You are on page 1of 5

Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport 19 (2016) 1010–1014

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jsams

Original research

Goal motives and multiple-goal striving in sport and academia: A


person-centered investigation of goal motives and inter-goal relations
Laura C. Healy a,∗ , Nikos Ntoumanis b , Joan L. Duda c
a
Physical Education and Sport Studies Department, Newman University, UK
b
Health Psychology & Behavioural Medicine Research Group, School of Psychology & Speech Pathology, Curtin University, Australia
c
School of Sport, Exercise and Rehabilitation Sciences, University of Birmingham, UK

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Objectives: This investigation extended the goal striving literature by examining motives for two goals
Received 29 May 2015 being pursued simultaneously. Grounded in self-determination theory, we examined how student-
Received in revised form 8 January 2016 athletes’ motives for their sporting and academic goals were associated with inter-goal facilitation and
Accepted 10 March 2016
interference.
Available online 17 March 2016
Design: Cross-sectional survey.
Methods: UK university student-athletes (n = 204) identified their most important sporting and academic
Keywords:
goals. They then rated their extrinsic, introjected, identified and intrinsic motives for these goals and
Goals
Motivation
completed questionnaires assessing inter-goal facilitation and interference.
Inter-goal facilitation Results: Using a person-centered approach via latent profile analysis, we identified three distinct profiles
Inter-goal interference of goal motives. Auxiliary analyses showed that the profile with high identified motives for both goals
Latent profile analysis reported greater inter-goal facilitation.
Self-determination theory Conclusions: Extending the previous literature, the findings demonstrate the benefits of autonomous
motives when simultaneously pursing goals in sport and academia.
© 2016 Published by Elsevier Ltd on behalf of Sports Medicine Australia.

1. Introduction et al.9 found that high goal self-concordance can protect individ-
uals from negative affect when experiencing goal conflict. Further,
Grounded in self-determination theory (SDT),1 a major princi- for self-concordant goals, conflict was viewed as challenging rather
ple of the self-concordance (SC) model2 is that goal motivation can than frustrating.
vary in both quality and quantity. Autonomous motivation, reflect- Gorges et al.’s9 findings show the importance of identifying
ing intrinsic and identified motivation regulations, is underpinned the motives underpinning concurrent goal pursuits. However, goal
by personal interest, importance or enjoyment in goal pursuit. Con- conflict was investigated in one domain only. Furthermore, the
trolled motivation is underpinned by internal or external pressures, relations between goals were not examined. When pursuing mul-
aligned with introjected and extrinsic motivation. SC model-based tiple goals, individuals may experience inter-goal facilitation or
research has generally examined one goal in a single domain, such interference.10 Inter-goal facilitation – the pursuit of one goal
as education,3 health,4 and sport.5–7 In reality, individuals often increasing the chance of success in the other goal – occurs through
simultaneously pursue multiple goals across contexts.8 Only one instrumental relations (progress in one goal resulting in progress
study has explored motivation in multiple-goal pursuit. Gorges toward the other goal) and overlapping goal strategies (actions
et al.9 linked goal self-concordance (autonomous minus controlled having positive effects on both goals). Inter-goal interference,
motives) to the affective responses associated with multiple goal whereby pursing one goal reduces the likelihood of attaining
conflict. To generate feelings of goal conflict, junior scientists con- another, operates through resources constraints (striving for one
sidered an instance where they had recently “felt torn” between goal detracts time, effort or resources from another goal) or
two activities in their research and teaching. Participants identified incompatible goal strategies (strategies for one goal conflict with
a goal and rated their motives for each of these activities. Gorges completing another goal). Facilitation is linked with higher lev-
els of goal pursuit, whereas interference is negatively associated
with well-being.10 The present study extends the literature by
∗ Corresponding author. examining the association between goal motivation and inter-goal
E-mail address: laura.healy@newman.ac.uk (L.C. Healy). relations.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jsams.2016.03.001
1440-2440/© 2016 Published by Elsevier Ltd on behalf of Sports Medicine Australia.
L.C. Healy et al. / Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport 19 (2016) 1010–1014 1011

