You are on page 1of 33

APA CENTENNIAL FEATURE

From Then to Now: The Development of Industrial-Organizational


Psychology in the United States

Raymond A. Katzell James T. Austin


New \brk University Ohio State University

This article reviews the development of industrial-organizational (I-O) psychology in the United
States since its beginnings at about the turn of the century. The history is divided into periods,
within which are described topics, roles, and forces, with the goal of explaining as well as describ-
ing developments. Among the conclusions are that I-O psychology is a dynamic field that has been
making contributions both to the science of behavior and to industrial society and that those
contributions have been underestimated by some and possibly overestimated by others. Among the
problems still facing the field is the disjunction between science and practice. However, in princi-
ple, that and other bipolarities can be made complementary.

Like human development, history can be studied either methodological topics; (b) the roles enacted by I-O psycholo-
cross-sectionally or longitudinally. That is, one can take a topic gists, which we frame in terms of training and employment;
and trace changes in its status over time or take a period of time and (c) salient aspects of the contemporary intellectual, social,
and describe the status of various topics during that interval. and political environment that may help to account for those
Although the two approaches are obviously complementary, features. As Mueller (1979) concluded from tracing the begin-
the latter has the advantage of depicting more clearly the rela- nings of scientific psychology, often what is gained from a con-
tionships among the various phenomena of interest. Because sideration of "where, when, and who" is knowledge about
that type of analysis has not, to our knowledge, been attempted "what" started. We may also evaluate change, with the lessons
for industrial-organizational (I-O) psychology, and because we learned providing guidance for the future.
think that it reveals a number of instructive relationships, the Although it is often fashionable to trace psychological devel-
cross-sectional strategy is the way we have chosen to look back opments back to ancient Greece, we start at the beginning of
at how the field developed. This undertaking is particularly the current century. Nevertheless, we cannot refrain from
appropriate in 1992, when the American Psychological Associa- pointing out that Plato, in The Republic, set forth a program of
tion (APA) celebrates its centennial (Evans, Sexton, & Cadwal- managerial selection and development that might have pleased
lader, 1992). We accordingly focus on developments in the Campbell, Dunnette, Lawler, and Weick (1970). He devised an
United States, although to be sure there was important work early taxonomy that placed citizens into three categories—
going on in other countries. guardians, auxiliaries, and workers—and also proposed meth-
We choose to divide history into several periods, each cover- ods for selecting and training members of each category. Also,
ing approximately two decades. In doing so, we briefly describe more than 3,000 years ago the Chinese developed a multiple-
each period in terms of the following features: (a) the subject hurdle selection system for bureaucrats that started with local
matter and methods of study, or noteworthy substantive and examinations, continued through provincial assessments, and
finished with final evaluations in the capital; this system was
used until 1905 (DuBois, 1965, 1970). A third selection-or-
This article is based in part on an invited address in 1986 by Ray- iented example is Hull's (1928) description of a French noble
mond A. Katzell at the initial midyear conference of the Society for who used tapestries depicting scenes of warlike and domestic
Industrial and Organizational Psychology, Chicago. Portions of the activities to assess applicants for court positions. In the organi-
article were completed while James T. Austin was a Visiting Assistant zational field, perhaps the earliest recorded consultant was the
Professor at New York University. Midianite priest, Jethro, who advised his son-in-law, Moses, on
We thank Roger M. Bellows, Richard J. Campbell, Tim Crespin,
Paula Larson, and James C. Naylor, as well as three anonymous ref-
how to staff and organize the ancient Israelites (Exod. 18). The
erees, for their helpful comments. We also thank Mildred E. Katzell for Catholic church represents another early example of a central-
her overall assistance throughout the writing of this article. ized and hierarchical organization. Nonetheless, those earliest
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to attempts were isolated and unintegrated (except for the Chi-
James T. Austin, Department of Psychology, Ohio State. University, 142 nese) and thus did not catch on in commerce. Turning to a time
Townshend Hall, 1885 Neil Avenue, Columbus, Ohio 43210-1222. when such work was more systematic, we may ask what were
Journal of Applied Psychology, 1992, Vol. 77, No. 6, 803-835
Copyright 1992 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 002l-90!0/92/$3.00

803
804 RAYMOND A. KATZELL AND JAMES T. AUSTIN

some features of American psychology from the turn of the was devoted to empirical literature on personnel psychology
century to World War I that prompted the germination of indus- and human factors. Textbooks by Link (1919) and Burtt (1929)
trial psychology. similarly made practically no reference to what we now call
organizational psychology. However, the lack of research did
not stop some psychologically oriented people from pontificat-
Turn of the Century to World War I ing about the subject. For example, Lillian Gilbreth (1914), a
Topics psychologist by training, and F. W Taylor (1911/1947), the fa-
ther of scientific management, each wrote prescriptive books
First, let us consider the emergence of subjects of direct inter- about organization and supervision, thus starting a tradition of
est to I-O psychology and the methods used to study them what Herbert Simon (1957) was to call "proverbs of administra-
between 1900 and 1917. Reviews of the early literature were tion" (p. xxxiii). The nearest approach to empirical investiga-
published in the Psychological Bulletin by Kornhauser (1922), tion of organizational factors at the time was the pioneering
Link (1920), and Viteles (1926a, 1928,1930). Some of the earli- work of Max Weber (1906/1946,1922/1947). Although he was
est studies dealt with the acquisition of such skills as telegraphy more of an economist and sociologist than a psychologist, he
(Bryan & Harter, 1897) and typing (Book, 1908). Personnel did consider such psychological topics as leadership, power,
selection, based on the concept of individual differences (E. L. norms, and bureaucratization.
Thorndike, 1911), was another popular subject for investiga- Methodological developments centered on psychological
tion, including such occupations as streetcar motormen measuring tools and on statistical analyses of the resulting data
(Miinsterberg, 1913) and salespersons (Scott, 1915). Studies of to identify individual differences (Cowles, 1989). Measurement
work methods and job design by industrial engineers were con- methods developed during this period included mental tests
cerned mainly with increasing capability and reducing fatigue, (Binet & Simon, 1905, 1908; Cattell, 1890; Ebbinghaus, 1897;
with efficiency a key dependent variable (Gilbreth & Gilbreth, Gallon, 1883; E. L. Thorndike, 1904; Whipple, 1910), direct
1916,1917); examples include the decomposition of work mo- observations of workers (Gilbreth, 1911; Taylor, 1911/1947),
tions into elementary "therbligs" and subsequent analysis of the and Weber's (1922/1947) use of case studies (cf. Huff, 1984).
"one best way" (F. Gilbreth, 1911) and time study (Taylor, 1911 / Statistical techniques developed at around that time included
1947). Dockeray's (1920) review suggested that psychologists regression, simple correlation, and partial correlation (Gallon,
could add a scientific perspective to the study of work and 1877; Pearson, 1894,1895; Yule, 1897) and factor analysis (Pear-
fatigue. E. L. Thorndike (1917) anticipated that proposal by son, 1901; Spearman, 1904,1914). The early roots of test valida-
publishing an analysis of the curve of work and the curve of tion were proposed by Miinsterberg (1913), Scott (1917), and
"satisfyingness" in the very first volume of the Journal of Ap- Binet and Simon (1905), who termed the process "testing the
plied Psychology. test." Experimental studies in the laboratory were used to sludy
A major institutional development was the establishment in memory (Ebbinghaus, 1885/1913) and fatigue (Mosso, 1888,
1915 of a Division of Applied Psychology at the Carnegie Insti- cited in Viteles, 1932). Quasi experiments in field situations
tute of Technology, now Carnegie-Mellon University. It was were done to improve efficiency (Gilbreth, 1911; Taylor, 1911/
headed by Walter VanDyke Bingham, who was hired away from 1947). Those various methodological developments suggest
Dartmouth. The next year, a consortium of 27 national organi- that Cronbach's (1957) "two disciplines" were alive and well
zations was formed, each pledging to contribute $500 a year for even at the beginning of the field.
5 years, with the resulting funds to be used to undertake scien-
tific research on salesmanship at that institute. The Bureau of Roles
Salesmanship Research, as it was called, was headed by Walter
Dill Scott in 1916-1917. Scott, who had been a professor at Nearly all psychologists at that time were trained in general
Northwestern University, and his colleagues quickly devised a experimental psychology and were primarily employed in aca-
multiple-component selection system for sales personnel that demia (Cattell, 1946). That, for instance, was the case with
included a scored personal history blank (biodata), a standard J. McKeen Cattell at Columbia, Hugo Miinsterberg at Harvard,
interview, a reference form, and a mental alertness test. Fergu- Scott at Northwestern, and Bingham at Dartmouth. With the
son (1952) reported how those instruments were later used by exception of Bingham, who received his doctorate at the Univer-
the Life Insurance Agency Management Association (LIAMA). sity of Chicago, the others had studied under Wilhelm Wundt
Indeed, the Aptitude Index Battery, a biodata questionnaire at Leipzig (Boring, 1950a). Their applied work was therefore
based on that early work, is still used by many companies in the typically done on a part-time basis. In some cases, their move-
United States and Canada (Brown, 1981). ment into the I-O field was due to approaches by employers
It was not until much later that American psychologists paid rather than at their own initiative. That seems to have been the
attention to social and organizational factors in the workplace. case with Bingham, who was recruited from the bucolic envi-
As late as 1932, Morris Viteles, in his classic text, Industrial rons of Dartmouth to head up the newly formed applied insti-
Psychology, described the situation as follows, "Unfortunately, tute at Carnegie Institute of Technology. Scott was asked in
there are few. . . experimental investigations in this field. The 1901 to give a speech at the Agate Club in Chicago on psycholog-
psychology of supervision and management represents largely a ical aspects of advertising, which resulted in his subsequent
rationalization of psychological principles and findings in engagement as a consultant as well as authorship of books on
terms of supervisory and management problems" (Note 12, p. the psychology of advertising (Scott, 1903) and industrial effi-
616). His own book reflected that situation as well, for the bulk ciency (Scott, 1911). This work helped to develop the founda-
APA CENTENNIAL: HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF I-O PSYCHOLOGY 805

tions for his subsequent personnel work and led Ferguson in turn was a mentor of Douglas Fryer, whom we shall later
(1962-1965) to refer to Scott as "America's first industrial psy- encounter as another of the early I-O psychologists.
chologist" (p. 1). However, credit for founding the field must be A third cultural force was the prevailing faith in capitalism
shared with Miinsterberg, who continued the applied work- that was rampant in the 19th and early 20th centuries. Capital-
after being called to Harvard in 1892—that he had started in ism placed a premium on efficiency, which in turn depended
his native Germany and whose textbook Psychology and Indus- on having hard-working, dedicated employees. Kaufman
trial Efficiency was published in English in 1913. (1913) characterized the early 20th century as the age of effi-
There were a few people working full-time outside of acade- ciency and charted the spread of this concept throughout soci-
mia on topics of an I-O nature but were not thought of as psy- ety; Callahan (1962) elaborated how the efficiency movement
chologists (e.g., the Gilbreths, who were full-time management came to influence educational administration. At the same
consultants, and Taylor, who was a senior executive at Bethle- time, people motivated by the capitalist ethic are likely to be
hem Steel). Incidentally, Taylor's status helps to explain how he achievement-oriented and entrepreneurial—behavior that fos-
was able to conduct some of the first rudimentary field experi- ters capitalist institutions (McClelland & Winter, 1969). Weber
ments (cf. Hoagland, 1964; Locke, 1982), something which to (1906/1946) analyzed the reciprocal relationship between the
this day is not easily arranged by I-O psychologists. spirit of capitalism and what he termed the "Protestant ethic,"
In those early decades, only a handful of people could be the notion that hard work is not only ethically good but a road
considered to be I-O psychologists. Cattell estimated that as to salvation. A recent detailed account of the work ethic has
late as 1917 only 16 of the more than 300 members of APA were been published by Furnham (1990).
engaged principally in the various applications of psychology, A fourth factor was the growth of industrialization (Morri-
whereas 272 were educators (Cattell, 1946). I-O psychologists, son, 1965), which in America was a consequence of the capital-
academic and otherwise, probably comprised those few we ist system, but of course is a property of all industrialized societ-
have already identified. Even allowing for several who may ies including communist ones. Industrialization calls for a sup-
have escaped our attention, the total was unlikely to have ex- ply of competent workers who are well managed. Mark Sullivan
ceeded 10. (1927) dramatically portrayed that development:
Throughout America, during the last half of the 19th century,
went on a process of little shops closing down, big factories grow-
Forces ing bigger; little one-man businesses giving up, great corporations
growing and expanding;. . .fewer craftsmen, more factory opera-
How do we account for the emergence of I-O psychology at tives . . . The process was essentially an adjustment of man to
that time? This question pertains to the Zeitgeist and Ortgeist, [technology], (p. 254)
or the spirits of the time and place. Our opinion is that four From those forces emerged a society that venerated efficient
cultural forces converged to establish the field. industrial organizations and people who were capable of adapt-
One was the flowering of science that occurred during the ing to and being successful in such institutions. Psychology too
period, which in turn led to empirical studies of mind and began marching to that drummer: No longer content to analyze
behavior (Roback, 1952). Another force was the influence of the structure of consciousness, it began to try to understand by
Darwinism (cf. Angell, 1907; Boring, 1950b), with its emphasis empirical investigation how people function so as to cope suc-
on fitness as the key to survival. In psychology, its clearest man- cessfully with their environments, which led to an interest in a
ifestation was in a school of thought called functionalism, scientifically based technology for facilitating such adaptation.
which emphasized the adaptive role of consciousness and be- Those dynamics—the scientific study of mind and behavior,
havior (Buxton, 1985; Heidbreder, 1933). Roback (1952) ob- Darwinism and its psychological reflection in functionalism,
served that functionalism was therefore attacked as being a the spirit of capitalism and growing industrialization—com-
mere technology, interested in the means of executing a task, in bined to create the scientific and practical field of industrial
contrast to pure science (note also the name of "psychotechnol- psychology, a child of its time.
ogy" adopted by several early I-O psychologists). However, func-
tionalists defended the scientific status of utility, pointing out
that it was often also a basis for evaluating pure science (Carr, World War I Through the 1920s
1930). Clearly the ideas of utility and technology were becom-
Topics
ing part of the American psychological scene. William James, a
leading exponent of pragmatism, was among the precursors of War has historically often turned out to be a catalyst of pro-
functionalist thought, and it is noteworthy that he was responsi- gress. This observation is true for psychology as well, because
ble for bringing Miinsterberg from Germany to Harvard, al- industrial psychology used the occasion of World War I to come
though not especially because of Miinsterberg's interest in in- out of the closet of academia (Scott, 1920; Yerkes, 1918). The
dustrial applications (on the other hand, Miinsterberg's debates wartime contribution of psychology has been comprehensively
with Wundt could only have raised his stock with James, as reported by Ferguson (1962-1965), Yerkes (1918,1919,1921),
noted by Roback, 1952, p. 148; but see Bringmann & Balance, E. L. Thorndike (1919), Chapman (1920), and Yoakum and
1973, for a counterperspective). James was also the mentor of \ferkes (1920). We rely heavily on those sources in describing
the prominent functionalists J. R. Angell and G. Stanley Hall World War I developments.
(Heidbreder, 1933). Hall became a proponent of applied psy- In April 1917, when the United States declared war on Ger-
chology and helped found the Journal of Applied Psychology. He many, a group of experimental psychologists was having its an-
806 RAYMOND A. KATZELL AND JAMES T. AUSTIN

nual meeting in Cambridge, Massachusetts. Upon hearing of trained by G. Stanley Hall and James Baird at Clark University,
the declaration of war, Yerkes, who was then president of the started his I-O career in the Morale Branch at Camp Greenleaf.
25-year-old APA, and several others quickly formed a Psychol- In addition to the aforementioned testing developments at
ogy Committee of the National Research Council, with the SGO, there were a number of other psychological products
members representing the National Academy of Sciences of its work, including a personality and psychopathology
(NAS), the American Association for the Advancement of checklist intended to screen recruits (Woodworth, 1918). As
Science (A A AS), and the APA (Von Mayrhauser, 1987). The reported by Strong (1918), Scott's AGO group devised qualifica-
Army accepted their proposal that a psychological examining tion cards for officer and enlisted ranks (forerunner of the ubiq-
program of recruits be evaluated, and Yerkes organized the Sur- uitous personnel file); catalogs of occupational needs for army
geon General's Section of Psychology with the rank of major. units (e.g., Bingham's "Table of Occupational Needs of an In-
Scott and Bingham, who were then faculty members of the fantry Division"; Committee on Classification of Personnel,
Carnegie Institute of Technology, disagreed with the majority 1918); the "man-to-man" and graphic scale methods for rating
of the group (Von Mayrhauser, 1987). They believed that it officer performance (Paterson, 1923); trade tests to evaluate
would be more important to aid in the selection of officer per- specific occupational skills (Chapman, 1920; Chapman &
sonnel and that psychology should be placed under the adjutant Toops, 1919; Robinson, 1919); and, more generally, the con-
general, the Army's personnel officer, not within the Surgeon cepts of person and job analysis. On the basis of the success of
General's Office (SGO). Scott persuaded Secretary of War Scott's committee, even Yerkes (1919) agreed that psychological
Newton Baker to establish a psychological program in the Ad- efforts might have been more useful had they originally been
jutant General's Office (AGO), with himself and Bingham in part of the AGO.
charge. Following World War I, additional improvements in person-
The Scott-Bingham group was called the Committee on Clas- nel psychology were made during the 1920s. Link (1919), Korn-
sification of Personnel and began with 10 academic psycholo- hauser and Kingsbury (1924), and Freyd (1923) discussed psy-
gists: Angell, Bingham, Raymond Dodge, Scott, J. E Shepard, chological techniques for personnel selection (Freyd's book be-
E. K. Strong, Lewis Terman, E. L. Thorndike, John B. Watson, ing noted by Guion in 1976 to be quite current). Viteles (1926b)
and Robert M. Yerkes, who were joined by two businessmen, and Bingham (1926) discussed the measurement of work perfor-
Robert Clothier and Horace Gardner. This committee probably mance, providing several warnings that are accepted as good
did as much work for the Army as Yerkes' group, with Scott practice today and showing early awareness of halo error. Korn-
rising to the rank of colonel and Bingham to lieutenant colonel. hauser (1926-1927) reported three empirical studies of ratings,
Their work was reported in The Personnel System of the United using as subjects senior undergraduates of the University of
States Army (1919). However, the SGO group under Yerkes re- Chicago who were rated by faculty. He addressed the reliability
ceived the larger share of publicity on the basis of their develop- of average ratings, comparisons of raters, and comparisons of
ment of the Army Alpha and Beta group mental ability exami- ratings on different traits. Millicent Pond (1927) published her
nations. Results of using those two scales demonstrated the dissertation in the Journal of Personnel Research, in which she
feasibility of large-scale selection testing and paved the way for evaluated the selection and placement of apprentice metal-
later expansion of the testing movement into government, in- workers at the Scoville Manufacturing Company.
dustry, and education (Hale, 1982; Sokal, 1987). Ferguson (1962-1965) recounted how the years after the war
On a darker note, the misinterpretation of the results of intel- saw the expansion of the Carnegie Institute of Technology work
ligence testing also led to proposals that could inflict wrongs into sales training, using as a vehicle the Research Bureau of
against certain ethnic subgroups. For example, Brigham's Retail Training under J. B. Miner and later W W Charters.
(1923) analysis of the Army data led him to conclude that race Although earlier there had been sporadic research done on
differences in intelligence existed and to argue that if un- training techniques, largely in the laboratory (cf. Viteles, 1932),
checked they could lead to deterioration of the national talent the approach at the bureau was focused in the field and in-
pool. Yerkes, in the forward to Brigham's (1923) book, noted volved job analysis, behavioral observation, and feedback. A
ominously the "menace of race deterioration" (p. vii). Those young psychologist associated with that work was E. K. Strong,
data were used in the debates of the early 1920s to try to reduce Jr. (Strong, 1922).
immigration from countries in which "intelligence" levels were Another significant postwar contribution of the Carnegie
lower. Lesser known is Brigham's recantation of those conclu- group was research on the measurement of vocational interests
sions (Brigham, 1930), based on self-acknowledged errors of (Fryer, 1931). Bruce V Moore's doctoral thesis, as well as that of
reification, inability to combine the Alpha and Beta scores into Merrill J. Ream and several others, examined the differentia-
a composite, and the recognition that both tests were verbally tion of occupational groups (for example, sales engineers and
and culturally loaded. Duels in the popular press between design engineers) by means of interest questionnaires. Moore's
Walter Lippmann and Lewis Terman were another part of this was almost certainly the first doctorate in industrial psychol-
debate (Pastore, 1978). The issue, one of ethnic differences ogy when it was awarded in 1921. He went on to teach at Penn-
in general mental ability, is recurrent (cf. Ackerman & sylvania State College for many years, eventually becoming de-
Humphreys, 1990; Gould, 1981; Herrnstein, 1971; Linn, 1985). partment chair, and was elected the first president of Division
Finally, late during the war a small program was established 14ofAPAinl945. Strong was also among those involved in the
in the Morale Branch of the SGO to facilitate the adjustment of work on interests, and, after moving to Stanford University in
soldiers to army life. Douglas Fryer, a young psychologist 1923, he developed with Karl Cowdery the famous Strong Vo-
APA CENTENNIAL: HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF I-O PSYCHOLOGY 807

cational Interest Blank (Strong, 1943), now known in revised the presidency in 1921, a position that he held for the next 19
form as the Strong-Campbell Interest Inventory. However, the years.
bright future of the Carnegie group ended when the Division of The other consulting organization founded in the wake of
Applied Psychology was dissolved in 1923 by an unsympathetic World War I was The Psychological Corporation. It was orga-
president (the I-O program revived in 1945 and continues today nized in 1921 by J. McKeen Cattell, with 20 influential psychol-
at Carnegie-Mellon University). Nonetheless, this group laid a ogists as directors and some 170 psychologists holding stock
foundation for the young discipline that was firmly grounded (Cattell, 1923). Sokal (1981) described the organization as simi-
in basic psychological principles and based on collaborative lar to a medieval guild, whereas Cattell himself used the term
research with industry. Many of its products and their students "holding company for psychologists." Cattell was chairman of
went on to expand the scope of I-O psychology through aca- the board from 1921-1926 and Henry C. Link was president.
demic training (e.g., Moore, Strong, Glen Cleeton) and practice There were several reasons that the firm did not flourish under
(e.g., Beardsley Ruml, Richard Uhrbrock), as well as through Cattell (Sokal, 1981), but subsequent leadership by Bingham,
research. Link, Paul Achilles, and George Bennett led to a major role for
the firm in disseminating and applying psychological tech-
niques for some 50 years. Divisions of tests, counseling, indus-
Roles trial consulting, and marketing served to propagate applied
psychology (e.g., Achilles, 1946). The Differential Aptitude
The period from after World War I to the Great Depression Tests and the Bennett Test of Mechanical Comprehension are
saw the extension of techniques and programs developed for examples of instruments developed by this firm that are still in
the Army into private industry. Those extensions were facili- wide use. The Psychological Corporation was acquired in 1970
tated by two consulting organizations founded expressly to ac- by the publishing firm of Harcourt, Brace, Jovanovich and sur-
complish those ends, which also provided employment and a vives to this day, although its major focus is now limited to test
critical mass for psychologists interested in I-O psychology. development and distribution.
One of those two organizations was The Scott Company, When \erkes was put in charge of the SGO Section of Psy-
founded in 1919 by Walter Dill Scott and colleagues from the chology as a major in the Sanitary Corps, he became one of the
Committee on Classification of Personnel and Carnegie Insti- first full-time I-O psychologists, although he might not have
tute of Technology. Its members adapted for industrial use a recognized the appellation. Similar full-time positions were oc-
number of the techniques and programs that had been devised cupied by a number of other psychologists in the military. It is
at Carnegie. The resulting armamentarium of psychological also noteworthy that I-O psychologists soon began to be em-
techniques included a group test of mental ability, job stan- ployed not only in academia, the military, and consulting orga-
dards for career progression and personnel planning, a perfor- nizations, but also full-time in government and private in-
mance rating system, oral trade tests and apprentice training dustry. Shortly after World War I, the U.S. Civil Service Com-
materials, and a program of personnel administration. The mission started looking into the feasibility of psychologically
company also promoted a forward-looking philosophy of coop- examining applicants, engaging as consultants the famous be-
erative labor relations that was later adopted in the men's gar- haviorist, John B. Watson, and The Scott Company's Beardsley
ment industry (cf. Ferguson, 1962; Gordon & Burt, 1981). An- Ruml. Those inquiries led to the establishment of a research
other important contribution of The Scott Company was the section under L. J. O'Rourke in 1922 (O'Rourke, 1926,1934),
concept of the "worker in his work" that foreshadowed the which has survived to this day under various designations. Job
development of the organizational branch of I-O psychology. redesign, "testing the tests" against criteria, and cost-benefit
Key individuals in The Scott Company included Robert analyses were all researched. However, obtaining the active sup-
Clothier, who later became president of Rutgers University and port of top management, the labor unions, and the Civil Ser-
coauthored a long-running book on personnel administration vice Commission was always a crucial first step for O'Rourke
with Scott (Scott & Clothier, 1923); Beardsley Ruml, who later (1950). O'Rourke (1950) and Kavruck (1956) described the early
became the treasurer of R. H. Macy and then an administrator projects undertaken by the research section. Ernest Primoff
of the Laura Spelman Rockefeller Memorial and who, as a (1991) recently provided an extensive review of Civil Service
member of F. D. Roosevelt's "brain trust" during the 1940s, Commission personnel activities from the late 1800s to World
devised the "pay as you go" income tax system; and Arthur War II.
Kornhauser, who later had a distinguished teaching and re- In private industry, too, psychologists began to find employ-
search career at Chicago University and Wayne State University. ment. Harry Hepner, later of Syracuse University, was em-
After a short but brilliant existence, The Scott Company went ployed by the Kaufman Department Store in Pittsburgh during
out of business in the early 1920s. Was this a failure of industrial the early 1920s, in the wake of the Carnegie Institute of Technol-
psychology? No. Partly, the firm was a victim of the depression ogy work on retail training. Ruml, as we have noted, went on
of 1921 -1922, but largely it was a victim of its own success, for from The Scott Company to Macy's, where Elsie O. Bregman
its key personnel received so much favorable attention that they had been evaluating clerical and sales personnel (Sokal, 1981).
were lured away by attractive offers, indicating that there were Several life insurance firms decided to hire in-house psycholo-
not enough I-O psychologists to satisfy the demand for their gists, Marion Bills at Aetna being a noteworthy example.
services. In addition to the new jobs already noted for various Among manufacturing companies, Winchester employed Link
staff, Scott returned to Northwestern University and accepted as a consultant from 1917-1919, after which he worked with the
808 RAYMOND A. KATZELL AND JAMES T. AUSTIN

