You are on page 1of 40

DOI: 10.4073/csr.2019.

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW

Effects of bystander programs on the prevention of sexual


assault among adolescents and college students: A systematic
review

Heather Hensman Kettrey 1 | Robert A. Marx 2 | Emily E. Tanner‐Smith3

1
Department of Sociology, Anthropology, and Criminal Justice, Clemson University, Clemson, South Carolina
2
Department of Child and Adolescent Development, Lurie College, San Jose State University, San Jose, California
3
Department of Counseling Psychology and Human Services, University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon

Correspondence
Heather Hensman Kettrey, PhD, Department of Sociology, Anthropology, and Criminal Justice, Clemson University, 132 Brackett Hall, Clemson, SC 29634‐1356.
Email: hkettre@clemson.edu

1 | PLA I N L ANGUAGE S UMMA RY

1.1 | Bystander programs increase bystander What is the aim of this review?
intervention but no effect on perpetrating sexual This Campbell systematic review examines the effects of
assault bystander programs on knowledge and attitudes concern-
ing sexual assault and bystander intervention, bystander
Bystander sexual assault prevention programs have beneficial effects
intervention when witnessing sexual assault or its warning
on bystander intervention but there is no evidence of effects on
signs, and the perpetration of sexual assault. The review
sexual assault perpetration. Effects on knowledge and attitudes are
summarizes evidence from 27 high‐quality studies, includ-
inconsistent across outcomes.
ing 21 randomized controlled trials.

1.2 | What is this review about?


2 | W HAT ARE TH E MA IN F IN D IN GS OF
Sexual assault is a significant problem among adolescents and
THIS REVIEW?
college students across the world. One promising strategy for
preventing these assaults is the implementation of bystander
2.1 | What studies are included?
sexual assault prevention programs, which encourage young
people to intervene when witnessing incidents or warning signs This review includes studies that evaluate the effects of bystander
of sexual assault. This review examines the effects bystander programs for young people on (a) knowledge and attitudes concerning
programs have on knowledge and attitudes concerning sexual sexual assault and bystander intervention, (b) bystander intervention
assault and bystander behavior, bystander intervention when behavior when witnessing sexual assault or its warning signs, and (c)
witnessing sexual assault or its warning signs, and participants’ perpetration of sexual assault. Twenty‐seven studies met the inclusion
rates of perpetration of sexual assault. criteria. These included studies span the period from 1997 to 2017 and

---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in any medium,
provided the original work is properly cited.
© 2019 The Authors. Campbell Systematic Reviews published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of The Campbell Collaboration

Campbell Systematic Reviews. 2019;15:e1013. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/cl2 | 1 of 40


https://doi.org/10.4073/csr.2019.1
2 of 40 | KETTREY ET AL.

were primarily conducted in the USA (one study was conducted in 5 | EXECUTIVE S UMMARY/ABSTRACT
Canada and one in India). Twenty‐one studies were randomized
controlled trials and six were high quality quasi‐experimental studies. 5.1 | Background
5.1.1 | Sexual assault among adolescents and
2.2 | Do bystander programs have an effect on college students
knowledge/attitudes, on bystander intervention, or
on sexual assault perpetration? Sexual assault is a significant problem among adolescents and college
students in the United States and globally. Findings from the Campus
Bystander programs have an effect on knowledge and attitudes for
Sexual Assault study estimated that 15.9% of college women had
some outcomes. The most pronounced beneficial effects are on rape
experienced attempted or completed sexual assault (i.e., unwanted
myth acceptance and bystander efficacy outcomes. There are also
sexual contact that could include sexual touching, oral sex,
delayed effects (i.e., 1 to 4 months after the intervention) on taking
intercourse, anal sex, or penetration with a finger or object) prior
responsibility for intervening/acting, knowing strategies for inter-
to entering college and 19% had experienced attempted or
vening, and intentions to intervene outcomes. There is little or no
completed sexual assault since entering college (Krebs, Lindquist,
evidence of effects on gender attitudes, victim empathy, date rape
Warner, Fisher, & Martin, 2009). Similar rates have been reported in
attitudes, and on noticing sexual assault outcomes.
Australia (Australian Human Rights Commission, 2017), Chile
Bystander programs have a beneficial effect on bystander
(Lehrer, Lehrer, & Koss, 2013), China (Su, Hao, Huang, Xiao, & Tao,
intervention. There is no evidence that bystander programs have
2011), Finland (Bjorklund, Hakkanen‐Nyholm, Huttunen, & Kunttu,
an effect on participants’ rates of sexual assault perpetration.
2010), Poland (Tomaszewska & Krahe, 2018), Rwanda (Van Decraen,
Michielsen, Herbots, Van Rossem, & Temmerman, 2012), Spain
(Vázquez, Torres, & Otero, 2012), and in a global survey of countries
3 | W H A T D O T H E FI N D I N G S O F TH I S
in Africa, Asia, and the Americas (Pengpid & Peltzer, 2016).
REVIEW MEAN ?