A central tenet of SDT1 is that autonomous motivation is more 2. Methods


adaptive because it reflects greater integration with the self. As
such, autonomous motivation can lead to a range of positive Following ethical approval from two UK universities, we
outcomes, and buffer negative outcomes. Conversely, controlled recruited 204 university students (103 male, 101 female, M
motivation is predicted to lead to negative outcomes, with no age = 21.00 years, SD age = 2.09) who had been participating in their
buffering effect. Goal motives research has generally supported sport for 7.69 ± 5.29 years. A questionnaire pack was completed
these propositions.7,11,12 Healy et al.12 found autonomous goal either online or on paper. Data collection occurred around 4–6
motives to be positively and negatively related to well- and ill- weeks into an academic semester, as we felt that students would
being, respectively. Furthermore, autonomous motives have been have commenced goal striving for both goals by this point.
shown to lead to enhanced persistence toward an increasing Participants identified their most important sporting and aca-
difficult goal5 and greater flexibility when goals have become demic goal for the remaining academic year, and rated their
unattainable.13 Therefore, it may be that when goals are pur- motivation for each goal. Four items (one for each goal motivation
sued for reasons of personal importance or enjoyment, individuals regulation) that have been used extensively in previous goal striv-
can be flexible in their allocation of resources. In a multiple goal ing research,2,7,12 tapped extrinsic (“Because someone else wants
context, autonomous motives may allow for greater facilitation you to”), introjected (“Because you would feel ashamed, guilty, or
between goals. In the present study we expected that autonomous anxious if you didn’t”), identified (“Because you personally believe
motives would be positively related to inter-goal facilitation and it’s an important goal to have”) and intrinsic (“Because of the fun
negatively associated with interference. Controlled motivation has and enjoyment the goal provides you”) goal motives on a 1 (not at
generally been found to be unrelated to goal attainment.5,14 In a all) to 7 (very much so) scale.
multiple goal context, this might be due to greater interference The Inter-goal Relations Questionnaire10 was completed to
between goals. Hence, we hypothesized that controlled motives assess facilitation and interference. The facilitation scale had one
would be positively associated with inter-goal interference, and item each for instrumental goal relations (“The pursuit of my sport-
unrelated to facilitation. We explored these hypotheses in univer- ing goal sets the stage for the realization of my academia goal”)
sity student-athletes striving for both sporting and academic goals, and overlapping goal attainment strategies (“How often has it hap-
as while some student-athletes struggle to balance their sporting pened that you did something in the pursuit of your sporting goal
and academic goals, others are more successful at managing multi- that was simultaneously beneficial for your academic goal?”). For
ple goal pursuits.15 Motivation can vary across different situations the interference scale, three items assessed resource constraints
and contexts,16 with individuals feeling more autonomous in one (e.g., “How often has it happened that because of the pursuit of
context and less so in another. Thus, variations in goal motivation your sporting goal, you could not invest as much energy into
across contexts might be associated with differences in student- your sporting goal as you would have liked to?”), and a fourth
athletes’ inter-goal relations. measured incompatible goal attainment strategies (“How often
In the original SC model, Sheldon and Elliott2 combined has it happened that you did something in the pursuit of your
autonomous and controlled motives to assess self-concordance. academic goal that was incompatible with your sporting goal?”).
Research has also examined autonomous and controlled motives Participants rated the impact of the sporting goal on their aca-
separately to explore their unique contribution to goal-related demic goal, and vice versa, in reference to the last month on a 1
outcomes.7,11,12 However, combinations of goal motives have not (Never or rarely) to 5 (Very often) scale. For each goal, mean facil-
been examined in the literature. In the wider SDT literature,17 itation and interference scores were created from the respective
examining general motivation rather than specific goal motivation, items.
it has been shown that people can experience varied combinations To create goal motives profiles, latent profile analysis (LPA) was
of motivation regulations. In this study we used a person-centered performed using MPlus 7.122 with MLR estimation. We included
approach, whereby we created goal motives profiles for both aca- in the analysis the four motivation regulations for each goal; eight
demic and sporting goals. variables were used in total. This approach is different to previous
Within the SDT literature, person-centered research has SC model research,7,11,12 where the extrinsic and introjected, and
demonstrated that more optimal motivation profiles (i.e. high identified and intrinsic scores have been aggregated to form con-
autonomous, low controlled motivation) are associated with bet- trolled and autonomous goal motives respectively. Our approach
ter outcomes (e.g. performance, effort) than those with less optimal was based on two reasons. First, the four items represent sepa-
profiles (i.e. low autonomous, high controlled motivation or mod- rate (albeit related) motivation regulations. Additionally, research
erate autonomous, moderate controlled motivation).17,18 However, has often found these goal motives aggregates have poor internal
other research has suggested that high levels of controlled motiva- reliability.7,12
tion may not be detrimental, as long as autonomous motivation While there is no “gold standard” for determining the opti-
regulations are also high.19–21 Within our research, it was also mum number of profiles in LPA, it is worthwhile to explore a
plausible that individuals would report different combinations of range of solutions and select the number of profiles based on the
goal motives across their academic and sporting goals. For exam- goodness-of-fit indices, the nature of the profiles, and theoretical
ple, student-athletes might enjoy their sporting goal, and therefore considerations.23,24 It is also possible to test if a more complex
report higher levels of autonomous and lower levels of controlled model offers a better fit to the data than a more parsimonious
motives in pursuit of this goal, whereas they might be pursuing one. We examined the model fit criteria from 1 to 5 profile solu-
their academic goal with different levels of autonomous and con- tions. We primarily used the bootstrapped log-likelihood ratio test
trolled motives. We expected that profiles in which intrinsic and (BLRT) as this is recommended for sample sizes of n < 200.25 We
identified goal regulations (i.e., autonomous motives) for both goals also inspected the entropy criterion values; higher values indicate
were high, would experience greater inter-goal facilitation and a better model fit.26 Furthermore, the goal motives means for each
lower interference, regardless of the level of extrinsic and intro- profile were examined in terms of relevance to theory. To examine
jected (i.e., controlled) motivation. Additionally, we hypothesized between profile differences in inter-goal interference and facilita-
that profiles with lower levels of autonomous goal motives, or with tion, we utilized the AUXILIARY command in MPlus. This allows
mixed motives for sporting and academic goals, would experience for the equality of outcome means hypothesis to be tested across
less inter-goal facilitation and more interference. profiles via a Wald chi-square test.27
1012 L.C. Healy et al. / Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport 19 (2016) 1010–1014

