having the most sex partners). Gender-role socialization is
research has demonstrated that college male may generally also an important factor in Objectification Theory, which
consume more alcohol than college women,13 more recent posits that over the course of a woman’s socialization, sexual
evidence suggests the prevalence of heavy alcohol use may parts and/or functions may become regarded as instrumental
be similar for college male and women (e.g., 12% vs. 11%, and exchangeable for favorable treatment. Men, on the other
respectively).20 Moreover, research has demonstrated that hand, are less likely to be socialized to view their relative
compared to college male with major depressive disorder attractiveness as exchangeable for favorable treatment. As
(MDD; a risk factor for substance use disorders), college such, women who tend to self-objectify may be less likely to
women with MDD engaged in more frequent binge drink- use condoms if it is perceived that the act will garner favor,
ing19: this finding may suggest that associations between and this may be particularly true for women higher in gran-
grandiosity and alcohol consumption could be moderated diosity who are seeking recognition and admiration from
by gender. their partners.
College may also constitute a period of sexual explor- Research has demonstrated positive associations between
ation. Many students report having multiple sexual partners, overall grandiosity and alcohol consumption in college stu-
including dating/romantic and casual/non-romantic partners dents,5,7,9,45 and some evidence suggests specific grandiose
(i.e., “hookups” without commitment or emotional attach- traits may differentially associate with alcohol consumption.
ment).28 In one study, roughly 78% of a student sample For example, one study demonstrated that “grandiose
reported having at least one casual sexual experience.11 In a fantasy” (which is conceptually similar to “leadership/
second study, students were just as likely to report engaging authority” in that it involves a preoccupation with fantasies
in casual sex since starting college as they were to report of unlimited power/success) was negatively related to heavy
dating.29 In yet another study, 21% of students reported that episodic drinking.46 On the other hand, “exploitativeness”
their most recent casual sexual experience was during a time was positively related to heavy episodic drinking.
when they were romantically involved with someone else.28 Importantly, the measure of grandiose traits used in this
Furthermore, the last decade has seen an increase in the use study was the Pathological Narcissism Inventory,47 which
of smartphone dating applications, and college students who lacks an assessment of exhibitionism (a critical component
use such applications may be more likely to have casual sex- of grandiosity). Thus, a more complete understanding of
ual experiences.30 Unfortunately, consistent condom use how specific grandiose traits associate with alcohol con-
among college students may be low (i.e., 29%–50%—well sumption requires an additional assessment of exhibition-
below what is ideal for preventing health consequences asso- ism, as well as an examination of whether gender acts as
ciated with unprotected sex),20,31 and college women may a moderator.
use condoms less consistently than male.17 Few studies have examined associations between grandi-
Given the high rates of alcohol consumption and risky osity (both overall and specific traits) and risky sexual
sexual behaviors among college students, it is important to behaviors in college students. In one study of adolescents
underscore that alcohol consumption and risky sexual aged 15–18, grandiosity was positively associated with num-
behaviors are often correlated.17,32–36 Having multiple sex ber of sex partners in girls, but not in boys.6 It remains
partners and irregular or no condom use increase one’s risk unclear whether grandiosity in college students similarly
for unintended pregnancy and sexually transmitted infection associates with number of sex partners after accounting for
(STI),37,38 and college students who have multiple sex part- gender as a moderator. Another study found no correlation
ners are at greater risk for perpetrating39 and being a victim between grandiosity and condom use in college students,5
of sexual assault,40 particularly when impaired by alco- and associations between specific grandiose traits and con-
hol.39–42 Thus, it is important to examine whether individual dom use were not examined. In a third study, college
JOURNAL OF AMERICAN COLLEGE HEALTH 3

students higher in exhibitionism reported more casual sexual characteristics, participants indicated total family incomes of
experiences,11 providing some preliminary evidence that spe- greater than $100 K (19%), between $40 K and $100 K
cific grandiose traits may relate to risky sexual behaviors; (29.8%), between $20 K and $40 K (12.4%), between $10 K
however, additional research is needed to examine grandiose and $20 K (7.4%), and less than $10 K (2.5%), or declined
traits in addition to exhibitionism while examining potential providing information on family income (29.5%). Most par-
gender effects. ticipants reported growing up in either suburban commun-
ities (46.3%) or large, metropolitan areas (39.7%), with a
minority reporting small, rural communities (14.1%).
Current study
Analyses regarding sexual behaviors were conducted on the
In this study, we examine associations between overall gran- subset of participants who identified as sexually active
diosity, specific grandiose traits (i.e., leadership/authority, (n ¼ 81); this subset had similar sociodemographic charac-
grandiose exhibitionism, and entitlement/exploitativeness), teristics to the full sample.
