You are on page 1of 13

G Model

BIOPSY-7253; No. of Pages 13 ARTICLE IN PRESS


Biological Psychology xxx (2016) xxx–xxx

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Biological Psychology
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/biopsycho

Top-down and bottom-up factors in threat-related perception and


attention in anxiety
Tamara J. Sussman, Jingwen Jin, Aprajita Mohanty ∗
Department of Psychology, Stony Brook University, United States

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Anxiety is characterized by the anticipation of aversive future events. The importance of prestimulus
Received 14 September 2015 anticipatory factors, such as goals and expectations, is well-established in both visual perception and
Received in revised form 10 August 2016 attention. Nevertheless, the prioritized perception of threatening stimuli in anxiety has been attributed
Accepted 17 August 2016
to the automatic processing of these stimuli and the role of prestimulus factors has been neglected. The
Available online xxx
present review will focus on the role of top-down processes that occur before stimulus onset in the
perceptual and attentional prioritization of threatening stimuli in anxiety. We will review both the cog-
Keywords:
nitive and neuroscience literature, showing how top-down factors, and interactions between top-down
Top-down
Endogenous and bottom-up factors may contribute to biased perception of threatening stimuli in normal function
Threat perception and anxiety. The shift in focus from stimulus-driven to endogenous factors and interactions between
Prestimulus processes top-down and bottom-up factors in the prioritization of threat-related stimuli represents an important
Attention conceptual advance. In addition, it may yield important clues into the development and maintenance of
Amygdala anxiety, as well as inform novel treatments for anxiety.
Anxiety © 2016 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
Sensory cortex
Prefrontal cortex
Perceptual bias
Attentional bias

1. Introduction stimuli shown rapidly in a stream of images are identified more


accurately than neutral stimuli (Anderson, 2005). While positive
Emotional stimuli require rapid adaptive responses, such as stimuli may also be associated with similar perceptual benefits, the
avoidance of threat or approach towards a rewarding stimulus. To effects tend to be smaller than those elicited by threatening stimuli
allow for these swift behavioural responses, our perceptual and (Carretie, Mercado, Tapia, & Hinojosa, 2001; Dijksterhuis & Aarts,
attentional system prioritizes emotional stimuli over stimuli that 2003; Smith, Cacioppo, Larsen, & Chartrand, 2003; Stefanics, Csukly,
are relatively unemotional in nature. Spiders, snakes and angry Komlosi, Czobor, & Czigler, 2012; Sussman, Weinberg, Szekely,
faces are hypothesized to belong to a special class of stimuli that Hajcak, & Mohanty, 2016).
are perceptually prioritized due to their importance for survival The facilitated perception of threatening stimuli has been
(Brosch, Pourtois, & Sander, 2010; New, Cosmides, & Tooby, 2007; attributed to bottom-up processing driven by the physical charac-
Seligman, 1971). Empirical research supporting this view shows teristics or evolutionary significance of these stimuli (Bannerman
that spiders and snakes are detected more rapidly than mush- et al., 2009; Ohman et al., 2001). In line with this view, research
rooms and flowers (Ohman, Flykt, & Esteves, 2001) and. angry faces in affective neuroscience has centered on examining the neural
are detected faster than neutral faces (Hansen & Hansen, 1988; pathways that promote ‘automatic’ perception of emotional stim-
Horstmann, 2007). Saccadic eye movements orient more quickly to uli (Fox, 2002; Mendez-Bertolo et al., 2016; Vuilleumier & Pourtois,
images of threatening compared to neutral faces and body postures 2007). It is hypothesized that threatening stimuli are prioritized
(Bannerman, Milders, de Gelder, & Sahraie, 2009). Threatening due to a processing bias (Bar-Haim, Lamy, Pergamin, Bakermans-
Kranenburg, & van, 2007; Cisler, Bacon, & Williams, 2009). This
processing bias is not measured directly, and instead is inferred
from accuracy and reaction time differences between the detec-
∗ Corresponding author at: Department of Psychology, Stony Brook University,
tion of threatening compared to neutral stimuli. Depending on the
Stony Brook, NY 11794, United States.
E-mail address: aprajita.mohanty@stonybrook.edu (A. Mohanty).
design of the task, the threat bias is hypothesized to facilitate detec-

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.08.006
0301-0511/© 2016 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

Please cite this article in press as: Sussman, T. J., et al. Top-down and bottom-up factors in threat-related perception and attention in
anxiety. Biol. Psychol. (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.08.006
G Model
BIOPSY-7253; No. of Pages 13 ARTICLE IN PRESS
2 T.J. Sussman et al. / Biological Psychology xxx (2016) xxx–xxx

tion of threatening stimuli in visual search and dot-probe-like tasks biasing process is a function of two mechanisms: a bottom-up, sen-
or impede performance when threatening stimuli distract from the sory driven mechanism that selects stimuli based on their physical
task at hand (Mathews & MacLeod, 1994; Ohman et al., 2001). Here, salience, and a top-down mechanism with variable selection cri-
we explore the possibility that in addition to processing biases that teria, which selects stimuli based on expectations, knowledge and
occur coincident with stimulus presentation, prioritized percep- goals. Unlike top-down mechanisms, bottom-up mechanisms are
tion of threatening stimuli in normal function and anxiety may be thought to operate by automatically shifting resources to salient
attributed to prestimulus biases. visual stimuli. For example, stimuli that create a local disconti-
The idea that prestimulus biases impact threat-perception is nuity in the visual environment, such as abrupt occurrence of a
consistent with research indicating that the process of perception new object (Jonides & Yantis, 1998), sudden motion and looming
starts prior to an encounter with a stimulus, and with research (Abrams & Christ, 2003; Franconeri & Simons, 2003), and luminance
demonstrating that perception is guided by top-down factors such contrast changes (Enns, Austen, Di Lollo, Rauschenberger, & Yantis,
as goals and expectations (Bacon & Egeth, 1994; Itti & Koch, 2001). 2001) are given more priority.
For example, both implicit and explicit prestimulus cues improve Similarly, emotional stimuli are considered another class of
target perception (Chen & Zelinsky, 2006; Wolfe, Butcher, Lee, stimuli that are hypothesized to be processed in a bottom-up man-
& Hyle, 2003). Similarly, in day-to-day life, we often use both ner. For example, in visual search arrays, snakes and spiders are
implicit and explicit emotional information to guide our percep- detected faster than flowers and mushrooms (Ohman et al., 2001);
tion, for example, while scanning for spiders in an uninhabited and angry faces are detected faster and more efficiently than neu-
room filled with cobwebs. These anticipatory search behaviors, tral and happy faces (Eastwood, Smilek, & Merikle, 2001; Tipples,
aimed at rapidly detecting sources of potential reward or threat, are Atkinson, & Young, 2002). Threatening faces are also processed ear-
deployed in a wide range of situations from driving on a highway to lier and receive more perceptual elaboration compared to other
navigating social gatherings. Prestimulus biases may be of partic- facial expressions (Schupp et al., 2004). Furthermore, saccadic reac-
ular importance in anxiety, as dispositional anxiety is associated tion times are faster towards an emotional compared to neutral
with overestimation of the likelihood and cost of future nega- faces and body postures (Bannerman et al., 2009), as well as towards
tive events (Aue & Okon-Singer, 2015; Grupe & Nitschke, 2013). emotional compared to neutral scenes (Nummenmaa, Hyona, &
The importance of top-down processes in anxiety has also been Calvo, 2009). Similarly, negative words are detected more accu-
demonstrated by studies showing that threat-related cues impact rately (Dijksterhuis & Aarts, 2003; Nasrallah, Carmel, & Lavie, 2009)
subsequent perception differently depending on type of anxiety and more quickly (Dijksterhuis & Aarts, 2003) than positive words.
(Sussman, Szekely et al., 2016). Attentional probes appearing in the same location as threatening
In the present review we first discuss the current affec- faces are detected faster than probes appearing in the opposite
tive neuroscience literature on exogenous, ‘bottom-up’ factors in location (Armony & Dolan, 2002; Mogg & Bradley, 1999; Pourtois,
understanding perceptual and attentional biases towards threaten- Grandjean, Sander, & Vuilleumier, 2004).
ing stimuli, both in normal function and in anxiety. While research It is hypothesized that emotional stimuli are prioritized due to
has examined the role of top-down factors that are non-emotional their salience, as proposed by appraisal, constructivist and, dimen-
in nature (for e.g., searching for matching Gabor patches) and their sional theories of emotion (Barrett, 2006; Brosch et al., 2010;
interaction with bottom-up processing of emotional stimuli (for Ellsworth & Scherer, 2003; Russell, 2003), or their physical char-
e.g., task-irrelevant emotional faces in the background), very few acteristics, as demonstrated by perceptual prioritization of shapes
studies have examined top-down factors that are themselves emo- associated with threats (Larson, Aronoff, Sarinopoulos, & Zhu, 2009;
tional in nature (e.g., cues indicating an upcoming threatening face) Larson, Aronoff, & Stearns, 2007). For example, in one study, par-
and their effect on perception. Hence, we discuss conceptual and ticipants were asked to detect and rate the valence of a discrepant
methodological issues in the research literature that arise from an threatening, happy or neutral schematic face in arrays of other-
exclusive focus on bottom-up factors in understanding prioritized wise identical faces (Lundqvist & Ohman, 2005). The schematic
perception of threatening stimuli. We then discuss the importance faces were manipulated such that three, two or one feature(s)
of endogenous, emotion-related ‘top-down’ factors, such as expec- of the schematic face conveyed emotion. Results showed better
tations and prior knowledge regarding threat, in guiding basic visual search performance for more negatively rated faces, even
human perception. We also discuss emerging evidence that under- if only one feature conveyed emotion, indicating that the threat-
scores the importance of endogenous processing in the perceptual ening meaning of the face drives improved detection (Lundqvist &
prioritization of threatening stimuli both in normal function and in Ohman, 2005). On the other hand, researchers have hypothesized
anxiety. Finally, we highlight the importance of shifting the empha- that the search advantage of threatening compared to neutral faces
sis from stimulus-driven to top-down mechanisms as well as their may be due to features such as upturned lip corners, open eyes, or
interaction with bottom-up mechanisms in the study of the percep- frowning that can be discriminated from neutral features (Calvo
tual prioritization of threatening stimuli both in normal function & Nummenmaa, 2008; Larson et al., 2007). This could be because
and in anxiety (Mohanty & Sussman, 2013). of the salience of the threat-related features, resulting from their
association with the holistic facial expression they come from (e.g.,
2. Bottom-up processes influencing the perception of Cave & Batty, 2006), or because of physical differences between
emotional stimuli features of threatening vs neutral faces regardless of emotional
meaning. Finally, some researchers have hypothesized that it is the
The human visual system is constantly bombarded with infor- configuration of threatening facial features, such as shape and posi-
mation. The limited capacity of this system makes it impossible tioning of the mouth relative to nose and eyes that aids visual search
to process all incoming information (Tsotsos, 1990). As a result, (Calder, Young, Keane, & Dean, 2000; Carey & Diamond, 1977),
stimuli entering the visual field compete for neural representa- others have concluded that specific features are responsible for
tion (Desimone & Duncan, 1995; Tsotsos, 1997). To deal with this improved detection (Calvo & Nummenmaa, 2008), and some stud-
overwhelming excess of information, the visual system biases the ies have presented results supporting both positions (Lundqvist,
competition between stimuli towards preferential representation Esteves, & Ohman, 2004).
of the most relevant stimuli (Desimone & Duncan, 1995). This

Please cite this article in press as: Sussman, T. J., et al. Top-down and bottom-up factors in threat-related perception and attention in
anxiety. Biol. Psychol. (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.08.006
G Model
BIOPSY-7253; No. of Pages 13 ARTICLE IN PRESS
T.J. Sussman et al. / Biological Psychology xxx (2016) xxx–xxx 3