U.S. Rubber Company, and Procter & Gamble hired H. G. Ke- for psychologists to obtain full-time employment as well as
nagy in 1923 to do research on sales personnel following the part-time consulting by others who were primarily academi-
demise of the Carnegie Institute of Technology program. cians. Those factors also helped spark increased attention to
Procter & Gamble in particular has maintained a long tradition scientific and technical topics relevant to industrial psychology,
of hiring psychologists, its staff later in the period including both in applied settings and academia. In the discipline of psy-
Richard Uhrbrock, a Carnegie Tech product, and Paul Horst, a chology, behaviorism (Watson, 1919,1925) gave further impe-
former Thurstone student. Uhrbrock established the psycholog- tus to objective studies of behavior and their practical applica-
ical research department at Procter & Gamble as part of the tions and also underscored the impact of environments on be-
Industrial Relations Division in 1930. Viteles, briefly, and after havior. However, we should not overlook the fact that some of
him Sadie Myers Shellow, were employed by the Milwaukee the thrust to take advantage of those opportunities came from
Electric Railway and Light Company during the 1920s. within the field. Academics like Bingham, Cattell, Scott, Vi-
In 1920 there was still only a handful of I-O psychologists. teles, and Yerkes were visionary in seeing ways in which their
Terman (1921), in analyzing the membership of APA, esti- fledgling discipline could be applied to industrial and national
mated that number to have been about 18, of whom only about needs (Ferguson, 1961; Viteles, 1967,1974) and in identifying
7 were clearly in industrial employment. There were a few addi- relevant subjects for scientific research and development. Such
tional I-O psychologists, like H. E. Burtt, whose principal em- vision and initiative will continue to be worthy of emulation.
ployment was centered at a university. Their number increased
later in the decade as employment opportunities expanded in
industry, government, and consulting firms. However, judging The 1930s to World War II
from APA membership, we estimate that the total number of Topics
I-O psychologists was still less than 50 by the decade's end.
Psychologists with strong I-O interests also were slowly being One of the salient developments of this decade actually be-
added to university faculties. Examples, in addition to those gan in the late 1920s. We refer to the Hawthorne studies, which
already cited, include Viteles at the University of Pennsylvania, again represent an instance in which a firm, in this case the
Miner at Ohio University, F. A. Kingsbury at the University of Western Electric Company, arranged to collaborate with a
Chicago, D. G. Paterson at the University of Minnesota, Elton group of academicians in an effort to improve operating effi-
Mayo at Harvard University, and Fryer at New York University. ciency. Those academics, led by Elton Mayo (1933) and T. N.
Graduate training in I-O psychology expanded slowly during Whitehead (1935, 1938), were on the faculty of the Harvard
the decade. The early history of the program at Ohio State Business School. The original experiments, as is well known,
University furnishes a typical example. Harold E. Burtt joined dealt with modifications of illumination, wage incentives, and
its faculty in 1919, having received a doctorate in general-exper- rest pauses—topics that would have gladdened the heart of
imental psychology at Harvard. He was soon followed by the F W Taylor. But what was to capture the attention of the disci-
psychometric specialist, Herbert Toops, and together they de- pline and the world was not those substantive results but the
veloped the first doctoral specialization in industrial psychol- partly serendipitous findings highlighting the importance of
ogy (Weiss, 1927) after the one at Carnegie Institute of Technol- social relationships: team development, supervision, and group
ogy. In addition to Carnegie Tech and Ohio State, institutions norms (Roethlisberger & Dickson, 1939). Less well-remem-
that turned out industrially oriented doctorates during the bered are the employee interviewing and counseling programs
1920s included the University of Minnesota and Stanford Uni- that continued long after the other experiments ended (Dick-
versity. son, 1950; Dickson & Roethlisberger, 1966).
Another noteworthy aspect of the Hawthorne studies was
that they involved scientific field research, including experi-
Forces
mental and observational techniques. We mentioned earlier the
Reflecting on what might help to account for those develop- field experiments of Taylor and Gilbreth, but they were quite
ments, we note several major external events. One, of course, unsophisticated in terms of research design and data analysis,
was World War I, which created an occasion and a need for probably because they were carried out in the tradition of man-
applying budding psychological concepts and methods. An- agement rather than behavioral science. On the other hand, in
other was the postwar period of prosperity. For example, de- the Hawthorne studies, the interventions were for the first time
spite a brief recession in 1921-1922, the gross national product planned by behavioral scientists, and there were systematic ef-
rose by 39% between 1919 and 1929 (Cashman, 1989). Growth forts at experimental and statistical controls, even if imperfect.
of employment accompanied that development, thereby creat- As a postscript, Hawthorne bashing became quite popular after
ing opportunities for I-O psychologists to practice their profes- the reports of the research were promulgated (e.g., Carey, 1967;
sion. Another factor was that wartime experiences alerted some Landsberger, 1958), even extending as far as a recent Marxist
forward-looking executives to the value of engaging psycholo- critique based on the concept of class struggle (Bramel &
gists as consultants or employees. The experiences of Viteles Friend, 1981). Often such critics may miss an important point,
(1967,1974) are illustrative. Additionally, the postwar growth for despite flaws of design and analysis, it was those studies and
of the field of employment management (described by Fer- their philosophy that helped to launch the organizational
guson, 1962-1965) was favorable to the applications of psy- branch of the I-O discipline.
chology. A second salient development of the 1930s was the study of
In combination, those circumstances created opportunities employee attitudes and morale, reflecting a concern with the
APA CENTENNIAL: HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF I-O PSYCHOLOGY 809

worker's perspective as well as that of employers. Although intervention in the Weldon unit to create a climate to support
there were earlier studies of employees' attitudes toward their the successful pattern of management then in place at the Har-
jobs and organizations, this subject now began to take wing wood unit (Marrow, Bowers, & Seashore, 1967). The Harwood
(Kornhauser, 1929b). Among the landmark studies of this era and Weldon studies were conducted under the auspices of
were ones by Uhrbrock (1934) at Procter & Gamble, by Korn- Alfred Marrow, who had in 1937 received a doctorate in psychol-
hauser and Sharp (1932) at the Kimberly-Clark Corporation, ogy from New York University and who at the time was a top
and by Hoppock (1935), who surveyed the job attitudes of the executive of the companies.
entire town of New Hope, Pennsylvania. Attitude surveys and Although work continued on selection, appraisal, and train-
structured interviews were a major tool of management consul- ing techniques as the industrial core of the field, it was charac-
tant J. D. Houser (1927,1938). The measurement of attitudes terized more by refinement of what had already been devised
was improved during this period by the scaling techniques of than by conceptual breakthroughs. Noteworthy was the estab-
L. L. Thurstone (1927; Thurstone & Chave, 1929) and Likert lishment of the Minnesota Employment Stabilization Research
(1932). We should note that Thurstone had been a member of Institute in 1931 (cf. Paterson, 1932; Paterson & Darley, 1936).
the Division of Applied Psychology and head of the Depart- That organization developed numerous tests (e.g., the Minne-
ment of Psychology at Carnegie Institute of Technology before sota Clerical Test) and the concept of occupational aptitude
moving to the University of Chicago and achieving fame as a patterns (Dvorak, 1935), as well as elaborating the "Minnesota
quantitative psychologist. However, despite this beginning, rela- point of view" that culminated in the theory of work adjust-
tively few companies conducted attitude surveys during this ment (Dawis & Lofquist, 1984). Another important method-
period (National Industrial Conference Board, 1951). ological contribution to mental measurement was the refine-
A third development centered around Kurt Lewin, one of the ment and growing use of exploratory factor analysis (cf. Guil-
notable expatriate psychologists from Nazi Germany. Although ford, 1936; Thurstone, 1931).
his initial American post was at the University of Iowa Child Worthy of note in the industrial core of the field was the
Welfare Research Station in 1935, Lewin was a researcher with reinvigoration of the U.S. Employment Service (USES), origi-
broad interests, among them the psychological aspects of work. nally founded in 1917, by the Wagner-Peyser Act of 1933. Both
One of his earliest papers, for example, examined the virtues the Minnesota and USES programs tried to bridge the gap
and limitations of Taylorism and scientific management between those unemployed as a result of the Depression and
(Lewin, 1920, cited by Marrow, 1969). Many of his students at the meager existing job openings. The USES is known for as-
Iowa also had I-O interests, including John French, Ronald Lip- sembling the General Aptitude Test Battery (GATB), the Dictio-
pitt, and Alex Bavelas. It is hardly surprising, then, that one of nary of Occupational Titles (DOT), and an influential book on
the famous experiments undertaken at Iowa by Lewin and his occupational placement and counseling techniques (Stead &
students was an investigation of the effects of leadership cli- Shartle, 1940). Person analysis within the Worker Analysis Sec-
mates on productivity and satisfaction (Lewin, Lippitt, & tion was guided by refinements of the factor-analytic results of
White, 1939). Other studies done under Lewin's influence dealt Thurstone's group and by job analysis using systematic obser-
with such aspects of group dynamics as cooperation and com- vation and recording of job behavior and worker attributes (Vi-
petition, group structure, communication patterns, and addi- teles, 1922) as further developed by Shartle's Occupational
tional topics that have come to play an important part in our Analysis Section. Those two branches continued the parallel
thinking about organizational life. Lewin also engaged in emphases of person and job analysis initiated by Scott's World
trailblazing theoretical work that led to expectancy theory, War I Committee on Classification of Personnel. It is of current
which he and his associates called "resultant valence" theory. interest that the GATB has been embroiled in a value- and
His research on individual as well as group levels of aspiration policy-laden dispute regarding employment discrimination
(Lewin, Dembo, Festinger, & Sears, 1944) was reintegrated into (Adler, 1991), specifically "race norming" as recommended by
his applied work and continues to exert an influence on contem- a National Academy of Sciences panel (Hartigan & Wigdor,
porary I-O psychology, including goal setting. 1989). However, the Civil Rights Act of 1991 now prohibits race
Methodologically, Lewin and his students espoused "action norming.
research"—experimentation jointly planned, executed, and
evaluated by researchers and participants and designed to illu- Roles
minate a problem so that solutions could be tried, evaluated,
and refined (cf. Clarke, 1980). Modern ideas of group feedback A new role grew out of the Hawthorne and Lewin research
methods (Heller, 1969), the "experimenting society" (D. Camp- programs, that of action researcher. This role combines active
bell, 1968), and the field of evaluation research can be viewed intervention with scientific research procedures, demonstrat-
as outgrowths of this beginning. Lewin also believed that the- ing and dependent on an interplay between science and prac-
ories and ideas needed to be investigated not only in small-scale tice.
laboratory experiments, but on a larger scale in the real world. Pennsylvania State College (now University) offered a doctor-
One illustration of that approach was the studies of resistance ate in I-O psychology beginning in 1930 under Bruce Moore,
to change and employee participation to facilitate change. Ini- thus joining Ohio State University, the University of Minnesota,
tial studies were done at the Harwood Manufacturing Com- and Stanford University as the major specialized programs. As
pany by Coch and French (1948), with Lewin as a consultant. the decade progressed, several additional institutions began
Subsequently, in 1962, Harwood acquired the Weldon Com- training students who were oriented toward I-O careers; these
pany, and a team of Lewin-influenced researchers designed an included Purdue University, Columbia University, University of
810 RAYMOND A. KATZELL AND JAMES T. AUSTIN

Pennsylvania, and New \brk University. The term program is work force unemployed (Manchester, 1973-1974). Hence,
perhaps too grand to be applied to those early endeavors, as there was less need for and interest in personnel selection and
prior to World War II doctoral training in psychology was not training. For example, more than 16% of companies that had
fragmented into the specialized programs existing today. To the used personnel tests dropped them during the Great Depres-
extent that specialization occurred, it consisted mainly of the sion (National Industrial Conference Board, 1940). Also, none
student's choice of (or assignment to) a principal mentor and of the references cited in an extensive review of the psychologi-
associated research and dissertation topics. A series of articles cal literature on training in industry was published during the
describing those early I-O doctoral training activities at various 1930s (McGehee & Thayer, 1961). Productivity enhancement
universities was published beginning in 1991 in The Industrial- also was less urgent, because there were no longer ready mar-
Organizational Psychologist, a publication of Division 14 of the kets.
APA (Katzell, 1991; Lawshe & Weiss, 1991; Prien, 1991). In The Great Depression and its adverse effects on people led to
addition to the graduates of those institutions, many I-O psy- heightened sensitivity to and concern for the human condition
chologists received their training in other areas of psychology and the humanization of work. This was personified by the
(Howard, 1982). The number of people identifying with I-O new president, E D. Roosevelt. "Identifying himself with the
psychology continued to increase. Perhaps the clearest evi- forgotten man at the base of the economic pyramid, he led the
dence of this was the establishment during 1937-1938 of the New Deal, a comprehensive program of legislation designed to
American Association for Applied Psychology (AAAP), which raise the Great Depression and remedy basic inequalities in the
included an Industrial and Business Psychology Section structure of American society . . ." (Cashman, 1989, p. 147).
(Bernreuter, Maxfield, Paterson, Reymert, & Fryer, 1937). By Those concerns were reflected as well in the growth of the
1943,79 people joined the industrial section of AAAP, suggest- social aspects of I-O psychology.
ing that the total number of I-O psychologists probably The work at both Western Electric's Hawthorne plant and at
amounted to about 100 in 1939—a doubling but hardly a dra- Harwood-Weldon has been accused of being antilabor (Baritz,
matic leap from 1929. The growth was not explosive in num- 1960). Those interventions were viewed by some as attempting
bers, but the field's identification as a profession did increase. to quell labor unrest and union activism by nonfinancial pal-
J. P. Symonds (1946) described the development of AAAP from liatives; from that standpoint, it has been argued that those
earlier roots in the Association of Consulting Psychologists and developments were really contrary to the long-run interests of
the Section of Clinical Psychology of the APA. Although workers. Our own evaluation is that while such work may in-
AAAP was founded to foster the interests of all branches of deed have mitigated labor-management antagonism, we do not
applied psychology, which many felt were not adequately served see why it would have hampered constructive relations between
by the APA, a large impetus came from industrial psychologist the parties. In fact, we think that the recent history of union-
Douglas Fryer. Fryer, by now a professor at New York Univer- management cooperation in such areas as work humanization
sity, was recognized by election as the first president of AAAP. (Banas, 1988) and production improvement (Hicks, 1992) bears
Harold E. Burtt of Ohio State University was elected the first out that belief.
president of the AAAP's Industrial and Business Psychology On the other hand, it cannot be gainsaid that most I-O psy-
Section. chologists were (and continue to be) managerially oriented, as
As a sidelight relevant to the APA centennial, it should be perusal of a random sample of I-O textbooks reveals. This is not
recalled that the reorganization of APA following World War II to say that I-O psychologists were or are generally antilabor.
involved a merger with AAAR This reformation gave explicit Most would probably still subscribe to the position set forth
recognition of the applied as well as the scientific concerns of early on by Munsterberg (1913), who argued that the results of
psychologists. AAAP's sectional structure was adopted and ex- industrial psychology would benefit workers as well as em-
panded by the reorganized APA (Doll, 1946), creating Division ployers. Nevertheless, even the studies of worker attitudes were
14 as the Division of Business and Industrial Psychology. generally motivated more by the interests of management than
by concern for employees (Baritz, 1960; Viteles, 1953). In any
case, few companies bothered to survey employee attitudes for
Forces whatever reason during the 1930s and early 1940s (National
The three aforementioned topical breakthroughs repre- Industrial Conference Board, 1951; Vroom, 1964a).
sented the early development of the organizational side of I-O The general noninvolvement of I-O psychology in issues of
psychology. Although those developments are often character- labor-management relations during that period is further re-
ized as applied social psychology, it is doubtful that they would flected in the book Industrial Conflict: A Psychological Inter-
have taken the form that they did had they not been studied in pretation (Hartmann & Newcomb, 1939). S. P. Hayes, Korn-
organizational contexts. We therefore consider them to be as hauser, and Moore were the only identifiable I-O psychologists
much contributions of I-O psychology to social psychology as among the 26 contributors. Moreover, it was sponsored not by
the reverse. By contrast, the refinements in personnel and in- the industrial section of AAAP, but by the Society for the Psy-
dustrial psychology, or the more traditional work in selection, chological Study of Social Issues, which was much involved in
training, and appraisal, were less dramatic during this period the issues of labor (Finison, 1979).
and thus could be characterized as an incremental or gradual It is also worth noting that, in a period when unemployment
process of development. The explanation again seems to be at was rampant, psychologists in general paid relatively little at-
least partly due to the socioeconomic context of the times. The tention to the unemployed. The rare exceptions included a
country was in the grips of a severe depression, with 25% Of its study of the psychological effects of unemployment by Jahoda,
APA CENTENNIAL: HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF I-O PSYCHOLOGY 811

Lazarsfeld, and Zeisel (1933/1972), but their study was con- settings (e.g., Sisson, 1950); the foundations of utility analysis
ducted in Austria before they emigrated to the United States. were proposed at this time as well (Brogden, 1946,1949; Brog-
Other exceptional examples included Rundquist and Sletto's den & Taylor, 1950). Subsequently, industry and government
(1936) examination of the attitudes and personalities of unem- expanded the use of these techniques after the war (Viteles,
ployed workers, a similar study by the Minnesota Unemploy- 1946). Women were integrated into the work force in large num-
ment Research Project (Paterson & Darley, 1936), and Hop- bers during the war to release men for combat, one side effect
pock's (1935) inclusion of unemployed people in his survey of being the demonstration that women could handle "male"
attitudes of New Hope residents. jobs. Finally, in a harbinger of the future, racial integration in
Why the relative inattention to the human condition? Partly the military was initiated late in the war by an executive order
the bias of the auspices. Industry was buying the services of I-O of President Truman.
psychologists to solve their problems, not those of the workers; Summaries of the wartime work include the 19-volume ac-
unions, at least until recently (cf. Gordon & Nurick, 1981; count of the Aviation Psychology Program edited by Flanagan
Lewin, 1986; "Psychologists and Unions," 1981), were more (1947-1949), R. L. Thorndike's (1949) codification of person-
concerned with economic issues than with psychological ones. nel selection procedures that emphasized criterion and opera-
Even our government has historically failed to subsidize inqui- tional issues, Stuit's (1947) report of work done for the Bureau
ries into the basic psychosocial aspects of work and unemploy- of Naval Personnel by the National Defense Research Commit-
ment (cf. Jahoda, 1981; Schneider, 1991; Terkel, 1972). How- tee and the College Entrance Examination Board, Murray and
ever, the blame should not just lie with our stars. Kenneth associates' account of situational and holistic testing for the
Clark's (1957) study of American psychologists showed that I-O Office of Strategic Services (Office of Strategic Services, 1948),
psychologists were more oriented toward data than toward peo- and the social psychological studies in the army reported by
ple. We suspect that, as a group, I-O psychologists have not been Stouffer et al. (1949). Accounts of the extensive research and
aroused much professionally to weep for the common person development (R&D) work conducted by the AGO have ap-
(cf. Baritz, 1960). On the other hand, the impetus toward in- peared in scattered articles (e.g., Harrell, in press; Sisson, 1950).
creased applications of psychology, as evidenced by the afore- Although those accounts of military developments generally
mentioned founding of AAAP, was probably motivated in part were full of optimism, Bingham (1949) provided a sobering
by the desire to involve psychology more directly in promoting "confession of shortcomings," echoed by Clark (1947) and by
human welfare. Cronbach and Neff (1949). Those authors suggested that many
opportunities had been lost due to poor planning, duplication
of work, inability to concentrate efforts where they were most
World War II to the Mid-1960s needed, and failures to measure criteria adequately. As was true
Topics after World War I, the fruits of World War II developments were
exported into both the public and private sectors. As we discuss
As with World War I, World War II turned out to be a tremen- later, a number of companies established psychological R&D
dous catalyst for the development of I-O psychology. Hundreds wings in their personnel departments, university-based centers
of psychologists, representing a variety of specialties, were em- were formed for research and training in the field, and many
ployed by the armed forces (Bingham, 1947; Britt & Morgan, consulting firms came into existence. To a considerable extent,
1946); many others were employed by civilian agencies sup- the work of I-O psychologists represented extensions of the I
porting the military. Bingham and Yerkes, so instrumental aspects of the field that were the main focus of the military; job
along with Scott in the effort during World War I, were called analysis, selection, assessment, performance appraisal, and
on to guide similar efforts for World War II (Bingham, 1947; training were major foci of attention.
Yerkes, 1941). Bingham served as chief psychologist of the War Another substantive set of topics was not just a linear extrapo-
Department from 1940-1947, among other things helping to lation from wartime developments. We noted earlier the bud-
plan the development of the Army General Classification Test ding of industrial social psychology during the 1930s. By 1950,
(AGCT), and his mediating style and mature experience may when Viteles began to revise his landmark 1932 book on indus-
have been responsible for the great impact of psychological trial psychology, he found that the field had changed so much in
principles and techniques (Bingham, 1947). Those efforts did the intervening two decades that he had to write quite a differ-
pay off, in terms of demonstrably valid selection techniques, ent book. The nature of that change is denoted by the resulting
including the AGCT (Harrell, in press), the beginnings of the title of the book, Motivation and Morale in Industry (Viteles,
assessment center method in the United States (Murray & 1953). In short, although work in what is conventionally called
MacKinnon, 1946; Office of Strategic Services, 1948), and the industrial and personnel psychology continued, the field that
Aircrew Classification Test battery (Flanagan, 1946, 1947- we now call organizational psychology had become a major
1949). Appraisal procedures (e.g., forced-choice ratings), train- partner. One contrast between the partners is between a focus
ing methods (e.g., job instructional training), team develop- on the individual worker and on working in social and organiza-
ment strategies, morale and attitude change methods (Stouffer tional contexts (Flanagan, 1981; Leavitt, 1962).
et al., 1949), and equipment design (e.g., airplane instrumenta- I-O psychology was now equally concerned with fitting peo-
tion) were other results of the systematic application of psycho- ple to their work and fitting work to people, at the level of the
logical principles to wartime problems. Moreover, it was dem- organization and work group as well as the job. Representing
onstrated conclusively that these approaches could be applied that approach was the development of several university-based
to a wide range of activities and in different kinds of work programs, including the Research Center for Group Dynamics,
812 RAYMOND A. KATZELL AND JAMES T. AUSTIN