and self-reported alcohol consumption, number of sex part-
ners over the last 3 months, and condom use with most
Measures
recent sex partner in college male and women. We hypothe-
size that overall grandiosity will positively associate with Grandiosity
alcohol consumption and number of sex partners, and nega- Grandiosity was assessed with the Narcissistic Personality
tively associate with condom use (i.e., individuals higher in Inventory-13 (NPI-13).15 The NPI-13 contains 13 forced-
grandiosity will be more likely to report not using a con- choice items (i.e., a “grandiose” response option vs. a “non-
dom). Based on evidence that leadership/authority (and con- grandiose” response option) from its 40-item parent meas-
ceptually similar traits)46 may be associated with better ure, the NPI.14 Participants were instructed to select the
functioning,48 we hypothesize that leadership/authority will response option that best described them, even if not a per-
negatively associate with alcohol consumption and number fect fit. The NPI-13 also includes three subscales to assess
of sex partners, and positively associate with condom use. specific grandiose traits: four items assess “leadership/
By contrast, grandiose exhibitionism and entitlement/exploi- authority” (e.g., “I am a born leader” vs. “Leadership is a
tativeness have been associated with worse functioning48; quality that takes a long time to develop”), five items assess
therefore, we hypothesize that grandiose exhibitionism and “grandiose exhibitionism” (e.g., “I will usually show off if I
entitlement/exploitativeness will positively associate with get the chance” vs. “I try not to show off”), and four items
alcohol consumption and number of sex partners, and nega- assess “entitlement/exploitativeness” (e.g., “I will never be
tively associate with condom use. Finally, given the limited satisfied until I get all that I deserve” vs. “I take my satisfac-
evidence base, moderation analyses will be conducted in an tions as they come”). Grandiose response options were
exploratory fashion to examine gender effects on associa- summed across subscales to generate an overall grandiosity
tions between grandiosity and risky health behaviors. score (a ¼ .67) and within subscales to generate scores for
leadership/authority (LA; a ¼ .70), grandiose exhibitionism
(GE; a ¼ .61), and entitlement/exploitativeness (EE; a ¼
Methods
.55). Internal consistencies were low to acceptable in the
Participants and procedures current sample. However, it has been noted that scales
derived from the NPI may exhibit low internal consistencies
This study was approved by all relevant institutional review but tend to correlate in theoretically meaningful ways with
boards, and informed consent was obtained from all partici- criterion variables.48 Importantly, the NPI has been utilized
pants prior to data collection. Participants were 130 under-
as a measure of grandiosity in college samples for roughly
graduate students at an urban public university recruited
40 years.51,52
through the university’s psychology subject pool.
Participants completed an online battery of self-report meas-
ures on individual differences, social interactions, and health Alcohol consumption
behaviors as part of the larger study, and were compensated Alcohol consumption was assessed with the Alcohol Use
with course participation credit which was awarded upon Disorders Identification Test-Consumption Questions
completion of the study. Because college undergraduates are (AUDIT-C).53 The AUDIT-C consists of the first 3 con-
typically between the ages of 18 and 24,49 eight participants sumption items of the AUDIT.54 The three items are: “How
were removed from the current analyses because they did often do you have a drink containing alcohol? (0 [never], 1
not fall within this age range, and “non-traditional” older [monthly or less], 2 [two to four times a month], 3 [two to
students may differ substantially regarding the enactment of three times per week], or 4 [four or more times a week])”
the health behaviors under investigation.32,50 The final sam- “How many standard drinks containing alcohol do you have
ple (n ¼ 122; Mage¼19.51, SD ¼ 1.70, 75.2% women) was on a typical day? (0 [1–2], 1 [3–4], 2 [5–6], 3 [7–9], or 4
racially diverse, with participants identifying as White [>10]),” and “How often do you have six or more drinks on
(46.3%), Asian (21.5%), Hispanic (15.7%), Black (11.6%), or one occasion? (0 [never], 1 [less than monthly], 2 [monthly],
“Other” (4.1%). One participant declined to provide racial 3 [weekly], or 4 [daily or almost daily]).” Items were
and gender identification. Regarding socioeconomic summed such that higher scores indicate greater alcohol
4 S. R. M. COLEMAN ET AL.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations.


N Mode Mean SD Skew 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
1.Grandiosity 122 2 3.57 2.55 .75 .
2.LA 122 0 1.41 1.38 .47 .73 .
3.GE 122 0 1.27 1.31 .89 .67 .18 .
4.EE 122 0 .89 1.10 1.17 .62 .22 .13 .
5.Problematic Alcohol Consumption 122 0 1.83 1.94 1.01 .25 .20 .19 .11 .
6.Number of Sex Partners 81 1 1.06 1.03 4.18 .24 .00 .18 .34 .02 .
7.Number of Sex Partners (log-transformed) 81 0 .69 1.30 1.61 .04 .08 .07 .08 .03 .57 .
8.Number of Sex Partners (categories) 81 1 .98 .63 .63 .13 .08 .14 .21 .00 .87 .80 .
9.Condom Use 81 1 .52 .50 .08 .23 .15 .17 .13 .09 .04 .14 .12 .
Note: p < .05, p < .01, p < .001. LA ¼ Leadership/Authority, GE ¼ Grandiose Exhibitionism, EE ¼ Entitlement/Exploitativeness.

consumption. The AUDIT-C has been shown to perform missing for “number of sex partners” for three of the partic-
better than the AUDIT in college samples.55 In the current ipants, and as in prior research, “number of sex partners”
sample, internal consistency for the AUDIT-C was accept- was highly positively skewed (Skew ¼ 4.18). The “number
able (a ¼ .72). of sex partners” variable was transformed in two ways in
order to select a transformation suitable for hypothesis test-
ing: First, “number of sex partners” was log-transformed
Sexual behavior (i.e., after adding a small constant of .0001 to preserve val-
Single item measures were used to identify sexually active ues of “0”), and second, “number of sex partners” was
participants, assess number of sex partners over the last recoded into four categories consistent with prior research
three months, and condom use with most recent sex part- (i.e., 0 ¼ zero, 1 ¼ one, 2 ¼ two or three, and 3 ¼ four or
ner. Single items measures such as these have been used to more sex partners).64 The log-transformed variable became
assess sexual behavior in community56–58 clinical,59 and col- negatively skewed (Skew ¼ 1.61) and the categorical vari-
lege samples5,60 and have shown meaningful associations able was less skewed than the log-transformed variable
with important health-related psychosocial factors and expe- (Skew ¼ 0.63). As the log-transformed and categorical varia-
riences, such as sexual abuse history,56 being exposed to dat- bles were highly positively correlated, the main hypothesis
ing violence,57 and substance use.58,59 In the current study, tests focused on the categorical “number of sex partners”
sexually active participants were identified using an open- variable. Bivariate correlations were run to test preliminary
ended response item: “Please indicate the age at which you associations between all variables.