Table 1
Descriptive statistics, scale reliabilities and bivariate correlations of study variables.

˛ M (SEM) 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13

1. Age (years) – 21.02 (.15) –


2. Years of experience in main sport – 7.69 (.38) .20** –
3. Sport extrinsic goal motives – 2.30 (.11) −.08 −.09 –
4. Sport introjected goal motives – 2.70 (.13) −.05 −.09 .45** –
5. Sport identified goal motives – 5.93 (.09) .01 .02 −.05 .14* –
6. Sport intrinsic goal motives – 6.24 (.07) −.05 .09 −.21** −.22** .31** –
7. Academic extrinsic goal motives – 3.13 (.13) −.03 −.002 .38** .16* .16* −.03 –
8. Academic introjected goal motives – 4.43 (.13) −.01 −.06 .17* .35** −.01 −.12 .24** –
9. Academic identified goal motives – 6.53 (.05) .03 −.06 .06 .17** .29** .04 .10 .26* –
10. Academic intrinsic goal motives – 4.53 (.12) −.02 −.003 −.16* −.02 −.04 .27** −.17* −.15* .24* –
11. Academic to sport goal interference .76 2.80 (.07) .05 .05 .04 .12 −.08 −.03 −.07 .02 −.15* −.07 –
12. Academic to sport goal facilitation .65 2.52 (.07) −.04 −.01 −.16* −.07 .26** .24** −.03 .11 .12 .06 .05 –
13. Sport to academic goal interference .73 2.70 (.06) −.06 −.11 .09 .10 .02 −.13 .11 .10 .08 −.02 .19* −.03 –
14. Sport to academic goal facilitation .63 2.62 (.07) −.16* .01 .08 .11 .20** .13 .06 .14 .05 .15* .11 .58** .24**

Note: The goal motives variables were all assessed on a 1–7 scale. The inter-goal interference and facilitation were measured on a 1–5 scale. M, mean, SEM, standard error of
the mean.
*
p < 0.05.
**
p < 0.01.