established under Lewin at Massachusetts Institute of Technol- from psychology. Psychologists active in that field also have
ogy and later moved to the University of Michigan as part of formed their own APA division, the Division of Consumer Psy-
Likert's Institute for Social Research; the leadership research chology (Division 23).
program at Ohio State University under C. L. Shartle and R. M. The postwar period witnessed a stirring of interest in labor
Stogdill; the Cornell University studies of satisfaction in work relations on the part of I-O psychologists (cf. Gordon & Burt,
and retirement led by Patricia C. Smith; and programs of educa- 1981). However, that interest, at least insofar as it led to publi-
tion and research at the Harvard Business School, Yale Univer- cations, was limited to a few I-O researchers, among them Wil-
sity, New York University, Carnegie-Mellon University, Michi- lard Kerr (1954; Keenan & Kerr, 1952), Arthur Kornhauser
gan State University, and other universities. The human rela- (1949), Theodore Purcell (1953), Hjalmar Rosen (1954; Rosen
tions perspective became salient with concerns expressed & Rosen, 1955), and Ross Stagner (1956; Institute of Labor and
regarding adverse effects on employees of traditional organiza- Industrial Relations, 1953-1954). Topics of attention included
tional policies and practices (Argyris, 1957; McGregor, 1960). conditions associated with cooperation and conflict between
Intergroup and intragroup relations, job satisfaction, commu- unions and management and attitudes of workers toward em-
nication, organizational factors, and other socially focused sub- ployers and unions, including the issue of dual allegiance. Nev-
jects became increasingly popular. Among these, the topic of ertheless, as Shostak (1964) concluded after reviewing efforts by
leadership rose to particular prominence; the subject of arm- management, I-O psychologists, and labor unions, the domi-
chair speculation and trait-based analyses before World War II, nant relationship between I-O psychologists and unions was
it now became the focus of much theory and research (e.g., one of mutual indifference.
Guetzkow, 1951). The work of Herzberg and his associates
(Herzberg, 1966; Herzberg, Mausner, & Snyderman, 1959) stim-
Roles
ulated attention to designing jobs so that they are positively
motivating, not just devoid of sources of dissatisfaction, and Even more than was the case after World War I, the momen-
thereby helped pave the way for the later quality of work life tum acquired by I-O psychology during World War II carried
movement. Two new journals, Human Relations, first pub- over into training and into employment opportunities. The pe-
lished in 1948 by the Tavistock Institute of Human Relations riod immediately after World War II saw the expansion of spe-
and the Research Center for Group Dynamics, and Administra- cialized I-O training, including the development of terminal
tive Science Quarterly, first published in 1955, gave researchers master's programs (Clark, 1957). Institutions already offering
additional outlets for their work and reports on these kinds of the doctorate expanded their programs (Ohio State University,
topics. as noted by Wherry, 1968) and new programs came into exis-
Another movement relating psychology to organizations was tence (e.g., George Washington University in 1948, University of
the interest in decision making. Initiated at Carnegie Institute Maryland in 1961, Michigan State University in 1951). Aca-
of Technology by Herbert Simon, James March, and Richard demic training continued to emphasize basic and methodologi-
Cyert, work on this subject was based on the thesis that infor- cal topics, although specialized courses in the I-O field became
mation and decisions lay at the heart of understanding organi- more feasible as research accumulated, theories were devel-
zations. The foundations were established in 1947 with the pub- oped, and the numbers of faculty members and students grew.
lication of Simon's Administrative Behavior, followed by March There was also a particularly marked increase in psychologists
and Simon's (1958) Organizations, and Cyert and March's (1963) on the faculties of business schools beginning in the 1960s. This
A Behavioral Theory of the Firm. That body of work helped to migration, which included many prominent psychologists, may
bring the perspective of cognitive psychology into the I-O field. have served as a counterforce to excessive professionalism in
The other major tradition of adapting work to workers, that those schools that was predicted in evaluative reports commis-
of human engineering or applied experimental psychology (er- sioned by the Ford Foundation and Carnegie Foundation in the
gonomics in Europe), began to shift its principal identification late 1950s (Gordon & Howell, 1959; Pierson, 1959).
with psychology to that of an interdisciplinary field after World Employment opportunities outside of academia also grew
War II (Chapanis, Garner, & Morgan, 1949; Fitts, 1951). The apace. In the military, there was no abrupt cessation of interest
hvbrid field involved contributions from biology, systems analy- in applied psychology as had occurred after World War I, al-
^is, engineering, and computer science, as well as psychology though the latter had been followed by a rapid demilitarization.
proper. Nevertheless, psychologists working in that field have Each branch of the armed services created centers of research
maintained their affiliation with the APA, but with a division activity to continue the benefits of the World War II efforts
separate from Division 14, namely Division 21, Applied Experi- (Hill, 1955; Melton, 1957). The Army Research Institute (ARI),
mental and Engineering Psychology. the Navy Personnel Research and Development Center
Consumer and advertising psychology, which Kuna (1976, (NPRDC), and the Air Force Human Resources Laboratory
1979) noted had predated I-O psychology (e.g., Scott, 1903; (AFHRL) are the current versions of the earlier centers, while
Strong, 1913), also developed an identity apart from I-O psy- others have spun off as private agencies (e.g., RAND Corpora-
chology after the war, affiliating with such fields as marketing, tion, Human Resources Research Organization). Government,
economics, and sociology as well as personality and social psy- especially at the federal level, expanded both the number of
chology (Ferber, 1977; Jacoby, 1976). A summary that illus- psychologists and their responsibilities.
trates aspects of this interface is a National Science Foundation As was the case with The Scott Company and The Psychologi-
report entitled Selected Aspects of Consumer Behavior (1977), cal Corporation, a number of psychological research organiza-
which adds perspectives from economics and sociology to those tions (e.g., American Institutes for Research; Flanagan, 1981)
APA CENTENNIAL: HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF I-O PSYCHOLOGY 813

and consulting firms (e.g., Richardson, Bellows, Henry, & Com- low's (1954) hierarchical theory of motives. In any case, those
pany; Dunlap & Associates) were formed. Many of those organi- trends were reflected in a spate of literature critical of the stulti-
zations continue to function today (McKeachie & Brim, 1984). fying nature of most jobs and calling for expanded opportuni-
Research centers devoted at least in part to I-O psychology ties for personal growth and self-expression, represented by
sprang up at universities, including at the University of Michi- influential books by Argyris (1957) and McGregor (1960).
gan under Likert, at Ohio State University under C. L. Shartle, The modest quickening of I-O psychologists' attention to la-
at Western Reserve University under J. L. Otis (1946), and at bor-management relations in the decade following the war also
Purdue University under Joseph Tiffin and Charles Lawshe. was more a reactive response to external events than an out-
Psychologists in increasing numbers were employed full- growth of theory. The strains of wartime dislocations in the
time by private industry (Canter, 1948; Haire, 1959; Katzell, economy and in conditions of employment resulted in wide-
1962). During the 1950s, a number of companies established spread strife. Manchester (197 3-1974) reported that starting in
sizable research groups, including AT&T, General Electric, Pru- 1946 there were "wave after wave of strikes from Montauk
dential Insurance, Metropolitan Life, Sears-Roebuck, and Point to Malibu Beach, paralyzing the oil, lumber, textile, and
Standard Oil of New Jersey (Dunnette, 1962). electrical industries [and] newspapers began talking about the
By Jeanneret's (1991) estimate, there were in 1960 756 psy- workers' 'revolt' and labor's 'rebellion'" (p. 491). Concerned
chologists who were members of APAs Division 14, of whom I-O psychologists got the message and responded to it. How-
approximately one-quarter were employed in academic posi- ever, the flurry of attention was rather short-lived. It faded as
tions. Of those employed in nonacademic settings, about 75% labor-management conflict faded during the 1960s. Although
were involved in personnel selection and related activities, 16% progressive labor leaders like Solomon Barkin (1949, 1957)
in training work, and 38% in activities broadly related to the called attention to numerous other psychological topics perti-
organizational domain of the I-O field (Howard, 1982), the nent to unions, including leadership, communication, and
total amounting to more than 100% because some undoubtedly loyalty, I-O psychology on the whole remained uninterested.
specialized in more than one field. The post-World War II
Another influence that began at this time was the shift from
growth was obviously enormous.
a manufacturing-based economy toward a service-based econ-
omy. It was during the late 1950s that the proportion of workers
Forces providing services increased to more than 50% of the work
force (Bass, 1965). This trend has continued to the present (Bell,
When we seek to explain those developments, some answers
1972) and has several implications for the discipline, for exam-
seem apparent. Once more a war created needs and opportuni-
ple, the use of customer satisfaction as a criterion. However, I-O
ties for psychological applications. This time psychologists did
psychology as a whole paid little attention to that trend until
not have to petition to help—most were drafted by a society
that had learned to appreciate their contributions. The expo- later.
sure of many people in military and civilian life to I-O psycholo- Growth occurred in other applied psychological areas as well
gists and their work, coupled with evidence of utility, aroused and with it a felt need for legal control of those claiming to be
interest within industry and government, and the rapidly ex- psychological practitioners to protect the public. In the 1950s,
panding postwar economy afforded increased opportunities states began enacting licensing or certification legislation that
for applications and research. Many psychologists, neither inter- essentially limited use of the title of psychologist by practi-
ested in nor trained in I-O psychology, learned as a result of tioners to those meeting certain standards of education, experi-
their wartime exposure that the field had much to offer, intrin- ence, and competence as assessed by tests. APA, in a move to
sically as well as extrinsically speaking. The increased emphasis encourage uniformity of those standards, issued model licens-
on personal, social, and organizational factors in I-O psychol- ing guidelines in 1955 and revised them in 1967. As most of the
ogy may be partly explained by the entry into the field of people laws did not differentiate among fields of specialization, I-O
who had originally been given an orientation to social or clini- practitioners were included in their purview.
cal psychology as part of their training—French, Stogdill, I-O psychologists have disagreed on the question of whether
Miner, and Fred Fiedler are examples. It was not until late in licensure is appropriate for their practitioners (Schmitt, 1991).
this period that the field became heavily populated by those Some have argued that users of the services of I-O psycholo-
who had been trained from the outset as I-O psychologists (cf. gists, unlike the general public, are able to assess their qualifica-
Howard, 1990). tions, and, besides, incompetent services are unlikely to have
In addition, the external Zeitgeist played an important role. serious adverse consequences for individuals (cf. Howard &
The general revulsion against fascism and authoritarianism led Lowman, 1985). Others, though, have taken contrary positions
to an aroused interest in democracy and autonomy in the work- on those points (e.g., Katzell & Thompson, 1958) and, further-
place, an interest that was supported by the earlier findings of more, argue that exemption of I-O practitioners from licensure
the Hawthorne and Lewin research. Heightened awareness of undesirably distances the field from the rest of psychology
post-World War II opportunities for the good life (prosperity, (Vandaveer, 1991). In any event, judging from the fact that 72%
leisure) stimulated increased concern with such noneconomic of nonacademic Division 14 members were licensed as late as
outcomes as job satisfaction and self-fulfillment (Katzell, 1980 and that the percentage has been declining, we estimate
1958). As the period of the 1950s and 1960s was generally one of that in the prior decades a large majority of I-O practitioners
economic prosperity, the increased salience of noneconomic became licensed. Although psychologists whose employment
and personal rewards may be interpreted in the light of Mas- is purely academic are not required to be licensed, the fact that
814 RAYMOND A. KATZELL AND JAMES T. AUSTIN

many academicians in Division 14 have become licensed re- Force personnel, they did not begin to get into the act until the
flects their part-time involvement in practice as well. mid-1960s, with studies reported by Lopez (1966), Tenopyr
(1967), and Kirkpatrick, Ewen, Barrett, and Katzell (1968).
This early literature on validity and fairness in testing re-
Mid-1960s to Mid-1980s sulted in a rather mixed picture: Instances were reported in
Topics which the validity and fairness of tests were essentially equal for
different ethnic groups, and other instances were reported in
There was a growing tendency for research and practice to fit which disparities were found. The conventional wisdom was
more neatly into either industrial or organizational categories, that the validity of employment devices should be investigated
but not very much to address their interface (Bass, 1968) until for each ethnic group that was appreciably represented in the
late in the period. Those changes were symbolized by the re- work force of each employing organization. That policy was
naming of Division 14 in 1970 as the Division of Industrial and soon promulgated in the 1970 guidelines issued by the Equal
Organizational Psychology. Employment Opportunity Commission and the Office of Fed-
Because the scope of science and practice grew rapidly dur- eral Contract Compliance (Byham & Spitzer, 1971, Appendixes
ing this period, we have been forced to be somewhat selective in A & B). Not much attention was paid to the issue of differential
our choices of topics to cover. The industrial branch topics of fairness until later, despite the point made by Kirkpatrick et al.
greatest fecundity appear to have been job analysis, behav- (1968) that "unfair discrimination between ethnic groups can-
iorally anchored rating scales (BARS), differential validity, and not be inferred from evidence of differences in validity alone
validity generalization. Job analysis was advanced by the publi- . . . unfair discrimination will occur only if the predicted criterion
cation of the Position Analysis Questionnaire (McCormick, scores of one ethnic group are lower than their actual criterion
Jeanneret, & Mecham, 1972), which reflected refinements and scores . . ." (p. 7). In subsequent years, concern with ethnic
empirical work dating from 1959 at Purdue's Occupational Re- differences in test-criteria relationships appears to have sub-
search Center. This worker-oriented and standardized instru- sided somewhat. That was due in part to findings of an aggre-
ment promised to assist in the discovery of generalized families gate analysis reporting that significant differences in validity
of jobs, job evaluation, and in the development of tailored se- coefficients of selection tests for White and Black employees
lection devices. This instrument has been in use ever since, occurred only with chance frequency (Schmidt, Berner, &
despite some criticism concerning cross-job comparisons (Har- Hunter, 1973), although another aggregate analysis that se-
vey, 1991). A second advance consisted of Fine's (1971) func- lected only studies that contained significant coefficients found
tional job analysis method, which was institutionalized by the the frequency of significant ethnic differences to exceed chance
U.S. Department of Labor (1965) as a part of the third edition of expectations (Katzell & Dyer, 1977).
the Dictionary of Occupational Titles. Standardized task state- In a related development, Schmidt and Hunter (1977) re-
ments and the classification of work into interactions with data, ported a meta-analysis showing considerable generalization of
people, and things characterized this technique. Job analysis criterion validity of tests of general cognitive ability across nu-
using Primoff's job elements method (Primoff, 1975) was devel- merous samples of employees. Their work applied a Bayesian
oped in the federal civil service. model to accumulated distributions of criterion validity esti-
BARS were an outgrowth of the work of Smith and Kendall mates and involved corrections for various artifacts such as
(1963) for the National League for Nursing. Two major contri- small samples, restrictions in range, and unreliability of tests
butions of this technique were the involvement of raters in scale and criteria. Several early application studies supported the
development and the scaling of critical incidents into verbal usefulness of the technique for such occupational groupings as
anchors that provided a frame of reference for observing and petroleum, clerical, and supervisory workers. However, meta-
evaluating performance. Research since that time has failed to analysis is not without its critics (cf. Guzzo, Jackson, & Katzell,
confirm the expected advantages of the BARS format, but the 1987; James, Demaree, & Mulaik, 1986), and Schmidt and
careful construction procedures do provide legal defensibility, Hunter's generalizability conclusions were limited to tests of
feedback capabilities, and a common frame of reference. Also general intellectual functioning. This last development can be
during this period, a major shift was the increased attention to taken as vindication of Spearman's (1914, 1927) theory of a
the cognitive processes involved in rating (Feldman, 1981; general intellectual factor underlying human performance on
Landy&Farr, 1980). most tasks (Gottfredson, 1986). Only recently has the issue of
One of the major thrusts of this period, affecting both re- test fairness across ethnic groups begun to receive more atten-
search and practice, was concern with ethnic differences in the tion (Hartigan & Wigdor, 1989; Hunter, Schmidt, & Hunter,
validity and fairness of employment tests. The background for 1979).
and onset of that line of activity has been described by Byham Among the organizational topics of greatest interest during
and Spitzer (1971). As we will explain further in our section on this period were work motivation (including goal setting, ex-
Forces, the impetus was provided by the civil rights movement pectancy theory, and job characteristics) and job attitudes. A
that started in the 1950s and was codified by later legal actions. major approach to work motivation involved expectancy
Porter (1966), in his review of the I-O literature, observed that theory. March and Simon (1958) and Katzell (1964) published
despite the social and political pressure, I-O psychologists had versions of their approach, but the formulation that received
been paying little attention to the problem. Although an earlier the greatest attention from I-O psychologists was the one by
isolated study by Gordon (1953) reported that the cutting score Vroom (1964b) in his book Work and Motivation. That formula-
on a qualifying test was applicable to both White and Black Air tion elaborated the expectancy model rooted in the work of
APA CENTENNIAL: HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF I-O PSYCHOLOGY 815

E. C. Tolman, Kurt Lewin, and Helen Peak. Research during including laboratory settings and measurement of intrinsic mo-
the next 15 years proceeded to study this theory in depth, but tivation through task persistence.
usually had methodological and measurement-based flaws. The influence of Skinnerian neobehaviorism on I-O psychol-
Naylor, Pritchard, and Ilgen (1980) proposed a more general ogy and on management also took wing during this period.
theory of behavior in organizations that refined expectancy Skinner had discussed the application of that methodology to
theory by adding other contingency relationships to those used educational, clinical, and industrial domains as early as the
in Vroom's model and by changing the conceptual definition of 1950s (Skinner, 1953), but industrial utilization was delayed
the contingency to a bivariate subjective distribution. However, until the 1960s. Books by Luthans and Kreitner (1975) and by
that theory has received relatively little research attention, possi- Frederiksen (1982) detailed the range of industrial applications
bly because of its complexity, several pending measurement of organizational behavior modification (OBM) based on oper-
problems, and the theory's deductive scope compared with ant techniques, although the approach generally remained out-
middle range ones (e.g., goal setting). side the mainstream of I-O psychology. However, Katzell and
A second motivational approach receiving much attention, Thompson (1990b) have recently included its reinforcement
goal setting, was proposed by Locke (1968). He drew on the techniques in their catalog of programs that can improve work
Wurzburg school of act psychology (cf. Locke & Latham, 1990), motivation, and it forms the foundation of behavior modeling.
F. W Taylor's scientific management (Locke, 1982), Lewin et The influence of neo-behaviorism was seen subsequently in the
al.'s (1944) level of aspiration paradigm, and T. A. Ryan's (1970) general social-cognitive paradigm (Bandura, 1986) and in spe-
experimental treatment of intentions. A plethora of studies in cific I-O adaptations of it (cf. Davis & Luthans, 1980; Fedor &
laboratory and field settings ensued, generally supporting the Ferris, 1981; Manz & Sims, 1981; Weiss, 1978).
effectiveness of setting clear, specific, challenging goals, espe- Somewhat contradictory to the growing use of neobehavioral
cially when coupled with feedback (Locke & Latham, 1990). concepts and techniques was an increased use of cognitive
However, the theoretical underpinnings of the approach are theory in the understanding of such diverse I-O subjects as
not altogether clear (Naylor & Ilgen, 1984), and there are unre- performance appraisal (Feldman, 1981; Landy & Farr, 1980),
solved research questions (Austin & Bobko, 1985). Recent work task design (Thomas & Griffin, 1983), and leadership (Lord &
by Bandura and his associates (Bandura, 1986; Bandura & Cer- Maher, 199 Ib), in addition to the expectancy and goal theories
vone, 1983), Lord and his associates (Campion & Lord, 1982; of motivation discussed earlier. As this is an escalating aspect of
Lord & Hanges, 1987), and Ajzen (1991) suggests that multiple the field, we will have more to say about it later.
theoretical perspectives can be brought to bear on the mass of Escalating the trends that had emerged in post-World War II
research data that has accumulated. years, I-O psychologists showed increasing interest in how
Another topic that stimulated much attention had to do with work affects people as human beings, not just as hired hands.
the scope or challenge of the job (Herzberg, 1966). Within this Kornhauser's (1965) study of the mental health of assembly-line
framework, Hackman and Oldham (1975, 1976) set forth a workers at different skill levels, the longitudinal studies of
model that prompted a considerable amount of research as well changes in AT&T managers' characteristics (Bray, Campbell, &
as practical application. The resulting picture suggested that Grant, 1974; Howard & Bray, 1988), the National Institute of
enriching jobs in terms of those and similar characteristics gen- Mental Health studies of the effects of work on cognitive pro-
erally has favorable effects on workers' attitudes but that their cesses and personality factors by Kohn and Schooler (1983),
effects on job performance are less consistent (cf. Stone, 1986). and the increased attention to quality of work life (Hackman &
Job satisfaction continued to be a topic of interest. Its rela- Suttle, 1977; OToole et al, 1973) all reflect at least in part that
tionships were generally found to be stronger with withdrawal trend.
behavior rather than performance. These findings have been During this period the focus of attention shifted further from
summarized in recent meta-analyses (laffaldano & Muchinsky, individual workers, their jobs, and work groups to properties of
1985; K. D. Scott & Taylor, 1985). The measurement of job organizations in relation to their member's attitudes and behav-
satisfaction was refined by Smith, Kendall, and Hulin (1969) at ior. Theory and research now often conceived of organizational
Cornell University and by Weiss, Dawis, England, and Lofquist behavior as embedded in open, sociotechnical systems (Emery,
(1967) at the University of Minnesota. Other aspects of workers' 1969; Katz & Kahn, 1966). Textbooks such as the ones by
attitudes that became the subjects of attention included organi- Porter, Lawler, and Hackman (1975) and Bass (1965) summa-
zational commitment (Mowday, Steers, & Porter, 1979) and job rized these kinds of concerns, which included communication
involvement (Rabinowitz & Hall, 1977). Although these three in organizations, conflict management, choice processes be-
attitudinal constructs can be differentiated operationally, the tween individuals and organizations, organizational socializa-
finding by Brooke, Russell, and Price (1988) of rather substan- tion, careers in organizations, and organizational influences on
tial intercorrelations among latent variables suggests the need individual work behavior. Interest also arose in the subject of
for further clarification. organizational climate, having to do with employees' consen-
Implicated in the conceptions of job enrichment and job sual perceptions of an organization's properties (Forehand &
involvement is intrinsic motivation. Deci (1975) theorized that Gilmer, 1964; James & Jones, 1974; Schneider & Bartlett, 1968)
intrinsic motivation is sapped by extrinsic rewards, thereby as well as the related subject of organizational culture (Schein,
stimulating a stream of research that has subsided lately as find- 1984,1985). Accompanying this trend was increased attention
ings support the theory only under special circumstances. to conceptualizing and analyzing phenomena that cross the
Hulin (1991) has reviewed some of the special circumstances, levels of individuals, groups, and organizations (Rousseau,
816 RAYMOND A. KATZELL AND JAMES T. AUSTIN