3. Neural mechanisms involved in bottom-up faces compared to healthy controls and participants with hip-
threat-perception pocampal lesions (Vuilleumier et al., 2004) and also lead to less
activation in the inferior temporal cortex, a region crucial for visual
At the neural level, the bottom-up processing of emotional stim- recognition, in monkeys (Hadj-Bouziane et al., 2012). Furthermore,
uli is believed to be mediated via the amygdala and its interactions greater connectivity between the amygdala and sensory regions
with the visual cortices (Cisler & Koster, 2010; Dolan, 2002; Ohman, has been found during threat perception (Lim, Padmala, & Pessoa,
2002, 2005). For example, results from studies using backward 2009; Morris, Friston et al., 1998; Pessoa, Gutierrez, & Ungerleider,
masking paradigms suggest that fearful faces activate the amygdala 2002). These empirical results support claims of quick and auto-
in the absence of conscious awareness (Morris, Ohman, & Dolan, matic processing of salient stimuli, and provide an amygdala-based
1998; Whalen et al., 1998). Using an event-related fMRI paradigm mechanism by which threat stimuli could be perceptually priori-
in which subjects fixated on a central cue and matched either tized.
two fearful or neutral faces or two houses presented eccentrically, In addition to evidence of an amygdala-driven bottom-up pro-
Vuilleumier and colleagues examined the hypothesis that emo- cessing of salient stimuli, studies suggest that a ventral network,
tion and voluntary attention reflect distinct influences that do not comprising the temporoparietal junction (TPJ) and the ventral
interact (Vuilleumier, Armony, Driver, & Dolan, 2001; Vuilleumier, frontal cortex (VFC), helps to reorient attention to salient incom-
Richardson, Armony, Driver, & Dolan, 2004). They found that while ing sensory information outside the current focus of processing
activity in the fusiform gyrus (FG), which is known to respond (Corbetta & Shulman, 2002). This network is recruited by infre-
strongly to faces, was modulated by voluntary attention, amyg- quent or unexpected events that are salient; for e.g., invalidly cued
dala responses to fearful faces were not (Vuilleumier et al., 2001). targets in the Posner task or oddballs. Studies show that during top-
Bottom-up appraisal of emotional stimuli is also associated with down attentional guidance activity in TPJ is suppressed (Shulman,
greater connectivity between the amygdala and limbic regions, Astafiev, McAvoy, d’Avossa, & Corbetta, 2007; Shulman et al., 2003;
such as the anterior cingulate cortex (Comte et al., 2014). Todd, Fougnie, & Marois, 2005) and in the presence of salient non-
The amygdala is thought to quickly detect relevant stim- targets stimulus activity in TPJ increases (Geng & Mangun, 2011).
uli, including threatening stimuli (Cunningham & Brosch, 2012; Hence, the TPJ and VFC function like circuit breakers that shift
Sander, Grafman, & Zalla, 2003), via a neural pathway, sometimes the focus to salient but task-irrelevant stimuli, even in case of
referred to as the ‘low road,’ that passes through the superior emotional stimuli (Dolcos & McCarthy, 2006). These studies sug-
colliculi and the thalamus, that does not require cortical input gest additional bottom-up stimulus-driven mechanisms by which
(LeDoux, 2000), and carries low-spatial frequency information. threats could be perceptually prioritized, and demonstrate a possi-
This theory has been supported by evidence demonstrating that bility of interactions between top-down and bottom-up processes
low spatial frequency images of fearful faces produce more amyg- via a brain network that is sensitive to both kinds of processing.
dala activation than fearful images displayed with high spatial
frequency (Vuilleumier, Armony, Driver, & Dolan, 2003) and by
evidence that emotional faces can be processed in the absence of 4. The influence of anxiety on bottom-up threat-perception
awareness in a subject with a lesioned striate visual cortex (de
Gelder, Vroomen, Pourtois, & Weiskrantz, 1999). Recent evidence While anxiety is typically conceptualized as an anticipatory
using human intracranial electrophysiological data showed fast response to future threatening events, fear is typically thought of
amygdala responses, beginning as early as 74-ms after the onset as the response to an immediate threat (Davis, Walker, Miles, &
of fearful, but not neutral or happy, facial expressions (Mendez- Grillon, 2010; Grupe & Nitschke, 2013; LeDoux, 2015). Neverthe-
Bertolo et al., 2016). The latency of fear responses in amygdala was less, perceptual processing in anxiety is typically conceptualized
much shorter than their latency in the visual cortices and relied on as driven by bottom-up processes (Mathews & MacLeod, 1994;
low spatial frequency information. However, this theory has been Ohman et al., 2001). Empirical studies examining perception of
challenged by a competing model, positing that during the pro- threatening stimuli in anxious individuals, like the studies that
cessing of emotional visual stimuli, the amygdala’s primary role is examine these phenomena in healthy individuals, tend to utilize
to coordinate cortical function and detect salient stimuli (Pessoa & tasks that exogenously drive perception and attention through
Adolphs, 2010). This model (discussed in greater detail below) has the use of unanticipated or task-irrelevant stimuli. Common tasks
been supported by evidence demonstrating that emotional faces employed to study this phenomenon present emotional stimuli
only produce greater amygdala activity when faces are actively that ‘pop out’ amongst non-emotional stimuli (Fox et al., 2000;
attended to (Pessoa, Kastner, & Ungerleider, 2002), and by evi- Ohman et al., 2001), are peripheral to fixation (Mogg & Bradley,
dence that rapid fear detection can rely on high spatial frequency 1999), appear rapidly in stream of images (Arend & Botella, 2002),
information, suggesting involvement of cortical visual areas (Stein, or are irrelevant to the task at hand (Williams, Mathews, &
Seymour, Hebart, & Sterzer, 2014). MacLeod, 1996).
After the amygdala evaluates incoming information as threat- Experimental results suggest that compared to healthy controls,
ening, it can boost processing in other brain regions, such as individuals with clinical anxiety (Klumpp & Amir, 2009; Mogg,
the sensory cortices, via re-entrant feedback (Vuilleumier, 2005). Millar, & Bradley, 2000; Ohman et al., 2001), dispositional anxi-
Hence, exogenously-driven perceptual prioritization of threat- ety (Eysenck, Derakshan, Santos, & Calvo, 2007; Mogg & Bradley,
related information is hypothesized to involve visual processing 1999; Richards, French, Johnson, Naparstek, & Williams, 1992), and
modulated by re-entrant feedback signals from the amygdala experimentally induced anxiety (Lim & Pessoa, 2008; Robinson,
(Anderson & Phelps, 2001; Davidson, 2002; Ohman et al., 2001; Letkiewicz, Overstreet, Ernst, & Grillon, 2011) detect or orient
Ohman, 2005; Vuilleumier & Pourtois, 2007). Since the amygdala towards threatening stimuli more quickly. Other studies have
both receives inputs from all sensory modalities and projects to found no difference in the initial orientation to threat, but have
numerous cortical and subcortical areas, it is well positioned to demonstrated that individual differences in anxiety increase dwell
influence a number of processes and behaviors (Fox, Oler, Tromp, time and the time required to disengage from a threatening stim-
Fudge, & Kalin, 2015; Freese & Amaral, 2009; Holland & Gallagher, ulus (Fox, Russo, Bowles, & Dutton, 2001; Yiend & Mathews,
1999). Supporting the theory that this rich connectivity is used 2001). However, there is some empirical evidence demonstrat-
during threat perception is evidence that lesions in the human ing that anxiety can both facilitate engagement with threatening
amygdala lead to less activation of the FG when viewing fearful stimuli and slow disengagement with threatening stimuli (Koster,

Please cite this article in press as: Sussman, T. J., et al. Top-down and bottom-up factors in threat-related perception and attention in
anxiety. Biol. Psychol. (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.08.006
G Model
BIOPSY-7253; No. of Pages 13 ARTICLE IN PRESS
4 T.J. Sussman et al. / Biological Psychology xxx (2016) xxx–xxx

Verschuere, Crombez, & Van Damme, 2005; Shackman, Maxwell, Other studies have demonstrated that very early sensory pro-
McMenamin, Greischar, & Davidson, 2011), suggesting that anx- cessing is boosted in clinically anxious individuals (Knott et al.,
iety influences both earlier and later sensory processes involved 1994), and in children higher in dispositional anxiety (Woodward
in threat perception. Finally, recent studies of soldiers experi- et al., 2001). Threatening stimuli have been shown to activate the
encing acute stressors, such as combat, show that avoidance of amygdala more for anxious than non-anxious subjects, in clinical,
threat, rather than a bias towards threats predicts subsequent PTSD dispositional and experimentally induced anxiety (Bishop, Duncan,
symptoms (Wald, Shechner et al., 2011; Wald, et al., 2011). These & Lawrence, 2004; Calder, Ewbank, & Passamonti, 2011; Etkin &
studies suggest that situationally induced anxiety may influence Wager, 2007; Etkin et al., 2004; Larson, Ruffalo, Nietert, & Davidson,
the relationship between orientation to threat and future anxiety 2005). However, the amygdala does not work in isolation. A recent
symptoms. study demonstrated that under threat of shock, greater functional
The well-documented, bidirectional relationship between connectivity between the amygdala and the dorsolateral prefrontal
attentional biases to threat and anxiety (Van Bockstaele et al., cortex (DLPFC) predicted faster threat detection, and was positively
2014) has led to the development of cognitive-bias modification of associated with trait anxiety (Robinson et al., 2012). Other studies
selective attention (CBM-A), a procedure with demonstrated thera- have found that individuals with anxiety disorders had less func-
peutic promise, and Attention Bias Modification Treatment (ABMT), tional connectivity between the amygdala and the DLPFC compared
a treatment for anxiety. Both of these techniques are based on cog- to healthy controls while resting or viewing threatening faces (Birn
nitive behavioural models positing that cognitive biases can lead to et al., 2014; Prater, Hosanagar, Klumpp, Angstadt, & Phan, 2013).
anxiety disorders, and therefore altering attentional biases could Furthermore, in one study, the connectivity between DLPFC and
subsequently reduce anxiety symptoms. Reviews of both CBM-A amygdala correlated negatively with measures of social anxiety
and Attention Bias Modification (ABM) have shown these proce- (Prater et al., 2013). Together, these studies suggest that different
dures reduce anxiety symptoms and vulnerability to anxiety in kinds of anxiety influence the relationship between the amygdala
adults (MacLeod & Clarke, 2015; MacLeod & Mathews, 2012) and in and the frontal cortex in distinct ways.
children (Eldar et al., 2012). Furthermore, meta-analyses of stud- While the literature describing the impact of anxiety on per-
ies examining the impact of CBM and ABMT on anxiety symptoms ception consistently demonstrates a bias for threatening stimuli,
has demonstrated that these treatments are effective (Hakamata precisely when this bias comes into play remains unclear. Since one
et al., 2010; Hallion & Ruscio, 2011). A meta-analysis of the efficacy of the core features of anxiety is a tendency to make inaccurate pre-
of ABMT on clinical anxiety demonstrated that more patients who dictions regarding the likelihood and costs of future negative events
received this treatment no longer met diagnostic criteria compared (Grupe & Nitschke, 2013), the effect of anxiety on threat perception
to patients in control conditions (Linetzky, Pergamin-Hight, Pine, & may start before stimulus presentation and may involve top-down
Bar-Haim, 2015). factors such as goals, expectations and prior knowledge. Therefore,
Different types of anxiety (e.g. dispositional vs situationally to gain a complete understanding of how anxiety impacts threat
induced) can interact, impacting perceptual biases for threatening detection, it is necessary to examine the impact of top-down pro-
images. For example, anxiety induced by an upcoming examination cessing on threat perception in anxiety.
increased a pre-existing tendency for those higher in dispositional
anxiety to respond more rapidly to targets following a threaten-
ing stimulus (MacLeod & Mathews, 1988). Similarly, in individuals 5. Conceptual and methodological issues regarding
high in trait-anxiety, a negative mood manipulation led to greater research on bottom-up factors effecting perception
interference from anxiety-related words on an emotional Stroop
task (Richards et al., 1992). However, some studies have found a Overall, studies of psychological and neural mechanisms
different pattern of interaction. For example, while participants involved in perception of emotional stimuli have reinforced the
high in trait anxiety responded more slowly to threatening words view that emotional salience-related, bottom-up effects are invol-
on an emotional Stroop task − indicating that attention had been untary and not under the control of attention (Vuilleumier & Driver,
captured by these threatening distractors − under stress, trait anx- 2007). However, this research raises methodological issues.
iety no longer influenced response times (Mogg, Mathews, Bird, & First, the majority of studies examining the influence of emo-
Macgregor-Morris, 1990). A subsequent study suggests that while tional stimuli on attention have used tasks in which emotional
transient stressors may wash out differences between high and low stimuli appear unexpectedly, such as modifications of a peripheral
anxious individuals, chronic stressors exaggerate the differences in or exogenous cuing task (Armony & Dolan, 2002; Holmes, Green, &
attention attributed to trait anxiety (Mogg, Bradley, & Hallowell, Vuilleumier, 2005; Keil, Moratti, Sabatinelli, Bradley, & Lang, 2005;
1994). Therefore, paying attention to the type of stressor at hand Mogg & Bradley, 1999; Mogg, McNamara et al., 2000; Stormark
could resolve the differences between studies that find that induced & Hugdahl, 1996, 1997). The dot probe paradigm is a very com-
anxiety either increases or decreases threat biases. monly used task in which both a threatening and neutral stimulus
Overall, clinical, dispositional and situationally induced anxiety are presented simultaneously and peripherally, and one of them
are associated with faster detection of threatening stimuli. This per- is followed by an attentional probe (Holmes et al., 2005; Mogg &
ceptual prioritization of threat in anxiety is attributed to attentional Bradley, 1999; Mogg, McNamara et al., 2000). A facilitated response
capture by the stimulus or automatic processing of threaten- to probes that appear at the same location of threat information
ing stimuli (Ohman et al., 2001; Robinson, Charney, Overstreet, (valid trial), in comparison with responses to probes at the oppo-
Vytal, & Grillon, 2012; Robinson et al., 2011). Studies that exam- site location of threat information (invalid trial), is interpreted as
ined brain activity while unanticipated or task-irrelevant stimuli vigilance for threat.
exogenously drive perception and attention demonstrate increased Secondly, bottom-up processing of emotional stimuli has been
amygdala and visual cortical activity for feared stimuli in anxi- shown to interact with top-down factors such as goals and task-
ety (Lipka, Miltner, & Straube, 2011; Straube, Mentzel, & Miltner, relevance. While bottom-up processing of emotional stimuli is
2005). It is hypothesized that these perceptual enhancements adaptive when they are relevant to our well-being, it is equally
share similarities with exogenous stimulus-driven mechanisms adaptive to ignore these stimuli and stay task-focused when they
and are mediated via amydalar feedback into visual sensory regions pose no danger. Hence, adaptive behavior requires constant inter-
(Pourtois, Schettino, & Vuilleumier, 2013). action between top-down goals and bottom-up processing. Studies
using paradigms in which emotional stimuli are task-irrelevant