The United States 2013 Campus Sexual Violence Elimination (SaVE) 5.1.2 | The bystander approach
Act requires postsecondary educational institutions participating in
One promising strategy for preventing sexual assault among
Title IX financial aid programs to provide incoming college students
adolescents and young adults is the implementation of bystander
with sexual violence prevention programming that includes a
programs, which encourage young people to intervene when
component on bystander intervention.
witnessing incidents or warning signs of sexual assault. Bystander
Bystander programs have a significant effect on bystander
programs seek to sensitize young people to warning signs of sexual
intervention. But there is no evidence that these programs have an
assault, create attitudinal changes that foster bystander responsi-
effect on rates of sexual assault perpetration. This suggests that
bility for intervening (e.g., creating empathy for victims), and build
bystander programs may be appropriate for targeting the behavior of
requisite skills and knowledge of tactics for taking action (Banyard,
potential bystanders but may not be appropriate for targeting the
2011; Banyard, Plante, & Moynihan, 2004; Burn, 2009; McMahon &
behavior of potential perpetrators.
Banyard, 2012). Many of these programs are implemented with large
Beneficial effects of bystander programs on bystander interven-
groups of adolescents or college students in the format of a single
tion were diminished by 6 months post‐intervention. Thus, booster
training/education session (e.g., as part of college orientation).
sessions may be needed to yield any sustained effects.
However, some programs use broader implementation strategies,
There are still important questions worth further exploration.
such as advertising campaigns that post signs across college
Namely, more research is needed to investigate the underlying causal
campuses to encourage students to act when witnessing signs of
mechanisms of program effects on bystander behavior (e.g., to model
violence.
relationships between specific knowledge/attitude effects and bystan-
By treating young people as potential allies in preventing sexual
der intervention effects), and to identify the most effective types of
assault, bystander programs have the capacity to be less threatening
bystander programs (e.g., using randomized controlled trials to compare
than traditional sexual assault prevention programs, which tend to
the effects of two alternate program models). Additionally, more
address young people as either potential perpetrators or victims of
research is needed in contexts outside of the USA so that researchers
sexual violence (Burn, 2009; Messner, 2015; [Jackson] Katz, 1995).
can better understand the role of bystander programs across the world.
Instead of placing emphasis on how young people may modify their
individual behavior to either respect the sexual boundaries of others
4 | HOW UP ‐TO ‐D A T E I S T H IS R E V IE W ? or reduce their personal risk for being sexually assaulted, bystander
programs aim to foster prerequisite knowledge and skills for
The review authors searched for studies up to June 2017. This intervening on behalf of potential victims. Thus, by treating young
Campbell systematic review was submitted in October 2017, revised people as part of the solution to sexual assault, rather than part of
in October 2018, and published January 2019. the problem, bystander programs may limit the risk of defensiveness
KETTREY ET AL. | 3 of 40

or backlash among participants (e.g., decreased empathy for victims, 5.4.2 | Types of participants
increased rape myth acceptance; Banyard et al., 2004; Katz, 1995).
The review focused on studies that examined outcomes of bystander
programs that targeted sexual assault and were implemented with
5.2 | Objectives adolescents and/or college students in educational settings. Eligible
participants included adolescents enrolled in grades 7 through 12
The overall objective of this systematic review and meta‐analysis was
and college students enrolled in any type of undergraduate
to examine what effects bystander programs have on preventing
postsecondary educational institution. The mean age of samples
sexual assault among adolescents and college students. More
could be no less than age 12 and no greater than age 25.
specifically, this review addressed three objectives.

1. The first objective was to assess the overall effects (including


5.4.3 | Types of interventions
adverse effects), and the variability of the effects, of bystander
programs on adolescents’ and college students’ attitudes and Eligible intervention programs were those that approached partici-
behaviors regarding sexual assault. pants as allies in preventing and/or alleviating sexual assault among
2. The second objective was to explore the comparative effective- adolescents and/or college students. Some part of the program had
ness of bystander programs for different profiles of participants to focus on ways that cultivate willingness for a person to respond to
(e.g., mean age of the sample, education level of the sample, others who are at risk for sexual assault. All delivery formats were
proportion of males/females in the sample, proportion of eligible for inclusion (e.g., in‐person training sessions, video programs,
fraternity/sorority members in the sample, proportion of athletic web‐based training, and advertising/poster campaigns). There were
team members in the sample). no intervention duration criteria for inclusion.
3. The third objective was to explore the comparative effective- Eligible comparison groups must have received no intervention
ness of different bystander programs in terms of gendered services targeting bystander attitudes/behavior or sexual assault.
content and approach (e.g., conceptualizing sexual assault as a
gendered or gender‐neutral problem, mixed‐ or single‐sex
group implementation). 5.4.4 | Types of outcome measures
We included studies that measured the effects of bystander
programs on at least one of the following primary outcome domains:
5.3 | Search methods
1. General attitudes toward sexual assault and victims (e.g., victim
Candidate studies were identified through searches of electronic
empathy, rape myth acceptance).
databases, relevant academic journals, and gray literature sources.
2. Prerequisite skills and knowledge for bystander intervention as
Gray literature searches included contacting leading authors and
defined by Burn (2009) (e.g., noticing sexual assault or its warning
experts on bystander programs to identify any current/ongoing
signs, identifying a situation as appropriate for intervention,
research that might be eligible for the review, screening bibliogra-
taking responsibility for acting/intervening, and knowing strate-
phies of eligible studies and relevant reviews to identify additional
gies for helping/intervening).
candidate studies, and conducting forward citation searches
3. Self‐efficacy with regard to bystander intervention (e.g., respon-
(searches for reports citing eligible studies) using the website Google
dents’ confidence in their ability to intervene).
Scholar.
4. Intentions to intervene when witnessing instances or warning
signs of sexual assault.
5.4 | Selection criteria 5. Actual intervention behavior when witnessing instances or
warning signs of sexual assault.
To be included in the review studies had to meet eligibility criteria in
6. Perpetration of sexual assault (i.e., participants’ rates of perpe-
the following domains: types of study, types of participants, types of
tration).
interventions, types of outcome measures, duration of follow‐up, and
types of settings.