first engaged in sexual intercourse (age in years).” A series of two-step regressions were conducted to test
Participants were excluded from analyses if they indicated the main hypotheses. Grandiosity (or grandiose trait) varia-
never engaging in sexual activity (e.g., “never,” “N/A,” “I bles were entered into the first step, and gender and inter-
haven’t had sex”) or responded “0” (i.e., a non-plausible action terms were entered into the second step. Linear
age). Number of sex partners over the last three months was regression models were used to test hypotheses regarding
assessed with an open-ended response item: “Please indicate alcohol consumption and number of sex partners. To probe
the number of sexual partners you have had during the past significant interactions in the linear regression models, over-
three months.” Finally, condom use with most recent sex all grandiosity and grandiose traits were categorized using a
partner was assessed with the item: “Please indicate whether quartile split and graphed to facilitate describing the charac-
you used a condom during your most recent instance of teristics of the sample: this method has been used in prior
sexual intercourse.” Participants responded to this item by studies examining associations between narcissism and
indicating “Yes” or “No,” coded as “1” and “0,” respectively. health-related variables.65,66 Logistic regression models were
used to test hypotheses regarding condom use. Odds ratios
(OR) and associated 95% confidence limits (CL) are pre-
Data analytic strategy sented for significant associations. ORs were computed to
indicate the event of participants’ reporting “not using a
A priori power calculations were conducted in GPower
condom” (i.e., the risky behavior) with their most recent
3.1.61 It was determined that a minimum sample size of 103
partner. Thus, an OR > “1” indicated that a participant was
participants would be needed to demonstrate significant
more likely to report not using a condom, and a 95% CL
effects of grandiose traits, gender, and their interactions on
that contained the value “1” was not significant.
the outcomes treated as continuous (i.e., alcohol consump-
tion and number of sex partners), and a minimum sample
size of 83 participants would be needed to demonstrate sig- Results
nificant effects of the independent variables on condom use
Bivariate correlations
(i.e., a dichotomous outcome). All hypothesis tests were con-
ducted in SAS 9.4.62 Bivariate correlations are presented in Table 1. As expected,
Variables were checked for normality (see Table 1). grandiosity and GE correlated positively with alcohol con-
Overall grandiosity, LA, and GE were normally distributed, sumption, grandiosity correlated positively with number of
and EE and alcohol consumption were positively skewed but sex partners and negatively with condom use, and EE corre-
within generally accepted limits (i.e., <2).63 Data were lated positively with number of sex partners. Unexpectedly,
JOURNAL OF AMERICAN COLLEGE HEALTH 5

Table 2. Linear regression models for hypothesis tests regarding associations between grandiosity (overall/traits), gender, and alcohol consumption and number
of sex partners.
Alcohol consumption Number of sex partners (categories)
b 95% confidence limits p b 95% confidence limits p
Model 1
Step 1
Grandiosity 0.19 0.06 0.32 <0.01 0.03 0.02 0.08 0.27
Step 2
Grandiosity 0.17 0.01 0.33 0.04 0.03 0.09 0.03 0.37
Gender 0.25 1.66 1.16 0.73 20.94 21.56 20.33 <0.01
Grandiositygender 0.08 0.22 0.37 0.61 0.23 0.11 0.36 <0.001
Model 2
Step 1
LA 0.23 0.03 0.49 0.08 0.72 0.18 0.03 0.18
GE 0.21 0.06 0.48 0.12 0.06 0.04 0.16 0.27
EE 0.11 0.21 0.43 0.51 0.14 0.01 0.27 0.04
Step 2
LA 0.20 0.11 0.50 0.20 20.10 20.21 2.00 0.05
GE 0.19 0.13 0.50 0.24 0.08 0.02 0.18 0.11
EE 0.07 0.36 0.50 0.76 0.10 0.25 0.06 0.21
Gender 0.53 2.12 1.05 0.51 0.36 1.11 0.38 0.33
LAgender 0.23 0.45 0.91 0.50 0.02 0.33 0.29 0.90
GEgender 0.23 0.49 0.95 0.52 0.12 0.39 0.15 0.38
EEgender 0.04 0.78 0.70 0.92 0.59 0.31 0.88 <0.001
Note: Bolded values indicate significant associations. LA ¼ Leadership/Authority, GE ¼ Grandiose Exhibitionism, EE ¼ Entitlement/Exploitativeness.

LA correlated positively with alcohol consumption, GE was specific traits, there was a significant GE by gender inter-
uncorrelated with numbed of sex partners and condom use, action associated with not using a condom. For college
and EE was uncorrelated with GE, alcohol consumption, male, GE was not associated with condom use (OR ¼ 0.39,
and condom use. Furthermore, among sexually active stu- 95% CL ¼ 0.12–1.24). For college women, every one unit
dents, alcohol consumption was uncorrelated with number increase in GE was associated with a 60% increase in the
of sex partners and condom use. likelihood of reporting not using a condom with the most
recent partner (OR ¼ 1.60, 95% CL ¼ 1.10–2.41).