1985). Here we see budding attention to the interface of indus- gists grew by more than 2'/2 times between 1960, when there
trial and organizational approaches. were 756 members, and 1980, when the number grew to 2,005
Paralleling those substantive interests in organizational psy- (Jeanneret, 1991). According to Howard (1982), 794 additional
chology, there emerged technologies for facilitating organiza- APA members who were not members of Division 14 desig-
tional change and development (OD). Techniques included lab- nated I-O psychology as their major field of interest. However,
oratory training (e.g., sensitivity training and T groups), survey the increase in numbers of Division 14 members as a propor-
feedback, diagnostic interviewing and discussion groups, pro- tion of total APA membership remained fairly constant be-
cess meetings, team development, and integrated technologies tween 1960 and 1985 (Jeanneret, 1991). Compared to 1960,
such as the Blake-Mouton Grid (Beer, 1976). A feature of I-O when 24% of Division 14 members held academic positions, the
research methodology was the increased reliance on laboratory proportion increased to 33% by 1980 and 36% by 1985 (Jean-
experimentation during this period (Sackett & Larson, 1990). neret, 1991), due largely to increased membership on faculties
Overall, there was some shifting in the field not only in sub- of schools of business administration. Of those who were not
ject matter and methodology, but in approach to the subject. academicians in 1980, approximately 62% held full-time posi-
Dunnette, in his introductory chapter for the 1976 Handbook of tions in industry, 12% were in the public sector, and 24% were in
Industrial and Organizational Psychology, entitled "Toward Fu- free-standing consulting or research organizations; by 1985, the
sion," compared the scope of that volume with that of its 1950 percentage employed full-time in industry had declined to 42%,
counterpart, the Handbook of Applied Psychology (Fryer & with corresponding growth in employment in consulting and
Henry, 1950). Dunnette observed that, whereas the 1950 hand- research organizations (Jeanneret, 1991). The reasons for that
book was largely devoted to practice with an emphasis on tech- shift are unclear, but it is possible that positions in industry
niques and applications, the 1976 handbook highlighted re- were increasingly being filled by people who did not identify
search and theory and dealt with practice as an outgrowth of with Division 14, given that the need for I-O services seems not
them. Another difference was that the earlier handbook dealt to have diminished (judging from the increase in numbers of
with the entire scope of applied psychology, including educa- those employed in consulting and applied research organiza-
tional psychology and individual adjustment as well as I-O top- tions). The percentage of Division 14 psychologists reporting an
ics. The increasing specialization of these domains of applied interest in personnel and training topics did not change mark-
psychology meant less contact among them. edly between 1960 and 1980. However, the percentage express-
ing interest in the organizational-type subjects grew substan-
tially; more than two thirds of them now listed topics in that
Roles
area as either their first or second specialty (Howard, 1982).
Doctoral training programs in the I-O field flourished dur-
ing this period as well, marking the continuation of sustained Forces
growth in the discipline. A number of new programs were inau-
gurated, including ones at Bowling Green State University in Several of the I-O developments during the period of 1965 to
1965, North Carolina State University in 1966, and the Univer- 1985 were outgrowths of developments in other branches of
sity of Akron in 1968. Additionally, there was more attention scholarship. We have already noted the indebtedness of expec-
given to training at the master's level. Division 14, following a tancy theory to the earlier writings of Tolman, Lewin, and
policy statement issued in 1959, published guidelines for doc- Peak; of goal setting to the Wurzburg school of act psychology,
toral training (APA, 1965), and discussion about the topic led to F. W Taylor, and Lewin; and of behavior management to Skin-
several symposia and debates. R. J. Campbell (1970) chaired a ner. The work in meta-analysis was rooted in Bayesian logic,
symposium that addressed the training of psychologists for in- and some of the earliest formulations by psychologists were
dustry. Naylor (1971) concluded that training was shifting away from people outside the I-O field (e.g., Glass, 1976; Rosenthal,
from the scientist and toward a scientist-professional model. 1978). The growing focus on cognitive processes in the fields of
His preference was to retain the scientist orientation, although experimental and social psychology stimulated a similar ap-
he was challenged by several commentators. Lawler (1971) pre- proach to the study of performance ratings and other I-O
sented a symposium in which training in general was ad- topics.
dressed; several participants presented survey data to formulate However, the attention to issues of ethnic differences in valid-
and buttress their contributions. No resolutions were reached, ity and fairness of assessment methods derived not so much
but training continued to emphasize the general academic from scholarly sources as from sociopolitical ones, namely the
model even as it incorporated special I-O topics. One hazard of civil rights movement and its counterpart in legislation and
such increasing specialization is neglect of the core of psychol- jurisprudence. A landmark decision in 1954 by the U.S. Su-
ogy (Naylor, 1971). (We note parenthetically that the training of preme Court, Brown v. Topeka Board of Education, struck down
I-O psychologists stretches back to Geissler, 1918, who in the the separate but equal doctrine in education. That was followed
second volume of the Journal of Applied Psychology had pub- in the 1960s by legislation, executive orders, and agency regula-
lished an article concerning methods of technical training for tions that prohibited discrimination in employment (Byham &
consulting psychologists.) Spitzer, 1971; Miner & Miner, 1978). In the Myart v. Motorola
Professional employment opportunities also continued to ex- case of 1963, the Illinois Fair Employment Practices Commis-
pand in nonacademic settings, as did academic opportunities sion ruled that a company was guilty of racial discrimination
based on the expansion of training in universities. Judging from because an experienced Black man had been denied employ-
the membership of Division 14, the number of I-O psycholo- ment on the basis of an intelligence test; though the ruling was
APA CENTENNIAL: HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF I-O PSYCHOLOGY 817

later overturned on the grounds of weak evidence, the case than field studies, academic I-O psychologists—especially
focused the attention of the civil rights movement on personnel those in psychology departments—may increasingly have
testing. Even more crucial was the Civil Rights Act of 1964, of turned to them in response to pressures to publish.
which Title VII permitted the use in employment of "any profes-
sionally developed ability test, provided that such t e s t . . . is Mid-1980s to the Present
not. . . used to discriminate because of race, color, religion,
sex, or national origin" (by the Tower Amendment). The ab- A glance at where we are now may provide some insights for
sence of rigorous definition of certain terms, including ability, the future as well as perspectives on how the three strands of
test, professionally developed, and discriminate, generated con- topics, roles, and forces have intertwined to create the disci-
siderable debate and confusion that federal agencies, notably pline. Philosophers have pointed out that there is really no such
the Equal Employment Opportunity Commission and the Of- time as the present. No sooner than one tries to observe it, it
fice of Federal Contract Compliance, attempted to dispel by becomes part of the past. Therefore, when in this section we try
issuing guidelines that incorporated the validation standards briefly to outline the present picture of I-O psychology, we are
defined by the APA (1974). Rulings by courts, especially the describing its recent history, primarily the past 5 or 6 years.
Griggs v. Duke Power Company decision of the U.S. Supreme Again, the scope and diversity of the discipline requires that we
Court in 1971, also lent clarification. Curiously, although Title offer no more than a sample.
VII specifically outlawed sex discrimination in employment,
and the problems faced by women were examined soon thereaf- Topics
ter in books by M. E. Katzell and Byham (1972) and by Astin,
Suniewick, and Dweck (1971), the subject did not receive nearly To a large extent, the recent past has witnessed increases in
as much attention until later (e.g., Heilman, 1983). As we shall the scope and depth of the topics that we have already cited.
see, issues raised during that period concerning discrimination The invigoration of topics in the industrial segment of the field
in employment survive in one form or another down to the continues apace (cf. Smith & Robertson, 1989). There is continu-
present. ing interest in validation strategies (Guion, 1991) and validity
The improvements in Americans' levels of affluence and edu- generalization (Hunter & Schmidt, 1990); assessment centers
cation, which were influential in shaping I-O psychology dur- (Gaugler, Rosenthal, Thornton, & Bentson, 1987) and individ-
ing the post-World War II period, continued to be operative ual assessment (Ryan & Sackett, 1987); criteria of performance
here as well, helping to account for high levels of interest in (Murphy & Cleveland, 1991; Wigdor & Green, 1991); job analy-
work motivation and attitudes and the quality of work life. sis (Harvey, 1991); training and development (Goldstein & As-
America's involvement in Vietnam and the Watergate scandal sociates, 1989); and fairness across several domains, including
raised further challenges to tradition and authority. Those so- employment, compensation, and promotion (Gottfredson &
cial, economic, and political factors led many to seek new and Sharf, 1988; Hartigan & Wigdor, 1989; Reynolds & Brown,
better ways to find meaning and fulfillment (Yankelovich, 1984). After having survived the skepticism of many psycholo-
1981) and contributed to the "greening" of America that was gists, the concept and assessment of personality are being reha-
characterized by breaks with tradition and with the established bilitated in I-O psychology (Hogan, 1991). A promising im-
order and by the apotheosis of individuals and their rights provement in personnel selection technology that uses model-
(Reich, 1970). In concert, these events lent further impetus to ing performance for prediction has been proposed by J. P.
I-O psychologists' concern with workers' attitudes and motiva- Campbell (1990a) and is being applied in a large-scale project
tion and with ways of improving life in the organizations that for the U.S. Army (J. P. Campbell, 1990b); this is an example of
employed them, hence the salience of such topics as work moti- the increased attention to how and why selection procedures
vation, job satisfaction, job involvement, job design, employee work (cf. Schmitt & Robertson, 1990), in lieu of the ad hoc
participation, and the organizational context of work. nature of most of the earlier work.
The stronger orientation toward theory development and On the organizational side, a review of the literature pub-
theory-related research, along with greater reliance on labora- lished between 1987 and 1990 led O'Reilly (1991) to conclude
tory investigation, may have reflected the field's growing matu- that considerable attention is still being paid to the familiar
ration. As noted by Dunnette (1976), the field was formerly "micro" topics of motivation, attitudes, leadership, and job de-
more inclined to be concerned with devising practical solutions sign. Most of that can be characterized as methodological or
to immediate problems in field situations. Perhaps having conceptual refinement of previous work, possibly because the
shown that even their improvisations were useful, more I-O accumulated research on those topics suggests that they are
psychologists now felt that they had the breathing space to try pretty well understood and demonstrably useful. That is not to
better to understand the nature of the phenomena in which say that there have not been some newer developments. They
they were interested and to use such understanding as a spring- include the addition of mood and general affective states to the
board to better practice. An additional force may have been a work attitudes being studied (e.g., George & Brief, in press;
growing distinction among I-O psychologists between those Levin & Stokes, 1989), growing examination of the construct of
whose primary focus was on science and those who were pri- organizational culture (Schneider, 1990), the extension of oper-
marily practitioners, with the former exhibiting greater interest ant behaviorism to the study of supervision and teams (Ko-
in theory and theory-oriented research. (We have more to say maki, Desselles, & Bowman, 1989), growing attention to re-
about this trend later.) Furthermore, because laboratory experi- source adequacy as a factor in work effectiveness (Schoorman &
ments are typically easier to arrange and quicker to complete Schneider, 1988) and in motivation (Katzell & Thompson,
818 RAYMOND A. KATZELL AND JAMES T. AUSTIN

1990a), and the inclusion of enduring dispositions and traits refinement of techniques that have been developed over the
among the causes of work attitudes (Arvey, Bouchard, Segal, & years in such familiar areas as productivity enhancement, indi-
Abraham, 1989; Staw & Ross, 1985). vidual assessment, training and development, and OD. How-
There has been renewed attention to psychologically based ever, these techniques are often being applied to new problems,
approaches to productivity enhancement (Brief, 1984; Camp- for example, those created by the changing demography of the
bell, Campbell, & Associates, 1988; Kopelman, 1986). Katzell work force, new technology, and the shift toward a service econ-
and Guzzo (1983) categorized them as dealing with selection, omy and service society. Attention is also being directed toward
training, supervision, goal setting, and management by objec- such emerging issues as sexual harassment, drug testing, and
tives, performance appraisal, work schedules, compensation, strategic planning. The disjunction between science and prac-
work design, decision making, and systemwide or OD types of tice continues to be of concern. Although many suggestions
interventions, which, as we noted earlier, have been found on emanate, mostly from university-based consultants on how to
the whole to have positive effects on productivity. It is notewor- develop a better partnership between the two, full-time practi-
thy that these approaches come from both the industrial and tioners seem less optimistic in the face of corporate realities.
the organizational branches of the discipline. In terms of statistical methodologies, there were several con-
The I-O field is also getting more involved with topics that tributions that flowered during this period: item response
are nontraditional, in the sense that they are not primarily re- theory (Drasgow & Hulin, 1990), structural equation modeling
lated to human performance in organizations. Among them are (Bollen, 1989; James & James, 1989), meta-analysis (Hunter &
global problems (Schmitt, 1990), off-the-job lives of workers Schmidt, 1990), event history analysis (Willett & Singer, 1991),
(Greenhaus & Beutell, 1985; Zedeck & Mosier, 1990), workers' and other methods for the study of change (Collins & Horn,
health and well-being (Ilgen, 1990; Kahn, 1981), and careers in 1991). These techniques have their roots in earlier eras and
organizations (Arthur, Hall, & Lawrence, 1989; Hall & Asso- different disciplines. In fact, path analysis, a form of structural
ciates, 1986). These expansions of purview suggest that systems equation modeling, can be traced back to work done by Sewall
conceptions are gaining a toehold, in that the possibility of Wright around 1921. In addition, these methods for analyzing
reciprocity between work and nonwork domains is being taken psychological data represent improvements over classical test
more seriously. Lewin's conception of life space is as relevant theory, regression analysis with manifest variables, narrative
here as it was during the 1940s. However, this kind of topic still literature reviews, and static and cross-sectional analyses, re-
commands only a small fraction of the attention of I-O scien- spectively. However, they offer potential for misuse (Austin &
tists and practitioners. Wolfle, 1991; Guzzo et al., 1987). Thus, they must be combined
The cognitive movement in I-O psychology deserves atten- with insightful conceptualizations and turned to substantive
tion because it is sweeping the field—as indeed it is sweeping problems to advance understanding. An example of this is
through psychology in general (Gardner, 1985). Without re- shown by Hanisch and Hulin (1991), who specified and evalu-
viewing the entire paradigm or the related interdisciplinary ated a structural model relating general job attitudes to organi-
cognitive science movement, which has been done in various zational withdrawal within the context of retirement. One re-
forms elsewhere (Estes, 1991; Hunt, 1989; Newell, 1990), we cent statistical technique is event history or survival analysis
present highlights. Two fundamental issues concern representa- (Willett & Singer, 1991), which estimates time-dependent be-
tion, or how information is encoded internally, and operation, or havioral processes when people are entering and leaving the
how the stored information is manipulated (Lord & Maher, sample across time, thus proving especially useful for studying
199la). Within I-O psychology, the applications of cognitive organizational adaptation and withdrawal processes (e.g., turn-
psychology have been noticeable in three areas: performance over, as suggested by Morita, Lee, & Mowday, 1989, or ab-
rating, leadership, and conceptions of ability. In the area of senteeism, as suggested by Fichman, 1988). In the area of test
performance rating, Landy and Farr called for the application fairness, developments in both technical (Humphreys, 1986;
of cognitive processes in 1980, followed by Feldman (1981). The Roznowski, 1987) and policy (Gottfredson & Sharf, 1988) issues
resultant models have been reviewed by DeNisi and Williams suggest sensitivity to a broader range of concerns.
(1988). In the area of leadership, Lord and his colleagues have The current birthing of the second edition of the I-O hand-
applied various cognitive principles and methods (Lord, 1985; book (Dunnette & Hough, 1990-1993), in four volumes, pro-
Lord & Maher, 1991b). A third topic concerns the merging of vides further evidence bearing on the refocusing identified by
psychometric and cognitive conceptions of ability, as repre- Dunnette (1976). The second edition will eventually consist of
sented in the work of Sternberg and Ackerman (Ackerman & three volumes covering theoretical, substantive, and method-
Humphreys, 1990). Ackerman (1987) combined a distinction ological topics and one volume describing I-O psychology as
between automatic and controlled processing with several conceived and practiced in other countries. The sheer length of
stages of task performance: initial, training, and final. Al- the second edition attests to the growth of the field.
though the jury is still out on the usefulness of cognitive ap- American Psychologist recently devoted a special issue to or-
proaches in I-O psychology (cf. Ilgen & Klein, 1989), the influ- ganizational psychology (Offermann & Gowing, 1990). Its table
ence is being felt across the spectrum of applied psychology, of contents (see Table 1). reflected the substantive topics that
from educational (Snow & Lohman, 1989) to clinical psychol- are currently central to the organizational side of the field. The
ogy (Kanfer & Hagerman, 1985). three topics at the bottom of Table 1 were added from the re-
Judging from the recent multiauthored volumes by Bray and view by O'Reilly (1991), who also noted many of the topics
Associates (1991) and by Murphy and Saal (1990), much con- covered by Offermann and Gowing (1990). Table 2 similarly
temporary practice of I-O psychology can be characterized as lists the main topics in the area of industrial and personnel
APA CENTENNIAL: HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF I-O PSYCHOLOGY 819

Table 1 alone. Comparing that with the figure of 79 members of the


Current Topics in Organizational Psychology Business and Industrial Division of AAAP in 1943 (plus some
nonmembers) yields a 30-fold increase in less than 50 years. Let
1. Organizations of the future: Changes and challenges (including us examine these demographics in greater detail. According to
work force changes, changes in organizational characteristics,
culture, climate, and organizational productivity) Howard's (1990,1991 a) analyses of the most recent membership
2. Training system issues survey, about 36% of SIOP members are employed in academic
3. Organizational culture settings, whereas 57% reported working primarily in industry,
4. Work teams and their effectiveness government, and consulting, and the remainder reported an-
5. Work motivation theory and practice other primary setting (e.g., health care). The academic-practi-
6. Developing the competitive organization: Interventions and
strategies tioner ratio has remained rather constant over the past 25 years.
7. Designing systems for resolving disputes in organizations However, Howard (1991b) and Jeanneret (1991) have called at-
8. The challenge of new work place technology tention to some migration of in-house practitioners to positions
9. Power and leadership in organizations as external consultants in recent years. The latter trend escapes
10. Developing managerial talent through simulation
11. Women and minorities in management ready explanation.
12. Entrepreneurship and intrapreneurship I-O psychologists working full-time in industry and consult-
13. Human resource planning ing spend more of their time doing surveys, assessments, and
14. Work in the family and employing organization problem diagnosis than they spend in applied scientific re-
15. Work site stress management interventions search. This picture emerges from job analyses recently done
16. Employee fitness and wellness programs
17. Health issues at work by members of Division 14 (Prien & Macey, 1984; Schmitt &
18. Organizational and group contexts of behavior Fine, 1982). As an observer of the I-O scene for more than 40
19. The person-situation debate years, Raymond A. Katzell has the impression that psycholo-
20. The impact of macro-level theory gists in such positions are doing less research than before, but
Note. Topics were drawn from a special issue on organizational psy- hard data on this point are not available. However, that impres-
chology published by the American Psychologist (Offermann & Gow- sion is supported by the fact that practitioners are publishing in
ing, 1990) and from a review by O'Reilly (1991). the Journal of Applied Psychology and Personnel Psychology at
only about half the rate that they did 25 years ago. We tabulated
the institutional affiliations of authors in those journals for
psychology, as featured in recent Annual Review of Psychology 1960-1961 and 1984-1985. In 1960-1961, 20% of Journal of
chapters (Schmidt, Ones, & Hunter, 1992; Schmitt & Robert- Applied Psychology authors had nonacademic affiliations,
son, 1990; Tannenbaum & Yukl, 1992). Together, these two whereas in 1984-1985, the percentage had declined to 10%. The
tables are reasonably complete representations of the subjects corresponding figures for Personnel Psychology were 38% in
of current interest to I-O psychologists. 1960-1961 versus 20% in 1984-1985, again a reduction of
Many of the symposia and papers to be presented at the 1992
midyear conference of the Society for Industrial and Organiza-
tional Psychology (SIOP) are indicative of new concerns that Table 2
would surprise a psychological Rip Van Winkle who slept since Current Topics in Industrial and Personnel Psychology
1972. Some of those concerns include the following: the config-
1. General cognitive ability versus differential aptitudes as predictors
uration of total systems, union socialization, transitional orga- 2. Developments in conventional assessment methods (e.g.,
nizations for unemployed people, diversity management, total assessment centers, interviews, tests, and biodata)
quality management, computer-mediated communication sys- 3. Methods of job analysis
tems, multilevel inferences in organizational research, later life 4. Psychometric and statistical procedures (e.g., meta-analysis,
transitions in careers, computer-based multimedia testing, and validity generalization, and item response theory)
5. Utility analysis
proactive organizational newcomers. This list should reassure 6. Legal and social issues
anyone who may wonder if I-O psychology has stagnated. 7. Cognitive processes in performance rating
8. Recruitment and attraction
9. Criteria of performance
Roles 10. Realistic job previews
By 1989, 88 universities were offering doctorates in I-O psy- 11. Training needs analysis
12. Cognitive models in design of training
chology (46) or organizational behavior (42); 26 psychology de- 13. Developments in training methods (e.g., high-technology
partments offered terminal master's degrees (SIOP, 1989). The methods, behavior modeling, and simulations)
SIOP Guidelines for Education and Training (SIOP, 1985) set 14. Effects of trainee characteristics, including aptitudes, motivation,
forth 21 areas of competence and five methods for acquiring self-efficacy levels, aptitude X treatment interactions, and
aptitude X motivation interactions
competence. Perhaps regrettably, only 6 of these competence 15. Facilitating transfer of training
areas were not specific to I-O psychology, thereby further delin- 16. Development experiences and mentoring
eating the distinction between the I-O psychologist as a general 17. Team training
psychological scientist and as an applied scientist-practitioner. 18. Evaluation of training
There are more I-O psychologists today than ever before—
Note. Topics were drawn from articles by Schmidt, Ones, and Hunter
2,500 in SIOP, plus probably several hundred more who are not (1992), Schmitt and Robertson (1990), and Tannenbaum and Yukl
members—thus there are some 3,000 in the United States (1992).
820 RAYMOND A. KATZELL AND JAMES T. AUSTIN

about one half. Similar trends have been reported by Sackett, A second force is the increasing diversity that characterizes
Callahan, DeMeuse, Ford, and Kozlowski (1986), and Cam- U.S. society. Because of changing demographic and immigra-
pion et al. (1986) provided descriptive data on the types of tion patterns, the cultural topography is shifting so that multi-
research being performed in nonacademic settings and reasons ple minority groups are increasing as a proportion of the work
for its lack of publication (e.g., lack of time and rewards). In force (Briggs, 1987). This shift is seen in census statistics be-
short, those psychologists employed full-time in industry, gov- tween 1970 and 1990 and in such think-tank forecasts as Work-
ernment, and consulting seem to be tilting more to the practi- force 2000 (Johnston & Packer, 1987). Also, because of health
tioner end of the scientist-practitioner continuum, a trend also care improvements and increased awareness of physical fitness
noted by Murphy and Saal (1990). Boehm (1980) and LaPointe and nutrition, the life expectancy for many subgroups is in-
(1990) said that practical exigencies of the organizational world creasing. This latter trend will interact with the revised Age
interfere with rigorous research, thereby suggesting at least part Discrimination in Employment Act of 1986 to create a situation
of the explanation of the relative dearth of publications by prac- that could be perceived as either a threat or an opportunity by
titioners in the leading journals. organizations. The Americans With Disabilities Act of 1990 is
"Macro" topics that are engaging the attention of practi- another regulatory influence.
tioners include strategic planning, management succession, or- Furthermore, the external environment has become increas-
ganization development, and companywide financial incen- ingly turbulent for the discipline and for organizations, just as it
tives. Such topics are more germane to the responsibilities of has for citizens (To filer, 1970). The pace of change is increasing
top management than are the more traditional concerns that and shows no signs of lessening (Pion & Lipsey, 1984). There-
principally concern human resources departments. Rousseau fore, the organizations, professions, and individuals that stand
(1985) is among those who called attention to the need for the- the best chance of surviving are those that are flexible and
ories and methods that incorporate cross-level effects if the adaptive. This development has prompted I-O psychologists to
field is to meld successfully those organization-level concerns study several correlates of change, mostly focusing on stress and
with its more familiar foci on individuals and small groups. coping strategies (Hurrell, Murphy, Sauter, & Cooper, 1988).
The U.S. Army project mentioned earlier is an example of Attention to unemployment has been stimulated by the recent
collaborative research on personnel selection by several organi- economic recession. A growing attention to nontraditional top-
zations (J. P. Campbell, 1990b). As long ago as 1923, Bingham ics, that is, those not addressing bottom-line performance, may
suggested the desirability of that consortium approach. As we be due to the influx of younger psychologists whose values and
have seen, it had been used successfully by the Carnegie Insti- interests were influenced by the greening of America. O'Reilly
tute of Technology. The advantages include cost sharing among (1991) suggested that the challenges of traditional topics, espe-
the participating organizations, larger sample sizes, multiple cially those at the micro level, have been pretty well met by now,
settings that permit dealing with issues of generalizability, and thus leading some people to seek new ones.
more adequate resources. The use of consortia has been fruitful As noted earlier, the disproportionate dearth of scientific
in the life insurance industry, as mentioned earlier (Brown, publications by I-O practitioners has been attributed in part to
1981; Ferguson, 1952), and recently has been adopted by the the practical exigencies of doing research in the organizational
electric power industry (Dunnette, 1989). This kind of under- world (Boehm, 1980; LaPointe, 1990). However, because that is
taking has valuable implications for both science and practice. no more true today than it was earlier, it does not fully explain
The relations between I-O psychology as science and as prac- the declining trend noted above. What may be different now
tice continue to pose a challenge, as witnessed by the fact that at than was formerly the case is the growing acceptance of I-O
the 1992 annual conference of SIOP, both its president (Kli- psychology as a profession, both by its practitioners and its
moski, 1992) and its distinguished scientist awardee (J. P. clients (Napoli, 1981). As a discipline becomes professional-
Campbell, 1992) devoted their addresses to that subject. Kli- ized, it is characterized by having a body of sanctioned knowl-
moski contended that one means for reenergizing the interface edge and practices that are deemed sufficiently well validated
between science and practice is to come to a more eclectic view as to no longer require experimental verification (cf. Abbott,
of science and practice; "inquiry from inside" is the term that 1988; Merton, 1982). The classic case of this is, of course, the
he used to try to convey an ethnographic knowledge acquisition practice of medicine in contrast to medical sciences such as
and verification process by practitioners. Campbell (1992) ar- bacteriology and biochemistry. In psychology, the field of clini-
gued that the latent needs of the two parties are actually more cal psychology has gone even further along the road of profes-
similar than their surface dissimilarities. sionalization than has I-O psychology, even eventuating in
schools of professional psychology and the Doctor of Psychol-
ogy (PsyD) degree. At least one school of professional psychol-
Forces ogy (Rutgers University) now offers the doctorate in the organi-
Major changes in emphasis in the past few years have in- zational field. The migration of much doctoral training in orga-
cluded the heightened concerns with productivity and utility. nizational behavior from departments of psychology to schools
That is clearly a response to America's rather stagnant produc- of business administration or management may also testify to
tivity and the consequent threat to our economic well-being growing professionalization.
from nations showing increasing productivity, especially Japan A feature possibly paralleling the last is that many psycholo-
(J. P. Campbell et al., 1988; Perloff& Nelson, 1983; Zeidner, gist-practitioners appear more prone to identify increasingly
1987). The interest in utility analysis (Cascio, 1987; Steffy & with management and correspondingly less with psychology.
Maurer, 1988) is undoubtedly related. This trend, of course, is hard to document, but let us make a
APA CENTENNIAL: HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF I-O PSYCHOLOGY 821