5.4.1 | Types of studies 5.4.5 | Duration of follow‐up


To be eligible for inclusion in the review, studies must have used an Studies reporting follow‐ups of any duration were eligible for
experimental or controlled quasi‐experimental research design to inclusion. When studies reported outcomes at more than one
compare an intervention group (i.e., students assigned to a bystander follow‐up wave, each wave was coded and identified by its
program) with a comparison group (i.e., students not assigned to a reported duration. Follow‐ups of similar durations were analyzed
bystander program). together.
4 of 40 | KETTREY ET AL.

5.4.6 | Types of settings 5.6 | Results


The review focused on studies that examined outcomes of 5.6.1 | Objective 1: Effects on knowledge, attitudes,
bystander programs that targeted sexual assault and were and behavior
implemented with adolescents and/or college students in educa-
Knowledge/Attitudes
tional settings. Eligible educational settings included secondary
Effects for knowledge and attitude outcomes varied widely across
schools (i.e., grades 7–12) and colleges or universities. There were
constructs. The most pronounced beneficial effect in this domain was
no geographic limitations on inclusion criteria. Research con-
on rape myth acceptance. The effect for this outcome was immediate
ducted in any country was eligible.
and sustained across all reported follow‐up waves (i.e., from
immediate posttest to 6‐ to 7‐month post‐intervention). Intervention
5.5 | Data collection and analysis effects on bystander efficacy were also fairly pronounced, with an
effect observed at both immediate posttest and 1‐ to 4‐month post‐
5.5.1 | Selection of studies
intervention. A significant effect was not observed for this outcome
Once candidate studies were identified, two reviewers independently 6 months post‐intervention; however, this should be interpreted with
screened each study title and abstract for eligibility; disagreements caution, as only one study reported bystander efficacy effects at this
between reviewers were resolved by discussion and consensus. follow‐up period.
Potentially eligible studies were then retrieved in full text, and these Effects on other knowledge and attitude outcomes were either
full texts were reviewed for eligibility, again using two independent delayed or unobserved. Intervention effects on taking responsibility
reviewers. for intervening or acting, knowing strategies for intervening, and
intentions to intervene were nonsignificant at immediate posttest,
but significant and beneficial by 1‐ to 4‐month post‐intervention. We
5.5.2 | Data extraction and management found limited or no evidence of significant intervention effects on

Two reviewers independently double‐coded all included studies, gender attitudes, victim empathy, date rape attitudes, and noticing

using a piloted codebook. Coding disagreements were resolved via sexual assault.

discussion and consensus. The primary categories for coding were as


follows: participant demographics and characteristics (e.g., age, Behavior
gender, education level, race/ethnicity, athletic team membership, The results indicated that bystander programs have a beneficial

fraternity/sorority membership); intervention setting (e.g., state, effect on bystander intervention behavior. However, this effect,

country, secondary or postsecondary institution, mixed‐ or single‐ which was observed at 1‐ to 4‐month post‐intervention, was not

sex group); study characteristics (e.g., attrition, duration of follow‐up, statistically significant at 6 months post‐intervention. Bystander

study design, participant dose, sample N); outcome construct (e.g., programs did not have a significant effect on sexual assault

type, description of measure); and outcome results (e.g., timing at perpetration.

measurement, baseline, and follow‐up means and standard deviations


or proportions). 5.6.2 | Objective 2: Effects for different participant
profiles

5.5.3 | Measures of treatment effect We had planned to conduct moderator analyses to assess a wide
range of participant characteristics as potential effect size modera-
During the coding process, relevant summary statistics (e.g., means
tors. However, our review only yielded a sufficient number of studies
and standard deviations, proportions, observed sample sizes) were
(n ≥ 10) to conduct moderator analyses for the bystander
extracted from research reports to calculate effect sizes. Effect sizes
intervention outcome domain. The results indicated that mean age,
were reported as standardized mean differences (SMD), adjusted for
education level, and proportion of males/female were not statistically
small sample size (Hedges’ g). Positive effect size values (i.e., greater
significant predictors of the magnitude of intervention effects.
than 0) indicate a beneficial outcome for the bystander intervention
group.
5.6.3 | Objective 3: Effects based on gendered
content/implementation
5.5.4 | Data synthesis
We conducted moderator analyses to assess any differential effects
Intervention effects for each outcome construct were synthesized via of bystander programs on measured outcomes based on (a) the
meta‐analyses using random‐effects inverse variance weights. All gender of perpetrators and victims in specific bystander programs
statistical analyses were conducted with the metafor package in R. and (b) whether programs were implemented in mixed‐ or single‐sex
Synthesis results are displayed using forest plots. Mean effect sizes settings. Our review only produced a sufficient number of studies
are reported with their 95% confidence intervals. (n ≥ 10) to conduct such moderator analyses for the bystander
KETTREY ET AL. | 5 of 40