Table 2
Fit indices, entropy, and model comparisons for estimated latent class analysis models.

Model BLRT BIC SSA-BIC Entropy LMR test

One class – 5559.26 5508.57 1.00 –


Two classes −2737.44* 5471.69 5392.50 .92 132.23*
Three classes −2669.94* 5220.67 5112.96 1.00 292.32
Four classes −2520.69* 5208.17 5071.96 .88 58.94
Five classes −2490.72* 5205.51 5040.78 .90 49.35

Note: BLRT, bootstrapped log-likelihood ratio test; BIC, Bayesian information criterion; SSA-BIC, sample size adjusted Bayesian information criterion; LMR, Lo-Mendell–Rubin
likelihood ratio test.
*
p < 0.01.

3. Results of experience in their sport (Table 1). There was a small, neg-
ative correlation between age and facilitation from the sporting
The data were screened for multivariate outliers using Maha- goal to the academic goal; however, no other variables were
lanobis distance. Consequently, we removed 9 participants, leaving related to age or years of experience. A MANOVA revealed no
a final sample of 195 participants. The internal reliabilities for both multivariate (Pillai’s V = .06, F (12, 182) = .99, p = 0.46, 2 = .06) or
facilitation variables were slightly lower than those for the interfer- univariate between gender differences in sport goal motives (all
ence variables (Table 1). This may be explained by the facilitation F (1, 195) < 2.23, p > 0.05, 2 < .02), academic goal motives (all F
subscale containing two items only whereas the interference sub- (1, 195) < 1.82, p > 0.05, 2 < .01) or inter-goal relations (all F (1,
scale contained four items.28 195) < 2.60, p > 0.05, 2 < .02).
We conducted preliminary analyses to ensure the LPA would Table 2 displays the fit indices for the LPA. Using the BLRT,
not be impacted by confounding variables. None of the goal motive entropy values and theoretical considerations, we accepted the 3-
regulations were correlated with the participants’ age or years profile solution (Fig. 1A). In all three profiles, participants reported

Fig. 1. Mean goal motivation regulations (A) and mean (±SEM) inter-goal interference and facilitation (B) across the different profiles. a – significantly different means to
other profiles p < 0.05.
L.C. Healy et al. / Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport 19 (2016) 1010–1014 1013