Main hypotheses tests
Discussion
Hypotheses tests for alcohol consumption and number of
sex partners are presented in Table 2. As hypothesized, This study examined associations between narcissistic gran-
grandiosity was associated with reporting greater alcohol diosity, including overall grandiosity and specific grandiose
consumption. Additionally, there was a significant gender by traits, and reported alcohol use and sexual behaviors in col-
grandiosity interaction associated with number of sex part- lege students. Results demonstrated positive associations
ners. Compared to male at “high/average” levels of grandios- between overall grandiosity and alcohol consumption and
ity, women at “high/average” levels of grandiosity reported number of sexual partners, as well as an association between
having more sex partners over the last three months (Figure overall grandiosity and reporting “no condom use.” These
1). By contrast, compared to women at “high” levels of results are consistent with the view that overall grandiosity
grandiosity, male at “high” levels of grandiosity reported may constitute a broad, risk-stratifying aspect of personality
having more sex partners over the last three months for college students. Thus, college students high in overall
(Figure 1). grandiosity may be at increased risk for health consequences
Regarding individual grandiose traits, as hypothesized, associated with alcohol consumption (e.g., development of
LA was not associated with alcohol consumption and was alcohol dependence, accidental injury) and risky sexual
negatively associated with number of sex partners (Table 2). behavior (e.g., unwanted pregnancy, contracting STIs, perpe-
EE was positively associated with number of sex partners. trating or being a victim of sexual assault). Importantly,
The association between EE and number of sex partners was these results add to a growing body of literature that sug-
moderated by gender. Compared to college women at “high” gests grandiosity (and narcissism more generally) may con-
levels of EE, college male at “high” levels of EE reported stitute an aspect of personality that affects a variety of
having more sex partners over the last three months health behaviors and outcomes.3,4
(Figure 2). Regarding specific grandiose traits, we found uneven sup-
Hypotheses tests for condom use are presented in Table port for the main hypotheses. As expected, Leadership/
3. As hypothesized, grandiosity was significantly associated authority (LA), Grandiose Exhibitionism (GE), and
with not using a condom (OR ¼ 1.22, 95% CL ¼ Entitlement/Exploitativeness (EE) exhibited differential and
1.01–1.47). Specifically, for every one unit increase in gran- theoretically meaningful associations with risky health
diosity, college students were 22% more likely to report not behaviors. LA was unrelated to alcohol consumption and
using a condom with their most recent partner. Regarding condom use and negatively associated with number of sex
6 S. R. M. COLEMAN ET AL.

Figure 1. Interaction between grandiosity and gender on number of sex partners in the last three months in college students. Note: the first quartile was catego-
rized as “Low” (n ¼ 11, 81.8% women), the second as “Low/Average” (n ¼ 25, 76% women), the third as “High/Average” (n ¼ 24, 75% women), and the fourth as
“High” (n ¼ 20; 75% women).

Figure 2. Interaction between entitlement/exploitativeness and gender on number of sex partners in the last three months in college students Note: the first quar-
tile was categorized as “Low” (n ¼ 20, 80% women), the second as “Low/Average” (n ¼ 21, 81% women), the third as “High/Average” (n ¼ 22, 72.7% women), and
the fourth as “High” (n ¼ 17; 70.6% women).

partners. These results generally support the notion that LA partners, such that at high levels of EE, college male
may constitute an adaptive (or less maladaptive) aspect of reported more sex partners over the past three months than
grandiosity.48 However, it is important to note that LA also college women. In one study examining how male react to
exhibited a positive zero-order correlation with reported sexual refusal from women, male higher in grandiosity
alcohol consumption. Given that LA is associated with cri- exhibited more punitive behavior (i.e., aggression) toward
terion variables such as striving to win at all costs, asserting women who refused to read sexually arousing passages out
dominance, and pursuing social status, one may speculate loud.67 In line with this study, one could speculate that male
that for both college male and women, LA could be quite higher in grandiosity in the current study reported more sex
maladaptive (e.g., increase risk for acute intoxication/impair- partners because they were more likely to use force or coer-
ment) in contexts where alcohol consumption is linked with cion than male lower in grandiosity. If this speculation is
winning and/or asserting dominance (e.g., during drinking correct, EE may have a dangerous influence on sexual
games). Using Ecological Momentary Assessment (EMA) to behavior in college students, potentially contributing to the
examine in-the-moment effects of grandiose traits on drink- incidence of sexual harassment and assault. That said, stud-
ing behaviors across different social/environmental contexts ies have shown that compared to women, male may be
may be an interesting avenue for future research to help to more likely to inflate reported number of sex partners.68
inform campus prevention and intervention efforts. Therefore, it is also possible that current results reflect
Gender was found to moderate an association between intentional misrepresentation among college male at higher
entitlement/exploitativeness (EE) and reported number sex levels of EE, particularly given the nature of EE (i.e., a
JOURNAL OF AMERICAN COLLEGE HEALTH 7

Table 3. Logistic regression models for hypothesis tests regarding associations highly motivated to self-enhance, and may inflate self-
between grandiosity (overall/traits), gender, and condom use.
reports of behaviors perceived to garner recognition and
B 95% confidence limits Wald v2 p
admiration. Because participants completed the question-
Model 1
Step 1
naires anonymously and online, the motivation to intention-
Grandiosity 0.20 0.01 0.38 4.13 0.04 ally misrepresent behaviors may have been reduced.