intervention outcome domain. We found that neither of these effects on bystander intervention behavior. These results cast
measures was a significant predictor of the effectiveness of uncertainty on the proposed relationship between knowledge/
bystander programs on bystander intervention. attitudes and bystander behavior. However, our methods do not
permit any formal evaluation of this relationship. The field’s under-
5.7 | Authors’ conclusions standing of the causal mechanisms of program effects on bystander
behavior would benefit from further analysis (e.g., path analysis
5.7.1 | Implications for practice and policy
mapping relationships between specific knowledge/attitude effects
The overwhelming majority of eligible studies assessing the effects of and bystander intervention).
bystander programs were conducted in the United States. This is not Second, bystander programs exhibit a great deal of content
necessarily surprising considering that the United States has variability, most notably in framing sexual assault as a gendered or
implemented public policy that encourages the implementation of gender‐neutral problem. That is, bystander programs tend to adopt
such programs on college campuses. The United States 2013 Campus one of two main approaches to addressing sexual assault: (a) they
SaVE Act requires postsecondary educational institutions participat- present sexual assault as a gendered problem (overwhelmingly
ing in Title IX financial aid programs to provide incoming college affecting women) or (b) they present sexual assault as a gender‐
students with primary prevention and awareness programs addres- neutral problem (affecting women and men alike). Differential effects
sing sexual violence. The Campus SaVE Act mandates that these of these two types of programs remain largely unexamined. Our
programs include a component on bystander intervention. Currently, analysis indicated that (a) the sex of victims/perpetrators presented
there is no comparable legislation regarding sexual assault among in interventions (i.e., portrayed in programs as gender neutral or male
adolescents (e.g., mandating bystander programs in secondary perpetrator and female victim) and (b) whether programs were
schools). implemented in mixed‐ or single‐sex settings were not significant
Findings from this review indicate that bystander programs have predictors of program effects on bystander intervention. However,
beneficial effects on bystander intervention behavior. This review these findings are limited to a single outcome and they should be
therefore provides important evidence of the effectiveness of considered preliminary, as they are based on a small sample (n = 11).
mandated programs on college campuses. Additionally, results from The field’s understanding of the differential effects of gendered
the moderator analyses indicated that the effects on bystander versus gender neutral programs would benefit from the design and
intervention are similar for adolescents and college students, which implementation of high‐quality primary studies that make direct
suggests that early implementation of bystander programs (i.e., in comparisons between these two types of programs (e.g., randomized
secondary schools with adolescents) may be warranted. controlled trials [RCTs] comparing the effects of two active
Importantly, although we found that bystander programs had a treatment arms that differ in their gendered approach).
significant effect on bystander intervention behavior, there was no Finally, as previously noted, all but two eligible studies were
evidence that these programs had an effect on participants’ sexual conducted in the United States. Thus, high‐quality studies conducted
assault perpetration. Although most bystander sexual assault outside of the United States are needed to provide a global
prevention programs aim to shift attitudes in the hopes of preventing perspective on the efficacy of bystander programs.
sexual assault perpetration, this review provided no evidence that
these programs decrease participants’ perpetration rates. This
suggests that bystander programs may be appropriate for targeting 6 | BACKGROUND
bystander behavior but may not be appropriate for targeting the
behavior of potential perpetrators. Additionally, effects of bystander 6.1 | The problem, condition, or issue
programs on bystander intervention diminished 6‐month post‐
6.1.1 | Sexual assault among adolescents and
intervention. Thus, booster sessions may be needed to yield any
college students
sustained intervention effects.
Sexual assault is a significant problem among adolescents and college
students in the United States and globally. Findings from the Campus
5.7.2 | Implications for research
Sexual Assault study estimated that 15.9% of American college
Findings from this review suggest there is a fairly strong body of women had experienced attempted or completed sexual assault (i.e.,
research assessing the effects of bystander programs on attitudes unwanted sexual contact that could include sexual touching, oral sex,
and behaviors. However, there are important questions worth intercourse, anal sex, or penetration with a finger or object) prior to
further exploration. entering college and 19% had experienced attempted or completed
First, according to one prominent logic model, bystander sexual assault since entering college (Krebs et al., 2009).
programs promote bystander intervention by fostering prerequisite Similar rates have been reported in Australia (Australian Human
knowledge and attitudes (Burn, 2009). Our meta‐analysis provides Rights Commission, 2017), Chile (Lehrer et al., 2013), China (Su et al.,
inconsistent evidence of the effects of bystander programs on 2011), Finland (Bjorklund et al., 2010), Poland (Tomaszewska & Krahe,
knowledge and attitudes, but promising evidence of short‐term 2018), Rwanda (Van Decraen et al., 2012), Spain (Vázquez et al., 2012),
6 of 40 | KETTREY ET AL.