few observations. Many, perhaps most, psychologists in in- Table 3


dustry do not use the title of doctor or even label themselves as Membership Figures for the Industrial and Organizational
psychologists; many have job titles that include the word man- Components of the American Association for Applied
ager (e.g., manager of human resources planning). Whereas in Psychology (1943) and the American Psychological
1972, 94% of new doctorates in I-O psychology joined APA Association (1948-1991)
within 3 years, the percentage in 1981 shrank to 66% (Howard
et al., 1986). Those numbers may not adequately represent grad- Year Members
uates of organizational behavior programs in schools of busi- 1943 79
ness, whose percentages would probably be even lower. Perhaps 1948 188
some of the recurrent talk about SIOP splitting off from APA 1950 289
may be due to a growing estrangement from the discipline of 1955 514
1960 734
psychology, in addition to frustration with APA politics or inef- 1965 934
ficiency. 1970 1,109
Apart from those recent trends in I-O science and practice, 1975 1,370
the principal force shaping the present is the history, or more 1980 1,953
precisely the prevailing culture of the field, comprising its val- 1985 2,499
1991 2,481
ues, beliefs, habits, and institutions. The topics I-O psycholo-
gists currently study and practice, the methods they use, and Note. 1943 number from Louttit (1943); other numbers from Director-
the roles they occupy, are in the main carryovers from those ies and Membership Registers of the American Psychological Associa-
described in the preceding two decades. In turn, those topics, tion.
methods, and roles were shaped by the forces described as oper-
ating during those earlier periods.
almost certainly not due to an overall reduction in numbers of
Some Professional History I-O psychologists, in view of the vitality of graduate programs
in the field. It is more likely due to a trend for fewer I-O psychol-
We now turn briefly to another facet of the field's history, ogists to join APA, coupled with the increased salience of other
namely, certain organizational and demographic features. societies that may be draining off some members, for example,
First, let us identify the major scientific and professional organi- the Academy of Management and the American Psychological
zations and the dates of their founding. The American Psycho- Society. Another factor may be the sharp increase in SIOP dues,
logical Association dates from 1892, the Society for Applied which may have caused the disaffiliation of psychologists
Psychology and the Economic Psychology Association from whose interest in the I-O field is only peripheral.
1915, The International Congress of Psychotechnology from
1920, the New "Vbrk State Association of Consulting Psycholo-
gists (now the New York State Psychological Association) and
Additional Historical Sources on I-O Psychology
the Personnel Research Federation from 1921, the Psychomet- Listed here are additional readings on the history of I-O psy-
ric Society from 1935, the Society for the Scientific Study of chology for those who may wish to delve further into the sub-
Social Issues and the Academy of Management from 1936, the ject. These readings focus on I-O psychology and are not in-
American Association for Applied Psychology (including a Sec- tended as an exhaustive list. Additional primary sources are the
tion of Industrial and Business Psychology) from 1937, Divi- materials stored in the Archives of the History of American
sion 14 of APA from 1945 (incorporated as the Society for Indus- Psychology at the University of Akron (Benjamin, 1980). There
trial and Organizational Psychology in 1983), and the Ameri- are also collections of papers and letters by Miinsterberg (Bos-
can Psychological Society from 1988. ton Public Library), Scott (Northwestern University), Bingham
As mentioned earlier, the field of consumer psychology, once (Carnegie-Mellon University), and Yerkes (Yale University).
part of I-O psychology, formed a separate Division of Con- Sources in addition to those cited in the following paragraphs
sumer Psychology within APA in 1960. Many consumer psy- include early issues of the Journal of Applied Psychology, the
chologists also identify with nonpsychological organizations, Journal of Personnel Research, and the Journal of Consulting
such as the American Marketing Association and the Ameri- Psychology.
can Association for Public Opinion Research. Likewise, psy- One important historian of the early period was Leonard
chologists interested in the interaction of workers with their Ferguson, although his coverage was somewhat focused. In a
physical and informational work setting (i.e., person-machine seriesof pamphlets (Ferguson, 1962-1965), he detailed the his-
systems) formed a separate division within APA, the Division of tory of the field during the first three decades of the century,
Engineering Psychology, and similarly have affiliated with ex- concentrating especially on three major themes: the faculty and
trapsychological organizations such as the Human Factors Soci- students of the Division of Applied Psychology at Carnegie
ety (Howell, 1991). Institute of Technology, psychological contributions to the mi-
As for population growth in the I-O field, we have already litary during World War I, and the profession of employment
estimated the numbers of I-O psychologists during various his- management. A brief summary of his work appeared in a book
torical periods, but Table 3 presents the counts of fellows and chapter (Ferguson, 1961). Ferguson (1952) also traced the his-
members of the I-O components of AAAP (1943) and APA for tory of the Life Insurance Agency Management Association
the years 1948-1991. The slight decline in the past few years is (LIAMA) back to the Bureau of Salesmanship Research at Car-
822 RAYMOND A. KATZELL AND JAMES T. AUSTIN

negie Institute of Technology. A later tribute to Walter V The aforementioned autobiographical chapter by Patricia
Bingham included an essay by Ferguson (1962) on the early Cain Smith is unique in its illustration of some of the special
relations between industrial psychology and labor. features of being a female I-O psychologist. We should note,
A comprehensive and recent review of the history of I-O however, that there were women among even the earliest I-O
psychology appeared as a chapter in Hilgard's (1987) monu- psychologists, including Marion Bills, Beatrice Dvorak, Kate
mental account of psychology in America. His perspective, that Gordon, Kate Lewis, Millicent Pond, and Sadie Myers Shellow.
of an experimental psychologist who has lived through many of Lillian Gilbreth also had a doctorate in psychology, although
those events and is familiar with many of the people involved she was later identified more with the field of industrial engi-
and with historical analysis (cf. Hilgard, Leary, & McGuire, neering and management. Furumoto (1987) provided one expla-
1991), makes this chapter especially one that I-O psychologists nation about the role of women in applied psychology during
could read profitably. the early professionalization of the field.
Several professional journals have devoted sections or entire
issues to the history of the I-O field at various times. Examples
Conclusion
include special issues in the Annals of the American Academy of
Political and Social Science (Crennan & Kingsbury, 1923), the What are some of the things we can learn from the nearly 100
Journal of Consulting Psychology (Viteles, 1944), and Profes- years of our history? Such lessons may help us to shape our
sional Psychology (Meltzer & Shulman, 1982; Meltzer & future rather than be shaped by it (Watson, 1966). Below we
Stagner, 1980). A recent issue of the American Psychologist de- present several conclusions and discuss some of the issues ema-
scribed current topics in organizational psychology (Offerman nating from our review.
& Gowing, 1990), and a section of an earlier issue addressed 1. I-O psychology has become a viable scientific discipline
productivity (Perloif & Nelson, 1983). Since its inception in that has added much to society's knowledge about work behav-
1950, the Annual Review of Psychology has featured variously ior (cf. Hilgard, 1987, p. 728). The four-volume second edition
titled chapters reviewing developments in various areas of I-O of the Handbook of Industrial and Organizational Psychology
psychology. Although the foregoing journal references were (Dunnette & Hough, 1990-1993) gives ample quantitative and
mainly concerned with describing current status at the time, qualitative evidence supporting these assertions, in addition to
they frequently contained historical material as well. Thus, a that reported earlier. Moreover, the field has contributed to the
comparison of their contents over the years furnishes a time- more general body of psychological knowledge. Subjects such
lapse picture of the field's history. as psychometrics, assessment methods, aptitudes, interests,
Other interesting time-lapse pictures can be developed by motivation, fatigue, stress, group dynamics, leadership, ethnic
comparing the contents of I-O journals through the years, espe- and gender comparisons, and decision making are but a few
cially the Journal of Applied Psychology (from 1917), the Jour- examples.
nal of Personnel Research, now Personnel Journal (from 1922), 2. From the scant attention that I-O topics receive in general
and Personnel Psychology (from 1948). Meltzer (1960) and Dar- psychology textbooks and the peripheral place that courses in
ley (1968) used this approach, respectively, to analyze Psycholog- the subject have in the typical psychology curriculum, it ap-
ical Abstracts and the Journal of Applied Psychology. Instructive pears that the scientific contributions of the field are underap-
inferences can similarly be drawn by comparing recent with preciated or perhaps ill-understood by many of our psychologi-
early textbooks, such as Munsterberg (1913), Link (1919), Burtt cal colleagues. This phenomenon may not be a new one, how-
(1926, 1929), Viteles (1932), and Jenkins (1935). In his 1932 ever, as illustrated by an anecdote recounted by Bruce V
book, Viteles devoted considerable attention to the historical Moore. He described how an eminent psychologist at the APA
roots of various I-O topics, organizing his review around eco- convention following the armistice told him, "Well, now that
nomic, psychological, and social foundations. His is one of the the war is over, psychologists ought to be getting back to the
very few American books that covered the foreign literature in real science of psychology" (Moore, 1962, p. 3).
any depth. The book by Jenkins also contained considerable 3. I-O psychology has become an important contributor to
historical content. Current texts, by comparison, are usually the practice of management. Evidence underlying this conclu-
more contemporary in their treatment. sion includes the employment of I-O psychologists in private
Autobiographical and biographical treatments of prominent (361) and public (128) organizations, as full-time consultants
I-O psychologists constitute another historical source. Autobio- (502), plus numerous academicians who serve as part-time con-
graphical book chapters have been written by the pioneering sultants (Howard, 1990), as well as the membership of psycholo-
British industrial psychologist, Charles S. Myers (1936), by W. V gists on faculties of management and their role in the education
Bingham (1952), by Morris S. Viteles (1967,1974), by Edwin E. of present and future managers, citation of psychological publi-
Ghiselli (1978), and by Patricia Cain Smith (1988). Biographi- cations in books on management, and so forth. The impact of
cal sources include treatments of Bingham (Kraus, 1986), Scott I-O psychology is reflected in the widespread use of more so-
(Jacobson, 1951), Munsterberg (Hale, 1980), and E. L. Thorn- phisticated techniques and practices for human resource and
dike (Clifford, 1984). Stagner (1981) summarized and analyzed general management, for example, tests, goal setting, perfor-
the autobiographies submitted by a number of former presi- mance appraisal, attitude surveys, OD programs, performance-
dents of APAs Division 14, updated by Thayer(1992). Biographi- based compensation plans, instructional methods, employee
cal sketches of noted I-O psychologists are also published from involvement, democratic supervision, sociotechnical systems,
time to time in the American Psychologist obituaries and in and so forth. The effectiveness of such applications is indicated
reports about APA award recipients. by the previously cited finding reported by Katzell and Guzzo
APA CENTENNIAL: HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF I-O PSYCHOLOGY 823

(1983) that 87% of 207 field experiments involving I-O inter- havior, so that theories and empirical investigations are more
ventions produced positive results on at least one objective prone to be multivariate, often involving contingencies and
measure of productivity. A subsequent meta-analysis of 98 of moderators as well as reciprocal relationships (e.g., leadership
those experiments by Guzzo, Jette, and Katzell (1985) found and goal theories);
the overall effect size to be what is traditionally regarded as (e) of late, there has been more sensitivity to cross-level and
"medium," which, the authors point out, is not often attained in aggregate effects (e.g., groups as more than collectivities of indi-
field studies. Even when one considers the bias against report- viduals and behavior as a function of organizational contexts);
ing negative results, those data are impressive. (f) there is more effort of late addressed to identifying com-
4. In contrast to the underappreciation of our scientific con- plexes of variables (e.g., taxonomies of abilities and jobs),
tributions, the perceived value of I-O psychology's contribu- integrating diverse conceptions (e.g., combining motivation the-
tions to management seems to be growing (Jones, StefFy, & Bray, ories), and combining variables from hitherto separate do-
1990), perhaps to a degree that even exceeds the field's actual mains (e.g., ability-motivation interactions, cognitive-psycho-
accomplishments. As a result, I-O professionals may at times be metric conceptions of ability);
pressed for premature advice or application (Finder, 1977) and (g) data-analytic methods have evolved from zero-order
their requests for further research met with impatience (Levy- correlation and critical ratio techniques through partial regres-
Leboyer, 1988). To quote Viteles (1974), "Perhaps the time has sion and analysis of variance to factor analysis, structural equa-
come for the psychologist to substitute wisdom for the appear- tion modeling, meta-analysis, and event history analysis;
ance of wisdom—at least, for distinguishing what we know (h) topics have emerged that are closer to the operations of
from what we do. . ." (p. 493). higher management (e.g., decision making and strategic plan-
5. I-O psychology has been contributing to the general well- ning) than the more traditional concerns of lower management
being of American society. It has done so by helping to select (e.g., supervision) or human resources management (e.g., selec-
people for work for which they are suited, training and develop- tion); paralleling this trend is more attention to bottom-line
ing them to be more effective in their work and careers, reduc- dependent variables such as productivity and utility;
ing bias in the employment of the disadvantaged, improving (i) more attention is being given to outcomes that do not
safety and comfort at work, and enhancing the quality of work relate directly to the bottom line, such as career development,
life. Furthermore, as noted by the economists Baumol, Black- stress, work-family relationships, and so forth; and
man, and Wolff (1989), in terms of human welfare, nothing (j) the armamentarium of professional practices is no longer
matters as much in the long run as improving productivity. limited to serving as expert technician and advisor, but now
6. The substance of I-O psychology has undergone much extends also to acting as action researcher and change agent.
development over its lifetime. As we reviewed the temporal 7. This catalog of accomplishments is not to say that all is
periods, we recounted the emergence of numerous topics and well. Dunnette (1984), adding his own observations to those of
techniques. Dunnette (1966) observed that some of the develop- colleagues, cited some 11 achievements that gave him reasons
ments turned out to be just passing fads (e.g., forced-choice for optimism about the I-O field, most of which are generally
ratings and the Theory X-Theory Y dichotomy). Still others consistent with the trends that we just described. But he also
seem to have a cyclical life, rising and falling at various times; reported a number of negative comments, most of which, in
job analysis and labor relations are examples. Even seemingly agreement, we summarize and paraphrase here:
significant theories (Miner, 1984) often depart the center stage (a) There still is wide use of techniques and conceptions of
after a few years. The fads appear to fade because they turn out unknown or uncertain validity;
to have transient or limited value. The cyclical topics seem to be (b) too many I-O psychologists are technicians rather than
correlated with cycles of social or political concern (e.g., labor scientists or scientist-practitioners;
relations with labor-management conflict, job analysis with (c) we collectively devote proportionately too much attention
fair employment issues). Fortunately, many developments have to trivial issues;
stood the test of time and are still with us, although usually in (d) we are often prone to use solutions that we have available
revised and improved form. Mental testing and leadership rather than addressing the real needs of the situation, that is,
theory are but two examples. We think that the following meta- our work is inclined to be technique-driven rather than prob-
trends are among those that can be discerned when early devel- lem-driven;
opments are compared with later ones: (e) I-O psychology is still oriented overwhelmingly to the
(a) More of the later ones are concerned with social (e.g., needs of management, and, despite occasional exceptions,
group dynamics) and organizational (e.g., climate) variables and shows proportionately little concern for the outcomes of work
constructs than the earlier ones, which were more focused on (or nonwork) on workers, their families, and their communities,
individual (e.g., aptitudes) or job (e.g., rest pauses) variables and nor is it very much interested in their unions;
constructs; (f) the state of the science of I-O psychology is better than is
(b) somewhat paradoxically, both cognitive and neobeha- its practice; and
vioral concepts and techniques have become more fashionable; (g) these problems are, to some extent, due to the training
(c) motivational and affective variables and constructs (e.g., received by I-O psychologists.
job involvement) loom larger in later work, although ability, of 8. After the early years (cf. Fryer, 1923; Kornhauser, 1929a;
course, continues its place as a central concern; Viteles, 1932), few American I-O psychologists have paid much
(d) more recently there has been a growing recognition of attention to what has been going on in other countries. The
complexity and overdetermination of worker attitudes and be- main exceptions to this lack of attention appear to have been in
824 RAYMOND A. KATZELL AND JAMES T. AUSTIN

the quality of work life and industrial democracy movements. tions should be brought back to the laboratory for further con-
More attention is warranted to non-American works such as the trolled experimentation, thereby fine-tuning our understand-
Handbook of Work and Organizational Psychology (Drenth, ing of the subject for both theory and practice. Additionally,
Thierry, Willems, & de Wolff, 1984) and the International Re- new or interesting phenomena observed in the field can serve
view of Industrial and Organizational Psychology (edited by as agenda for intensive study in the lab.
Cooper and Robertson since 1986). The contents of these publi- 11. Related to the aforementioned is our impression that I-O
cations identify some differences and similarities between psychology does too little quasi experimentation. Sackett and
American and European I-O psychology. The forthcoming Larson (1990) characterized only 4% of 577 studies under this
final volume of the Dunnette and Hough (1990-1993) Hand- strategy. Interestingly, most of those quasi experiments were
book oj'Industrial and Organizational Psychologyshould help to longitudinal. One of the reasons that we deplore the disuse of
remedy that problem by illustrating the diversity of I-O re- quasi experimentation is precisely the absence of the longer
search and applications in other countries. term dimension from the bulk of I-O research. There is evi-
9. The field's history reveals a proclivity for science and dence that relationships between variables can change over
practice in I-O psychology to be shaped more by external forces time (cf. Hulin, Henry, & Noon, 1990), but I-O science has little
than by theories, foresight, or imagination. In short, I-O psy- to say about that topic. Actually, we suspect that there may be a
chologists tend to be more reactive than proactive, but whether lot more quasi experimentation conducted by I-O practitioners
more so than most applied fields is moot. The neglect of prob- and consultants, but it goes unpublished. Many planned or un-
lems in ethnic and gender employment, until pressed by law planned changes could be developed into respectable quasi ex-
and society, is a case in point. That is not to say that there have periments, which could add to our science as well as our prac-
been no internally generated innovations. Among the numer- tice. Katzell (1959), for instance, suggested that I-O psycholo-
ous laudable examples are the subject of leadership in the area gists, as action researchers, might borrow a leaf from the page
of theory and research and assessment centers in the realm of of operations researchers (Churchman, Ackoff, & Arnoff, 1957)
practice. Graduate education might help stimulate more such by determining whether results of treatments in field settings
developments if it were to challenge students to anticipate accord with predictions based on previous scientific research
emerging needs, to conjecture with anomalies or puzzles that and theory, assuming unfeasibility or even inadvisability (Ar-
are not explained by existing theories (Kuhn, 1970), to envision gyris, 1968) of control groups and some of the other require-
applications of cutting-edge developments in theory, or to de- ments of rigorous experimental design. The interrupted time-
vise new or better methods for studying important phenomena. series quasi-experimental design used by Lawler and Hackman
Scientific and professional groups could contribute by identify- (1969) to evaluate the effects of employee participation in the
ing emerging issues, as illustrated by the Human Capital Initia- development of an incentive plan is a prototype that would be
tive recently launched by the National Behavioral Science Re- appropriate for many such applications; other possibilities are
search Agenda Committee (1992). described by Cook, Campbell, and Peracchio (1990). That we
10. Our impression that there has been a pronounced in- do not see more quasi experiments may be due in part to defi-
crease over the past 25 years in the use of laboratory experi- ciencies in training. But we suspect that, more important, quasi
ments is supported by Sackett and Larson (1990). Those au- experimentation has not been fully accepted into the norms of
thors surveyed 577 empirical studies published in 1977,1982, I-O science, thereby discouraging I-O psychologists from tak-
and 1987 and found that about half were conducted in labora- ing it seriously. Similarly, McCall and Bobko (1990) suggested
tory settings, typically with college students as participants. that greater open-mindedness about methods of inquiry would
Although there is evidence of generalizability from the labora- better serve the ends of discovery than does slavish devotion to
tory to the field (cf. Locke, 1986), we share the concern of Sack- rigorous scientific methodology, and Van Maanen, Dabbs, and
ett and Larson that it is often difficult to establish one or more Faulkner (1982) have espoused greater use of qualitative re-
critical boundary conditions in the laboratory. Moreover, cer- search methods. The conventional methods remain the strate-
tain significant boundary conditions may not even be recog- gies of choice under the right circumstances, but that may not
nized until later; the importance of the demand characteristics always be the case in field situations or for certain problems.
of the experiment (Orne, 1962) is a classic example. To ques- Our journal editors and our academies should take a fresh look
tions of the external validity of laboratory findings should be at these issues.
added the question of their face validity, a factor that may im- 12. As mentioned earlier, one of the criticisms reported by
pede the adoption of I-O research by management and may Dunnette (1984) is that our science is better than our practice.
account for the aforementioned gap between what we know and However, in certain areas, such as OD, our useful practices may
what we practice. This argument is not to be taken as a polemic exceed scientific understanding (cf. Porras & Robertson, in
against laboratory research, in which we too have engaged. press). Moreover, the survey of I-O research topics by Camp-
Rather, it is an argument for planned exchange between the bell, Daft, and Hulin (1982) indicated that significant research
laboratory and field to which we have alluded above. Interven- was often an outgrowth of involvement with organizational
tions based on laboratory research might well be introduced in problems and that the substantive theory or knowledge used in
the field as small-scale pilot experiments; alternatively, or addi- generating milestone research generally did not originate
tionally, evidence of their applicability could be sought by a within I-O psychology. Significant research often had intuitive
correlational approach. Conversely, where there are reasons to or ad hoc inspiration, that is, it did not follow the hypothetico-
question the generality of findings from field research (Dip- deductive model traditionally espoused in the scientific
boye & Flanagan, 1979), the hypothesized boundary condi- method. In combination, the foregoing imply some sort of dis-
APA CENTENNIAL: HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF I-O PSYCHOLOGY 825