Please cite this article in press as: Sussman, T. J., et al. Top-down and bottom-up factors in threat-related perception and attention in
anxiety. Biol. Psychol. (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.08.006
G Model
BIOPSY-7253; No. of Pages 13 ARTICLE IN PRESS
T.J. Sussman et al. / Biological Psychology xxx (2016) xxx–xxx 5

hypothesize that emotional stimuli distract from the task at hand in perception (Barrett & Simmons, 2015; Summerfield & de Lange,
because they are processed automatically, leading to impaired task- 2014). Perceptual decision-making is heavily influenced by factors
relevant performance. For example, task-irrelevant emotional faces that occur prior to physical encounter with the stimulus, such as,
slow reaction times (Vuilleumier et al., 2001) and decrease accuracy expectations regarding what is contextually relevant or likely. This
(McHugo, Olatunji, & Zald, 2013). The presentation of emotional is exemplified by faster and more accurate recognition of objects
stimuli exogenously or as distractors in experimental paradigms that occur in familiar contexts (Bar, 2004; Brattico, Naatanen, &
has contributed to the view that emotional stimuli are processed Tervaniemi, 2002; Enns & Lleras, 2008), hence, a loaf of bread is
involuntarily in a bottom-up manner and are immune to the effect identified more accurately than a drum in the kitchen (Palmer,
attention or cognitive control. 1975). More recently, research is showing that emotional top-down
However, studies also show that strong as it may be, bottom-up factors can also influence perception (e.g., Sussman et al., 2016;
processing of an emotional stimulus is also susceptible to top-down Sussman, Szekely et al., 2016). In the brain, top-down factors such
control. For example, happy and threatening facial expressions as expectation, context, attention, and learning have been shown
have been shown to capture attention when they are the tar- to influence amygdala activity. The amygdala is one of the most
get of search but not when they are in opposition to task goals highly connected regions of the brain and shows connectivity con-
(Hahn & Gronlund, 2007; Williams, Moss, Bradshaw, & Mattingley, sistent with that of a ‘hub’ region (Barbas, 1995; Stephan et al.,
2005). This indicates that in addition to stimulus characteristics, 2000; Swanson, 2003; Young, Scannell, Burns, & Blakemore, 1994)
top-down goals guide the efficiency of emotional facial expression indicating that it is well situated to influence processing in other
search. Similarly, reward and punishment can modulate bottom- regions.
up capture of attention (Engelmann & Pessoa, 2007) and distractor In summary, while much has been learned regarding the
inhibition (Della Libera & Chelazzi, 2006). For more detailed cov- bottom-up factors involved in the perceptual prioritization of
erage on the interaction between top-down goals and bottom-up emotional stimuli, increasing evidence is showing that 1) studies
processing of emotional stimuli we would refer readers to more examining attentional and perceptual biases for emotional stim-
comprehensive reviews on this topic by Aue, Chauvigne, Bristle, uli have largely utilized paradigms that capitalize on the use of
Okon-Singer, & Guex, 2016; Pessoa, 2009; Pessoa & Ungerleider, bottom-up processes, 2) bottom-up perception and related neu-
2005 and Pessoa et al., 2002; Pessoa, Gutierrez et al., 2002. ral mechanisms are sensitive to top-down influence, 3) top-down
Neural evidence also indicates that bottom-up subcortical pro- factors play a critical role in human visual perception. Together,
cessing of emotional stimuli is susceptible to top-down control. these lines of evidence underscore the importance of examining the
For example, amygdala and its influence on the visual cortex, is role of top-down prestimulus biases in the prirotized perception of
impacted by top-down factors like task-context and attentional threatening stimuli.
control (Pessoa & Adolphs, 2010; Pessoa, 2008). Amygdala response
is modulated via top-down input from prefrontal brain regions 6. Top-down guidance of perception
during emotional conflict (Etkin, Egner, Peraza, Kandel, & Hirsch,
2006) and reappraisal (Ochsner, Bunge, Gross, & Gabrieli, 2002; In contrast to bottom-up processes, top-down processes are
Ochsner et al., 2004; Phan, Wager, Taylor, & Liberzon, 2004); for endogenous and driven by contexts or goals. According to the
a review see (Ochsner & Gross, 2005). While some studies have top-down perspective, visual perception involves a process of
argued that amygdala response to threatening stimuli is indepen- hypothesis testing in which predictive perceptual models are
dent of voluntary attention (e.g. Vuilleumier et al., 2001), these proposed based on prior knowledge and incoming sensory infor-
studies tend to use tasks in which threatening stimuli are presented mation is compared to these predicted models (Gregory, 1968;
in an unexpected manner, and have not examined the contributions Summerfield et al., 2006). Therefore, past knowledge and expe-
top-down factors, such as those implemented via projections from rience create expectations of what is relevant or likely, helping
the prefrontal cortex to the amygdala. Hence, Pessoa and Adolphs facilitate the speed and accuracy of subsequent perceptual judg-
(2010) propose an alternative model, which underlines the numer- ments. This expectation takes the form of predictive neural
ous connections between the prefrontal cortex, the visual cortex, representations that may be based on perceptual templates that
the amygdala and other subcortical structures, and posits that the consist of important discriminating features (Neisser, 2014) used
amygdala is primarily involved in the coordination of cortical pro- to aid stimulus recognition.
cessing rather than just the detection of threat. These predictive representations are implemented via two
The interaction between bottom-up emotional processing and important top-down mechanisms: 1) by an attention-related pri-
top-down factors also plays an important role in anxiety as outlined oritization of stimulus processing based on the relevance of the
by the attentional control theory (Eysenck et al., 2007). This theory stimulus to goals and 2) by an expectation-related interpretation
posits that trait anxiety increases the attention given to threatening of stimulus based on the likelihood of encountering an antici-
stimuli and impairs attentional control. In other words, trait anxi- pated stimulus (Summerfield & de Lange, 2014; Summerfield et al.,
ety impairs top-down guidance of attention and boosts bottom-up 2006). The function of top-down attention is to allocate cognitive
processes related to threat detection. More specifically, according resources based on the relevance or salience of a stimulus given the
to attentional control theory, trait anxiety impairs the efficiency current context. Common manipulations of attention by top-down
with which non-threatening stimuli are processed, rather than processes include spatial attention, feature-based attention, and
always impacting performance. This is consistent with studies that object-based attention (Kanwisher & Wojciulik, 2000). For exam-
found that accuracy (Calvo, Eysenck, Ramos, & Jimenez, 1994; Ikeda, ple, prior knowledge of the target stimulus location enhances the
Iwanaga, & Seiwa, 1996; Markham & Darke, 1991) or reaction detection of stimuli at the attended location (Carrasco, Ling, & Read,
time was not negatively impacted by anxiety (Bishop et al., 2004; 2004; Posner, Snyder, & Davidson, 1980), even when attention is
Compton et al., 2003; Whalen et al., 1998). Attentional control the- ‘covert’ (i.e. in the absence of saccades to attended locations). Sim-
ory also posits that the impact of trait anxiety on performance is ilarly, top-down spatial biasing towards the location reduces the
exacerbated as demands on the executive control increase (Ashcraft distracting effects of salient stimuli at other locations (Theeuwes,
& Kirk, 2001; Eysenck, 1985). 1991; Yantis & Jonides, 1984). In contrast to attention, the effect
Finally, in addition to evidence indicating that emotional of expectation is often studied by manipulating the likelihood of
bottom-up factors constantly interact with top-down factors, the occurrence of a stimulus. Studies have robustly demonstrated
research increasingly shows that top-down factors play a vital role that responses to visual stimuli are facilitated by the conditional

Please cite this article in press as: Sussman, T. J., et al. Top-down and bottom-up factors in threat-related perception and attention in
anxiety. Biol. Psychol. (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.08.006
G Model
BIOPSY-7253; No. of Pages 13 ARTICLE IN PRESS
6 T.J. Sussman et al. / Biological Psychology xxx (2016) xxx–xxx

probability of occurrence in a given context. Objects are recog- Tanaka, 1996). Downstream (higher order) brain regions feed infor-
nized more quickly in a context in which they are likely to be mation back to upstream (lower order) regions, influencing how
found (Gold & Shadlen, 2007; Heekeren, Marrett, & Ungerleider, information is processed. These feedback projections outnumber
2008). Research also demonstrates that objects in familiar or pre- the feedforward projections in most stages of the hierarchy, and
dictable contexts are recognized faster and more accurately than therefore are likely to have a major influence on the processing of
objects seen in unpredictable contexts (Bar, 2004; Brattico et al., incoming information (Angelucci et al., 2002). Since attention and
2002; Enns & Lleras, 2008); for example, a loaf of bread is iden- expectation are two top-down influences that may play a crucial
tified more accurately than a drum in the kitchen (Palmer, 1975). role in perceptual biases towards threatening stimuli, this review
Additionally, humans integrate and weigh prior knowledge about will focus on these two factors.
likelihood and uncertainty in combination with current sensory Top-down modulation of perception impacts most known
information in perceptual decision-making following Bayes’ the- stages of visual information processing, even in brain regions that
orem (Bach & Dolan, 2012). In contrast, unexpected visual objects process basic visual information, such as V1 (Li, Piech, & Gilbert,
in a complex scene are often detected more slowly and with more 2006; Motter, 1993; Roelfsema, Lamme, & Spekreijse, 1998). In fact,
errors (Biederman, Mezzanotte, & Rabinowitz, 1982). top-down modulation has been found to occur even before the sen-
sory information reaches the cortex in the subcortical region of
7. Top-down guidance of perception by emotional cues lateral geniculate nucleus (McAlonan, Cavanaugh, & Wurtz, 2008;
O’Connor, Fukui, Pinsk, & Kastner, 2002); for review see (Gilbert
While threatening stimuli can take us by surprise, we often & Li, 2013). Top-down modulation is more obvious in extrastri-
detect these stimuli within contexts or following cues that indi- ate areas (V2/V3 and V4) (McAdams & Maunsell, 1999; Nienborg &
cate an upcoming threat. Implicit and explicit environmental cues Cumming, 2009), and in the medial (Womelsdorf, Anton-Erxleben,
direct us to stimuli that are relevant or likely given the context, & Treue, 2008) and ventral visual streams (Chelazzi, Miller, Duncan,
subsequently improving perception. For example, a context such & Desimone, 1993). The effect of attention and expectation on visual
as a dense forest path in Colorado can create expectations of see- cortical activity prior to stimulus onset has been demonstrated
ing a bear, or an overt cue, such as a sign warning that a floor was in multiple human studies (Esterman & Yantis, 2010; Peelen, Fei-
recently washed, encourages us to look for slippery spots, resulting Fei, & Kastner, 2009; Stokes, Thompson, Nobre, & Duncan, 2009;
in faster detection of the expected stimulus. Emotional and moti- Summerfield et al., 2006). For example, using a cued face/house
vational top-down factors (e.g. searching for threat or anticipating discrimination task, one study demonstrated increase of blood-
reward) have been shown to influence target detection. When emo- oxygen-level dependent (BOLD) signal in object-category-specific
tional stimuli are task-relevant, in other words, when they are visual cortical areas during expectation (Esterman & Yantis, 2010).
prioritized both by top-down and bottom-up processes, detection Anticipation increases prestimulus neuronal activity in sensory
of these stimuli are improved. For example, happy and threaten- and decision-related neurons (Kastner, Pinsk, De Weerd, Desimone,
ing facial expressions are prioritized when they are the target of & Ungerleider, 1999; Ress, Backus, & Heeger, 2000; Summerfield
search, but not when they are in opposition to task goals, (Hahn & de Lange, 2014). For example, studies show that neurons in
& Gronlund, 2007; Williams et al., 2005). Furthermore, cues indi- the inferior temporal lobe that encode the expected stimulus
cating an upcoming threat-related perceptual decision improve the show increased activation following a predictive cue (Erickson &
sensitivity and speed of subsequent perceptual decisions (Sussman Desimone, 1999). Similarly, neurons in medial temporal lobe (sen-
et al., 2016; Sussman, Szekely et al., 2016), specifically in case of sitive to object motion) are activated prior to a predicted motion
subsequent fearful faces (Sussman, et al., 2016). These studies indi- stimulus (Albright, 2012; K. Sakai & Miyashita, 1991). Prestimulus
cate that, in addition to stimulus characteristics, emotion-related BOLD signal in extrastriate visual cortex is associated with sub-
top-down goals guide the efficiency of facial expression search, and sequent decisions regarding whether subjects report seeing the
can improve target detection. Rubin’s vase illusion as a face or a vase (Hesselmann, Kell, Eger,
Top-down processes could improve threat perception by guid- & Kleinschmidt, 2008), and face-related cues elicit increased BOLD
ing attention to a spatial location, or to a specific feature. For signal in fusiform face area (FFA) prior to face stimulus onset (Bar
example, on a visual search task, cues correctly predicting the et al., 2001; Esterman & Yantis, 2010; Puri, Wojciulik, & Ranganath,
spatial location and threat value of faces improved reaction time; 2009). In a random dot classification task, BOLD signal in motion
performance improved both when spatial cues were accurate and sensitive visual cortex prior to stimulus onset predicts the subject’s
when cues accurately predicted an angry face, demonstrating that response (Hesselmann, Kell, & Kleinschmidt, 2008). Another study
endogenous processes related to both spatial and feature-based examining the prestimulus oscillatory activity over motor cortex
attention can enhance threat detection (Mohanty, Egner, Monti, found that both endogenous expectation (without explicit cue) and
& Mesulam, 2009). Additionally, on a cued word identification expectation induced by explicit cues biases the starting point of
task, emotional words were more accurately identified than neutral decision-related activity before the accumulation of sensory evi-
words, while emotional distractors had no impact on performance, dence (de Lange, Rahnev, Donner, & Lau, 2013).
suggesting that when directed to looking for a specific emotional Studies examining the ensemble activity patterns of BOLD signal
stimulus via cues, perceptual processing of that emotional stim- have shown that top-down attention to a target activates target-
ulus is enhanced (Zeelenberg, Wagenmakers, & Rotteveel, 2006). specific representations in shape-sensitive visual areas (Peelen
The arousal-biased competition theory suggests a mechanism by et al., 2009), and brain regions involved in olfactory perception
which top-down processes may perceptually prioritize emotional (Zelano, Mohanty, & Gottfried, 2011), indicating a preparatory bias
stimuli. It posits that emotional cues increase arousal, biasing selec- favoring the attended stimulus over competing ones.
tive attention toward perception of the stimuli relevant to the goal Increased prestimulus activity may reflect increased attention
at hand (Mather & Sutherland, 2011). prior to stimulus onset, thereby improving subsequent detection
(Hesselmann, Sadaghiani, Friston, & Kleinschmidt, 2010). Alter-
8. Brain mechanisms of top-down guidance of perception natively, according to sequential sampling models of perceptual
decision-making, like the drift diffusion model (Ratcliff & Smith,
After sensory information hits the retina it is processed in hier- 2004; Ratcliff, 1978), increases in prestimulus activity may reflect a
archically organized regions with increasing level of abstractness bias or shift in the starting point for evidence accumulation towards
and complexity before a percept is formed (Deco & Rolls, 2004; a specific decision boundary, or may reflect a change in the rate of