Step 2 However, future studies would benefit from control meas-
Grandiosity 0.13 0.08 0.35 1.46 0.23
Gender 0.57 0.49 1.63 1.12 0.90 ures on social desirability (e.g., Marlowe-Crowne Social
Grandiositygender 0.16 0.37 0.06 2.04 0.15 Desirability Scale)70 and the use of methods to reduce prob-
Model 2 lems with retrospective reporting bias (e.g., objective data
Step 1
LA 0.18 0.17 0.52 1.02 0.31 where possible; naturalistic methods that utilize intensive
GE 0.22 0.11 0.55 1.68 0.20 sampling to reduce the recall interval).71 Power analyses
EE 0.19 0.25 0.63 0.71 0.40
Step 2
indicated that the current study may have been somewhat
LA 0.08 0.32 0.47 0.14 0.71 underpowered for some statistical tests; thus, future studies
GE 0.47 0.06 0.88 5.14 0.02 on this topic should seek to recruit larger samples, particu-
EE 0.37 0.23 0.96 1.48 0.22
Gender 2.24 0.67 5.14 2.28 0.13 larly with respect to sexually active college students. Finally,
LAgender 0.01 1.19 1.18 0.00 0.99 it was noted in the measures section that low internal con-
GEgender 21.42 22.65 20.19 5.11 0.02 sistencies on subscales derived from the NPI may be accept-
EEgender 0.15 1.28 0.98 0.80 0.80
able because they tend to show meaningful associations with
Note: Bolded values indicate significant associations. LA ¼ Leadership/
Authority, GE ¼ Grandiose Exhibitionism, EE ¼ Entitlement/Exploitativeness. criterion variables.48 GE and EE are considered maladaptive
aspects of grandiosity, and LA may capture (at least in part)
general willingness to use deception/manipulation to obtain potentially adaptive aspects of grandiosity; thus, GE and EE
recognition and admiration). Importantly, both interpreta- should correlate more closely with each other than with LA.
tions are consistent with some theories of gender69 and nar- In the present study, however, EE was uncorrelated with
cissism,2,43,67 and could be examined in future studies using GE. Therefore, it is possible that the NPI-13 did not
measures to help guard against self-report biases (e.g., adequately capture GE or EE. As such, it would be beneficial
Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale).70 to replicate the present findings in other college samples
As hypothesized, grandiosity was associated with report- using scales with better internal consistency (e.g.,
ing no condom use with most recent sex partner. As such, Psychological Entitlement Scale).72
college students at higher levels of grandiosity may be at
higher risk for irregular or no condom use and, conse-
Conclusions
quently, at higher risk for unwanted pregnancy and STIs.
Regarding specific traits, women (but not male) who were Given the observed associations between grandiosity (overall
higher in GE were more likely to report not using a condom and specific traits), alcohol consumption, and risky sexual
with their most recent partner. Three out of the five items behaviors in college students, this study may help to inform
included in the NPI-13 GE subscale are associated with body prevention and treatment efforts on college campuses. For
image and may indicate a tendency to self-objectify (i.e., “I example, some of the reported associations were moderated
like to show off my body,” “I like to look at my body,” and by gender; thus, it may be important to consider the poten-
“I like to look at myself in the mirror.”). Given these items, tial benefit of tailoring prevention and treatment efforts for
it may be reasonable to suggest that women (more than male and women. Additionally, the behaviors of individuals
male) who are higher in GE tend to self-objectify given gen- high in grandiosity tend to be driven by the need to obtain
der differences in socialization and sexual experiences. This recognition and admiration from others. Thus, it may also
interpretation might help explain why GE was a risk factor be important to develop health and risk messages that chal-
for no condom use in women but not male, and also gener- lenge students’ perceptions of which behaviors will garner
ally aligns with Objectification Theory and associated peer recognition and admiration.73 More generally, the
research65,66 Future research on this topic may benefit from development of strategies to reduce risky health behaviors
using longitudinal study designs with more intensive sam- on college campuses may benefit from the inclusion of
pling schemes (e.g., daily diary) to help delineate gender dif- measures to track associations between grandiosity and
ferences in GE and self-objectification, and examine how health behaviors over time.
potential differences affect the reporting of sexual behaviors
in daily life.
Conflict of interest disclosure
The authors have no conflicts of interest to report. The
Limitations and future directions
authors confirm that the research presented in this article
The current results are cross-sectional which precludes met the ethical guidelines, including adherence to the legal
causal inferencing. In future studies, causal inferencing on requirements, of the United States and received approval
this topic may be improved by using longitudinal methods. from the Institutional Review Board of The Pennsylvania
As discussed above, individuals high in grandiosity are State University.
8 S. R. M. COLEMAN ET AL.

Funding 16. Grijalva E, Newman DA, Tay L, et al. Gender differences in nar-
cissism: a meta-analytic review. Psychol Bull. 2015;141(2):
No funding was used to support this research and/or the 261–310. doi:10.1037/a0038231.
preparation of the manuscript. 17. Bontempi JB, Mugno R, Bulmer SM, Danvers K, Vancour ML.
Exploring gender differences in the relationship between HIV/
STD testing and condom use among undergraduate college stu-
ORCID dents. Am J Health Ed. 2009;40(2):97–105. doi:10.1080/19325037.
2009.10599084.
Joshua M. Smyth http://orcid.org/0000-0002-0904-5390 18. Napper LE, Montes KS, Kenney SR, LaBrie JW. Assessing the
personal negative impacts of hooking up experienced by college
students: gender differences and mental health. J Sex Res. 2016;
References 53(7):766–775. doi:10.1080/00224499.2015.1065951.