junction between I-O psychological science and practice. One case, there clearly exist forces for fragmentation of the field of
clue to this situation is the indication that the problems ad- I-O psychology.
dressed by I-O research have less of an applied flavor than 14. There is evidence of progress in reducing the distance
desired by practitioners (J. P. Campbell et al., 1982). Another between nomothetic and idiographic poles. Several partici-
factor, as we suggested earlier, may be the real or perceived pants in the conference that led to the book Doing Research
limits to generalizability from laboratory research to field ap- that is Useful for Theory and Practice (Lawler et al., 1985) offered
plication. We also have the impression that, for whatever rea- examples of case studies in which practical application was
sons, many practitioners do not assiduously mine the theoreti- guided by scientific knowledge largely derived from nomo-
cal and research literature for potentially useful ideas. thetic strategies. Conversely, the observed consequences of such
13. We have noted enduring differences in the interests and applications can, in principle, serve the purposes of replication
roles of I-O scientists and practitioners. The disparity in their and generalization involved in the development of nomologies,
research agendas poses a further threat to the conventional although those potential contributions have not as yet been
image of the I-O psychologist as scientist-practitioner. How- clearly demonstrated. Also, Lamiell and associates (Lamiell,
ever, that disparity may be misguided, for, as J. P. Campbell 1981; Lamiell, Foss, Larsen, & Hempel, 1983) have described a
(1992) has suggested, they share the same underlying interests mixed, or "idiothetic," strategy that further suggests ways of
in the subject matter of I-O psychology. In addition to the scien- relating the methodologies; Roberson (1989,1990) used a simi-
tist versus practitioner bipolarity, we discern certain other di- lar approach in studying personal work goals.
mensions along which the field of I-O psychology has become 15. There is also evidence that the industrial and the organi-
differentiated. One has to do with cognitive style, which paral- zational branches of the field are more often being combined
lels the nomothetic versus idiographic approaches to knowl- when relevant to the problem. Examples include the potential
edge; in the I-O field, the latter is more characteristic of those merging of job analysis and job design in examining the task
who deal with such topics as individual assessment and OD, environment of the worker (Ilgen & Hollenbeck, 1991), the con-
whereas the former is illustrated by those working on goal set- sideration of turnover in the organizational context (Krack-
ting and test development. Another bipolarity is between those hardt & Porter, 1986), and the growing use of cross-level analy-
who are concerned with the industrial aspect of I-O psychology sis previously noted.
and those dealing with organizational issues. Of course none of 16. Evidence of the merging of the scientist and practitioner
those dichotomies is absolute; there are individuals who take wings of I-O psychology is not as persuasive. Indeed, some of
both nomothetic and idiographic approaches, many who work the trends we have described suggest that the poles may be
in both the industrial and organizational domains, and some moving further apart. Dunnette (1990) recently discussed those
who function as both scientists and practitioners. But we be- issues bearing on the relationship between I-O science and
lieve that most I-O psychologists and their work fall preponder- practice, reported his discomfort with the continuing schism
antly on one side or the other of these dimensions, as exempli- between them, but remained optimistic about their ultimate
fied by the following signs: fusion. He further suggested constructive measures that should
(a) the hyphen between the I and O, plus the differentiation be taken to bring that fusion about, including creating ap-
of subspecialties within each; praisal and reward systems that foster it, creating better commu-
(b) the growing identification of organizational psycholo- nication between scientists and practitioners, doing research
gists and those in schools of business with the Academy of that is relevant to both the academic and nonacademic worlds,
Management, with corresponding weakening of ties with APA; and increasing collaboration between those who are tradition-
(c) the felt need of SIOP to have two Frontiers series, one ally scientists and those who are primarily practitioners. How-
dealing with scientific and the other with professional matters; ever, Hackman (1985) identified several forces that contribute
to the bipolarity, including corporate practices that reward their
(d) the diminution of publishing in standard I-O journals on
I-O psychologists more for behaving like practitioners than as
the part of practitioners, resulting in proportionately greater
scientists, differences in the conceptual and research para-
prominence of publication by academicians;
digms of scientists and practitioners, and the insufficiency of
(e) the growing trend for publications to be based on labora- laboratory and field experiments to serve as guides for practice.
tory rather than field research; Moreover, at least some of the factors underlying them seem
(f) increasing uneasiness with dependence on the standard intransigent. For example, Hackman (1985) pointed out that
nomothetic, paradigmatic scientific methodology as the route the reward systems of most corporations reinforce I-O psycholo-
to I-O knowledge, coupled with calls for alternative approaches; gists for behaving as professionals and practitioners rather than
(g) the expansion of doctoral training in schools of manage- as scientists. Given corporate realities, those reward systems are
ment and of professional psychology, more or less separate likely to endure. He noted that those forces are strong and en-
from departments of psychology; and during and would require new methodological approaches if
(h) the complaints of psychological practitioners about the they are to be overcome.
irrelevance of much scientific research and methodology, while It may be that the resolution will lie not in the integration of
there are also the reverse complaints that practitioners do not science and practice as much as in symbiosis, that is, acceptance
make as much use as they should of I-O science. of the prospect that the roles of scientists and practitioners may
Although some of those observations are symptomatic of the in fact become increasingly different from, although not inde-
scientist-practitioner schism, we think that they also may re- pendent of, each other. To be sure, some I-O psychologists are
flect one or both of the other dimensions of cleavage. In any and will be capable of and comfortable with alternating be-
826 RAYMOND A. KATZELL AND JAMES T. AUSTIN

tween or combining those roles. That is often true of academic Argyris, C. (1957). Personality and organization. New York: Harper.
I-O psychologists who also have a part-time practice. The Argyris, C. (1968). Some unintended consequences of rigorous re-
emerging profession of organizational behavior may produce search. Psychological Bulletin, 70, 185-197.
people who can serve as bridges by carrying reciprocal feed- Arthur, M. B., Hall, D. T, & Lawrence, B. S. (Eds.). (1989). Handbook of
back between the worlds of discovery and application. A fur- career theory. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
ther bridging mechanism was suggested by Seashore (1985) Arvey, R. D, Bouchard, X, Segal, N. L., & Abraham, L. M. (1989). Job
when he observed that much of the scientific work done by him satisfaction: Environmental and genetic components. Journal of Ap-
and his colleagues had its impact on practice through its ab- plied Psychology, 74, 187-194.
sorption into the common culture. The recent spates of scien- Astin, H. S., Suniewick, N., & Dweck, S. (1971). Women: A bibliography
on their education and careers. Washington, DC: Human Service
tific work on fair employment and on productivity illustrate the
Press.
reverse flow. Communication and education are manifestly es-
Austin, J. X, & Bobko, P. (1985). Goal-setting theory: Unexplored areas
sential vehicles for such mutual influences to occur. If the gap and future research needs. Journal of Occupational Psychology, 58,
between science and practice cannot be eliminated, it can at 289-308.
least be bridged by pursuing some of the measures suggested. Austin, J. X, & Wolfle, L. M. (1991). Annotated bibliography of struc-
The need for intercourse between the two domains of I-O psy- tural equation modeling: Technical work. British Journal of Mathe-
chology was aptly stated by Morris Viteles, now 93 years old, in matical and Statistical Psychology, 44, 93-152.
a recent conversation with one of us: "If it isn't scientific, it's not Banas, P. A. (1988). Employee involvement: A sustained labor/manage-
good practice, and if it isn't practical, it's not good science." ment initiative at the Ford Motor Company. In J. P. Campbell & R. J.
17. In principle, the poles of all three of those aforemen- Campbell (Eds.), Productivity in organizations (pp. 388-416). San
tioned dichotomies are complementary rather than antagonis- Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
tic. The centrifugal forces that operate to keep them apart there- Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action. Engle-
fore will need to be outweighed by centripetal forces (Spence, wood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
1987) if I-O psychology is to continue on the upward trajectory Bandura, A., & Cervone, D. (1983). Self-evaluative and self-efficacy
mechanisms governing the effects of goal systems. Journal of Person-
as science and practice that its history has so far manifested.
ality and Social Psychology, 45, 1017-1028.
Attention to the suggestions and examples that we have noted Baritz, L. (1960). The servants of power. New \brk: Wiley.
would, we hope, help effect that outcome. Barkin, S. (1949). Applied social science in the American trade-union
movement. Philosophy of Science, 16, 193-197.
References Barkin, S. (1957). Human relations in the trade unions. In C. M. Arens-
berg, S. Barkins, W E. Chalmers, H. L. Wilensky, J. C. Worthy, &
Abbott, A. (1988). The system of professions. Chicago: University of B. D. Dennis (Eds.), Research in industrial human relations (pp. 192-
Chicago Press. 213). New York: Harper.
Achilles, P. S. (1946). The consulting services of the Psychological Cor- Bass, B. M. (1965). Organizational psychology. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
poration. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 10,120-126. Bass, B. M. (1968). Interface between personnel and organizational
Ackerman, P. L. (1987). Individual differences in skill learning: An psychology. Journal of Applied Psychology, 52, 81-88.
integration of psychometric and information processing perspec- Baumol, W J., Blackman, S. A. B., & Wolff, E. N. (1989). Productivity
tives. Psychological Bulletin, 102, 3-27. and American leadership: The long run. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Ackerman, P. L., & Humphreys, L. G. (1990). Individual differences Beer, M. (1976). The technology of organizational development. In
theory in industrial and organizational psychology. In M. D. Dun- M. D. Dunnette (Ed.), Handbook of industrial and organizational
nette & L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organiza- psychology (pp. 937-993). Chicago: Rand McNally.
tional psychology (2nd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 223-282). Palo Alto, CA: Con- Bell, D. (1972). The coming of post-industrial society. New York: Basic
sulting Psychologists' Press. Books.
Adler, T. (1991, June). Tug-of-war develops over use of GATE. APA
Benjamin, L. X (1980). Research at the archives of the history of Ameri-
Monitor, p. 8.
can psychology: A case history. In J. Brozek & L. J. Pongratz (Eds.),
Age Discrimination in Employment Act of 1986, 29 U.S.C. §621-634
Historiography of modern psychology: Aims, resources, approaches
(1986).
(pp. 241-251). Toronto: C. J. Hogrefe.
Ajzen, 1. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behav-
Bernreuter, R. G., Maxfield, F N., Paterson, D. G., Reymert, M. L., &
ior and Human Decision Processes, 50,179-211.
American Psychological Association (1948, 1950, 1955, 1960, 1965, Fryer, D. H. (1937). The proposed American Association of Applied
1985). Directory. Washington, DC: Author. and Professional Psychology. Journal of Applied Psychology, 21,
American Psychological Association (1970,1975). Biographical direc- 320-341.
tory. Washington, DC: Author. Binet, A., & Simon, T. (1905). Methodes nouvelles pour le diagnostic
American Psychological Association. (1974). Standards for educa- du niveau intellectuel des anormaux [New methods for the diagnosis
tional and psychological testing. Washington, DC: Author. of intellectual level among abnormal children]. LAnnee Psychologi-
American Psychological Association (1980,1991). Membership regis- que, 11, 191-244.
ter. Washington, DC: Author. Binet, A., & Simon, T. (1908). Le developpement de 1'intelligence chez
American Psychological Association (Division 14). (1965). Guidelines les enfants [The development of intelligence among children ]. LAn-
for doctoral education in industrial psychology. American Psycholo- nee Psychologique, 14, 1-94.
gist, 20,822-831. Bingham, W V (1926). Measures of occupational success. Harvard
Americans With Disabilities Act of 1990, 42 US.C. §12101 et seq. Business Review, 5, 1-10.
(1990). Bingham, W V (1947). Military psychology in war and peace. Science,
Angell, J. R. (1907). The province of functional psychology. Psychologi- 106,155-160.
cal Review, 14, 61-91. Bingham, W V (1949). Lessons from the Second World War. In F.
APA CENTENNIAL: HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF I-O PSYCHOLOGY 827

Baumgarten (Ed.), Progress of psychotechnics 1939-1945 (pp. 139- Callahan, R. E. (1962). Education and the cult of efficiency. Chicago:
146). Bern, Switzerland: Francke. University of Chicago Press.
Bingham, W V (1952). Walter Van Dyke Bingham. In E. G. Boring, Campbell, D. T. (1968). Reforms as experiments. American Psycholo-
H. S. Langfeld, H. Werner, & R. M. Yerkes (Eds.), A history of psy- gist, 24, 409-429.
chology in autobiography (Vol. 4, pp. 1-26). New York: Appleton- Campbell, J. P. (1990a). Modeling the performance prediction problem
Century-Crofts. in industrial and organizational psychology. In M. D. Dunnette &
Boehm, V R. (1980). Research in the real world: A conceptual model. L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psy-
Personnel Psychology, 35, 495-504. chology (2nd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 687-732). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting
Bollen, K. E. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. New Psychologists' Press.
York: Wiley. Campbell, J. P. (1990b). An overview of the Army selection and classifi-
Book, W F. (1908). The psychology of skill. Missoula: University of cation project (Project A). Personnel Psychology, 43, 231-239.
Montana Publications in Psychology. Campbell, J. P. (1992, May). The latent structure of science and practice:
Boring, E. G. (1950a). A history of experimental psychology (2nd ed.). A confirmatory test. Paper presented at the Seventh Annual Confer-
New York: Century. ence of the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology,
Boring, E. G. (1950b). The influence of evolutionary thought upon Montreal, Canada.
American psychological thought. In S. Persons (Ed.), Evolutionary Campbell, J. P., Campbell, R. J., & Associates. (1988). Productivity in
thought in America (pp. 268-299). New Haven, CT: Yale University organizations. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Press. Campbell, J. P., Daft, R. L., & Hulin, C. L. (1982). What to study:
Bramel, D, & Friend, R. (1981). Hawthorne, the myth of the docile Generating and developing research questions. Beverly Hills, CA:
worker, and class bias in psychology. American Psychologist, 36, Sage.
867-878. Campbell, J. P., Dunnette, M. D, Lawler, E. E., Ill, & Weick, K. E., Jr.
Bray, D. W, & Associates. (1991). Working with organizations and their (1970). Managerial behavior, performance, and effectiveness. New
people. New York: Guilford. York: McGraw-Hill.
Bray, D. W, Campbell, R. J., & Grant, D. L. (1974). Formative years in Campbell, R. J. (1970). On becoming a psychologist in industry (A
business: A long-term AT&T study of managerial lives. New York: symposium). Personnel Psychology, 23, 191-221.
Wiley. Campion, M. A., Adams, E. F, Morrison, R. F, Spool, M. D, Tornow,
Brief, A. P. (Ed.). (1984). Productivity research in the behavioral and W W, & Wijting, J. P. (1986). I/O psychology research conducted in
social sciences. New \brk: Praeger. nonacademic settings and reasons for nonpublication. The Indus-
Briggs, V M., Jr. (1987). The growth and composition of the U.S. labor trial-Organizational Psychologist, 24(1), 44-49.
force. Science, 238, 176-180. Campion, M. A., & Lord, R. G. (1982). A control systems conceptual-
Brigham, C. A. (1923). A study of American intelligence. Princeton, NJ: ization of the goal-setting and changing process. Organizational Be-
Princeton University Press. havior and Human Performance, 30, 265-287.
Brigham, C. A. (1930). Intelligence tests of immigrant groups. Psycho- Canter, R. R. (1948). Psychologists in industry. Personnel Psychology, 1,
logical Review, 37,158-165. 145-161.
Bringmann, W G, & Balance, W D. G. (1973). Wundt vs. Munsterberg: Carey, A. (1967). The Hawthorne studies: A radical criticism. Ameri-
Roback's version challenged. American Psychologist, 27, 849-850. can Sociological Review, 32, 413-416.
Britt, S. H., & Morgan, J. D. (1946). Military psychologists in World
Carr, H. A. (1930). Functionalism. In C. Murchison (Ed.), Psychologies
War II. American Psychologist, 1, 423-437.
of 1930. Worcester, MA: Clark University Press.
Brogden, H. E. (1946). On the interpretation of the correlation coeffi-
Cascio, W F. (1987). Costing human resources (2nd ed). Boston: PWS-
cient as a measure of predictive efficiency. Journal of Educational
Kent.
Psychology, 37, 64-76.
Cashman, S. D. (1989). America in the twenties and thirties. New \brk:
Brogden, H. E. (1949). When testing pays off. Personnel Psychology, 2,
New York University Press.
171-183.
Cattell, J. McK. (1890). Mental tests and measurements. Mind, 1, 373-
Brogden, H. E., & Taylor, E. K. (1950). The dollar criterion: Applying
380.
the cost accounting concept to criterion construction. Personnel Psy-
chology, 3, 133-154. Cattell, J. McK. (1923). The Psychological Corporation. Annals of the
Brooke, R P., Jr., Russell, D. W, & Price, J. L. (1988). Discriminant American Academy of Political and Social Science, 110, 165-171.
validation of measures of job satisfaction, job involvement, and orga- Cattell, J. McK. (1946). Retrospect: Psychology as a profession. Jour-
nizational commitment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 73,139-145. nal of Consulting Psychology, 10, 289-291.
Brown, S. H. (1981). Validity generalization and situational specificity Chapanis, A., Garner, W R., & Morgan, C. (1949). Applied experimen-
in the life insurance industry. Journal of Applied Psychology, 66, tal psychology. New York: Wiley.
664-670. Chapman, J. C. (1920). Army trade tests. New York: Holt.
Brown v. Topeka Board of Education, 347 U.S. 483 (1954). Chapman, J. C., & Toops, H. A. (1919). A written trade test: Multiple
Bryan, W L., & Harter, N. (1897). Studies in the physiology and psy- choice method. Journal of Applied Psychology, 3, 358-365.
chology of the telegraphic language. Psychological Review, 4, 27-35. Churchman, C. W, Ackoff, R. L., & Arnoff, E. L. (1957). Introduction to
Burtt, H. E. (1926). Principles of employment psychology. New York: operations research. New York: Wiley.
Harper. Civil Rights Act of 1964, 42 U.S.C. §2000e-2000e-17 (1964).
Burtt, H. E. (1929). Psychology and industrial efficiency. New \brk: Civil Rights Act of 1991, P. L. 102-166,105 Stat. 1071 (1991).
Appleton. Clark, K. E. (1947). Applied research that might have been. Journal of
Buxton, C. E. (1985). American functionalism. In C. E. Buxton (Ed.), Applied Psychology, 31, 94-98.
Points of view in the modern history of psychology (pp. 113-140). New Clark, K. E. (1957). America's psychologists: A survey of a growing
York: Academic Press. profession. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Byham, W C., & Spitzer, M. E. (1971). The law and personnel testing. Clarke, A. W (1980). Action research: Theory, practice, and values.
New York: American Management Association. Journal of Occupational Behavior, 1, 151-157.
828 RAYMOND A. KATZELL AND JAMES T. AUSTIN

Clifford, G. J. (1984). Edward L. Thorndike: The sane positivist. Middle- Dunnette, M. D. (1962). Personnel management. Annual Review of
town, CT: Wesleyan Press. Psychology, 13, 285-314.
Coch, L., & French, J. R. P. (1948). Overcoming resistance to change. Dunnette, M. D. (1966). Fads, fashions, and folderol in psychology.
Human Relations, 1, 512-532. American Psychologist, 21, 343-352.
Collins, L. M., & Horn, J. L. (Eds.). (1991). Best methods for the analy- Dunnette, M. D. (Ed.). (1976). Handbook of industrial and organiza-
sis of change. Washington, DC: American Psychological Associa- tional psychology. Chicago: Rand McNally.
tion. Dunnette, M. D. (1984, August). I/O psychology in the 80s: Fads, fash-
Committee on Classification of Personnel. (1918). How the Army uses ions, and folderol revisited. Invited address presented at the 92nd
individual differences in experience. Psychological Bulletin, 15, Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, To-
187-206. ronto, Ontario, Canada.
Cook, T. D., Campbell, D. X, & Peracchio, L. (1990). Quasi-experimen- Dunnette, M. D. (1989). Validation of selection tests for electrical
tation. In M. D. Dunnette & L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of indus- power plant operators. In B. Fallon, H. Pfister, & J. Brebner (Eds.),
trial and organizational psychology (2nd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 492-576). Advances in industrial-organizational psychology (pp. 377-387).
Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists' Press. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Cowles, M. (1989). Statistics in psychology: An historical perspective. Dunnette, M. D. (1990). Blending the science and practice of industrial
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. and organizational psychology: Where are we and where are we go-
Crennan, C. H., & Kingsbury, F. A. (1923). Psychology in business ing? In M. D. Dunnette & L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of indus-
[Special issue]. Annals of the American Academy of Political and trial and organizational psychology (2nd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 492-576).
Social Science, 110, 2-232. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists' Press.
Cronbach, L. J. (1957). The two disciplines of scientific psychology. Dunnette, M. D, & Hough, L. (Eds.). (1990-1993). Handbook of indus-
American Psychologist, 12, 671-684. trial and organizational psychology (2nd ed.). Palo Alto, CA: Con-
Cronbach, L. J., & Neff, W. D. (1949). Selection and training. In Panel sulting Psychologists' Press.
on Psychology and Physiology (Ed.), Human factors in undersea war- Dvorak, B. (1935). Differential occupational ability patterns (Bulletins,
fare (pp. 491 -513). Washington, DC: National Research Council. Employment Stabilization and Research Institute). Minneapolis:
Cyert, R. M., & March, J. G. (1963). A behavioral theory of the firm. University of Minnesota.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Ebbinghaus, H. (1897). Uber eine neue Methode zur Prufunggeistiger
Darley, J. G. (1968). 1917: A journal is born. Journal of Applied Psychol- Fahigkeiten und ihre Anwendung bei Schulkindern [Concerning a
ogy, 52, 1-9. new method of testing intellectual capacities and its use with school-
Davis, T. R. V, & Luthans, F. (1980). A social learning approach to children]. Zeitschrift fur Psychologic und Physiologic der Sinnesor-
organizational behavior. Academy of Management Review, 5, 281- gane, 13, 401-459.
290. Ebbinghaus, H. (1913). Uber das gedachtniss [Memory: A contribution
Dawis, R. V, & Lofquist, L. H. (1984). A psychological theory of work to experimental psychology] (H. Ruger&C. Bussenius, Trans.). New
adjustment. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. York: Teachers College. (Original work published 1885)
Deci, E. L. (1975). Intrinsic motivation. New \brk: Plenum. Emery, F. E. (Ed.). (1969). Systems thinking. Baltimore, MD: Penguin.
DeNisi, A. S., & Williams, K. J. (1988). Cognitive approaches to perfor-
Estes, W K. (1991). Cognitive architectures from the standpoint of an
mance appraisal. In G. R. Ferris & K. M. Rowland (Eds.), Research experimental psychologist. Annual Review of Psychology, 42, 1-28.
in personnel and human resources management (Vol. 6, pp. 109-155).
Evans, R. B., Sexton, V S., & Cadwallader, T. S. (Eds.). (1992). The
Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
American Psychological Association: A historical perspective. Wash-
Dickson, W J. (1950). The Hawthorne plan of personnel counseling. In
ington, DC: American Psychological Association.
A. H. Brayfield (Ed.), Readings in modern methods of counseling (pp.
Fedor, D. B., & Ferris, G. R. (1981). Integrating OB Mod with cognitive
133-138). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
approaches to motivation. Academy of Management Review, 6,115-
Dickson, W J., & Roethlisberger, F. J. (1966). Counseling in an organiza-
125.
tion: A sequel to the Hawthorne researches. Boston: Harvard Gradu-
ate School of Business Administration. Feldman, J. M. (1981). Beyond attribution theory: Cognitive processes
Dipboye, R. L., & Flanagan, M. F. (1979). Research settings in indus- in performance appraisal. Journal of Applied Psychology, 66, 127-
trial and organizational psychology. American Psychologist, 34,141 - 148.
150. Ferber, R. (1977). Coverage and scope of consumer behavior. In Se-
Dockeray, F. C. (1920). Work, fatigue, and inhibition. Psychological lected aspects of consumer behavior (pp. 3-7). Washington, DC: Na-
Bulletin, 17, 322-330. tional Science Foundation.
Doll, E. A. (1946). The divisional structure of the APA. American Psy- Ferguson, L. W (1952). A look across the years 1920 to 1950. In L. L.
chologist, 1, 336-345. Thurstone (Ed.), Applications of psychology: Essays to honor Walter
Drasgow, E, & Hulin, C. L. (1990). Item response theory. In M. D. V. Bingham (pp. 1-17). New York: Harper.
Dunnette & L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organi- Ferguson, L. W (1961). The development of industrial psychology. In
zational psychology (2nd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 577-636). Palo Alto, CA: B. von H. Gilmer (Ed.), Industrial psychology (pp. 18-37). New
Consulting Psychologists' Press. York: McGraw-Hill.
Drenth, P J. D, Thierry, H., Willems, P. J., & de Wolff, C. J. (Eds.). Ferguson, L. W (1962). Industrial psychology and labor. In B. von H.
(1984). Handbook of work and organizational psychology. New\brk: Gilmer (Ed.), Walter Vandyke Bingham (pp. 7-22). Pittsburgh, PA:
Wiley. Department of Psychology, Carnegie Institute of Technology.
DuBois, P. H. (1965). A test-dominated society: China, 1115 B.C.-1905 Ferguson, L. W (1962-1965). The heritage of 'industrial psychology (14
A.D. In Proceedings of the 1964 invitational conference on testing pamphlets). Hartford, CT: Finlay Press.
problems (pp. 3-11). Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service. Fichman, M. (1988). Motivational consequences of absence and atten-
DuBois, P. H. (1970). A history of psychological testing. Boston: Allyn& dance: Proportional hazards estimation of a dynamic motivation
Bacon. model. Journal of Applied Psychology, 73, 119-134.
APA CENTENNIAL: HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF I-O PSYCHOLOGY 829