Please cite this article in press as: Sussman, T. J., et al. Top-down and bottom-up factors in threat-related perception and attention in
anxiety. Biol. Psychol. (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.08.006
G Model
BIOPSY-7253; No. of Pages 13 ARTICLE IN PRESS
T.J. Sussman et al. / Biological Psychology xxx (2016) xxx–xxx 7

evidence accumulation (Summerfield & de Lange, 2014). Behav- Separate from the effects of attention, expectations regarding
iorally, studies have shown that a predicted stimulus is associated upcoming targets can enhance their perception (Summerfield &
with faster reaction time, higher accuracy, and higher perceptual Egner, 2009). According to the ‘predictive coding’ theory, rather
sensitivity compared to unpredicted stimuli (Bar, 2004; Geisler, than passively absorbing sensory input, the brain actively predicts
2008; Polat & Sagi, 1994). what is coming, generating a prestimulus template against which
In addition to sensory areas, top-down modulation of visual observed sensory information is matched (Summerfield et al.,
processing involves prefrontal and parietal areas. For example, 2006; Zelano et al., 2011). In an fMRI study in which human subjects
top-down biasing of attention in space involves a network of decided whether visual objects were faces or not, predictive neural
frontoparietal regions that include the intra-parietal sulcus in representations related to faces were reported in medial prefrontal
the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) and the frontal eye fields cortex (mPFC; Summerfield et al., 2006). Interestingly, perceptual
(FEF), the anterior cingulate cortex and supplementary motor area decisions about faces were associated with an increase in top-down
(ACC/SMA), and the thalamus and superior colliculus (Corbetta connectivity from the mPFC to face-sensitive visual cortices, includ-
& Shulman, 2002; Gitelman et al., 1999; Kastner, De Weerd, ing FFA, consistent with the idea that the prefrontal cortex codes
Desimone, & Ungerleider, 1998; Kastner et al., 1999; Mesulam, for the predicted representations and sends top-down signals that
1981, 1999; Reynolds, Chelazzi, & Desimone, 1999). This spatial guide the sensory regions in collecting relevant evidence to make
attention network is hypothesized to form an integrated search the perceptual decision. Using multivariate pattern (MVP) analy-
template combining the spatial coordinates and the relevance of ses of prestimulus ensemble patterns, another study showed that
the anticipated stimulus to bias visual neurons in preparation target-specific ensemble patterns emerge prior to encountering the
for the search process (Egner, 2008; Gottlieb, 2007; Thompson target stimulus in the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) and in sensory
& Bichot, 2005). Brain regions involved with assessing the moti- cortices. Furthermore, these prestimulus patterns reliably predict
vational value of a stimulus include neurons in the inferior subsequent behavioural performance (Zelano et al., 2011).
parietal lobule and the intra-parietal sulcus (Bushnell, Goldberg, In a study that examined the impact of threat compared to
& Robinson, 1981; Mountcastle, Lynch, Georgopoulos, Sakata, & neutral prestimulus cues on brain activity and subsequent perfor-
Acuna, 1975; Sugrue, Corrado, & Newsome, 2004) and limbic mance, threat cues increased both cue- and stimulus-related brain
regions such as amygdala (Pessoa et al., 2002; Pessoa, Gutierrez activation and improved subsequent stimulus detection (Sussman
et al., 2002; Vuilleumier & Driver, 2007). However, whether these et al., 2016). More specifically, threat cues resulted in a larger late
regions communicate is unclear. The cingulate gyrus may be a con- positive potential (LPP) and in increased superior temporal sul-
duit for information on motivational salience used by the spatial cus (STS) activity, both of which are measures of emotional face
attention network (Mesulam, Van Hoesen, Pandya, & Geschwind, processing. In addition, threat cues specifically increased amyg-
1977), as the limbic parts of the cingulate gyrus send projec- dala activity for subsequently presented threatening vs neutral
tions to frontoparietal regions and posterior cingulate neurons faces. Furthermore, brain activity, as measured by the LPP and
signal reward outcomes associated with eye movements (McCoy, STS activity, predicted subsequent improvement in the speed and
Crowley, Haghighian, Dean, & Platt, 2003) and preferences guiding precision of perceptual decisions about threatening faces. These
visual orienting (McCoy & Platt, 2005). results demonstrate how top-down processing elicited by pres-
timulus threat-related cues can enhance subsequent perceptual
decision-making. It has also been hypothesized that this enhance-
ment may be due to arousal-induced release of norepinephrine
9. Brain mechanisms of top-down guidance of perception into the locus coeruleus leading to increased levels of glutamate
by emotional cues and norepinephrine at the site of the goal-relevant representation,
thereby enhancing the representation of the goal-relevant stimulus
While most studies have focused on the brain mechanisms (Mather, Clewett, Sakaki, & Harley, 2015).
of bottom-up perception of emotional stimuli, newer evidence Overall, while the neural mechanisms involved in prestimulus
is beginning to uncover the brain circuitry that is involved threat-related biases are relatively unexplored, emerging evidence
in top-down guidance by emotional information. In one study, indicates that top-down factors may impact threat perception both
endogenous guidance of attention was manipulated by predic- by changes in prestimulus activity in sensory regions as well as
tive cues offering both probabilistic information related to the prestimulus biasing via templates instantiated in higher order
location of a subsequently presented stimulus, and information regions (including PPC, FEF, and other prefrontal regions) and inter-
regarding the emotional salience of that stimulus (Mohanty et al., actions of these regions with limbic and sensory regions.
2009). Spatially valid cues enhanced target detection. In addition,
cues accurately predicting angry face targets were associated with
faster responses than uninformative cues, indicating an endoge- 10. Top-down guidance of the perception by emotional
nous mediation of improved target detection, driven by emotional cues in anxiety
cues. Functional imaging showed that prior to the stimulus pre-
sentation, spatially informative cues activated the frontoparietal Top-down processes may also play a crucial role in the devel-
spatial attention network including the intra-parietal sulcus and opment and maintenance of perceptual biases in anxiety disorders.
FEF, as well as FG. Cues predicting angry faces also activated brain Anticipation of negative future events is one of the cardinal features
regions associated with emotional processing, including limbic of anxiety. For example, people with anxiety tend to overestimate
areas, such as the amygdala. Additive effects of spatial and emo- both the likelihood of negative events occurring and the cost of
tional cueing were identified in the intra-parietal sulcus, FEF and these negative events (Grupe & Nitschke, 2013). Thus, a person with
FG indicating that cues activate regions involved in directing spatial clinical anxiety or spider-related fear standing in a room with cob-
attention prior to arrival of the threat are activated in preparation. webs might have higher expectation of spiders being present and
These regions also had increased connectivity with the amygdala overestimate their dangerousness compared to a non-anxious per-
following angry face cues. This study demonstrates that prestimu- son in the same room. As a result of this overestimation, anxious
lus threat-related cues elicit amygdala input to the spatial attention individuals will scan the environment for spiders and will detect
network and inferotemporal visual areas, thereby facilitating threat them faster if present. Researchers have proposed that focusing
detection. on the anticipatory phase in anxiety may be an effective strategy

Please cite this article in press as: Sussman, T. J., et al. Top-down and bottom-up factors in threat-related perception and attention in
anxiety. Biol. Psychol. (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.08.006
G Model
BIOPSY-7253; No. of Pages 13 ARTICLE IN PRESS
8 T.J. Sussman et al. / Biological Psychology xxx (2016) xxx–xxx

to determine psychological and neurobiological factors involved in Making use of prior threat related information requires the
anxiety development and maintenance (Davis et al., 2010). maintenance of task-relevant representations online in work-
According to the ‘uncertainty and anticipation model of anxi- ing memory to match against incoming stimuli (Sreenivasan,
ety,’ anxiety influences several cognitive processes, two of which Sambhara, & Jha, 2011). Because emotional representations are
relate directly to threat perception (Grupe & Nitschke, 2013). First, maintained with greater vividness (Bywaters, Andrade, & Turpin,
hypervigilance, or increased attention to threatening information 2004), they may tax working memory resources more than neu-
even before a stimulus is presented can lead to both faster detection tral internal representations. Therefore, tasks that require the
of threatening stimuli and a misinterpretation of neutral stimuli. maintenance of threat-related information, or that encourage the
For example, anxiety caused by the threat of shock leads to faster unnecessary entry of threat-related information into working
detection of negative stimuli (Robinson et al., 2011), interpretation memory may result in deficits for individuals high in trait anx-
of neutral faces as being negative in socially anxious individuals iety (Stout, Shackman, & Larson, 2013; Sussman, Szekely et al.,
(Yoon & Zinbarg, 2008) and interpretation of ambiguous intero- 2016). Trait anxiety is thought to particularly impact the efficiency
ceptive experiences as being negative in people high in anxiety on tasks involving the inhibition function (supporting empiri-
sensitivity (Richards, Austin, & Alvarenga, 2001). Second, inflated cal research includes Calvo & Eysenck, 1996; Fox, 2002; Yiend &
estimates of threat probability and the costs of threats can lead to Mathews, 2001), the shifting function (Eysenck et al., 2007; Gopher,
improved performance (Paulus & Yu, 2012), via overweighting of Armony, & Greenshpan, 2000), and, to a certain extent, the updating
low probability events (Mukherjee, 2010). function (Duff & Logie, 2001).
Recent evidence provides direct support for the view that anx- Neurally, anxiety is associated with reduced recruitment of
iety can improve perception by influencing top-down processes regions involved in top-down control. For example, predictive
such as top-down attention. This research shows improvement representations of upcoming target stimuli are maintained in pre-
in perceptual sensitivity due to prior threat-related information frontal regions of the brain, specifically, the dorsal and ventral
is dependent on individual differences in trait anxiety and cur- medial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC & VMPFC; Summerfield et al.,
rent levels of induced anxiety (Sussman, Szekely et al., 2016). In 2006). Studies show that anxiety is associated with poorer recruit-
this study, two groups of participants varying in levels of trait ment of DMPFC (Shin et al., 2005) and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
anxiety (dispositional anxiety) identified degraded emotional and (DLPFC; Bishop, 2009), possibly contributing to an impaired abil-
neutral stimuli in a cued two-alternative forced-choice perceptual ity to maintain and deploy threat-related perceptual templates in
task. One group completed the perceptual tasks in the presence the service of threat perception. In line with this, low trait anxious
of the threat of shock (high situationally induced anxiety), while individuals have been found to benefit from cues preceding a visual
the other group completed the same task in the absence of threat search task, whereas individuals high in trait anxiety are not able
of shock (low situationally induced anxiety). Individual differences to use these cues as effectively (Berggren & Derakshan, 2013). In
in trait anxiety moderated gains in perceptual sensitivity follow- a recent study, decreased perceptual sensitivity in high trait anx-
ing the threat cue, such that higher trait anxiety was associated iety was observed for threatening but not neutral cues (Sussman,
with larger gains in perceptual sensitivity in the presence of shock Szekely et al., 2016). Since the adverse impact of anxiety on per-
(Sussman, Szekely et al., 2016) but worse perceptual sensitivity in formance becomes greater with increasing task demands on the
the absence of shock. Overall, results from this study demonstrated central executive (Eysenck et al., 2007), it is possible that main-
that distinct types of anxiety (dispositional and induced) interact tenance of a perceptual set for threatening stimuli which might
with each other in influencing how prior threat-related informa- be more complex may be more demanding than maintaining a
tion is used in a top-down manner to guide perception (Sussman, perceptual set for neutral stimuli.
Szekely et al., 2016). Prestimulus threat-related information can Few studies have examined neural mechanisms of threat related
influence perception by effecting attention or expectation, both of guidance of attention or expectation in anxiety. However, exami-
which are hypothesized to involve different psychological and neu- nation of neural activity at rest or prior to stimulus onset in anxiety
ral mechanisms (Summerfield & de Lange, 2014). The cues in the provides clues into potential neural mechanisms. Neuroimaging
aforementioned Sussman, Szekely et al. (2016) indicated what to studies have demonstrated that the amygdala is more active
look for (threatening or neutral faces) but the cues did not provide for people with anxiety disorders compared to healthy controls
information regarding the likelihood of targets. (Furmark et al., 2002; Sakai et al., 2005; Semple et al., 2000), and
Aue and colleagues (2013, 2016) manipulated the probabil- for people with a short variant of the 5-HT transported gene com-
ity or likelihood of upcoming targets to examine whether threat pared to individuals homozygous for the long variant (Canli et al.,
expectancy (inflated estimates of threat probability) impacts detec- 2006), when at rest. Anticipatory amygdala and anterior cingulate
tion of a spider or birds in individuals with or without spider fear cortex activity prior to treatment predicted treatment outcome
(Aue et al., 2016; Aue, Guex, Chauvigne & Okon-Singer, 2013). In 8 weeks later (Nitschke et al., 2009). In one experiment, subjects
two studies, they manipulated expectancy via a prestimulus cue with social phobia were asked to imagine giving a public speech.
telling participants there was a 90% or 50% likelihood of seeing a Individuals with social phobia had hyperactivity in limbic and par-
spider or a bird. Cues indicating the likelihood of spiders did not alimbic regions compared to healthy controls (Lorberbaum et al.,
have a significant impact on the speed of spider detection for indi- 2004). While anticipating giving a public speech, socially anxious
viduals with or without spider fear. However, prestimulus cues individuals also showed increased limbic activation and decreased
regarding the likelihood of seeing a bird on the subsequent trial striatal activation (Boehme et al., 2014), as well as reduced func-
lead to observable differences in reaction time, error rates, pupil tional connectivity between cortical regions involved in emotion
diameter and heart rate for both groups of subjects (Aue et al., regulation and limbic regions (Cremers et al., 2015). Furthermore,
2016; Aue, Guex, Chauvigne & Okon-Singer, 2013). These stud- symptom severity was found to correlate with changes in activa-
ies indicate that top-down manipulation of expectancies does not tion and connectivity (Boehme et al., 2014; Cremers et al., 2015).
impact detection of threatening stimuli the way it affects detec- Overall, these studies show greater limbic activity prior to stim-
tion of non-threatening stimuli. Taken together, the Sussman et al. ulus onset in anxiety suggesting that anxiety may impact threat
(2016, 2016) and Aue et al. (2013, 2016) studies indicate that anxi- perception by changes in prestimulus activity in limbic regions,
ety is associated with differential utilization of attention but not possibly leading individuals with anxiety to interpret cues regard-
probability-related information regarding upcoming threatening ing salience and the likelihood of upcoming threatening stimuli
stimuli. differently than individuals without anxiety. Enhanced perceptual