relatively adaptive motives for their sporting goal (i.e. lower extrin- with goal progress,10 it could be expected that when individuals
sic and introjected, and higher identified and intrinsic motives). The find personal importance in their goal pursuits, they experience
academic goal motives across the profiles were more diverse. In benefits such as inter-goal facilitation, enabling them to success-
Profile 1 (10.3% of the sample), individuals reported low extrinsic, fully achieve multiple goals.
moderate introjected and high identified and intrinsic motives for An interesting aspect of our findings is that the “Dual-Identified
the sport goal. For the academic goal, they reported moderate levels Motive Strivers” reported moderate levels of controlled (e.g. extrin-
of all goal motive regulations. Therefore, this profile was labeled as sic and introjected) motives for their academic goal. Indeed,
“Mixed-Motive Strivers”. In Profile 2 (25.1%) individuals reported individuals within this group reported the highest level of intro-
low extrinsic and introjected, moderate identified and high intrin- jected motives for the academic goal of the three profiles. Despite
sic motives for their sporting goal. For their academic goal, they feeling internal pressures to pursue their academic goal, student-
reported high identified, and moderate extrinsic, introjected and athletes within this profile reported the highest levels facilitation.
intrinsic motives. This profile was labeled “Intrinsic-Identified These findings are aligned with the SDT literature. Studies in phys-
Motive Strivers”. The final profile was the largest (64.6%). Indi- ical education have shown that students with higher autonomous
viduals within this group pursued their sporting goal with low motivation reported more adaptive experiences, regardless of their
extrinsic and introjected, and high identified and intrinsic motives. controlled motivation levels.17,18 In a sport setting, no differences
They also reported high identified motives for their academic goal, were found in objectively-assessed performance between two pro-
along with moderate levels of extrinsic, introjected and intrinsic files with high autonomous motivation, which had varying levels of
motives. Given their high level of identified motivation for both controlled motivation.20 Recent research in sport which explored
goals, we labeled this class as “Dual-Identified Motive Strivers”. motivation profiles in relation to well-being has suggested that
The results of the AUXILIARY analyses (Fig. 1B) showed that high controlled motivation can lead to adaptive outcomes when
all profiles reported similar levels of academic to sporting (Global coupled with high autonomous motivation.21 In multiple goal
Wald 2 = 1.68, p = 0.43) and sporting to academic (Global Wald pursuit, it seems that introjected motives are not detrimental to
2 = 2.60, p = 0.21) goal interference. Different levels of facilitation facilitation, as long as both goals are perceived to be personally
were reported between the profiles. From the academic to the important.
sporting goal, the “Mixed-Motive Strivers” reported lower levels Contrary to our expectations, our findings suggest that differ-
of facilitation than both the “Intrinsic-Identified Motives Strivers” ences in goal motivation profiles are not associated with differential
(Wald 2 = 11.0, p = 0.001, Cohen’s d = .75) and the “Dual-Identified levels of inter-goal interference. Individuals in all profiles reported
Motive Strivers” (Wald 2 = 23.68, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = .98), with moderate levels of interference between their academic and sport-
no difference between the latter two profiles (Wald 2 = .70, ing goals. This suggests that, in relation to the pursuit of multiple
p = 0.41, Cohen’s d = .15; Global Wald 2 = 23.74, p < 0.001). For facil- goals across domains, more adaptive forms of motivation can-
itation from the sporting to the academic goal, the “Dual-Identified not protect individuals from interference, contrary to the tenets