19. Pedrelli P, Borsari B, Lipson SK, Heinze JE, Eisenberg D. Gender
1. Pincus AL, Lukowitsky MR. Pathological narcissism and narcis- differences in the relationships among major depressive disorder,
sistic personality disorder. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2010;6(1): heavy alcohol use, and mental health treatment engagement
421–446. doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131215.
among college students. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2016;77(4):
2. Pincus AL, Roche MJ, Good EW. Narcissistic personality dis-
620–628. doi:10.15288/jsad.2016.77.620.
order and pathological narcissism. In: Blaney PH, Krueger RF,
20. American College Health Association. American College Health
Millon T, eds. Oxford Textbook of Psychopathology. 3rd ed. New
Association-National College Health Assessment II: Reference
York: Oxford University Press; 2015:791–813.
Group Executive Summary Fall 2018. Silver Spring, MD:
3. Konrath S, Bonadonna JP. Physiological and health-related cor-
American College Health Association; 2018.
relates of the narcissistic personality. In: Besser A, ed. Handbook
21. Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration.
of the Psychology of Narcissism: Diverse Perspectives. New York,
Alcohol Use: Facts & Resources. 2020. https://www.samhsa.gov/
NY: Nova Science Publishers, Incorporated; 2014:175–214.
sites/default/files/alcohol-use-facts-resources-fact-sheet.pdf.
4. Coleman SRM, Pincus AL, Smyth JM. Narcissism and stress-
Accessed April 30, 2020.
reactivity: a biobehavioural health perspective. Health Psychology
22. Knight JR, Wechsler H, Kuo M, Seibring M, Weitzman ER,
Rev. 2019;13(1):35–72. doi:10.1080/17437199.2018.1547118.
Schuckit MA. Alcohol abuse and dependence among US college
5. Hill E. The role of narcissism in health-risk and health-protective
students. J Stud Alcohol. 2002;63(3):263–270. doi:10.15288/jsa.
behaviors. J Health Psychol. 2016;21(9):2021–2032. doi:10.1177/
2002.63.263.
1359105315569858.
23. Wechsler H, Davenport A, Dowdall G, Moeykens B, Castillo S.
6. Lavan H, Johnson JG. The association between axis I and II psy-
chiatric symptoms and high-risk sexual behavior during adoles- Health and behavioral consequences of binge drinking in college:
cence. J Pers Disord. 2002;16(1):73–94. doi:10.1521/pedi.16.1.73. a national survey of students at 140 campuses. JAMA. 1994;
22559. 272(21):1672–1677. doi:10.1001/jama.1994.03520210056032.
7. Luhtanen RK, Crocker J. Alcohol use in college students: effects 24. White A, Hingson R. The burden of alcohol use: excessive alco-
of level of self-esteem, narcissism, and contingencies of self- hol consumption and related consequences among college stu-
worth. Psychol Addict Behav. 2005;19(1):99–103. doi:10.1037/ dents. Alcohol Res. 2013;35(2):201–2018.
0893-164X.19.1.99. 25. Hingson RW, Heeren T, Zakocs RC, Kopstein A, Wechsler H.
8. Martin AM, Benotsch EG, Lance SP, Green M. Transmission Magnitude of alcohol-related mortality and morbidity among US
risk behaviors in a subset of HIV-positive individuals: the role of college students ages 18-24. J Stud Alcohol. 2002;63(2):136–144.
narcissistic personality features. Pers Indiv Dif. 2013;54(2): doi:10.15288/jsa.2002.63.136.
256–260. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2012.09.006. 26. Grant BF. The impact of a family history of alcoholism on the
9. Welker LE, Simons RM, Simons JS. Grandiose and vulnerable relationship between age at onset of alcohol use and DSM-IV
narcissism: associations with alcohol use, alcohol problems and alcohol dependence: results from the National Longitudinal
problem recognition. J Am Coll Health. 2019;67(3):226–234. doi: Alcohol Epidemiologic Survey. Alcohol Health Res World. 1998;
10.1080/07448481.2018.1470092. 22(2):144–148.
10. Wechsler H, Nelson TF. What we have learned from the 27. Maddrey WC. Alcohol-induced liver disease. Clin Liver Dis.
Harvard School of Public Health College Alcohol Study: focusing 2000;4(1):115–131. doi:10.1016/S1089-3261(05)70099-4.
attention on college student alcohol consumption and the envir- 28. Grello CM, Welsh DP, Harper MS. No strings attached: the
onmental conditions that promote it. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2008; nature of casual sex in college students. J Sex Res. 2006;43(3):
69(4):481–490. doi:10.15288/jsad.2008.69.481. 255–267. doi:10.1080/00224490609552324.
11. Paul EL, McManus B, Hayes A. “Hookups”: characteristics and 29. Kuperberg A, Padgett JE. Dating and hooking up in college:
correlates of college students’ spontaneous and anonymous sex- meeting contexts, sex, and variation by gender, partner’s gender,
ual experiences. J Sex Res. 2000;37(1):76–88. doi:10.1080/ and class standing. J Sex Res. 2015;52(5):517–531. doi:10.1080/
00224490009552023. 00224499.2014.901284.
12. Caldeira KM, Singer BJ, O’Grady KE, Vincent KB, Arria AM. 30. Choi EPH, Wong J, Lo HHM, Wong W, Chio JHM, Fong D.
HIV testing in recent college students: prevalence and correlates. The association between smartphone dating applications and col-
AIDS Education Prev. 2012;24(4):363–376. doi:10.1521/aeap.2012. lege students’ casual sex encounters and condom use. Sex Reprod
24.4.363. Healthc. 2016;9:38–41. doi:10.1016/j.srhc.2016.07.001.
13. O’Malley PM, Johnston LD. Epidemiology of alcohol and other 31. Bird Y, Solis LH, Mbonu CA. Sexual attitudes, norms, condom
drug use among American college students. J Stud Alcohol Suppl. use, and adherence of Hispanic and non-Hispanic undergraduate
2002;14(s14):23–39. doi:10.15288/jsas.2002.s14.23. students: a cross-sectional study of three community colleges in
14. Raskin R, Hall CS. The Narcissistic Personality Inventory: alterna- southwestern US. Patient Prefer Adherence. 2016;10:1501–1508.
tive form reliability and further evidence of construct validity. J doi:10.2147/PPA.S108688.