Please cite this article in press as: Sussman, T. J., et al. Top-down and bottom-up factors in threat-related perception and attention in
anxiety. Biol. Psychol. (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.08.006
G Model
BIOPSY-7253; No. of Pages 13 ARTICLE IN PRESS
T.J. Sussman et al. / Biological Psychology xxx (2016) xxx–xxx 9

sensitivity in threatening contexts, or following threat-related cues Furthermore, the impact of other types of top-down processes
may also be due to enhanced sensory-perceptual functions, which on threat perception should be examined. For example, since we
have been observed in both high trait anxiety and induced anxi- typically can infer what kinds of threats and rewards are more
ety (Robinson, Vytal, Cornwell, & Grillon, 2013). Results from one relevant or likely depending on the context we currently inhabit,
study demonstrate that the threat of shock changes neural process- exploring the impact of context on threat perception will allow
ing to a sensory-vigilance mode that prioritizes threatening stimuli for more ecologically valid examination of how emotional stimuli
(Arnsten, 2009; Shackman et al., 2011). Finally, models of decision- are perceptually prioritized. Some work has been conducted in this
making suggest a few mechanisms that could drive the perceptual vein. A recent study demonstrated that negative contexts evoked
prioritization of threatening images in anxiety by biasing decisions by recalling a real-life threat (the Boston Marathon Bombings) led
towards a threaten response (Sussman, Szekely et al., 2016). to an increased false alarm rate on a shooting task (Wormwood,
Overall, the role of top-down factors in threat perception in anxi- Lynn, Feldman Barrett, & Quigley, 2016). This study demonstrates
ety is not well understood. While greater activity in threat-sensitive that by studying the impact of context, we can add considerably to
limbic and sensory brain regions at rest and during anticipatory our understanding of how threats are perceptually prioritized, and
periods in anxiety suggests a possible mechanism by which threat how neutral stimuli may be misperceived as threatening in day-
perception could be prioritized in anxiety, the impact of anxiety to-day life. The interaction of situational contexts (such as the one
on top-down attention or expectation of threat is not yet known. described above), and internal contexts, such as anxiety, or other
Some empirical evidence supports enhancements in perceptual moods should also be explored to gain a more complete under-
sensitivity due to prior threat-related information; however, these standing of how top-down factors impact perception. While some
perceptual benefits depend on the type of anxiety. In disposi- research has been done in this area, many questions remain. One
tional anxiety, impairment of top-down mechanisms may make study demonstrated that a fearful mood, induced via film clip, could
utilization and maintenance of top-down threat-related informa- speed reaction times over and above the impact of low-level visual
tion harder; whereas this information may be more effectively information (LoBue, 2014). However, little is known about how pos-
utilized in case of clinical and situationally induced anxiety. itive moods impact threat perception, or about how situational and
internal contexts interact in their impact on threat-perception.
Future research should also aim to better isolate the impact of
11. Conclusion top-down from bottom-up factors on threat perception, as results
could advance our understanding of how these factors interact to
The perceptual prioritization of threatening stimuli, often perceptually prioritize emotional stimuli. Drawing these distinc-
described as a bias for these stimuli, in dispositional, clinical and tions is especially crucial when studying the influence of anxiety on
induced anxiety has been observed as faster detection of threat- perception, as anxiety is associated with inaccurate estimates of the
related stimuli (Lim & Pessoa, 2008; Mogg & Bradley, 1999; Mogg likelihood and costs of future negative events (Grupe & Nitschke,
et al., 2000; Ohman et al., 2001; Robinson et al., 2011), or greater 2013). Elucidating how individual differences in anxiety impact
activation in fear-sensitive brain regions (Bishop et al., 2004; Etkin the interaction between top-down and bottom-up factors, both
et al., 2004; Larson et al., 2005). This perceptual advantage has in terms of behavioural performance and neural processing, could
generally been studied as a bottom-up phenomenon driven by provide clues about how basic perceptual processes are associated
the physical characteristics of the threatening stimulus. Because with clinical symptoms. This, in turn, may provide valuable new
bottom-up processes were thought to drive the prioritized percep- information about how to create new treatments for clinical anxi-
tion of threat, experiments designed to study a bias for threatening ety, and how to improve existing ones. For example, a recent study
stimuli have often relied on tasks that only tested the effects of found that delivering an ABMT designed to enhance vigilance for
exogenous factors. However, top-down processes, such as prior threat prior to combat exposure protected participants, reducing
knowledge, expectations and goals, influence perception (Brosch post-traumatic stress and depression symptoms following com-
et al., 2010; Pessoa & Adolphs, 2010). Furthermore, these endoge- bat (Wald et al., 2016). This demonstrates that acknowledging the
nous factors are of particular importance in anxiety, which is role of top-down processes in anxiety could lead to novel forms of
characterized by exaggerated and inaccurate estimates of the prob- therapy, some which could even function preventatively.
ability and costs of future negative events (Grupe & Nitschke, 2013).
Therefore, examining the impact of top-down factors on threat per-
ception in anxiety is a crucial step towards shedding light on how References
basic processes of cognition, such as perception, may shape the
Abrams, R. A., & Christ, S. E. (2003). Motion onset captures attention. Psychological
development and maintenance of anxiety and related disorders. Science, 14(5), 427–432.
Future studies should focus on examining the differential impact Albright, T. D. (2012). On the perception of probable things: neural substrates of
of threat-related top-down and bottom-up mechanisms (e.g., cues associative memory, imagery, and perception. Neuron, 74(2), 227–245.
Anderson, A. K., & Phelps, E. A. (2001). Lesions of the human amygdala impair
guiding one to look for threatening faces and threatening faces enhanced perception of emotionally salient events. Nature, 411(6835),
themselves) on perception and attention. Distinguishing the impact 305–309.
of prestimulus attention from the impact of prestimulus expec- Anderson, A. K. (2005). Affective influences on the attentional dynamics supporting
awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 134(2), 258–281.
tation of seeing a salient target could provide more fine-grained Angelucci, A., Levitt, J. B., Walton, E. J., Hupe, J. M., Bullier, J., & Lund, J. S. (2002).
detail about how top-down factors influence perception in normal Circuits for local and global signal integration in primary visual cortex. The
function and in anxiety. Examination of neural and computational Journal of Neuroscience, 22(19), 8633–8646.
Arend, I., & Botella, J. (2002). Emotional stimuli reduce the attentional blink in
models would help elucidate the mechanisms implementing spe-
sub-clinical anxious subjects. Psicothema, 14(2), 209–214.
cific top-down and bottom-up factors involved in the perceptual Armony, J. L., & Dolan, R. J. (2002). Modulation of spatial attention by
prioritization of emotional stimuli. For example, future studies fear-conditioned stimuli: an event-related fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 40(7),
817–826.
could compare pre- and post-stimulus neural representations of
Arnsten, A. F. (2009). Stress signalling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex
emotional vs neutral stimuli in visual and prefrontal regions of the structure and function. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(6), 410–422.
brain, allowing us to determine how each contributes to perceptual Ashcraft, M. H., & Kirk, E. P. (2001). The relationships among working memory,
prioritization of threat in anxiety. The use of computational models math anxiety, and performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General,
130(2), 224–237.
would allow us to examine the specific mechanisms that contribute Aue, T., & Okon-Singer, H. (2015). Expectancy biases in fear and anxiety and their
to the perceptual prioritization. link to biases in attention. Clinical Psychology Review, 42, 83–95.

Please cite this article in press as: Sussman, T. J., et al. Top-down and bottom-up factors in threat-related perception and attention in
anxiety. Biol. Psychol. (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.08.006
G Model
BIOPSY-7253; No. of Pages 13 ARTICLE IN PRESS
10 T.J. Sussman et al. / Biological Psychology xxx (2016) xxx–xxx