Fine, S. A. (1971). An introduction to functional job analysis. Kalama- Goldstein, L, & Associates. (1989). Training and development in organi-
zoo, MI: W E. Upjohn Institute for Employment Research. zations. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Finison, L. J. (1979). An aspect of the early history of the Society for the Gordon, M. A. (1953). A study of the applicability of the same minimum
Psychological Study of Social Issues: Psychologists and labor. Jour- qualifying scores for technical schools to white males, WAF,and Negro
nal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 15, 29-37. males (T.R. 53-34). Lackland Air Force Base, TX: Personnel Re-
Fitts, P. M. (1951). Engineering psychology and equipment design. In search Laboratory.
S. S. Stevens (Ed.), Handbook of'experimental psychology (pp. 1287- Gordon, M. E., & Burt, R. (1981). A history of industrial psychology's
1340). New York: Wiley. relationship with American unions: Lessons from the past and direc-
Flanagan, J. C. (1946). The experimental evaluation of a selection pro- tions for the future. International Review of Applied Psychology, 30,
cedure. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 6, 445-466. 137-156.
Flanagan, J. C. (Ed.). (1947-1949). The aviation psychology program in Gordon, M. E., & Nurick, A. J. (1981). Psychological approaches to the
the Army Air Forces (19 vols). Washington, DC: U.S. Government study of unions and union-management relations. Psychological
Printing Office. Bulletin, 90, 293-306.
Flanagan, J. C. (1981). Organizational psychology in perspective. In Gordon, R. A., & Howell, J. E. (1959). Higher education for business.
J. A. Sgro (Ed.), Virginia Tech symposium on applied behavioral New York: Columbia University Press.
science (pp. 17-35). Lexington, MA: Heath. Gottfredson, L. (Ed.). (1986). The g factor in employment [Special
Forehand, G. A., & Gilmer, B. von H. (1964). Environmental variation issue]. Journal of'Vocational Behavior, 29, 293-450.
in studies of organizational behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 62, Gottfredson, L., & Sharf, J. (Eds.). (1988). Fairness in employment
361-382. testing [Special issue]. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 33, 225-477.
Frederiksen, L. W(Ed.). (1982). Handbook of organizational behavior Gould, S. J. (1981). The mismeasure of man. New York: Norton.
management. New York: Wiley. Greenhaus, J. H., & Beutell, N. V (1985). Sources of conflict in work
Freyd, M. (1923). Measurement in vocational selection: An outline of and family roles. Academy of Management Review, 10, 76-88.
research procedure. Journal of Personnel Research, 2, 215-284,377- Griggs v. Duke Power Company, 401 U.S. 424 (1971).
385. Guetzkow, H. (Ed.). (1951). Groups, leadership, and men. Pittsburgh,
Fryer, D. H. (1923). Psychology and industry in France and Great Brit- PA: Carnegie Press.
ain. Journal of Personnel Research, 2, 396-402. Guilford, J. P. (1936). Psychometric methods. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Fryer, D. H. (1931). The measurement of interests in relation to voca- Guion, R. M. (1976). Recruiting, selection, and job placement. In M.
tional adjustment. New \brk: Holt. D. Dunnette (Ed.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psy-
Fryer, D. H., & Henry, E. R. (Eds.). (1950). Handbook of applied psychol- chology (pp. 777-828). Chicago: Rand McNally.
ogy (2 vols.). New \brk: Rinehart. Guion, R. M. (1991). Assessment, selection, and placement. In M. D.
Furnham, A. (1990). The Protestant work ethic: The psychology of work- Dunnette & L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organi-
related attitudes and behaviors. London: Routledge. zational psychology (2nd ed., Vol. 2, pp. 327-397). Palo Alto, CA:
Furumoto, L. (1987). On the margins: Women and the professionaliza- Consulting Psychologists' Press.
tion of psychology in the United States, 1890-1940. In M. G. Ash & Guzzo, R. A., Jackson, S. E., & Katzell, R. A. (1987). Meta-analysis. In
W R. Woodward (Eds.), Psychology in twentieth-century thought and B. M. Staw & L. L. Cummings (Eds.), Research in organizational
society (pp. 93-113). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University behavior (Vol. 9, pp. 407-442). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Press. Guzzo, R. A., Jette, R. D.,&Katzell, R. A. (1985). The effectsof psycho-
Gallon, F. (1877). Studies of types of character. Nature, 16, 244-247. logically-based intervention programs on worker productivity: A
Gallon, F. (1883). Inquiries into human faculty and its development. meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, 38, 275-291.
London: Dent. Hackman, J. R. (1985). Doing research that makes a difference. In E. E.
Gardner, H. (1985). The mind's new science: A history of the cognitive Lawler, III, A. M. Mohrman, Jr., S. A. Mohrman, G. E. Ledford, Jr.,
revolution. New York: Basic Books. & T. G. Cummings (Eds.), Doing research that is useful for theory and
Gaugler, B. B., Rosenthal, D. B., Thornton, G. C., HI, & Bentson, C. practice (pp. 126-149). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
(1987). Meta-analysis of assessment center validity. Journal of Ap- Hackman, J. R., & Oldham, G. R. (1975). Development of the Job
plied Psychology, 72, 493-511. Diagnostic Survey. Journal of Applied Psychology, 60,159-170.
Geissler, L. R. (1918). A plan for the technical training of consulting Hackman, J. R., & Oldham, G. R. (1976). Motivation through the de-
psychologists. Journal of Applied Psychology, 2, 77-83. sign of work: Test of a theory. Organizational Behavior and Human
George, J. M., & Brief, A. P. (in press). Feeling good-doing good: A Performance, 16, 250-279.
conceptual analysis of the mood at work-organizational spontaneity Hackman, J. R., & Suttle, J. L. (Eds.). (1977). Improving life at work.
relationship. Psychological Bulletin. Santa Monica, CA: Goodyear.
Ghiselli, E. E. (1978). From Jack London to Luigi Pirandello: Influ- Haire, M. (1959). Psychology and the study of business: Joint behav-
ences in the career of an industrial psychologist. In T. Krawiec (Ed.), ioral sciences. In R. A. Dahl, M. Haire, & P. Lazarsfeld (Eds.), Social
The psychologists (Vol. 3, pp. 61 -83). Brandon, VT: Clinical Psychol- science research on business: Product and potential (pp. 47-98). New
ogy Publishing Company. York: Columbia University Press.
Gilbreth, F. B. (1911). Motion study. New York: Van Nostrand. Hale, M., Jr. (1980). Human science and social order: Hugo Mttnsterberg
Gilbrelh, F. B., & Gilbreth, L. M. (1916). Fatigue study. New York: and the origins of applied psychology. Philadelphia: Temple Univer-
Sturgis & Walton. sity Press.
Gilbreth, F. B., & Gilbreth, L. M. (1917). Applied motion study. New Hale, M., Jr. (1982). History of employment testing. In A. K. Wigdor &
York: Sturgis & Walton. W R. Garner (Eds.), Ability testing: Uses, consequences, and contro-
Gilbreth, L. M. (1914). The psychology of management. New York: versies (pp. 3-38). Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Sturgis & Walton. Hall, D. T, & Associates. (1986). Career development in organizations.
Glass, G. V (1976). Primary, secondary, and meta-analysis of research. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Educational Researcher, 5, 3-8. Hanisch, K. A., & Hulin, C. L. (1991). General attitudes and organiza-
830 RAYMOND A. KATZELL AND JAMES T. AUSTIN

tional withdrawal: An evaluation of a causal model. Journal of Voca- face of American Psychology. American Psychologist, 41. 1311-
tional Behavior, 39, 110-128. 1327.
Harrell, T. W (in press). Personnel research for World War II: Army Howell, W C. (1991). Human factors in the workplace. In M. D. Dun-
General Classification Test and some other products. Journal of Ap- nette & L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organiza-
plied Psychology. tional psychology (2nd ed., Vol. 2, pp. 209-269). Palo Alto, CA: Con-
Hartigan, J. A., & Wigdor, A. K. (Eds.). (1989). Fairness in employment sulting Psychologists' Press.
testing. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Huff, T. (1984). Max Weber and the methodology of the social sciences.
Hartmann, G. W, & Newcomb, T. W (Eds.). (1939). Industrial conflict: New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Books.
A psychological interpretation. New York: Corden. Hulin, C. L. (1991). Adaptation, persistence, and commitment in orga-
Harvey, R. J. (1991). Job analysis. In M. D. Dunnette & L. M. Hough nizations. In M. D. Dunnette & L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of
(Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology. (2nd industrial and organizational psychology (2nd ed., Vol. 2, pp. 445-
ed., Vol. 2, pp. 71-163). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists' 505). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists' Press.
Press. Hulin, C. L., Henry, R., & Noon, S. (1990). Adding a dimension: Time
Heidbreder, E. (1933). Seven psychologies. New York: Century. as a factor in the generalizability of predictive relationships. Psycho-
Heilman, M. E. (1983). Sex bias in work settings: The lack of fit model. logical Bulletin, 107, 328-340.
In L. L. Cummings & B. M. Staw (Eds.), Research in organizational Hull, C. L. (1928). Aptitude testing. Yonkers, NY: World Book.
behavior (Vol. 5, pp. 269-298). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Humphreys, L. G. (1986). An analysis and evaluation of test and item
Heller, F. A. (1969). Group feedback analysis: A method of field re- bias in the prediction context. Journal of Applied Psychology, 71,
search. Psychological Bulletin, 72,108-117. 327-333.
Herrnstein, R. J. (1971, September). IQ. Atlantic Monthly, pp. 43-64. Hunt, E. (1989). Cognitive science: Definition, status, and questions.
Herzberg, F. (1966). Work and the nature of man. Cleveland, OH: World. Annual Review of Psychology, 40, 603-629.
Herzberg, F., Mausner, B., & Snyderman, B. (1959). The motivation to Hunter, J. E., & Schmidt, F. L. (1990). Methods ofmela-analysis. Beverly
work (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley. Hills, CA: Sage.
Hicks, J. P. (1992, April 3). The steel man with the kid gloves. New York
Hunter, J. E., Schmidt, F. L., & Hunter, R. (1979). Differential validity
Times, pp. Dl, D3.
of employment tests by race: A comprehensive review and analysis.
Hilgard, E. R. (1987). Psychology in America: A historical survey. New Psychological Bulletin, 86, 721-735.
York: Harcourt, Brace, Jovanovich.
Hurrell, J. J., Jr., Murphy, L. R., Sauter, S. L., & Cooper, C. L. (Eds.).
Hilgard, E. R., Leary, D. E., & McGuire, G. R. (1991). History of psy-
(1988). Occupational stress: Issues and developments in research.
chology: A survey and critical assessment. Annual Review of Psychol-
London: Taylor & Francis.
ogy, 42, 79-108.
laffaldano, M. T, & Muchinsky, P. M. (1985). Job satisfaction and job
Hill, C. (1955). Psychological research within the armed services.
performance: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 67, 251-273.
American Psychologist, 10, 238-242.
Ilgen, D. R. (1990). Health issues at work: Opportunities for industrial/
Hoagland, J. H. (1964). Historical antecedents of organization re-
organizational psychology. American Psychologist, 45, 273-283.
search. In W Cooper, H. Leavitt, & P. Shelley (Eds.), New perspectives
in organization research (pp. 27-38). New \fork: Wiley. Ilgen, D. R., & Hollenbeck, J. R. (1991). The structure of work: Job
Hogan, R. T. (1991). Personality and personality measurement. In design and roles. In M. D. Dunnette & L. M. Hough (Eds.), Hand-
M. D. Dunnette & L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and book of industrial and organizational psychology (2nd ed., Vol. 2, pp.
organizational psychology (2nd ed., Vol. 2, pp. 873-919). Palo Alto, 165-208). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists' Press.
CA: Consulting Psychologists' Press. Ilgen, D. R., & Klein, H. J. (1989). Organizational behavior. Annual
Hoppock, R. M. (1935). Job satisfaction. New York: Harper. Review of Psychology, 40, 327-351.
Houser, J. D. (1927). What the employer thinks. Cambridge, MA: Har- Institute of Labor and Industrial Relations. (1953-1954). Labor-mana-
vard University Press. gement relations in Illini City (2 vols.). Champaign: University of
Houser, J. D. (1938). What people want from business. New York: Illinois Press.
McGraw-Hill. Jacobson, J. Z. (1951). Scott of Northwestern. Chicago: Louis Mariano.
Howard, A. (1982). Who are the industrial/organizational psycholo- Jacoby, J. (1976). Consumer and industrial psychology: Prospects for
gists? An analysis of data from the 1981APA Directory survey. Arling- theory corroboration and mutual contribution. In M. D. Dunnette
ton Heights, IL: Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychol- (Ed.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (pp.
ogy. 1031-1061). Chicago: Rand McNally.
Howard, A. (1990). The multiple facets of industrial-organizational psy- Jahoda, M. (1981). Work, employment, and unemployment: Values,
chology. Arlington Heights, IL: Society for Industrial and Organiza- theories, and approaches in social research. American Psychologist,
tional Psychology. 56,184-191.
Howard, A. (199 la). Industrial/organizational psychologists as practi- Jahoda, M., Lazarsfeld, P. F, & Zeisel, H. (1972). Marienthal: The so-
tioners. In D. W Bray & Associates (Eds.), Working with organizations ciography of an unemployed community. London: Tavistock. (Origi-
and their people (pp. 13-44). New \brk: Guilford Press. nal work published 1933)
Howard, A. (1991b). New directions for human resources practice. In James, L. R., Demaree, R. G., & Mulaik, S. A. (1986). A note on validity
D. W Bray & Associates (Eds.), Working with organizations and their generalization procedures. Journal of Applied Psychology, 71, 440-
people (pp. 219-251). New York: Guilford Press. 450.
Howard, A., & Bray, D. W (1988). Managerial lives in transition. New James, L. R., & James, L. A. (1989). Causal modelling in organiza-
York: Guilford Press. tional research. In C. L. Cooper & I. Robertson (Eds.), International
Howard, A., & Lowman, R. L. (1985). Should industrial/organiza- review of industrial and organizational psychology 1989 (Vol. 4, pp.
tional psychologists be licensed? American Psychologist, 40,40-47. 371-404). New York: Wiley.
Howard, A., Pion, G. M., Gottfredson, G. D., Flattau, P. E., Oskamp, S., James, L. R., & Jones, A. P. (1974). Organizational climate: A review of
Pfafflin, S. M., Bray, D. W, & Burstein, A. G. (1986). The changing theory and research. Psychological Bulletin, 81, 1096-1112.
APA CENTENNIAL: HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF I-O PSYCHOLOGY 831

Jeanneret, P. R. (1991). Growth trends in I/O psychology. The Indus- Kopelman, R. E. (1986). Managing productivity in organizations: A
trial-Organizational Psychologist, 29(2), 47-52. practical, people-oriented perspective. New \brk: McGraw-Hill.
Jenkins, J. G. (1935). Psychology in business and industry: An introduc- Kornhauser, A. W (1922). The psychology of vocational selection. Psy-
tion to psychotechnology. New \brk: Wiley. chological Bulletin, 19,192-229.
Johnston, W B., & Packer, A. E. (Eds.). (1987). Workforce 2000: Work Kornhauser, A. W (1926-1927). What are rating scales good for? Jour-
and workers for the 21st century. Indianapolis, IN: Hudson Institute. nal of Personnel Research, 5,189-193, 309-317, 338-344,440-446.
Jones, J. W, Steffy, B. W, & Bray, D. W (Eds.). (1990). Applying psychol- Kornhauser, A. W (1929a). Industrial psychology in England, Ger-
ogy in business: The handbook for managers and human resource many, and the United States. Personnel Journal, 8, 421-434.
professionals. Lexington, MA: Lexington. Kornhauser, A. W (1929b). The study of work feelings. Personnel Jour-
Kahn, R. L. (1981). Work and health. New York: Wiley. nal.8, 348-351.
Kanfer, F. H., & Hagerman, S. M. (1985). Behavior therapy and the Kornhauser, A. W (Ed.). (1949). Psychology of labor-management rela-
information-processing paradigm. In R. Reiss & R. Bootzin (Eds.), tions. Madison, WI: Industrial Relations Research Association.
Theoretical issues in behavior therapy (pp. 3-33). New \brk: Aca- Kornhauser, A. W (1965). Mental health of the industrial worker. New
demic Press. York: Wiley.
Katz, D., & Kahn, R. A. (1966). The social psychology of organizations. Kornhauser, A. W, & Kingsbury, F. A. (1924). Psychological tests in
New York: Wiley. business. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Katzell, M. E., & Byham, W C. (Eds.). (1972). Women in the workforce. Kornhauser, A. W, & Sharp, A. A. (1932). Employee attitudes: Sugges-
New York: Behavioral Publications. tions from a study in a factory. Personnel Journal, 10, 393-404.
Katzell, R. A. (1958). Looking around: Is individualism disappearing? Krackhardt, D., & Porter, L. W (1986). The snowball effect: Turnover
Harvard Business Review, 36, 139-143. embedded in communication networks. Journal of Applied Psychol-
Katzell, R. A. (1959). Blueprinting the next ten years of industrial ogy, 71, 50-55.
psychology (A symposium): Some additional prospects. Personnel Kraus, M. P. (1986). Walter Van Dyke Bingham and the Bureau of Per-
Psychology, 12, 45-48. sonnel Research. New York: Garland.
Katzell, R. A. (1962). Psychologists in industry. In W Webb (Ed.), The Kuhn, T. S. (1970). The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: Uni-
profession of psychology (pp. 180-211). New \brk: Holt, Rinehart, & versity of Chicago Press.
Winston. Kuna, D. P. (1976). The psychology of advertising, 1896-1916. Unpub-
Katzell, R. A. (1964). Personal values, job satisfaction, and job perfor- lished doctoral dissertation, University of New Hampshire, Dur-
mance. In H. Borow (Ed.), Man in a world of work (pp. 341-363). ham.
Boston: Houghton-Mifflin. Kuna, D. P. (1979). Early advertising applications of the Gale-Cattell
order-of-merit method. Journal of the History of the Behavioral
Katzell, R. A. (1991). History of early I-O doctoral programs. The
Sciences, 15, 38-46.
Industrial Organizational Psychologist, 28(4), 51.
Lamiell, J. T. (1981). Toward an idiothetic psychology of personality.
Katzell, R. A., & Dyer, F. J. (1977). Differential validity revived. Jour-
American Psychologist, 36, 276-289.
nal of Applied Psychology, 62,137-145.
Lamiell, J. T, Foss, M. A., Larsen, R. J., & Hempel, A. M. (1983).
Katzell, R. A., & Guzzo, R. A. (1983). Psychological approaches to
Studies in intuitive personology from an idiothetic point of view:
productivity improvement. American Psychologist, 38, 468-472.
Implications for personality theory. Journal of Personality, 51, 438-
Katzell, R. A., & Thompson, A. S. (1958). Some comments on "After 467.
legislation. . ." American Psychologist, 13, 652-654. Landsberger, H. A. (1958). Hawthorne revisited. Ithaca, NY: Cornell
Katzell, R. A., & Thompson, D. E. (1990a). An integrative model of University Press.
work attitudes, motivation, and performance. Human Performance, Landy, F. J., & Farr, J. L. (1980). Performance rating. Psychological
3, 63-85. Bulletin, 87, 72-107.
Katzell, R. A., & Thompson, D. E. (1990b). Work motivation: Theory LaPointe, J. B. (1990). Industrial/organizational psychology: A view
and practice. American Psychologist, 45,144-153. from the field. In K. R. Murphy & F. B. Saal (Eds.), Psychology in
Kaufman, H. (1913). The efficient age. New York: Doubleday. organizations: Integrating science and practice (pp. 7-24). Hillsdale,
Kavruck, S. (1956). Thirty-three years of test development: A short NJ: Erlbaum.
history of test development in the U.S. Civil Service Commission. Lawler, E. E., III. (1971). The changing role of industrial psychology in
American Psychologist, 11, 329-333. university education: A symposium. Professional Psychology, 2,
Keenan, V, & Kerr, W A. (1952). Unfair employment practice as 2-22.
viewed by private employment counselors. Journal of Applied Psy- Lawler, E. E., Ill, & Hackman, J. R. (1969). Impact of employee partici-
chology, 36, 361-364. pation in the development of pay incentive plans: A field experi-
Kerr, W A. (1954). Dual allegiance and emotional acceptance-rejec- ment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 53, 467-471.
tion in industry. Personnel Psychology, 7, 59-66. Lawler, E. E., Ill, Mohrman, A. M., Jr., Mohrman, S. A., Ledford, G. E.,
Kirkpatrick, J. J., Ewen, R. B., Barrett, R. S., & Katzell, R. A. (1968). Jr., Cummings, T. G., & Associates. (1985). Doing research that is
Testing and fair employment. New York: New York University Press. useful for theory and practice. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Klimoski, R. J. (1992, May). Revitalizing the interface between science Lawshe, C. H., & Weiss, H. M. (1991). History of industrial/organiza-
and practice. Presidential address at the Seventh Annual Conference tional psychology at Purdue University. The Industrial-Organiza-
of the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology, Mon- tional Psychologist, 28(4), 52-57.
treal, Canada. Leavitt, H. J. (1962). Toward organizational psychology. In B. von H.
Kohn, M. L., & Schooler, C. (1983). Work and personality: An inquiry Gilmer (Ed.), Walter Vandyke Bingham (pp. 23-30). Pittsburgh, PA:
into the impact of social stratification. Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Department of Psychology, Carnegie Institute of Technology.
Komaki, J. L., Desselles, M. L., & Bowman, E. D. (1989). Definitely not Levin, I., & Stokes, J. P. (1989). Dispositional approach to job satisfac-
a breeze: Extending an operant model of effective supervision to tion: Role of negative affectivity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74,
teams. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 522-531. 752-758.
832 RAYMOND A. KATZELL AND JAMES T. AUSTIN

Levy-Leboyer, C. (1988). Success and failure in applying psychology. McCormick, E. J., Jeanneret, P. R., & Mecham, R. (1972). A study of
American Psychologist, 43, 779-785. job characteristics and job dimensions as based on the Position Anal-
Lewin, D. (1986). Summary of the symposium "Industrial/organiza- ysis Questionnaire (PAQ). Journal of Applied Psychology, 56, 347-
tional psychologists and industrial relations: A symposium." Indus- 368.
trial Relations Research Association Proceedings, 39, 507-512. McGehee, W, & Thayer, P. W (1961). Training in business and industry.
Lewin, K., Dembo, T., Festinger, L., & Sears, P. S. (1944). Level of New York: Wiley.
aspiration. In J. McV Hunt (Ed.), Personality and the behavior dis- McGregor, D. (1960). The human side of enterprise. New York:
orders (pp. 333-378). New York: Ronald. McGraw-Hill.
Lewin, K., Lippitt, R., & White, R. K. (1939). Patterns of aggressive McKeachie, W J., & Brim, O. G, Jr. (1984). Lessons to be learned from
behavior in experimentally created "social climates." Journal oj'So- large behavioral research organizations. American Psychologist, 39,
cial Psychology, 10, 271-299. 1254-1255.
Likert, R. (1932). A technique for the measurement of attitudes. Ar- Melton, A. W (1957). Military psychology in the United States of
chives of Psychology, 22, 1-55. America. American Psychologist, 12, 740-746.
Link, H. C. (1919). Employment psychology. New York: Macmillan. Meltzer, H. (1960). Industrial psychology in Psychological Abstracts,
Link, H. C. (1920). The applications of psychology to industry. Psycho- 1927-1959. Journal of Applied Psychology, 44, 111 -114.
logical Bulletin, 17, 335-346. Meltzer, H., & Shulman, A. D. (Eds.). (1982). Special section on indus-
Linn, R. L. (Ed.). (1985). Intelligence. Urbana: University of Illinois trial-organizational psychology. Professional Psychology, 13, 889-
Press. 953.
Locke, E. A. (1968). Toward a theory of task motivation and incentives. Meltzer, H., & Stagner, R. (Eds.). (1980). Industrial-organizational psy-
Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 3, 157-189. chology: 1980 overview [Special issue]. Professional Psychology, 11,
Locke, E. A. (1982). The ideas of Frederick W Taylor: An evaluation. 347-546.
Academy of Management Review, 7, 14-24. Merton, R. K. (1982). Social change and the practicing professions.
Locke, E. A. (Ed.). (1986). Generalizing from laboratory to field settings. Cambridge, MA: Abt Books.
Lexington, MA: Heath. Miner, J. B. (1984). The validity and usefulness of theories in the
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting and task emerging organizational sciences. Academy of Management Review,
performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. 9, 296-306.
Lopez, F. M., Jr. (1966). Current problems in test performance of job Miner, M. G, & Miner, J. B. (1978). Employee selection within the law.
applicants. Personnel Psychology, 19, 10-18. Washington, DC: Bureau of National Affairs.
Lord, R. G. (1985). An information processing approach to social per- Moore, B. V (1962). Some beginnings of industrial psychology. In
ceptions, leadership, and behavioral measurement in organizations. B. von H. Gilmer (Ed.), Walter VanDyke Bingham (pp. 1-5). Pitts-
In B. M. Staw & L. L. Cummings (Eds.), Research in organizational burgh, PA: Carnegie Institute of Technology.
behavior (Vol. 1, pp. 87-128). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Morita, J. G, Lee, T. W, & Mowday, R. T. (1989). Introducing survival
Lord, R. G., & Hanges, P. J. (1987). A control systems model of organi- analysis to organizational researchers: A selected application to turn-
zational motivation: Theoretical development and applied implica- over research. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 280-292.
tions. Behavioral Science, 32, 161-178. Morrison, S. E. (1965). The Oxford history of the American people. New
Lord, R. G., & Maher, K. J. (199 la). Cognitive theory in industrial and York: Oxford University Press.
organizational psychology. In M. D. Dunnette & L. M. Hough (Eds.), Mowday, R. T., Steers, R. M., & Porter, L. W (1979). The measurement
Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology. (2nded., Vol. of organizational commitment. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 14,
2, pp. 1-62). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists' Press. 224-247.
Lord, R. G., & Maher, K. J. (199 Ib). Leadership and information pro-
Mueller, C. G. (1979). Some origins of psychology as a science. Annual
cessing: Linking perceptions and performance. New \ork: Harper-
Review of Psychology, 30, 9-29.
Collins.
Miinsterberg, H. (1913). Psychology and industrial efficiency. Boston:
Louttit, C. M. (1943). Directory of applied psychologists (2nd ed.) New
Houghton-Mifflin.
York: American Association for Applied Psychology.
Murphy, K. R., & Cleveland, J. N. (1991). Performance appraisal: An
Luthans, F, & Kreitner, R. (1975). Organizational behavior modifica-
organizational perspective. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
tion. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman.
Murphy, K. R., &Saal, F. E. (Eds.). (1990). Psychology in organizations:
Manchester, W (1973-1974). The glory and the dream (2 vols.). Boston:
Integrating science and practice. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Little, Brown.
Manz, C. C., & Sims, H. P., Jr. (1981). Vicarious learning: The influence Murray, H. A., & MacKinnon, D. W (1946). Assessment of OSS per-
of modeling on organizational behavior. Academy of Management sonnel. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 10, 76-80.
Review, 6, 105-113. Myart v. Motorola. 110 Congressional Record, 5662-5664 (1964).
March, J. G., & Simon, H. A. (1958). Organizations. New York: Wiley. Myers, C. S. (1936). Charles Samuels Myers. In C. Murchison (Ed.), A
Marrow, A. J. (1969). The practical theorist: The life and work of Kurt history of psychology in autobiography (Vol. 3, pp. 215-230). Worces-
Lewin. New \brk: Basic Books. ter, MA: Clark University Press.
Marrow, A. J., Bowers, D. G., & Seashore, S. E. (1967). Management by Napoli, D. S. (1981). Architects of adjustment: The history of the psycho-
participation. New \brk: Harper & Row. logical profession in the United States. Port Washington, NY: Kenni-
Maslow, A. H. (1954). Motivation and personality. New York: Harper. kat Press.
Mayo, E. (1933). The human problems of an industrial civilization. New National Behavioral Science Research Agenda Committee. (1992, Feb-
York: Macmillan. ruary). The human capital initiative [Special issue]. APS Observer.
McCall, M. W, Jr., & Bobko, P. (1990). Research methods in the service National Industrial Conference Board. (1940). Training solutions of com-
of discovery. In M. D. Dunnette & L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of pany problems. Studies in Personnel Policy, No. 18.
industrial and organizational psychology (2nd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 381- National Industrial Conference Board. (1951). Experience with em-
418). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists' Press. ployee attitude surveys. Studies in Personnel Policy, No. 115.
McClelland, D. C, & Winter, D. G. (1969). Motivating economic achieve- Naylor, J. C. (1971). Hickory, dickory, dock! Let's turn back the clock!
ment. New York: Free Press. Professional Psychology, 2, 217-234.
APA CENTENNIAL: HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF I-O PSYCHOLOGY 833