Pers Assess. 1981;45(2):159–162. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa4502_10. 32. Wechsler H, Dowdall GW, Davenport A, Castillo S. Correlates
15. Gentile B, Miller JD, Hoffman BJ, Reidy DE, Zeichner A, of college student binge drinking. Am J Public Health. 1995;
Campbell WK. A test of two brief measures of grandiose narcis- 85(7):921–926. doi:10.2105/AJPH.85.7.921.
sism: the Narcissistic Personality Inventory–13 and the 33. Mair C, Ponicki WR, Gruenewald PJ. Reducing risky sex among
Narcissistic Personality Inventory-16. Psychol Assess. 2013;25(4): college students: prospects for context-specific interventions. AIDS
1120–1136. doi:10.1037/a0033192. Behav. 2016;20(S1):109–118. doi:10.1007/s10461-015-1147-2.
JOURNAL OF AMERICAN COLLEGE HEALTH 9

34. Lewis MA, Litt DM, Cronce JM, Blayney JA, Gilmore AK. 51. Twenge JM, Konrath S, Foster JD, Campbell WK, Bushman BJ.
Underestimating protection and overestimating risk: examining Egos inflating over time: a cross-temporal meta-analysis of the
descriptive normative perceptions and their association with Narcissistic Personality Inventory. J Pers. 2008;76(4):875–902.
drinking and sexual behaviors. J Sex Res. 2014;51(1):86–96. doi: doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00507.x.
10.1080/00224499.2012.710664. 52. Wetzel E, Brown A, Hill PL, Chung JM, Robins RW, Roberts
35. Desiderato LL, Crawford HJ. Risky sexual behavior in college BW. The narcissism epidemic is dead; long live the narcissism
students: relationships between number of sexual partners, dis- epidemic. Psychol Sci. 2017;28(12):1833–1847. doi:10.1177/
closure of previous risky behavior, and alcohol use. J Youth 0956797617724208.
Adolesc. 1995;24(1):55–68. doi:10.1007/BF01537560. 53. Bush K, Kivlahan DR, McDonell MB, Fihn SD, Bradley KA. The
36. Snipes DJ, Benotsch EG. High-risk cocktails and high-risk sex: AUDIT alcohol consumption questions (AUDIT-C): an effective
examining the relation between alcohol mixed with energy drink brief screening test for problem drinking. Arch Intern Med. 1998;
consumption, sexual behavior, and drug use in college students. 158(16):1789–1795. doi:10.1001/archinte.158.16.1789.
Addict Behav. 2013;38(1):1418–1423. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2012. 54. Saunders JB, Aasland OG, Babor TF, De La Fuente JR, Grant M.
07.011. Development of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test
37. Wellings K, Jones KG, Mercer CH, et al. The prevalence of (AUDIT): WHO collaborative project on early detection of per-
unplanned pregnancy and associated factors in Britain: findings sons with harmful alcohol consumption-II. Addiction. 1993;
from the third National Survey of Sexual Attitudes and Lifestyles 88(6):791–804. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.1993.tb02093.x.
(Natsal-3). Lancet. 2013;382(9907):1807–1816. doi:10.1016/S0140- 55. DeMartini KS, Carey KB. Optimizing the use of the AUDIT for
6736(13)62071-1. alcohol screening in college students. Psychol Assess. 2012;24(4):
38. McNicholas CP, Klugman JB, Zhao Q, Peipert JF. Condom use 954–963. doi:10.1037/a0028519.
and incident sexually transmitted infection after initiation of 56. Luster T, Small SA. Sexual abuse history and number of sex
long-acting reversible contraception. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2017; partners among female adolescents. Fam Plann Perspect. 1997;
217(6):672.e1–672.e6. 29(5):204–211. doi:10.2307/2953396.
39. Abbey A, McAuslan P, Ross LT. Sexual assault perpetration by 57. Martin-Storey A. Prevalence of dating violence among sexual
college men: the role of alcohol, misperception of sexual intent, minority youth: variation across gender, sexual minority identity
and sexual beliefs and experiences. J Soc Clin Psychol. 1998;17(2): and gender of sexual partners. J Youth Adolescence. 2015;44(1):
167–195. doi:10.1521/jscp.1998.17.2.167. 211–224. doi:10.1007/s10964-013-0089-0.
40. Abbey A, Ross LT, McDuffie D, McAuslan P. Alcohol and dating 58. Mustanski B, Andrews R, Herrick A, Stall R, Schnarrs PW. A
risk factors for sexual assault among college women. Psychol Women syndemic of psychosocial health disparities and associations with
Q. 1996;20(1):147–169. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.1996.tb00669.x.
risk for attempting suicide among young sexual minority men.