and in a global survey of countries in Africa, Asia, and the Americas when they do seek help they often seek it from their peers (Ashley &
(Pengpid & Peltzer, 2016). Foshee, 2005; Black, Tolman, Callahan, Saunders, & Weisz, 2008;
These rates are problematic, as sexual assault in adolescence Molidor & Tolman, 1998; Weisz, Tolman, Callahan, Saunders, & Black,
and/or young adulthood is associated with numerous adverse 2007). Victims may trust their peers to provide a valuable source of
outcomes, including risk of repeated victimization, depressive support after an assault has occurred, and as such, peers have the
symptomology, heavy drinking, and suicidal ideation (Exner‐ Cortens, potential to play a pivotal role in the prevention of sexual assault by
Eckenrode, & Rothman, 2013; Cui, Ueno, Gordon, & Fincham, 2013; intervening when they witness its warning signs. In fact, in a
Halpern, Spriggs, Martin, & Kupper, 2009). Importantly, there is contemporary “hookup culture” adolescents and young adults are
evidence that indicates that experiences of sexual assault during more likely to meet and socialize in groups than they are to date in
these two life phases are related as victimization and perpetration pairs and, therefore, warning signs of assault are frequently exhibited
during adolescence, respectively, associated with increased risk of in communal spaces (Bogle, 2007; 2008; England & Ronen, 2015;
victimization and perpetration during young adulthood (Cui et al., Molidor & Tolman, 1998; Wade, 2017). Thus, the social nature of
2013). Thus, early prevention efforts are of paramount importance. intimate relationships during these life stages can make peers pivotal
Reviews of research on the effectiveness of programs designed to actors in the prevention of sexual assault.
prevent sexual assault among adolescents and college students have However, the potential for peer intervention can be undermined by a
noted both a dearth of high‐quality studies, such as RCTs and general “bystander effect” that diffuses responsibility for action in group
minimal evidence that these prevention programs have meaningful settings (Darley & Latane, 1968). To intervene as a witness to sexual
effects on young people’s behavior (De Koker, Mathews, Zuch, assault, individuals must notice the event (or its warning signs), define the
Bastien, & Mason‐Jones, 2014; DeGue et al., 2014). Concerning the event as warranting action/intervention, take responsibility for acting (i.e.,
latter point, evaluations of such programs tend to measure attitudinal feel a sense of personal duty), and demonstrate a sufficient level of self‐
outcomes (e.g., rape supportive attitudes, rape myth acceptance) efficacy (i.e., perceived competence to successfully intervene; Latane &
more frequently than behavioral outcomes (e.g., perpetration or Darley, 1969). Studies have indicated that, as witnesses to sexual assault,
victimization; Anderson & Whiston, 2005; Cornelius & Resseguie, young people often fail to meet these criteria (Banyard, 2008; Bennett,
2007; DeGue et al., 2014). Additionally, findings from a meta‐analysis Banyard, & Garnhart, 2014; Burn, 2009; Casey & Ohler, 2012; Exner &
of studies assessing outcomes of college sexual assault prevention Cummings, 2011; McCauley et al., 2013; McMahon, 2010; Noonan &
programs suggested that effects are larger for attitudinal outcomes Charles, 2009), with males being less likely than females to intervene
than for the actual incidence of sexual assault (Anderson & Whiston, (Banyard, 2008; Burn, 2009; Edwards, Rodenhizer‐Stampfli, & Eckstein,
2005). 2015; Exner & Cummings, 2011; McMahon, 2010).
Thus, bystander programs seek to sensitize young people to
6.2 | The intervention warning signs of sexual assault, create attitudinal change that fosters
bystander responsibility for intervening (e.g., creating empathy for
6.2.1 | The bystander approach
victims), and build requisite skills/tactics for taking action (Banyard
Given this paucity of evidence regarding behavior change, it is et al., 2004; Banyard, 2011; Burn, 2009; McMahon & Banyard, 2012).
imperative to identify effective strategies for preventing sexual Many of these programs are implemented with large groups of
assault among adolescents and young adults. One promising adolescents or college students in the format of a single training/
strategy is the implementation of bystander programs, which education session (e.g., as part of college orientation). However, some
encourage young people to intervene when witnessing incidents or programs use broader implementation strategies, such as advertising
warning signs of sexual assault (e.g., controlling behavior, such as campaigns where signs are posted across college campuses to
intervening with a would‐be perpetrator leading an intoxicated encourage students to act when witnessing signs of violence. The
person into an isolated area). The strength of the bystander model bystander model was developed and popularized in the US but has
lies in its emphasis on the role of peers in the prevention of been adapted for use in global contexts.
violence. Peers are a salient influence on young people’s intimate
relationships (Adelman & Kil, 2007; Giordano, 2003). In some
6.3 | How the intervention might work
respects, this influence can be detrimental, as having friends
involved in violent intimate relationships (i.e., characterized by By treating young people as potential allies in preventing sexual
sexual or physical violence) is a risk factor for becoming both a assault, bystander programs have the potential to be less threatening
perpetrator and victim of violence (Arriaga & Foshee, 2004; than traditional sexual assault prevention programs, which tend to
Foshee, Benefield, Ennett, Bauman, & Suchindran, 2004; Foshee, approach young people as either potential perpetrators or victims of
Linder, MacDougall, & Bangdiwala, 2001; Foshee, Reyes, & sexual violence (Burn, 2009; [Jackson] Katz, 1995; Messner, 2015).
Ennett, 2010; McCauley et al., 2013). However, peers can also Instead of placing emphasis on how young people may modify their
have a positive impact on intimate relationships. individual behavior to either respect the sexual boundaries of others
Young victims and perpetrators of violence are often reluctant to or reduce their personal risk for being sexually assaulted, bystander
divulge their experience or to seek help (especially from adults), but programs aim to foster prerequisite knowledge and skills for
KETTREY ET AL. | 7 of 40