Aue, T., Guex, R., Chauvigne, L., & Okon-Singer, H. (2013). Varying expectancies and Chen, X., & Zelinsky, G. J. (2006). Real-world visual search is dominated by
attention bias in phobic and non-phobic individuals. Frontiers in Human top-down guidance. Vision Research, 46(24), 4118–4133.
Neuroscience. Cisler, J. M., & Koster, E. H. (2010). Mechanisms of attentional biases towards threat
Aue, T., Chauvigne, L. A., Bristle, M., Okon-Singer, H., & Guex, R. (2016). Expectancy in anxiety disorders: an integrative review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(2),
influences on attention to threat are only weak and transient: behavioral and 203–216.
physiological evidence. Biological Psychology. Cisler, J. M., Bacon, A. K., & Williams, N. L. (2009). Phenomenological characteristics
Bach, D. R., & Dolan, R. J. (2012). Knowing how much you don’t know: a neural of attentional biases towards threat: a critical review. Cognitive Therapy and
organization of uncertainty estimates. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13(8), Research, 33(2), 221–234.
572–586. Compton, R. J., Banich, M. T., Mohanty, A., Milham, M. P., Herrington, J., Miller, G. A.,
Bacon, W. F., & Egeth, H. E. (1994). Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture. . . . & Heller, W. (2003). Paying attention to emotion: an fMRI investigation of
Perception & Psychophysics, 55(5), 485–496. cognitive and emotional Stroop tasks. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral
Bannerman, R. L., Milders, M., de Gelder, B., & Sahraie, A. (2009). Orienting to Neuroscience, 3(2), 81–96.
threat: faster localization of fearful facial expressions and body postures Comte, M., Schon, D., Coull, J. T., Reynaud, E., Khalfa, S., Belzeaux, R., . . . & Fakra, E.
revealed by saccadic eye movements. Proceedings of the Royal Society (2014). Dissociating bottom-up and top-down mechanisms in the
B-Biological Sciences, 276(1662), 1635–1641. cortico-limbic system during emotion processing. European Psychiatry, 29.
Bar, M., Tootell, R. B., Schacter, D. L., Greve, D. N., Fischl, B., Mendola, J. D., . . . & Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven
Dale, A. M. (2001). Cortical mechanisms specific to explicit visual object attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(3), 201–215.
recognition. Neuron, 29(2), 529–535. Cremers, H. R., Veer, I. M., Spinhoven, P., Rombouts, S. A., Yarkoni, T., Wager, T. D., &
Bar, M. (2004). Visual objects in context. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5(8), Roelofs, K. (2015). Altered cortical-amygdala coupling in social anxiety
617–629. disorder during the anticipation of giving a public speech. Psychological
Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van, I. M. H. Medicine, 45(7), 1521–1529.
(2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: Cunningham, W. A., & Brosch, T. (2012). Motivational salience: amygdala tuning
a meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133(1), 1–24. from traits, needs, values, and goals. Current Directions in Psychological Science,
Barbas, H. (1995). Anatomic basis of cognitive-emotional interactions in the 21(1), 54–59.
primate prefrontal cortex. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 19(3), Davidson, R. J. (2002). Anxiety and affective style: role of prefrontal cortex and
499–510. amygdala. Biological Psychiatry, 51(1), 68–80.
Barrett, L. F., & Simmons, W. K. (2015). Interoceptive predictions in the brain. Davis, M., Walker, D. L., Miles, L., & Grillon, C. (2010). Phasic vs sustained fear in
Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(7), 419–429. rats and humans: role of the extended amygdala in fear vs anxiety.
Barrett, L. F. (2006). Are emotions natural kinds? Perspectives on Psychological Neuropsychopharmacology, 35(1), 105–135.
Science, 1(1), 28–58. Deco, G., & Rolls, E. T. (2004). A neurodynamical cortical model of visual attention
Berggren, N., & Derakshan, N. (2013). Trait anxiety reduces implicit expectancy and invariant object recognition. Vision Research, 44(6), 621–642.
during target spatial probability cueing. Emotion, 13(2), 345–349. de Gelder, B., Vroomen, J., Pourtois, G., & Weiskrantz, L. (1999). Non-conscious
Biederman, I., Mezzanotte, R. J., & Rabinowitz, J. C. (1982). Scene perception: recognition of affect in the absence of striate cortex. Neuroreport, 10(18),
detecting and judging objects undergoing relational violations. Cognitive 3759–3763.
psychology, 14(2), 143–177. de Lange, F. P., Rahnev, D. A., Donner, T. H., & Lau, H. (2013). Prestimulus oscillatory
Birn, R. M., Shackman, A. J., Oler, J. A., Williams, L. E., McFarlin, D. R., Rogers, G. M., activity over motor cortex reflects perceptual expectations. Journal of
. . . & Kalin, N. H. (2014). Evolutionarily conserved prefrontal-amygdalar Neuroscience, 33(4), 1400–1410.
dysfunction in early-life anxiety. Molecular Psychiatry, 19(8), 915–922. Della Libera, C., & Chelazzi, L. (2006). Visual selective attention and the effects of
Bishop, S. J., Duncan, J., & Lawrence, A. D. (2004). State anxiety modulation of the monetary rewards. Psychological Science, 17(3), 222–227.
amygdala response to unattended threat-related stimuli. Journal of Desimone, R., & Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention.
Neuroscience, 24(46), 10364–10368. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18, 193–222.
Bishop, S. J. (2009). Trait anxiety and impoverished prefrontal control of attention. Dijksterhuis, A., & Aarts, H. (2003). On wildebeests and humans: the preferential
Nature Neuroscience, 12(1), 92–98. detection of negative stimuli. Psychological Science, 14(1), 14–18.
Boehme, S., Ritter, V., Tefikow, S., Stangier, U., Strauss, B., Miltner, W. H., & Straube, Dolan, R. J. (2002). Emotion, cognition, and behavior. Science, 298(5596),
T. (2014). Brain activation during anticipatory anxiety in social anxiety 1191–1194.
disorder. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(9), 1413–1418. Dolcos, F., & McCarthy, G. (2006). Brain systems mediating cognitive interference
Brattico, E., Naatanen, R., & Tervaniemi, M. (2002). Context effects on pitch by emotional distraction. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(7), 2072–2079.
perception in musicians and nonmusicians: evidence from Duff, S. C., & Logie, R. H. (2001). Processing and storage in working memory span.
event-related-potential recordings. Music Perception, 19(2), 199–222. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. A, Human Experimental Psychology,
Brosch, T., Pourtois, G., & Sander, D. (2010). The perception and categorisation of 54(1), 31–48.
emotional stimuli: a review. Cognition & Emotion, 24(3), 377–400. Eastwood, J. D., Smilek, D., & Merikle, P. M. (2001). Differential attentional
Bushnell, M. C., Goldberg, M. E., & Robinson, D. L. (1981). Behavioral enhancement guidance by unattended faces expressing positive and negative emotion.
of visual responses in monkey cerebral cortex. I. Modulation in posterior Perception and Psychophysics, 63(6), 1004–1013.
parietal cortex related to selective visual attention. Journal of Neurophysiology, Egner, T. (2008). Multiple conflict-driven control mechanisms in the human brain.
46(4), 755–772. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(10), 374–380.
Bywaters, M., Andrade, J., & Turpin, G. (2004). Determinants of the vividness of Eldar, S., Apter, A., Lotan, D., Edgar, K. P., Naim, R., Fox, N. A., . . . & Bar-Haim, Y.
visual imagery: the effects of delayed recall, stimulus affect and individual (2012). Attention bias modification treatment for pediatric anxiety disorders: a
differences. Memory, 12(4), 479–488. randomized controlled trial. American Journal of Psychiatry, 169(2), 213–220.
Calder, A. J., Young, A. W., Keane, J., & Dean, M. (2000). Configural information in Ellsworth, P. C., & Scherer, K. R. (2003). Appraisal processes in emotion. In H. H. G.
facial expression perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human R. J. Davidson, & K. R. Scherer (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp.
Perception and Performance, 26(2), 527–551. 572–595). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Calder, A. J., Ewbank, M., & Passamonti, L. (2011). Personality influences the neural Engelmann, J. B., & Pessoa, L. (2007). Motivation sharpens exogenous spatial
responses to viewing facial expressions of emotion. Philosophical Transactions attention. Emotion, 7(3), 668–674.
of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences, 366(1571), 1684–1701. Enns, J. T., & Lleras, A. (2008). What’s next? New evidence for prediction in human
Calvo, M. G., & Eysenck, M. W. (1996). Phonological working memory and reading vision. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(9), 327–333.
in test anxiety. Memory, 4(3), 289–305. Enns, J. T., Austen, E. L., Di Lollo, V., Rauschenberger, R., & Yantis, S. (2001). New
Calvo, M. G., & Nummenmaa, L. (2008). Detection of emotional faces: salient objects dominate luminance transients in setting attentional priority. Journal of
physical features guide effective visual search. Journal of Experimental Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 27(6), 1287–1302.
Psychology-General, 137(3), 471–494. Erickson, C. A., & Desimone, R. (1999). Responses of macaque perirhinal neurons
Calvo, M. G., Eysenck, M. W., Ramos, P. M., & Jimenez, A. (1994). Compensatory during and after visual stimulus association learning. Journal of Neuroscience,
reading strategies in test anxiety. Anxiety Stress and Coping, 7(2), 99–116. 19(23), 10404–10416.
Canli, T., Qiu, M., Omura, K., Congdon, E., Haas, B. W., Amin, Z., . . . & Lesch, K. P. Esterman, M., & Yantis, S. (2010). Perceptual expectation evokes category-selective
(2006). Neural correlates of epigenesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of cortical activity. Cerebral Cortex, 20(5), 1245–1253.
Sciences of the United States of America, 103(43), 16033–16038. Etkin, A., & Wager, T. D. (2007). Functional neuroimaging of anxiety: a
Carey, S., & Diamond, R. (1977). From piecemeal to configurational representation meta-analysis of emotional processing in PTSD, social anxiety disorder, and
of faces. Science, 195(4275), 312–314. specific phobia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 164(10), 1476–1488.
Carrasco, M., Ling, S., & Read, S. (2004). Attention alters appearance. Nature Etkin, A., Klemenhagen, K. C., Dudman, J. T., Rogan, M. T., Hen, R., Kandel, E. R., &
Neuroscience, 7(3), 308–313. Hirsch, J. (2004). Individual differences in trait anxiety predict the response of
Carretie, L., Mercado, F., Tapia, M., & Hinojosa, J. A. (2001). Emotion, attention, and the basolateral amygdala to unconsciously processed fearful faces. Neuron,
the ‘negativity bias’, studied through event-related potentials. International 44(6), 1043–1055.
Journal of Psychophysiology, 41(1), 75–85. Etkin, A., Egner, T., Peraza, D. M., Kandel, E. R., & Hirsch, J. (2006). Resolving
Cave, K. R., & Batty, M. J. (2006). From searching for features to searching for emotional conflict: a role for the rostral anterior cingulate cortex in
threat: drawing the boundary between preattentive and attentive vision. modulating activity in the amygdala. Neuron, 51(6), 871–882.
Visual Cognition, 14(4–8), 629–646. Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and
Chelazzi, L., Miller, E. K., Duncan, J., & Desimone, R. (1993). A neural basis for visual cognitive performance: attentional control theory. Emotion, 7(2), 336–353.
search in inferior temporal cortex. Nature, 363(6427), 345–347.

Please cite this article in press as: Sussman, T. J., et al. Top-down and bottom-up factors in threat-related perception and attention in
anxiety. Biol. Psychol. (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.08.006
G Model
BIOPSY-7253; No. of Pages 13 ARTICLE IN PRESS
T.J. Sussman et al. / Biological Psychology xxx (2016) xxx–xxx 11

Eysenck, M. W. (1985). Anxiety and cognitive-task performance. Personality and Kanwisher, N., & Wojciulik, E. (2000). Visual attention: insights from brain
Individual Differences, 6(5), 579–586. imaging. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 1(2), 91–100.
Fox, E., Lester, V., Russo, R., Bowles, R. J., Pichler, A., & Dutton, K. (2000). Facial Kastner, S., De Weerd, P., Desimone, R., & Ungerleider, L. G. (1998). Mechanisms of
expressions of emotion: are angry faces detected more efficiently? Cognition & directed attention in the human extrastriate cortex as revealed by functional
Emotion, 14(1), 61–92. MRI. Science, 282(5386), 108–111.
Fox, E., Russo, R., Bowles, R., & Dutton, K. (2001). Do threatening stimuli draw or Kastner, S., Pinsk, M. A., De Weerd, P., Desimone, R., & Ungerleider, L. G. (1999).
hold visual attention in subclinical anxiety? Journal of Experimental Increased activity in human visual cortex during directed attention in the
Psychology-General, 130(4), 681–700. absence of visual stimulation. Neuron, 22(4), 751–761.
Fox, A. S., Oler, J. A., Tromp, D. P. M., Fudge, J. L., & Kalin, N. H. (2015). Extending the Keil, A., Moratti, S., Sabatinelli, D., Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (2005). Additive
amygdala in theories of threat processing. Trends in Neurosciences, 38(5), effects of emotional content and spatial selective attention on electrocortical
319–329. facilitation. Cerebral Cortex, 15(8), 1187–1197.
Fox, E. (2002). Processing emotional facial expressions: the role of anxiety and Klumpp, H., & Amir, N. (2009). Examination of vigilance and disengagement of
awareness. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 2(1), 52–63. threat in social anxiety with a probe detection task. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping,
Franconeri, S. L., & Simons, D. J. (2003). Moving and looming stimuli capture 22(3), 283–296.
attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 65(7), 999–1010. Knott, C., Beyer, J., Villarruel, A., Denyes, M., Erickson, V., & Willard, G. (1994).
Freese, J. L., & Amaral, D. G. (2009). The human amygdala. In P. J. Whalen, & E. A. Using the Oucher developmental approach to pain assessment in children.
Phelps (Eds.), (pp. 3–42). New York: The Guilford Press. MCN; American Journal of Maternal Child Nursing, 19(6), 314–320.
Furmark, T., Tillfors, M., Marteinsdottir, I., Fischer, H., Pissiota, A., Langstrom, B., & Koster, E. H., Verschuere, B., Crombez, G., & Van Damme, S. (2005). Time-course of
Fredrikson, M. (2002). Common changes in cerebral blood flow in patients attention for threatening pictures in high and low trait anxiety. Behaviour
with social phobia treated with citalopram or cognitive-behavioural therapy. Research and Therapy, 43(8), 1087–1098.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 59(5), 425–433. Larson, C. L., Ruffalo, D., Nietert, J. Y., & Davidson, R. J. (2005). Stability of
Geisler, W. S. (2008). Visual perception and the statistical properties of natural emotion-modulated startle during short and long picture presentation.
scenes. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 167–192. Psychophysiology, 42(5), 604–610.
Geng, J. J., & Mangun, G. R. (2011). Right temporoparietal junction activation by a Larson, C. L., Aronoff, J., & Stearns, J. J. (2007). The shape of threat: simple geometric
salient contextual cue facilitates target discrimination. Neuroimage, 54(1), forms evoke rapid and sustained capture of attention. Emotion, 7(3), 526–534.
594–601. Larson, C. L., Aronoff, J., Sarinopoulos, I. C., & Zhu, D. C. (2009). Recognizing threat:
Gilbert, C. D., & Li, W. (2013). Top-down influences on visual processing. Nature a simple geometric shape activates neural circuitry for threat detection. Journal
Reviews Neuroscience, 14(5), 350–363. of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(8), 1523–1535.
Gitelman, D. R., Nobre, A. C., Parrish, T. B., LaBar, K. S., Kim, Y. H., Meyer, J. R., & LeDoux, J. E. (2000). Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience,
Mesulam, M. (1999). A large-scale distributed network for covert spatial 23, 155–184.
attention: further anatomical delineation based on stringent behavioural and LeDoux, J. E. (2015). Anxious. Oneworld Publications: London.
cognitive controls. Brain, 122(Pt. 6), 1093–1106. Li, W., Piech, V., & Gilbert, C. D. (2006). Contour saliency in primary visual cortex.
Gold, J. I., & Shadlen, M. N. (2007). The neural basis of decision making. Annual Neuron, 50(6), 951–962.
Review of Neuroscience, 30, 535–574. Lim, S. L., & Pessoa, L. (2008). Affective learning increases sensitivity to graded
Gopher, D., Armony, L., & Greenshpan, Y. (2000). Switching tasks and attention emotional faces. Emotion, 8(1), 96–103.
policies. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 129(3), 308–339. Lim, S. L., Padmala, S., & Pessoa, L. (2009). Segregating the significant from the
Gottlieb, J. (2007). From thought to action: the parietal cortex as a bridge between mundane on a moment-to-moment basis via direct and indirect amygdala
perception, action, and cognition. Neuron, 53(1), 9–16. contributions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United
Gregory, R. L. (1968). Perceptual illusions and brain models. Proceedings of the States of America, 106(39), 16841–16846.
Royal Society B-Biological Sciences, 171(1024), 279–296. Linetzky, M., Pergamin-Hight, L., Pine, D. S., & Bar-Haim, Y. (2015). Quantitative
Grupe, D. W., & Nitschke, J. B. (2013). Uncertainty and anticipation in anxiety: an evaluation of the clinical efficacy of attention bias modification treatment for
integrated neurobiological and psychological perspective. Nature Reviews anxiety disorders. Depression and Anxiety, 32(6), 383–391.
Neuroscience, 14(7), 488–501. Lipka, J., Miltner, W. H., & Straube, T. (2011). Vigilance for threat interacts with
Hadj-Bouziane, F., Liu, N., Bell, A. H., Gothard, K. M., Luh, W. M., Tootell, R. B., . . . & amygdala responses to subliminal threat cues in specific phobia. Biological
Ungerleider, L. G. (2012). Amygdala lesions disrupt modulation of functional Psychiatry, 70(5), 472–478.
MRI activity evoked by facial expression in the monkey inferior temporal LoBue, V. (2014). Deconstructing the snake: the relative roles of perception,
cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of cognition, and emotion on threat detection. Emotion, 14(4), 701–711.
America, 109(52), E3640–E3648. Lorberbaum, J. P., Kose, S., Johnson, M. R., Arana, G. W., Sullivan, L. K., Hamner, M.
Hahn, S., & Gronlund, S. D. (2007). Top-down guidance in visual search for facial B., . . . & George, M. S. (2004). Neural correlates of speech anticipatory anxiety
expressions. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(1), 159–165. in generalized social phobia. Neuroreport, 15(18), 2701–2705.
Hakamata, Y., Lissek, S., Bar-Haim, Y., Britton, J. C., Fox, N. A., Leibenluft, E., . . . & Lundqvist, D., & Ohman, A. (2005). Emotion regulates attention: the relation
Pine, D. S. (2010). Attention bias modification treatment: a meta-analysis between facial configurations, facial emotion, and visual attention. Visual
toward the establishment of novel treatment for anxiety. Biological Psychiatry, Cognition, 12(1), 51–84.
68(11), 982–990. Lundqvist, D., Esteves, F., & Ohman, A. (2004). The face of wrath: the role of
Hallion, L. S., & Ruscio, A. M. (2011). A meta-analysis of the effect of cognitive bias features and configurations in conveying social threat. Cognition & Emotion,
modification on anxiety and depression. Psychological Bulletin, 137(6), 18(2), 161–182.
940–958. MacLeod, C., & Clarke, P. J. F. (2015). The attentional bias modification approach to
Hansen, C. H., & Hansen, R. D. (1988). Finding the face in the crowd − an anger anxiety intervention. Clinical Psychological Science, 3(1), 58–78.
superiority effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 917–924. MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (1988). Anxiety and the allocation of attention to
Heekeren, H. R., Marrett, S., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2008). The neural systems that threat. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. A, Human Experimental
mediate human perceptual decision making. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(6), Psychology, 40(4), 653–670.
467–479. MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (2012). Cognitive bias modification approaches to
Hesselmann, G., Kell, C. A., Eger, E., & Kleinschmidt, A. (2008). Spontaneous local anxiety. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 8, 189–217.
variations in ongoing neural activity bias perceptual decisions. Proceedings of Markham, R., & Darke, S. (1991). The effects of anxiety on verbal and spatial
the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(31), task-performance. Australian Journal of Psychology, 43(2), 107–111.
10984–10989. Mather, M., & Sutherland, M. R. (2011). Arousal-biased competition in perception
Hesselmann, G., Sadaghiani, S., Friston, K. J., & Kleinschmidt, A. (2010). Predictive and memory. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(2), 114–133.
coding or evidence accumulation? False inference and neuronal fluctuations. Mather, M., Clewett, D., Sakaki, M., & Harley, C. W. (2015). Norepinephrine ignites
Public Library of Science, 5(3), e9926. local hot spots of neuronal excitation: how arousal amplifies selectivity in
Hesselmann, G., Kell, C. A., & Kleinschmidt, A. (2008). Ongoing activity fluctuations perception and memory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1–100.
in hMT+ bias the perception of coherent visual motion. Journal of Neuroscience, Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1994). Cognitive approaches to emotion and
28(53), 14481–14485. emotional disorders. Annual Review of Psychology, 45, 25–50.
Holland, P. C., & Gallagher, M. (1999). Amygdala circuitry in attentional and McAdams, C. J., & Maunsell, J. H. (1999). Effects of attention on orientation-tuning
representational processes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(2), 65–73. functions of single neurons in macaque cortical area V4. Journal of
Holmes, A., Green, S., & Vuilleumier, P. (2005). The involvement of distinct visual Neuroscience, 19(1), 431–441.
channels in rapid attention towards fearful facial expressions. Cognition McAlonan, K., Cavanaugh, J., & Wurtz, R. H. (2008). Guarding the gateway to cortex
Emotion, 19, 899–922. with attention in visual thalamus. Nature, 456(7220), 391–394.
Horstmann, G. (2007). Preattentive face processing: what do visual search McCoy, A. N., & Platt, M. L. (2005). Risk-sensitive neurons in macaque posterior
experiments with schematic faces tell us? Visual Cognition, 15(7), 799–833. cingulate cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 8(9), 1220–1227.
Ikeda, M., Iwanaga, M., & Seiwa, H. (1996). Test anxiety and working memory McCoy, A. N., Crowley, J. C., Haghighian, G., Dean, H. L., & Platt, M. L. (2003). Saccade
system. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 82(3), 1223–1231. reward signals in posterior cingulate cortex. Neuron, 40(5), 1031–1040.
Itti, L., & Koch, C. (2001). Computational modelling of visual attention. Nature McHugo, M., Olatunji, B. O., & Zald, D. H. (2013). The emotional attentional blink:
Reviews Neuroscience, 2(3), 194–203. what we know so far. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 151.
Jonides, J., & Yantis, S. (1998). Uniqueness of abrupt visual onset in capturing Mendez-Bertolo, C., Moratti, S., Toledano, R., Lopez-Sosa, F., Martinez-Alvarez, R.,
attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 43, 346–354. Mah, Y. H., . . . & Strange, B. A. (2016). A fast pathway for fear in human
amygdala. Nature Neuroscience, 19(8), 1041–1049.