41. Testa M, Cleveland MJ. Does alcohol contribute to college men’s
Am J Public Health. 2014;104(2):287–294. doi:10.2105/AJPH.
sexual assault perpetration? Between-and within-person effects
2013.301744.
over five semesters. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2017;78(1):5–13. doi:
59. Benotsch EG, Pinkerton SD, Dyatlov RV, et al. HIV risk behav-
10.15288/jsad.2017.78.5.
ior in male and female Russian sexually transmitted disease
42. Tyler KA, Schmitz RM, Adams SA. Alcohol expectancy, drinking
clinic patients. Int J Behav Med. 2006;13(1):26–33. doi:10.1207/
behavior, and sexual victimization among female and male col-
s15327558ijbm1301_4.
lege students. J Interpers Violence. 2017;32(15):2298–2322. doi:10.
60. Arnold P, Fletcher S, Farrow R. Condom use and psychological
1177/0886260515591280.
sensation seeking by college students. Sex Relation Therapy.
43. Morf CC, Rhodewalt F. Unraveling the paradoxes of narcissism:
2002;17(4):355–365. doi:10.1080/1468199021000017209.
a dynamic self-regulatory processing model. Psychol Inq. 2001;
61. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, Lang AG. Statistical power anal-
12(4):177–196. doi:10.1207/S15327965PLI1204_1.
44. Fredrickson BL, Roberts TA. Objectification theory: toward yses using G Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression
understanding women’s lived experiences and mental health analyses. Behav Research Methods. 2009;41(4):1149–1160. doi:10.
risks. Psychol Women Q. 1997;21(2):173–206. doi:10.1111/j.1471- 3758/BRM.41.4.1149.
6402.1997.tb00108.x. 62. SAS Institute Inc. Base SASV R 9.4 Procedures Guide: Statistical

45. Kramer MP, Wilborn DD, Spencer CC, Stevenson BL, Dvorak Procedures. 2nd ed. Cary, NC: SAS Institute; 2013.
RD. Protective behavioral strategies moderate the association 63. Kim HY. Statistical notes for clinical researchers: assessing nor-
between narcissistic traits and alcohol pathology in college stu- mal distribution (2) using skewness and kurtosis. Restor Dent
dent drinkers. Subst Use Misuse. 2019;54(5):863–867. doi:10. Endod. 2013;38(1):52–54. doi:10.5395/rde.2013.38.1.52.
1080/10826084.2018.1547909. 64. Valois RF, Oeltmann JE, Waller J, Hussey JR. Relationship
46. Naidu ES, Patock-Peckham JA, Ruof A, et al. Narcissism and between number of sexual intercourse partners and selected
devaluing others: an exploration of impaired control over drink- health risk behaviors among public high school adolescents. J
ing as a mediating mechanism of alcohol-related problems. Pers Adolesc Health. 1999;25(5):328–335. doi:10.1016/S1054-
Individ Dif. 2019;139:39–45. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2018.10.039. 139X(99)00051-8.
47. Pincus AL, Ansell EB, Pimentel CA, Cain NM, Wright AG, Levy 65. Fernie BA, Fung A, Nikcevic AV. Different coping strategies
KN. Initial construction and validation of the Pathological amongst individuals with grandiose and vulnerable narcissistic
Narcissism Inventory. Psychol Assess. 2009;21(3):327–365. doi:10. traits. J Affect Disord. 2016;205:301–305. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2016.
1037/a0016530. 08.009.
48. Ackerman RA, Witt EA, Donnellan MB, Trzesniewski KH, 66. Kalliopuska M. Attitudes towards health, health behaviour, and
Robins RW, Kashy DA. What does the Narcissistic Personality personality factors among school students very high on empathy.
Inventory really measure? Assessment. 2011;18(1):67–87. doi:10. Psychol Rep. 1992;70(4):1119–1122. doi:10.2466/PR0.70.4.1119-
1177/1073191110382845. 1122.
49. Snyder TD, Dillow SA. Digest of Education Statistics 2011. 67. Bushman BJ, Bonacci AM, Van Dijk M, Baumeister RF.
National Center for Education Statistics, Institute of Education Narcissism, sexual refusal, and aggression: testing a narcissistic
Sciences, US Department of Education. Washington, DC: US reactance model of sexual coercion. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2003;
Government Printing Office; 2011. NCES 2011–015. 84(5):1027–1040. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.5.1027.
50. Certain HE, Harahan BJ, Saewyc EM, Fleming MF. Condom use 68. Wiederman MW. The truth must be in here somewhere: exam-
in heavy drinking college students: the importance of always ining the gender discrepancy in self-reported lifetime number of
using condoms. J Am Coll Health. 2009;58(3):187–194. doi:10. sex partners. J Sex Res. 1997;34(4):375–386. doi:10.1080/
1080/07448480903295284. 00224499709551905.
10 S. R. M. COLEMAN ET AL.

69. Courtenay WH. Constructions of masculinity and their influence across the lifespan. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2017;11(10):
on men’s well-being: a theory of gender and health. Soc Sci Med. e12356. doi:10.1111/spc3.12356.
2000;50(10):1385–1401. doi:10.1016/S0277-9536(99)00390-1. 72. Campbell WK, Bonacci AM, Shelton J, Exline JJ, Bushman BJ.
70. Reynolds WM. Development of reliable and valid short forms of Psychological entitlement: interpersonal consequences and valid-
the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale. J Clin Psychol. ation of a self-report measure. J Pers Assess. 2004;83(1):29–45.
1982;38(1):119–125. doi:10.1002/1097-4679(198201)38:1<119:: doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa8301_04.
AID-JCLP2270380118>3.0.CO;2-I. 73. Hill EM. Narcissism as a consideration when designing health and
71. Smyth JM, Juth V, Ma J, Sliwinski M. A slice of life: ecologically risk messages. In: Parrott RL, ed. Oxford Research Encyclopedia of
valid methods for research on social relationships and health Communication. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2017:1–33.

You might also like