intervening on behalf of victims. Thus, by treating young people as Since the inception of MVP a number of programs have emerged
part of the solution to sexual assault, rather than part of the problem, to address barriers to bystander intervention among adolescents and
bystander programs limit the risk of defensiveness or backlash college students. Although they all share the common goal of
among participants (e.g., decreased empathy for victims, increased inspiring bystanders to act in ways that prevent sexual assault, these
rape myth acceptance; Banyard et al., 2004; Katz, 1995). programs exhibit a great deal of variation in scope pertaining to their
In addition to encouraging young people to prevent sexual violence target bystander populations (i.e., males and/or females, secondary
through bystander intervention, these programs may reduce the school or college students), sex of victims, and gendered versus
likelihood of participants committing sexual assault themselves. To gender‐neutral approach. For example, some programs use a
illustrate, Katz (1995) describes the rationale behind the mentors in gendered approach by (a) critiquing gender norms that can promote
violence prevention (MVP) bystander program as follows: “rather than violence against women and (b) encouraging males to intervene on
focus on men as actual or potential perpetrators (emphasis in original), behalf of female victims (e.g., MVP, see Katz, 1995). Others use a
we focus on them in their role as potential bystanders (emphasis in gender‐neutral approach to build a sense of community responsibility
original). This shift in emphasis greatly reduces the participants’ to intervene on behalf of both male and female victims of sexual
defensiveness” (p. 168). In other words, young men may be more assault (e.g., Bringing in the Bystander, see Banyard, Moynihan, &
receptive to prevention programming, and thus less likely to commit Crossman, 2009; Banyard, Moynihan, & Plante, 2007). One of the
sexual assault, when they are approached as part of the solution rather major differences between gendered and gender‐neutral bystander
than part of the problem to sexual assault. Consistent with this line of programs is that the former places socio‐cultural forces, such as
reasoning, studies on the effects of bystander programs often measure gender norms, at the center of discussions of violence whereas the
participants’ rates of sexual assault as a program outcome (Foubert, latter places the bystander, and individual cognitive processes when
2000; Foubert, Newberry, & Tatum, 2007; Gidycz, Orchowski, & encountering violence, at the center of discussions of violence
Berkowitz, 2011; Miller et al., 2013; Miller et al., 2014; Salazar, ([Jackson] Katz, Heisterkamp, & Fleming, 2011; Messner, 2015).
Vivolo‐Kantor, Hardin, & Berkowitz, 2014). Comparing the effects of gendered and gender‐neutral programs
As outlined by Burn (2009) bystander programs are designed has the potential to identify important determinants of the success of
to promote the following prerequisites for intervention: noticing bystander programs. Although there is no empirical examination of
an event, identifying a situation as warranting intervention, the different effects of these programs, there are theoretical reasons
taking responsibility for acting, and deciding how to help. This to believe that each has the potential to be successful under certain
often involves educating young people about what constitutes conditions. Namely, gendered approaches to bystander education
sexual assault, portraying victims as worthy of assistance, and programs may be better suited to target socio‐cultural facilitators of
building skills necessary to intervene (e.g., providing strategies sexual assault against women and address different patterns of
for what to do and say). Although most bystander programs bystander behaviors exhibited by males and females (Banyard, 2008;
share the common goal of promoting such prerequisites for Burn, 2009; Exner & Cummings, 2011; Katz, Heisterkamp, & Fleming,
intervention, their specific program content and framing of 2011; [Jennifer] Katz, 2015; [Jennifer] Katz, Colbert, & Colangelo,
sexual assault varies. 2015; McCauley et al., 2013; McMahon, 2010; Messner, 2015). On
Research has indicated that, relative to males, females are the other hand, gender‐neutral programs may have the benefit of
overwhelmingly the victims of sexual assault (Foshee, 1996; deflecting the criticism that prevention programs utilizing a gendered
Gressard, Swahn, & Tharp, 2015; Harned, 2001; Howard, Wang, & approach are inherently anti‐male (Katz et al., 2011; Messner, 2015).
Yan, 2007). Thus, the earliest bystander programs tended to apply a Proponents of such programs assert that avoidance of such criticism
gendered perspective to the prevention of sexual assault among is paramount to the success of sexual assault prevention programs. In
adolescents and college students. For example, Katz (1995) devel- their view, adolescents and young adults who are coming of age in a
oped MVP with the goal of inspiring male college athletes to “post‐feminist” era may be likely to reject gendered explanations of
challenge sociocultural definitions of masculinity that equate men’s sexual assault and, instead, may respond more positively to gender‐
strength with dominance over women. At the time of its inception, neutral programs that use inclusive language that can be applied to a
MVP was unique in its explicit focus on masculinity as well as its broad range of victims and perpetrators (Barreto & Ellemers, 2005;
nonthreatening bystander approach that encouraged young men to Kettrey, 2016; Swim, Aikin, Hall, & Hunter, 1995).
intervene when witnessing acts (or warning signs) of violence against
women. As Katz explained, MVP reduces young men’s defensiveness
6.4 | Why it is important to do the review
to violence prevention efforts by focusing on men as potential
bystanders to violence, rather than potential perpetrators of violence. In Policymakers in the United States perceive bystander programs to be
addition to reducing men’s defensiveness to intervention efforts, this beneficial, as evidenced by the 2013 Campus SaVE Act’s requirement
bystander approach emphasizes the point that “when men don’t that postsecondary educational institutions participating in Title IX
speak up or take action in the face of other men’s abusive behavior financial aid programs provide incoming college students with
toward women, that constitutes implicit consent of such behavior” primary prevention and awareness programs addressing sexual
(Katz, 1995, p. 168). violence. The Campus SaVE Act mandates that these programs
8 of 40 | KETTREY ET AL.