Please cite this article in press as: Sussman, T. J., et al. Top-down and bottom-up factors in threat-related perception and attention in
anxiety. Biol. Psychol. (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.08.006
G Model
BIOPSY-7253; No. of Pages 13 ARTICLE IN PRESS
12 T.J. Sussman et al. / Biological Psychology xxx (2016) xxx–xxx

Mesulam, M. M., Van Hoesen, G. W., Pandya, D. N., & Geschwind, N. (1977). Limbic Pessoa, L., & Adolphs, R. (2010). Emotion processing and the amygdala: from a ‘low
and sensory connections of the inferior parietal lobule (area PG) in the rhesus road’ to ‘many roads’ of evaluating biological significance. Nature Reviews
monkey: a study with a new method for horseradish peroxidase Neuroscience, 11(11), 773–783.
histochemistry. Brain Research, 136(3), 393–414. Pessoa, L., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2005). Visual attention and emotional perception.
Mesulam, M. M. (1981). A cortical network for directed attention and unilateral In G. R. L. Itti, & J. K. Tsotsos (Eds.), Neurobiology of attention. San Diego, CA:
neglect. Annals of Neurology, 10(4), 309–325. Elsevier.
Mesulam, M. M. (1999). Spatial attention and neglect: parietal, frontal and Pessoa, L., Kastner, S., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2002). Attentional control of the
cingulate contributions to the mental representation and attentional targeting processing of neural and emotional stimuli. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain
of salient extrapersonal events. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of Research, 15(1), 31–45.
London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 354(1387), 1325–1346. Pessoa, L. (2008). On the relationship between emotion and cognition. Nature
Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (1999). Orienting of attention to threatening facial Reviews Neuroscience, 9(2), 148–158.
expressions presented under conditions of restricted awareness. Cognition & Pessoa, L. (2009). How do emotion and motivation direct executive control? Trends
Emotion, 13(6), 713–740. in Cognitive Sciences, 13(4), 160–166.
Mogg, K., Mathews, A., Bird, C., & Macgregor-Morris, R. (1990). Effects of stress and Pessoa, L. M., Gutierrez, E., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2002). Neural processing of
anxiety on the processing of threat stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social emotional faces requires attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Psychology, 59(6), 1230–1237. Sciences of the United States of America, 99(17), 11458–11463.
Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., & Hallowell, N. (1994). Attentional bias to threat: roles of Phan, K. L., Wager, T. D., Taylor, S. F., & Liberzon, I. (2004). Functional neuroimaging
trait anxiety, stressful events, and awareness. Quarterly Journal of Experimental studies of human emotions. CNS Spectrums, 9(4), 258–266.
Psychology. A, Human Experimental Psychology, 47(4), 841–864. Polat, U., & Sagi, D. (1994). Spatial interactions in human vision: from near to far
Mogg, K., Millar, N., & Bradley, B. P. (2000). Biases in eye movements to threatening via experience-dependent cascades of connections. Proceedings of the National
facial expressions in generalized anxiety disorder and depressive disorder. Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 91(4), 1206–1209.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109(4), 695–704. Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R., & Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of
Mogg, K., McNamara, J., Powys, M., Rawlinson, H., Seiffer, A., & Bradley, B. P. (2000). signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 109(2), 160–174.
Selective attention to threat: a test of two cognitive models of anxiety. Pourtois, G., Grandjean, D., Sander, D., & Vuilleumier, P. (2004).
Cognition Emotion, 14, 375–399. Electrophysiological correlates of rapid spatial orienting towards fearful faces.
Mohanty, A., & Sussman, T. J. (2013). Top-down modulation of attention by Cerebral Cortex, 14(6), 619–633.
emotion. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 102. Pourtois, G., Schettino, A., & Vuilleumier, P. (2013). Brain mechanisms for
Mohanty, A., Egner, T., Monti, J. M., & Mesulam, M. M. (2009). Search for a emotional influences on perception and attention: what is magic and what is
threatening target triggers limbic guidance of spatial attention. Journal of not. Biological Psychology, 92(3), 492–512.
Neuroscience, 29(34), 10563–10572. Prater, K. E., Hosanagar, A., Klumpp, H., Angstadt, M., & Phan, K. L. (2013). Aberrant
Morris, J. S., Ohman, A., & Dolan, R. J. (1998). Conscious and unconscious emotional amygdala-frontal cortex connectivity during perception of fearful faces and at
learning in the human amygdala. Nature, 393(6684), 467–470. rest in generalized social anxiety disorder. Depression and Anxiety, 30(3),
Morris, J. S., Friston, K. J., Buchel, C., Frith, C. D., Young, A. W., Calder, A. J., & Dolan, 234–241.
R. J. (1998). A neuromodulatory role for the human amygdala in processing Puri, A. M., Wojciulik, E., & Ranganath, C. (2009). Category expectation modulates
emotional facial expressions. Brain, 121(Pt. 1), 47–57. baseline and stimulus-evoked activity in human inferotemporal cortex. Brain
Motter, B. C. (1993). Focal attention produces spatially selective processing in Research, 1301, 89–99.
visual cortical areas V1, V2, and V4 in the presence of competing stimuli. Ratcliff, R., & Smith, P. L. (2004). A comparison of sequential sampling models for
Journal of Neurophysiology, 70(3), 909–919. two-choice reaction time. Psychological Review, 111(2), 333–367.
Mountcastle, V. B., Lynch, J. C., Georgopoulos, A., Sakata, H., & Acuna, C. (1975). Ratcliff, R. (1978). A theory of memory retrieval. Psychological Review, 85, 59–108.
Posterior parietal association cortex of the monkey: command functions for Ress, D., Backus, B. T., & Heeger, D. J. (2000). Activity in primary visual cortex
operations within extrapersonal space. Journal of Neurophysiology, 38(4), predicts performance in a visual detection task. Nature Neuroscience, 3(9),
871–908. 940–945.
Mukherjee, K. (2010). A dual system model of preferences under risk. Psychological Reynolds, J. H., Chelazzi, L., & Desimone, R. (1999). Competitive mechanisms
Review, 117(1), 243–255. subserve attention in macaque areas V2 and V4. The Journal of Neuroscience,
Nasrallah, M., Carmel, D., & Lavie, N. (2009). Murder, she wrote: enhanced 19(5), 1736–1753.
sensitivity to negative word valence. Emotion, 9(5), 609–618. Richards, A., French, C. C., Johnson, W., Naparstek, J., & Williams, J. (1992). Effects of
Neisser, U. (2014). Cognitive psychology: classic edition. New York, NY: Psychology mood manipulation and anxiety on performance of an emotional stroop task.
Press. British Journal of Psychology, 83, 479–491.
New, J., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2007). Category-specific attention for animals Richards, J. C., Austin, D. W., & Alvarenga, M. E. (2001). Interpretation of ambiguous
reflects ancestral priorities, not expertise. Proceedings of the National Academy interoceptive stimuli in panic disorder and nonclinical panic. Cognitive Therapy
of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(42), 16598–16603. and Research, 25(3), 235–246.
Nienborg, H., & Cumming, B. G. (2009). Decision-related activity in sensory Robinson, O. J., Letkiewicz, A. M., Overstreet, C., Ernst, M., & Grillon, C. (2011). The
neurons reflects more than a neuron’s causal effect. Nature, 459(7243), 89–92. effect of induced anxiety on cognition: threat of shock enhances aversive
Nitschke, J. B., Sarinopoulos, I., Oathes, D. J., Johnstone, T., Whalen, P. J., Davidson, processing in healthy individuals. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral
R. J., & Kalin, N. H. (2009). Anticipatory activation in the amygdala and anterior Neuroscience, 11(2), 217–227.
cingulate in generalized anxiety disorder and prediction of treatment Robinson, O. J., Charney, D. R., Overstreet, C., Vytal, K., & Grillon, C. (2012). The
response. American Journal of Psychiatry, 166(3), 302–310. adaptive threat bias in anxiety: amygdala-dorsomedial prefrontal cortex
Nummenmaa, L., Hyona, J., & Calvo, M. G. (2009). Emotional scene content drives coupling and aversive amplification. Neuroimage, 60(1), 523–529.
the saccade generation system reflexively. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Robinson, O. J., Vytal, K., Cornwell, B. R., & Grillon, C. (2013). The impact of anxiety
Human Perception and Performance, 35(2), 305–323. upon cognition: perspectives from human threat of shock studies. Frontiers in
O’Connor, D. H., Fukui, M. M., Pinsk, M. A., & Kastner, S. (2002). Attention Human Neuroscience, 7.
modulates responses in the human lateral geniculate nucleus. Nature Roelfsema, P. R., Lamme, V. A. F., & Spekreijse, H. (1998). Object-based attention in
Neuroscience, 5(11), 1203–1209. the primary visual cortex of the macaque monkey. Nature, 395(6700), 376–381.
Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2005). The cognitive control of emotion. Trends in Russell, J. A. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion.
Cognitive Sciences, 9(5), 242–249. Psychological Review, 110(1), 145–172.
Ochsner, K. N., Bunge, S. A., Gross, J. J., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2002). Rethinking feelings: Sakai, K., & Miyashita, Y. (1991). Neural organization for the long-term memory of
an FMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emotion. Journal of Cognitive paired associates. Nature, 354(6349), 152–155.
Neuroscience, 14(8), 1215–1229. Sakai, Y., Kumano, H., Nishikawa, M., Sakano, Y., Kaiya, H., Imabayashi, E., . . . &
Ochsner, K. N., Ray, R. D., Cooper, J. C., Robertson, E. R., Chopra, S., Gabrieli, J. D., & Kuboki, T. (2005). Cerebral glucose metabolism associated with a fear network
Gross, J. J. (2004). For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the in panic disorder. Neuroreport, 16(9), 927–931.
cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion. Neuroimage, 23(2), Sander, D., Grafman, J., & Zalla, T. (2003). The human amygdala: an evolved system
483–499. for relevance detection. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 14(4), 303–316.
Ohman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: detecting the Schupp, H. T., Ohman, A., Junghofer, M., Weike, A. I., Stockburger, J., & Hamm, A. O.
snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 130(3), 466–478. (2004). The facilitated processing of threatening faces: an ERP analysis.
Ohman, A. (2002). Automaticity and the amygdala: nonconscious responses to Emotion, 4(2), 189–200.
emotional faces. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(2), 62–66. Seligman, M. E. (1971). Phobias and preparedness. Behavior Therapy, 2(3), 307–320.
Ohman, A. (2005). The role of the amygdala in human fear: automatic detection of Semple, W. E., Goyer, P. F., McCormick, R., Donovan, B., Muzic, R. F., Jr., Rugle, L., . . .
threat. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30(10), 953–958. & Schulz, S. C. (2000). Higher brain blood flow at amygdala and lower frontal
Palmer, S. E. (1975). Effects of contextual scenes on identification of objects. cortex blood flow in PTSD patients with comorbid cocaine and alcohol abuse
Memory & Cognition, 3(5), 519–526. compared with normals. Psychiatry, 63(1), 65–74.
Paulus, M. P., & Yu, A. J. (2012). Emotion and decision-making: affect-driven belief Shackman, A. J., Maxwell, J. S., McMenamin, B. W., Greischar, L. L., & Davidson, R. J.
systems in anxiety and depression. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(9), 476–483. (2011). Stress potentiates early and attenuates late stages of visual processing.
Peelen, M. V., Fei-Fei, L., & Kastner, S. (2009). Neural mechanisms of rapid natural Journal of Neuroscience, 31(3), 1156–1161.
scene categorization in human visual cortex. Nature, 460(7251), 94–U105. Shin, L. M., Wright, C. I., Cannistraro, P. A., Wedig, M. M., McMullin, K., Martis, B., . . .
& Rauch, S. L. (2005). A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of
amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex responses to overtly presented fearful