Naylor, J. C, & Ilgen, D. R. (1984). Goal-setting: A theoretical analysis Porter, L. W, Lawler, E. E., Ill, & Hackman, J. R. (1975). Behavior in
of a motivational technology. In B. Staw & L. L. Cummings (Eds.), organizations. New \brk: McGraw-Hill.
Research in organizational behavior (Vol. 6, pp. 95-140). Greenwich, Prien, E. P. (1991). The Division of Applied Psychology at Carnegie
CT: JAI Press. Institute of Technology. The Industrial-Organizational Psychologist,
Naylor, J. C., Pritchard, R., & Ilgen, D. R. (1980). A theory of behavior in 29(2), 41-45.
organizations. New York: Academic Press. Prien, E. P., & Macey, W H. (1984, August). Multi-domain job analysis
Newell, A. C. (1990). Unified theories of cognition. Cambridge, MA: of the industrial-organizational psychologist job. Paper presented at
Harvard University Press. the 92nd Annual Convention of the American Psychological Associ-
Offermann, L. R., & Gowing, M. K. (Eds.). (1990). Organizational ation, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.
psychology [Special issue]. American Psychologist, 45, 95-283. Primoff, E. S. (1975). How to prepare and conduct job element examina-
Office of Strategic Services. (1948). Assessment of men. New York: tions. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Rinehart. Primoff, E. S. (1991). History of psychological examining in the Civil
O'Reilly, C. A., III. (1991). Organizational behavior: Where we've Service. Unpublished manuscript, Office of Personnel Manage-
been, where we're going. Annual Review of Psychology, 42, 427-458. ment, Washington, DC.
Orne, M. T. (1962). On the social psychology of the psychology experi- Psychologists and unions. (1981). International Review of Applied Psy-
ment. American Psychologist, 17, 776-783. chology, 30, 137-328.
O'Rourke, L. J. (1926). Saving dollars and energy by personnel re- Purcell.T. V. (1953).The worker speaks his mind on company andunion.
search. Journal of Personnel Research, 4, 351-358. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
O'Rourke, L. J. (1934). Scientific personnel selection in the United Rabinowitz, S., & Hall, D. T. (1977). Organizational research on job
States Civil Service. Occupations, 12, 29-39. involvement. Psychological Bulletin, 84, 265-288.
O'Rourke, L. J. (1950). Federal civilian personnel administration. In Reich, C. A. (1970). The greening of America. New York: Random
D. H. Fryer & E. R. Henry (Eds.), Handbook of applied psychology House.
(Vol. 1, pp. 229-235). New York: Rinehart. Reynolds, C. R., & Brown, R. T. (1984). Perspectives on bias in mental
Otis, J. L. (1946). The personnel research institute of Western Reserve testing. New York: Plenum Press.
University. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 10, 131-135. Roback, A. A. (1952). A history of American psychology. New York:
O'Toole, J. D., Hansot, E., Herman, W, Herrick, N., Liebow, E., Lusig- Collier-Macmillan.
nan, B., Richman, H., Sheppard, H., Stephansky, B., & Wright, J. Roberson, L. (1989). Assessing personal work goals in the organiza-
(1973). Work in America. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. tional setting: Development and evaluation of the Work Concerns
Pastore, N. (1978). The Army intelligence tests and Walter Lippman. Inventory. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes,
Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 14, 316-327. 44, 345-367.
Paterson, D. G. (1923). The Scott Company graphic rating scale. Jour- Roberson, L. (1990). Prediction of job satisfaction from characteristics
nal of Personnel Research, 1, 361-376. of personal work goals. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 11, 29-
Paterson, D. G. (1932). The Minnesota unemployment research proj- 41.
ect. Personnel Journal, 10, 318-328. Robinson, E. S. (1919). The analysis of trade ability. Journal of Applied
Paterson, D. G., & Darley, J. G. (1936). Men, women, and jobs. Minneap- Psychology, 3, 352-357.
olis: University of Minnesota Press. Roethlisberger, F. J., & Dickson, W J. (1939). Management and the
Pearson, K. (1894). Contributions to the mathematical theory of evo- worker. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University, Graduate School of
lution. Dissection of asymmetrical frequency curves. Philosophical Business Administration.
Transactions of the Royal Society, A, 185, 71-110. Rosen, H. (1954). Dual allegiance: A critique and a proposed ap-
Pearson, K. (1895). Contributions to the mathematical theory of evo- proach. Personnel Psychology, 7, 67-71.
lution. II. Skew variations in homogeneous material. Philosophical Rosen, H., & Rosen, R. A. (1955). The union member speaks. New York:
Transactions of the Royal Society, A, 186, 343-414. Prentice-Hall.
Pearson, K. (1901). On lines and planes of closest fit to systems of Rosenthal, R. (1978). Combining results of independent studies. Psy-
points in space. Philosophical Magazine, 6, 559-572. chological Bulletin, 85,185-193.
Perloff, R., & Nelson, S. D. (1983). Economic productivity and the Rousseau, D. (1985). Issues of level in organizational research: Multi-
behavioral sciences. American Psychologist, 38, 451-453. level and cross-level perspectives. In L. L. Cummings & B. M. Staw
The personnel system of the United States Army (2 vols.). (1919). Wash- (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior (Vol. 7, pp. 1-38). Green-
ington, DC: War Department. wich, CT: JAI Press.
Pierson, F. C. (1959). The education of American businessmen. New Roznowski, M.(1987). Useoftests manifesting sex differences as mea-
York: McGraw-Hill. sures of intelligence: Implications for measurement bias. Journal of
Pinder, C. C. (1977). Concerning the application of human motivation Applied Psychology, 72, 480-483.
theories in organizational settings. Academy of Management Review, Rundquist, E. A., &Sletto, R. F. (1936). Personality and the depression.
2, 384-397. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
Pion, G. M., & Lipsey, M. W (1984). Psychology and society: The chal- Ryan, A. M., & Sackett, P. R. (1987). A survey of individual assessment
lenge of change. American Psychologist, 39, 739-754. practices by I/O psychologists. Personnel Psychology, 40, 455-488.
Pond, M. (1927). Selective placement of metalworkers. Journal of Per- Ryan, T. A. (1970). Intentional behavior. New York: Ronald Press.
sonnel Research, 5, 345-368, 405-417, 452-466. Sackett, P. R., Callahan, C., DeMeuse, K., Ford, J. K., & Kozlowski,
Porras, J. I., & Robertson, P. J. (in press). Organizational development: S. W J. (1986). Changes over time in research involvement by aca-
Theory, practice, and research. In M. D. Dunnette & L. M. Hough demic and nonacademic psychologists. The Industrial-Organiza-
(Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (Vol 3). tional Psychologist, 24(1), 40-43.
Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists' Press. Sackett, P. R., & Larson, J. R., Jr. (1990). Research strategies and tactics
Porter, L. W (1966). Personnel management. Annual Review of'Psychol- in industrial and organizational psychology. In M. D. Dunnette &
ogy, 77,395-422. L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psy-
834 RAYMOND A. KATZELL AND JAMES T. AUSTIN

chology. (2nd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 419-489). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Smith, M., & Robertson, I. (Eds.). (1989). Advances in selection and
Psychologists' Press. assessment. Chichester, England: Wiley.
Schein, E. (1984). Coming to a new awareness of organizational cul- Smith, P. C. (1988). Patricia Cain Smith. In A. N. O'Connell & N. F.
ture. Sloan Management Review, 25, 3-16. Russo (Eds.), Models of achievement: Reflections of eminent women
Schein, E. (1985). Organizational culture and leadership. San Francisco, in psychology (Vol 2, pp. 139-153). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
CA: Jossey-Bass. Smith, P. C., & Kendall, L. M. (1963). Retranslation of expectations: An
Schmidt, F. L., Berner, J. G., & Hunter, J. E. (1973). Racial differences approach to the construction of unambiguous anchors for rating
in validity of employment tests: Reality or illusion? Journal of Ap- scales. Journal of Applied Psychology, 47, 149-155.
plied Psychology, 58, 5-9. Smith, P. C, Kendall, L. M., & Hulin, C. L. (1969). The measurement of
Schmidt, F. L., & Hunter, J. E. (1977). Development of a general solu- satisfaction in work and retirement. Chicago: Rand McNally.
tion to the problem of validity generalization. Journal of Applied Snow, R. E., & Lohman, D. F. (1989). Implications of cognitive psychol-
Psychology, 62, 529-540. ogy for educational measurement. In R. L. Linn (Ed.), Educational
Schmidt, F. L., Ones, D. S., & Hunter, J. E. (1992). Personnel selection. measurement (3rd ed., pp. 263-331). New York: Macmillan.
Annual Review of Psychology, 43, 627-670. Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology. (1985). Guide-
Schmitt, N. (1990, April). What should we be doing? Presidential ad- lines for education and training at the doctoral level in industrial/or-
dress to the Fifth Annual Conference of the Society for Industrial ganizational psychology. College Park, MD: Author.
and Organizational Psychology, Miami Beach, FL. Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology. (1989). Graduate
Schmitt, N. (1991). Report on efforts to revise SIOP's licensure posi- training programs in industrial/organizational psychology and organi-
tion. The Industrial-Organizational Psychologist, 29(1), 33-50. zational behavior. College Park, MD: Author.
Schmitt, N., & Fine, S. (1982). Functional job analysis of the work of an Sokal, M. M. (1981). The origins of The Psychological Corporation.
industrial and organizational psychologist. Journal Supplemental Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 17, 54-67.
Abstract Service, 12, Ms. 2457. Sokal, M. M. (Ed.). (1987). Psychological testing and American society,
Schmitt, N., & Robertson, I. (1990). Personnel selection. Annual Re- 1890-1930. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
view of Psychology, 41, 289-319. Spearman, C. (1904). General intelligence objectively determined and
Schneider, B. J. (Ed.). (1990). Organizational climate and culture. San measured. American Journal of Psychology, 15, 201-293.
Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Spearman, C. (1914). The theory of two factors. Psychological Review,
Schneider, B. J. (1991, July-August). Funding for basic industrial and 21, 101-115.
organizational psychological research: The lack and the need. APA Spearman, C. (1927). The abilities of man. London: Macmillan.
Psychological Science Agenda, 4(4), 10-12. Spence, J. T. (1987). Centrifugal vs. centripetal tendencies in psychol-
Schneider, B. J., & Bartlett, C. J. (1968). Individual differences and ogy: Will the center hold? American Psychologist, 42, 1052-1054.
organizational climate I: The research plan and questionnaire devel- Stagner, R. (1956). Psychology of industrial conflict. New York: Wiley.
opment. Personnel Psychology, 21, 323-333. Stagner, R. (1981). Training and experiences of some distinguished
Schoorman, F. D, & Schneider, B. J. (Eds.). (1988). Facilitating work industrial psychologists. American Psychologist, 36, 497-505.
effectiveness. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books. Staw, B. M., & Ross, J. (1985). Stability in the midst of change: A dispo-
Scott, K. D., & Taylor, D. S. (1985). An examination of conflicting sitional approach to job attitudes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 70,
findings on the relationship of job satisfaction and absenteeism: A 469-480.
meta-analysis. Academy of Management Journal, 28, 599-612. Stead, W H., & Shartle, C. L. (Eds.). (1940). Occupational counseling
Scott, W D. (1903). The theory of advertising. Boston: Small, Maynard. techniques. New York: American Book Company.
Scott, W D. (1911). Increasing human efficiency in business. New York: Steffy, B. D., & Maurer, S. D. (1988). Conceptualizing and measuring
Macmillan. the effectiveness of human resource activities. Academy of Manage-
Scott, W D. (1915). The scientific selection of salesmen. Advertising & ment Review, 13, 271-296.
Selling, 5(5-7). Stone, E. F. (1986). Job scope-job satisfaction and job scope-job perfor-
Scott, W D. (1917). A fourth method of checking results in vocational mance relationships. In E. A. Locke (Ed.), Generalizing from labora-
selection. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1, 61-66. tory to field settings (pp. 189-206). Lexington, MA: Lexington
Scott, W D. (1920). Changes in some of our conceptions and practices Books.
of personnel. Psychological Review, 27, 81-94. Stouffer, S., Lumsdaine, A. A., Lumsdaine, M. H., Williams, R. M., Jr.,
Scott, W D., & Clothier, R. C. (1923). Personnel management. Chicago: Smith, M. B., Janis, I. L., Star, S. A., & Cottrell, L. A., Jr. (1949). The
Shaw. American soldier (2 vols.). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Seashore, S. E. (1985). Institutional and organizational issues in doing Strong, E. K., Jr. (1913). Psychological methods as applied to advertis-
useful research. In E. E. Lawler III, A. M. Mohrman, Jr., S. A. Mohr- ing. Journal of Educational Psychology, 4, 393-404.
man, G. E. Ledford, Jr., T. G. Cummings, & Associates (Eds.), Doing Strong, E. K., Jr. (1918). Work of the Committee on Classification of
research that is useful for theory and practice (pp. 45-60). San Fran- Personnel. Journal of Applied Psychology, 2,130-139.
cisco: Jossey-Bass. Strong, E. K., Jr. (1922). The psychology of selling insurance. New York:
Selected aspects of consumer behavior. (1977). Washington, DC: Na- Harper.
tional Science Foundation. Strong, E. K., Jr. (1943). Vocational interests of men and women. Stan-
Shostak, A. B. (1964). Industrial psychology and the trade unions: A ford, CA: Stanford University Press.
matter of mutual indifference. In G. Fisk (Ed.), The frontiers of man- Stuit, D. B. (1947). Personnel research and test development in the
agement psychology (pp. 144-155). New \brk: Harper & Row. Bureau of Naval Personnel. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University
Simon, H. A. (1947). Administrative behavior (1st ed.). New York: Mac- Press.
millan. Sullivan, M. (1927). Our times: The United States 1900-1925. London:
Simon, H. A. (1957). Administrative behavior (2nd ed.). New York: Scribner's Sons.
Macmillan. Symonds, J. P. (1946). Ten years of journalism in psychology: 1937-
Sisson, E. D. (1950). Military personnel management. In D. H. Fryer & 1946. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 10, 335-374.
E. R. Henry (Eds.), Handbook of applied psychology (Vo\. 1, pp. 236- Tannenbaum, S. I., & Yukl, G. (1992). Training and development in
246). New York: Rinehart. work organizations. Annual Review of Psychology, 43, 399-441.
Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: Macmil- Taylor, F. W (1947). Principles of scientific management. New York:
lan. Harper. (Original work published 1911)
APA CENTENNIAL: HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF I-O PSYCHOLOGY 835

Tenopyr, M. (1967, September). Race and socio-economic status as Vroom, V (1964a). Employee attitudes. In G. Fisk (Ed.), The frontiers of
moderators in predicting machine-shop training success. Paper pre- management psychology (pp. 127-143). New York: Harper & Row.
sented at the 75th Annual Convention of the American Psychologi- Vroom, V (1964b). Work and motivation. New York: Wiley.
cal Association, Washington, DC. Watson, J. B. (1919). Psychology from the standpoint of a behaviorist.
Terkel, S. (1972). Working. New York: Pantheon Books. Philadelphia: Lippincott.
Terman, L. (1921). The status of applied psychology in the United Wagner-Peyser Act of 1933, 29 U.S.C. §49 et seq.
States. Journal of Applied Psychology, 5,1-4. Watson, J. B. (1925). Behaviorism. New York: Norton.
Thayer, P. W (1992). Autobiographies of past presidents of SIOP. The Watson, R. I. (1966). The role and use of history in the psychology
Industrial-Organizational Psychologist, 29(4), 42-50. curriculum. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 2, 64-
Thomas, J. G, & Griffin, R. (1983). The social information processing 69.
model of task design: A review of the literature. Academy of Manage- Weber, M. (1946). The protestant sects and the spirit of capitalism. In
ment Review, 8, 672-682. H. H. Gerth & C. W Mills (Eds. & Trans.), From Max Weber: Essays
Thorndike, E. L. (1904). Mental and social measurements. New York: in sociology (pp. 302-322). New York: Oxford University Press. (Orig-
Columbia University, Teachers College. inal work published 1906)
Thorndike, E. L. (1911). Individuality. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin. Weber, M. (1947). The theory of social and economic organizations
Thorndike, E. L. (1917). The curve of work and the curve of satisfying- (A. M. Henderson & T. Parsons, Trans.). New York: Oxford Univer-
ness. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1, 265-267. sity Press. (Original work published 1922)
Thorndike, E. L. (1919). Scientific personnel work in the army. Weiss, A. P. (1927). The psychological laboratory of Ohio State Univer-
Science, 49, 53-61. sity. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 10, 434-445.
Thorndike, R. L. (1949). Personnel selection. New York: Wiley. Weiss, D. J., Dawis, R. V, England, G. W, & Lofquist, L. H. (1967).
Thurstone, L. L. (1927). A law of comparative judgment. Psychological Manual for the Minnesota Satisfaction Questionnaire. Minnesota
Review, 34, 273-286. Studies in Vocational Adjustment, XXII.
Thurstone, L. L. (1931). Multiple factor analysis. Psychological Review, Weiss, H. M. (1978). Social learning of work values in organizations.
38, 406-427. Journal of Applied Psychology, 63,111 -718.
Thurstone, L. L., & Chave, E. J. (1929). The measurement of attitudes. Wherry, R. S., Sr. (1968). A statistical history of the Department of Psy-
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. chology at the Ohio State University: 1907-1968. Unpublished manu-
Tomer, A. (1970). Future shock. New York: Random House. script, Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, Co-
Uhrbrock, R. S. (1934). Attitudes of 4430 employees. Journal of Social lumbus.
Psychology, 5, 365-377. Whipple, G. M. (1910). Manual of mental and physical tests. Baltimore,
U.S. Department of Labor. (1965). Dictionary of occupational titles (3rd MD: Warwick & York.
ed.). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Whitehead, T. N. (1935). Social relationships in the factory: A study of
Vandaveer, V (1991). A letter to my pro-exemptionist colleagues. The an industrial group. The Human Factor, 9, 381-394.
Industrial-Organizational Psychologist, 29(1), 51-58. Whitehead, T. N. (1938). The industrial worker. Cambridge, MA: Har-
Van Maanen, J., Dabbs, J. M., Jr., & Faulkner, R. R. (1982). Varieties of vard University Press.
qualitative research. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Wigdor, A. K., & Green, B. F., Jr. (Eds.). (1991). Performance assessment
Viteles, M. S. (1922). Job specifications and diagnostic tests of job for the workplace (2 vols.). Washington, DC: National Academy
competence designed for the auditing division of a street railway Press.
company. Psychological Clinic, 14, 83-105. Willett, J. B., & Singer, J. D. (1991). From whether to when: New meth-
Viteles, M. S. (1926a). Psychology in industry. Psychological Bulletin, ods for studying student dropout and teacher attrition. Review of
25,631-680. Educational Research, 61, 407-450.
Viteles, M. S. (1926b). Standards of accomplishment: Criteria of voca- Woodworth, R. S. (1918). Personal data sheet. Chicago: Stoelting.
tional success. Journal of Personnel Research, 4, 483-487. Yankelovich, D. (1981). New rules: Searching for self-fulfillment in a
Viteles, M. S. (1928). Psychology in industry. Psychological Bulletin, 25, world turned upside down. New York: Random House.
309-340. Yerkes, R. M. (1918). Psychology in relation to the war. Psychological
Viteles, M. S. (1930). Psychology in industry. Psychological Bulletin, 27, Review, 25,85-115.
567-635. Yerkes, R. M. (1919). Report of the Psychology Committee of the Na-
Viteles, M. S. (1932). Industrial psychology. New 'York: Norton. tional Research Council. Psychological Review, 26, 83-149.
Viteles, M. S. (Ed.). (1944). Industrial psychology [Special issue]. Jour- Yerkes, R. M. (Ed.). (1921). Psychological examining in the United
nal of Consulting Psychology, 8,1-185. States Army. Memoirs of the National Academy of Sciences (XV).
Viteles, M. S. (1946). Industrial implications of wartime developments Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
in psychology. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 10, 85-92. \erkes, R. M. (1941). Man-power and military efficiency: The case for
Viteles, M. S. (1953). Motivation and morale in industry. New York: human engineering. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 5, 205-209.
Norton. Yoakum, C. S., & Yerkes, R. M. (1920). Army mental tests. New York:
Viteles, M. S. (1967). Morris S. Viteles. In E. G. Boring & G. Lindzey Holt.
(Eds.), A history of psychology in autobiography (Vo\. 5, pp. 417-500). Yule, G. U. (1897). On the theory of correlation. Journal of the Royal
New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Statistical Society, 60, 812-854.
Viteles, M. S. (1974). Industrial psychology: Reminiscences of an aca- Zedeck, S., & Mosier, K. L. (1990). Work in the family and employing
demic moonlighter. In T. Krawiec (Ed.), The psychologists (Vol. 2, organization. American Psychologist, 45, 240-251.
pp. 441-500). New York: Oxford University Press. Zeidner, J. (Ed.). (1987). Human productivity enhancement (2 vols.).
Von Mayrhauser, R. (1987). The manager, the medic, and the media- New York: Praeger.
tor: The clash of professional psychological styles and the wartime
origins of group mental testing. In M. M. Sokal (Ed.), Psychological Received July 8,1991
testing and American society, 1890-1930(pp. 128-157). New Bruns- Revision received May 1,1992
wick, NJ: Rutgers University Press. Accepted May 5,1992 •

You might also like