include a component on bystander intervention. Currently, there is noted less evidence of beneficial effects on actual bystander
no comparable legislation regarding sexual assault among adoles- behavior or perpetration of violence. While informative, each of
cents (e.g., mandating bystander programs in secondary schools). This these reviews has limitations. Katz & Moore’s (2013) meta‐
is an unfortunate oversight, as adolescents who experience sexual analysis focused exclusively on sexual assault on college
assault are at an increased risk of repeated victimization in young campuses and did not examine effects of such programs among
adulthood (Cui et al., 2013). Thus, the implementation of bystander adolescents. Although Storer et al. (2015) focused on studies
programs in secondary schools not only has the potential to reduce examining violence among both adolescents and young adults,
sexual assault among adolescents but may also have the long‐term their sample was limited in that it was exclusively comprised of
potential to reduce sexual assault on college campuses. peer‐reviewed articles (i.e., the sample explicitly excluded theses,
Findings from this systematic review will provide valuable dissertations, and other gray literature). Additionally, the authors
evidence of the extent to which bystander programs, as mandated specified no research design criteria for inclusion (i.e., the sample
by the Campus SaVE Act, are effective in preventing sexual assault included low‐quality studies such as those utilizing single group
among college students. Additionally, by examining effects of these pre‐ and post‐test designs), limiting the strength of their
programs among adolescents, this review will provide educators and conclusions. Importantly, Storer et al. reported no meta‐analytic
policy makers with information for determining whether such findings. Thus, to our knowledge there are currently no existing
programs should be widely implemented in secondary schools. meta‐analyses examining the effects of bystander programs on
Currently, there are no Campbell or Cochrane Collaboration attitudes and behaviors regarding sexual assault among both
Reviews evaluating the effects of bystander programs on sexual college students and adolescents. Additionally, Katz & Moore’s
assault among adolescents and/or college students. Of modest early meta‐analysis only included studies published/reported
relevance to the proposed review, the Campbell and Cochrane through 2011 (2 years prior to the 2013 Campus SaVE Act) and
Collaboration libraries include meta‐analyses of the effects of more did not evaluate program content as a moderator.
general programs (not bystander programs) designed to prevent or Our review examined the effects of bystander programs on
reduce relationship/dating violence among adolescents and/or young attitudes (i.e., perceptions of violence/victims, self‐efficacy to
adults (De La Rue, Polanin, Espelage, & Pigott, 2014; Fellmeth, intervene, and intentions to intervene) and behaviors (i.e., actual
Heffernan, Nurse, Habibula, & Sethi, 2013). Both of these reviews intervention behavior, perpetration) regarding sexual assault
reported violence outcomes as aggregate measures that do not among adolescents and college students. Importantly, we present
distinguish sexual violence from other forms of violence. Although meta‐analytic findings to quantitatively assess the influence of
they each found some evidence of significant effects on knowledge or moderators (e.g., gender composition of sample, mean age of
attitudes pertinent to violence, neither found evidence of significant sample, education level of sample, single‐ or mixed‐sex implemen-
effects on young people’s behavior (i.e., rates of perpetration or tation, gendered content of program, fraternity/sorority member-
victimization). ship, and athletic team membership) on the effects of bystander
Two reviews published outside of the Campbell and Cochrane programs.
Collaboration libraries are of closer relevance to this review. These
include a meta‐analysis of the effects of bystander programs on
7 | O B J EC T I V E S
sexual assault on college campuses (Katz & Moore, 2013) and a
narrative review of studies examining the effects of bystander
7.1 | The problem, condition or issue
programs on dating and sexual violence among adolescents and
young adults (Storer, Casey, & Herrenkohl, 2015). The overall objective of this systematic review and meta‐analysis was
In what they called an “initial” meta‐analysis of experimental to examine the effects bystander programs have on preventing
and quasi‐experimental studies published through 2011, Katz & sexual assault among adolescents and college students. More
Moore (2013) found moderate beneficial effects of bystander specifically, and given the study designs that were included, this
programs on participants’ self‐efficacy and intentions to intervene, review addressed three objectives.
and small (but significant) effects on bystander behavior, rape‐
supportive attitudes, and rape proclivity (but not perpetration). 1. The first objective was to assess the overall effects (including
Effects were generally stronger among younger samples and adverse effects), and the variability of the effects, of bystander
samples containing a higher percentage of males. The stronger programs on adolescents’ and college students’ attitudes and
effect for younger participants (i.e., younger college students) behaviors regarding sexual assault. This included general
suggests such programs may be particularly effective with attitudes toward violence and victims, prerequisite skills and
adolescents. knowledge to intervene, self‐efficacy to intervene, intentions/
In a narrative review of studies examining the effects of willingness to intervene when witnessing signs of sexual assault,
bystander programs on dating violence and sexual assault among actual intervention behavior, and perpetration of sexual assault.
adolescents and young adults, Storer et al. (2015) highlighted 2. The second objective was to explore the comparative effective-
beneficial effects on bystander self‐efficacy and intentions but ness of bystander programs for different profiles of participants
KETTREY ET AL. | 9 of 40