Motive Strivers” reported higher facilitation than both the “Mixed- of SDT.1 It may be that in goal pursuit across multiple domains,
Motive Strivers” (Wald 2 = 4.38, p = 0.04, Cohen’s d = .50) and high autonomous motivation does not have the same buffering
the “Intrinsic-Identified Motive Strivers” (Wald 2 = 4.81, p = 0.03, effect as found in previous literature looking at motivation for
Cohen’s d = .37). There were no differences between the latter pursuits within the same domain.12 This unexpected finding warr-
two profiles (Wald 2 = .29, p = 0.59, Cohen’s d = .14; Global Wald ants investigation to fully understand the association between goal
2 = 7.67, p = 0.02). To summarize, we found partial support for our motivation and inter-goal relations in multiple domains.
hypotheses, as the profiles with higher levels of autonomous goal This study makes a novel contribution to the literature by exam-
motives for their goals experienced greater facilitation, but there ining goal motives in multiple-goal situations. However, as the
were no differences in interference. analyses used cross-sectional data, we were unable to determine
if goal motives can prospectively predict multiple goal attainment.
4. Discussion Given that facilitation is positively associated with goal progress,10
we might infer from our findings that, over time, those with an
This was the first study to explore combinations of motivation adaptive goal motive profile would have higher levels of attain-
regulations for sporting and academic goals. Our results sug- ment for both goals. It is important that research examines the
gest that facilitation between goals occurs when identified goal associations between goal motives, inter-goal relations, and goal
motives are high. Within the “Dual-Identified Motive Strivers” and attainment via a longitudinal design. A further limitation is the
“Intrinsic-Identified Motive Strivers” profiles, individuals reported use of single-item measures for each goal motivation regulation.
high identified motives for their academic goal. Furthermore, While this approach is consistent with the literature,2,7,12 it would
they experienced greater facilitation from their academic goal be worthwhile to develop multiple items for each goal motive and
to their sporting goal than the “Mixed-Motives Strivers”, who incorporate these into future research.
reported moderate identified goal motives. Similarly, only the Research could also examine how an individual’s goal motives
“Dual-Identified Motive Strivers” reported high identified sport can explain differences in inter-goal relations when pursuing mul-
goal motives. Individuals within this group experienced greater tiple goals in a single domain. In sport, goal setting may be more
facilitation from their sporting goal to their academic goal than effective when athletes set goals to work toward across different
the “Mixed-Motive Strivers” and the “Intrinsic-Identified Motive sport-related contexts (e.g. training and competition) and over dif-
Strivers”. ferent time scales (e.g. short-, medium- and long-term).30 Pursuing
Research from the SDT literature has shown that identified and these questions would extend the SC model research conducted to
intrinsic motivation regulations can lead to different outcomes.29 It date.
has also been suggested that identified motivation might be more
beneficial than intrinsic motivation when tasks are not perceived
to be inherently interesting.16 It is plausible that for our partici- 5. Conclusion
pants, their sporting goal was more inherently enjoyable than their
academic goal. As such, understanding the importance of achieving Our findings extend the SC model literature by showing that
both goals may have resulted in facilitation between both the sport- adaptive goal motivation is also important in multiple-goal pur-
ing and academic goal. Given that facilitation is positively linked suit, particularly in relation to facilitation of academic and sporting
1014 L.C. Healy et al. / Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport 19 (2016) 1010–1014