Please cite this article in press as: Sussman, T. J., et al. Top-down and bottom-up factors in threat-related perception and attention in
anxiety. Biol. Psychol. (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.08.006
G Model
BIOPSY-7253; No. of Pages 13 ARTICLE IN PRESS
T.J. Sussman et al. / Biological Psychology xxx (2016) xxx–xxx 13

faces in posttraumatic stress disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(3), Van Bockstaele, B., Verschuere, B., Tibboel, H., De Houwer, J., Crombez, G., & Koster,
273–281. E. H. (2014). A review of current evidence for the causal impact of attentional
Shulman, G. L., McAvoy, M. P., Cowan, M. C., Astafiev, S. V., Tansy, A. P., d’Avossa, G., bias on fear and anxiety. Psychological Bulletin, 140(3), 682–721.
& Corbetta, M. (2003). Quantitative analysis of attention and detection signals Vuilleumier, P., & Driver, J. (2007). Modulation of visual processing by attention
during visual search. Journal of Neurophysiology, 90(5), 3384–3397. and emotion: windows on causal interactions between human brain regions.
Shulman, G. L., Astafiev, S. V., McAvoy, M. P., d’Avossa, G., & Corbetta, M. (2007). Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological
Right TPJ deactivation during visual search: functional significance and Sciences, 362(1481), 837–855.
support for a filter hypothesis. Cerebral Cortex, 17(11), 2625–2633. Vuilleumier, P., & Pourtois, G. (2007). Distributed and interactive brain
Smith, N. K., Cacioppo, J. T., Larsen, J. T., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). May I have your mechanisms during emotion face perception: evidence from functional
attention, please: electrocortical responses to positive and negative stimuli. neuroimaging. Neuropsychologia, 45(1), 174–194.
Neuropsychologia, 41(2), 171–183. Vuilleumier, P., Armony, J. L., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2001). Effects of attention and
Sreenivasan, K. K., Sambhara, D., & Jha, A. P. (2011). Working memory templates emotion on face processing in the human brain: an event-related fMRI study.
are maintained as feature-specific perceptual codes. Journal of Neurophysiology, Neuron, 30(3), 829–841.
106(1), 115–121. Vuilleumier, P., Armony, J. L., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2003). Distinct spatial
Stefanics, G., Csukly, G., Komlosi, S., Czobor, P., & Czigler, I. (2012). Processing of frequency sensitivities for processing faces and emotional expressions. Nature
unattended facial emotions: a visual mismatch negativity study. Neuroimage, Neuroscience, 6(6), 624–631.
59(3), 3042–3049. Vuilleumier, P., Richardson, M. P., Armony, J. L., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2004).
Stein, T., Seymour, K., Hebart, M. N., & Sterzer, P. (2014). Rapid fear detection relies Distant influences of amygdala lesion on visual cortical activation during
on high spatial frequencies. Psychological Science, 25(2), 566–574. emotional face processing. Nature Neuroscience, 7(11), 1271–1278.
Stephan, K. E., Hilgetag, C. C., Burns, G. A., O’Neill, M. A., Young, M. P., & Kotter, R. Vuilleumier, P. (2005). How brains beware: neural mechanisms of emotional
(2000). Computational analysis of functional connectivity between areas of attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(12), 585–594.
primate cerebral cortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of Wald, I., Lubin, G., Holoshitz, Y., Muller, D., Fruchter, E., Pine, D. S., . . . & Bar-Haim,
London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 355(1393), 111–126. Y. (2011). Battlefield-like stress following simulated combat and suppression
Stokes, M., Thompson, R., Nobre, A. C., & Duncan, J. (2009). Shape-specific of attention bias to threat. Psychological Medicine, 41(4), 699–707.
preparatory activity mediates attention to targets in human visual cortex. Wald, I., Bitton, S., Levi, O., Zusmanovich, S., Fruchter, E., Ginat, K., . . . & Bar-Haim,
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, Y. (2016). Acute delivery of attention bias modification training (ABMT)
106(46), 19569–19574. moderates the association between combat exposure and posttraumatic
Stormark, K. M., & Hugdahl, K. (1996). Peripheral cuing of covert spatial attention symptoms: a feasibility study. Biological Psychology.
before and after emotional conditioning of the cue. International Journal of Wald, I., Shechner, T., Bitton, S., Holoshitz, Y., Charney, D. S., Muller, D., . . . &
Neuroscience, 86(3–4), 225–240. Bar-Haim, Y. (2011). Attention bias away from threat during life threatening
Stormark, K. M., & Hugdahl, K. (1997). Conditioned emotional cueing of spatial danger predicts ptsd symptoms at one-year follow-up. Depression and Anxiety,
attentional shifts in a go/no-go RT task. International Journal of 28(5), 406–411.
Psychophysiology, 27(3), 241–248. Whalen, P. J., Rauch, S. L., Etcoff, N. L., McInerney, S. C., Lee, M. B., & Jenike, M. A.
Stout, D. M., Shackman, A. J., & Larson, C. L. (2013). Failure to filter: anxious (1998). Masked presentations of emotional facial expressions modulate
individuals show inefficient gating of threat from working memory of threat amygdala activity without explicit knowledge. The Journal of Neuroscience,
from working memory. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7. 18(1), 411–418.
Straube, T., Mentzel, H. J., & Miltner, W. H. (2005). Common and distinct brain Williams, J. M. G., Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional stroop task
activation to threat and safety signals in social phobia. Neuropsychobiology, and psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 120(1), 3–24.
52(3), 163–168. Williams, M. A., Moss, S. A., Bradshaw, J. L., & Mattingley, J. B. (2005). Look at me,
Sugrue, L. P., Corrado, G. S., & Newsome, W. T. (2004). Matching behavior and the I’m smiling: visual search for threatening and nonthreatening facial
representation of value in the parietal cortex. Science, 304(5678), 1782–1787. expressions. Visual Cognition, 12(1), 29–50.
Summerfield, C., & Egner, T. (2009). Expectation (and attention) in visual cognition. Wolfe, J. M., Butcher, S. J., Lee, C., & Hyle, M. (2003). Changing your mind: on the
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(9), 403–409. contributions of top-down and bottom-up guidance in visual search for feature
Summerfield, C., & de Lange, F. P. (2014). Expectation in perceptual decision singletons. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and
making: neural and computational mechanisms. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, Performance, 29(2), 483–502.
15(11), 745–756. Womelsdorf, T., Anton-Erxleben, K., & Treue, S. (2008). Receptive field shift and
Summerfield, C., Egner, T., Greene, M., Koechlin, E., Mangels, J., & Hirsch, J. (2006). shrinkage in macaque middle temporal area through attentional gain
Predictive codes for forthcoming perception in the frontal cortex. Science, modulation. Journal of Neuroscience, 28(36), 8934–8944.
314(5803), 1311–1314. Woodward, S. A., McManis, M. H., Kagan, J., Deldin, P., Snidman, N., Lewis, M., &
Sussman, T. J., Weinberg, A., Szekely, A., Hajcak, G., & Mohanty, A. (2016). Here Kahn, V. (2001). Infant temperament and the brainstem auditory evoked
comes trouble: prestimulus brain activity predicts enhanced perception of response in later childhood. Developmental Psychology, 37(4), 533–538.
threat. Cerebral Cortex. Wormwood, J. B., Lynn, S. K., Feldman Barrett, L., & Quigley, K. S. (2016). Threat
Sussman, T. J., Szekely, A., Hajcak, G., & Mohanty, A. (2016). It’s all in the perception after the Boston Marathon bombings: the effects of personal
anticipation: how perception of threat is enhanced in anxiety. Emotion, 16(3), relevance and conceptual framing. Cognition & Emotion, 30(3), 539–549.
320–327. Yantis, S., & Jonides, J. (1984). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention:
Swanson, L. W. (2003). The amygdala and its place in the cerebral hemisphere. evidence from visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 985, 174–184. Perception & Performance, 10(5), 601–621.
Tanaka, K. (1996). Inferotemporal cortex and object vision. Annual Review of Yiend, J., & Mathews, A. (2001). Anxiety and attention to threatening pictures.
Neuroscience, 19, 109–139. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. A, Human Experimental Psychology,
Theeuwes, J. (1991). Exogenous and endogenous control of attention: the effect of 54(3), 665–681.
visual onsets and offsets. Perception and Psychophysics, 49(1), 83–90. Yoon, K. L., & Zinbarg, R. E. (2008). Interpreting neutral faces as threatening is a
Thompson, K. G., & Bichot, N. P. (2005). A visual salience map in the primate frontal default mode for socially anxious individuals. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
eye field. Progress in Brain Research, 147, 251–262. 117(3), 680–685.
Tipples, J., Atkinson, A. P., & Young, A. W. (2002). The eyebrow frown: a salient Young, M. P., Scannell, J. W., Burns, G. A., & Blakemore, C. (1994). Analysis of
social signal. Emotion, 2(3), 288–296. connectivity: neural systems in the cerebral cortex. Reviews in the
Todd, J. J., Fougnie, D., & Marois, R. (2005). Visual short-term memory load Neurosciences, 5(3), 227–250.
suppresses temporo-parietal junction activity and induces inattentional Zeelenberg, R., Wagenmakers, E. J., & Rotteveel, M. (2006). The impact of emotion
blindness. Psychological Science, 16(12), 965–972. on perception: bias or enhanced processing? Psychological Science, 17(4),
Tsotsos, J. K. (1990). Analyzing vision at the complexity level. Behavioral and Brain 287–291.
Sciences, 13, 423–469. Zelano, C., Mohanty, A., & Gottfried, J. A. (2011). Olfactory predictive codes and
Tsotsos, J. K. (1997). Limited capacity of any realizable perceptual system is a stimulus templates in piriform cortex. Neuron, 72(1), 178–187.
sufficient reason for attentive behavior. Consciousness and Cognition, 6(2/3),
429–436.

Please cite this article in press as: Sussman, T. J., et al. Top-down and bottom-up factors in threat-related perception and attention in
anxiety. Biol. Psychol. (2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.08.006

You might also like