goals. To find balance in pursuits across different settings, it is 10. Riediger M, Freund AM. Interference and facilitation among personal goals: dif-
important for individuals to find personal importance in their goals ferential associations with subjective well-being and persistent goal pursuit.
Pers Soc Psychol B 2004; 30(12):1511–1523.
within each domain. 11. Smith A, Ntoumanis N, Duda JL et al. Goal striving, coping, and well-being. A
prospective investigation of the self-concordance model in sport. J Sport Exerc
Practical implications Psychol 2011; 33(1):124–145.
12. Healy LC, Ntoumanis N, Veldhuijzen van Zanten JJCS et al. Goal striving and
well-being in sport: the role of contextual and personal motivation. J Sport Exerc
• Student-athletes strive for their sporting and academic goals for Psychol 2014; 36(5):446–459.
different reasons. 13. Ntoumanis N, Healy LC, Sedikides C. Self-regulatory responses to unattainable
goals: the role of goal motives. Self Identity 2014; 13(5):594–612.
• To experience optimum relations between sporting and academic
14. Koestner R, Otis N, Powers TA et al. Autonomous motivation, controlled moti-
goals, student-athletes should try to find personal importance in vation, and goal progress. J Pers 2008; 76(5):1201–1229.
both goals. 15. Cosh S, Tully PJ. All I have to do is pass. A discursive analysis of student athletes’
• Striving for goals as a result of pressure or for the avoidance of talk about prioritising sport to the detriment of education to overcome stressors
encountered in combining elite sport and tertiary education. Psychol Sport Exerc
unpleasant emotions may not necessarily be detrimental for goal 2014; 15(2):180–189.
relations, as long as the goals are also important to the individual. 16. Vallerand RJ. A hierarchical model of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation in sport
and exercise, Chapter 8, in Advances in Motivation in Sport and Exercise, Treasure
GC, editor, Champaign, IL, Human Kinetics Publishers, 2001, p. 263–319.
Acknowledgements 17. Vansteenkiste M, Sierens E, Soenens B et al. Motivational profiles from a self-
determination perspective: the quality of motivation matters. J Educ Psychol
2009; 101(3):671–688.
The research in this manuscript was supported by a PhD stu-
18. Ullrich-French S, Cox A. Using cluster analysis to examine the combinations
dentship for the first author from the Economic and Social Research of motivation regulations of physical education students. J Sport Exerc Psychol
Council (Award No: ES/J50001X/1) while all authors were at the 2009; 31(3):358–379.
University of Birmingham. 19. Van den Berghe L, Soenens B, Aelterman N et al. Within-person profiles of
teachers’ motivation to teach: associations with need satisfaction at work,
need-supportive teaching, and burnout. Psychol Sport Exerc 2014; 15(4):
Appendix A. Supplementary data 407–417.
20. Gillet N, Vallerand RJ, Paty B. Situational motivational profiles and performance
with elite performers. J Appl Soc Psychol 2013; 43(6):1200–1210.
Supplementary data associated with this article can be found, in 21. Langan E, Hodge K, McGown S et al. The influence of controlled motivation along-
the online version, at doi:10.1016/j.jsams.2016.03.001. side autonomous motivation: maladaptive, buffering, or additive effects? Int J
Sport Exerc Psychol 2015. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1612197X.2015.1016084.
22. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus User’s Guide, Los Angeles, CA, Muthén & Muthén,
References 1998–2011.
23. Gerber M, Jonsdottir IH, Lindwall M et al. Physical activity in employees with
1. Deci EL, Ryan RM. The “What” and “Why” of goal pursuits: human needs and differing occupational stress and mental health profiles: a latent profile analysis.
the self-determination of behaviour. Psychol Inq 2000; 11(4):227–268. Psychol Sport Exerc 2014; 15(6):649–658.
2. Sheldon KM, Elliot AJ. Goal striving, need satisfaction, and longitudinal well- 24. Marsh HW, Lüdtke O, Trautwein U et al. Classical latent profile analysis of
being: the self-concordance model. J Pers Soc Psychol 1999; 76(3):482–497. academic self-concept dimensions: synergy of person- and variable-centered
3. Vasalampi K, Nurmi JE, Jokisaari M et al. The role of goal-related autonomous approaches to theoretical models of self-concept. Struct Equ Model 2009;
motivation, effort and progress in the transition to university. Eur J Psychol Educ 16(2):191–225.
2012; 27(4):591–604. 25. Nylund KL, Asparouhov T, Muthén BO. Deciding on the number of classes in
4. Miquelon P, Vallerand RJ. Goal motives, well-being, and physical health: happi- latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: a Monte Carlo simulation
ness and self-realization as psychological resources under challenge. Motiv Emot study. Struct Equ Model 2007; 14(4):535–569.
2006; 30(4):259–272. 26. Aldrige AA, Roesch SC. Developing coping typologies of minority adolescents: a
5. Ntoumanis N, Healy LC, Sedikides C et al. When the goal gets tough: the “why” latent profile analysis. J Adolesc 2008; 31(4):499–517.
of goal striving matters. J Pers 2014; 82(3):225–236. 27. Morin AJS, Morizot J, Boudrias J-S et al. A multifoci person-centered perspective
6. Smith A, Ntoumanis N, Duda JL. Goal striving, goal attainment, and well-being: on workplace affective commitment: a latent profile/factor mixture analysis.
adapting and testing the self-concordance model in sport. J Sport Exerc Psychol Organ Res Methods 2011; 14(1):58–90.
2007; 29(6):763–782. 28. Cortina JM. What is a coefficient alpha? An examination of theory and applica-
7. Smith A, Ntoumanis N, Duda J. An investigation of coach behaviors, goal motives, tions. J Appl Psychol 1993; 78(1):98–104.
and implementation intentions as predictors of well-being in sport. J Appl Sport 29. Burton KD, Lydon JE, D’Alessandro DU et al. The differential effects of intrinsic
Psychol 2010; 22(1):17–35. and identified motivation on well-being and performance: prospective, experi-
8. Louro MJ, Pieters R, Zeelenberg M. Dynamics of multiple-goal pursuit. J Pers Soc mental and implicit approaches to self-determination theory. J Pers Soc Psychol
Psychol 2007; 93(2):174–193. 2006; 91(4):750–762.
9. Gorges J, Esdar W, Wild W. Linking self-concordance and affective responses to 30. Weinberg RS. Goal setting in sport and exercise: research and practical applica-
goal conflict. Motiv Emot 2014; 38(4):475–484. tions. Rev Educ Fis 2013; 24(2):171–179.

You might also like