(e.g., mean age of the sample, education level of the sample, Consistent with Campbell Collaboration policies and proce-
proportion of males/females in the sample, proportion of dures, studies using experimental and quasi‐experimental
fraternity/sorority members in the sample, and proportion of research designs were synthesized separately in the meta‐
athletic team members in the sample). analyses, given that experimental study designs have the highest
3. The third objective was to explore the comparative effective- level of internal validity. Furthermore, we collected extensive
ness of different bystander programs in terms of gendered data on the risk of bias and study quality of all eligible studies,
content and approach (e.g., conceptualizing sexual assault as a which we attended to when interpreting the findings from the
gendered or gender‐neutral problem, mixed‐ or single‐sex systematic review and meta‐analyses (described in greater detail
group implementation). below).

8 | METHODS 8.1.2 | Types of participants


The review focused on studies that examined outcomes of bystander
8.1 | Criteria for considering studies for this review programs and target sexual assault and are implemented with
8.1.1 | Types of studies adolescents and/or college students in educational settings. Eligible
participants included adolescents enrolled in grades 7 through 12
To be eligible for inclusion in the review, studies must have used an and college students enrolled in any type of undergraduate
experimental or controlled quasi‐experimental research design to postsecondary educational institution.
compare an intervention group (i.e., students assigned to a bystander Eligible participant populations included studies that reported on
program) with a comparison group (e.g., students not assigned to a general samples of adolescents and/or college students as well as
bystander program). We limited our review to such study designs studies using specialized samples such as those primarily consisting
because these typically have lower risk of bias relative to other study of college athletes, fraternity/sorority members, and single‐sex
designs (e.g., single group designs). More specifically, we included the samples. Study samples primarily consisting of postgraduate students
following designs: were ineligible for inclusion; the mean age of samples could be no
less than 12 and no greater than 25 to be included in the review.
1. Randomized controlled trials: Studies in which individuals, class-
rooms, schools, or other groups were randomly assigned to
intervention and comparison conditions.
8.1.3 | Types of interventions
2. Quasi‐randomized controlled trials: Studies where assignment to
conditions was quasi‐random, for example, by birth date, date of Eligible intervention programs were those that approached partici-
week, student identification number, month, or some other pants as allies in preventing and/or alleviating sexual assault among
alternation method. adolescents and/or college students. Some part of the program had
3. Controlled quasi‐experimental designs: Studies where partici- to focus on ways that cultivate willingness for a person to respond to
pants were not assigned to conditions randomly or quasi‐ others who are at risk for sexual assault. All delivery formats were
randomly (e.g., participants self‐selected into groups). Given the eligible for inclusion (e.g., in‐person training sessions, video programs,
potential selection biases inherent in these controlled quasi‐ web‐based training, and advertising/poster campaigns). There were
experimental design, we only included those that also met one of no interventions duration criteria for inclusion.
the following criteria: Studies that reported bystander outcomes but did not meet the
a. Regression discontinuity designs: Studies that used a cutoff on aforementioned intervention inclusion criterion were not eligible
a forcing variable to assign participants to intervention and for inclusion. Additionally, studies that assessed outcomes of
comparison groups, and assessed program impacts around the programs that aimed to facilitate pro‐social bystander behavior,
cutoff of the forcing variable. but that did not explicitly include a component addressing sexual
b. Studies that used propensity score or other matching proce- assault (e.g., programs to prevent bullying) were not eligible for
dures to create a matched sample of participants in the inclusion.
intervention and comparison groups. To be eligible for Eligible comparison groups must have received no intervention
inclusion, these studies must have also provided enough services targeting bystander attitudes/behavior or sexual assault.
statistical information to permit estimation of pretest effect Thus, treatment–treatment studies that compared outcomes of
sizes for the matched groups. individuals assigned to a bystander program versus those assigned
c. For studies where participants in the intervention to a general sexual assault prevention program were not eligible for
and comparison groups were not matched, enough statistical inclusion. Eligible comparison groups may have received a sham or
information must have been reported to permit estimation of attention treatment expected to have no effect on bystander
pretest effect sizes for at least one outcome measure. outcomes or attitudes/behaviors regarding sexual assault.
10 of 40 | KETTREY ET AL.

8.1.4 | Types of outcome measures programs implemented with adolescents and young adults outside
of educational institutions (e.g., in community settings, military
We included studies that measured the effects of bystander
settings) were ineligible for inclusion in the review. There were no
programs on at least one of the following primary outcome domains:
geographic limitations on inclusion criteria. Research conducted in
any country was eligible.
1. General attitudes toward sexual assault and victims (e.g., victim
empathy, rape myth acceptance).
8.2 | Search methods for identification of studies
2. Prerequisite skills and knowledge for bystander intervention as
defined by Burn (2009) (e.g., noticing sexual assault or its warning 8.2.1 | Search strategy
signs, identifying a situation as appropriate for intervention,
We identified candidate studies through searches of electronic
taking responsibility for acting/intervening, knowing strategies for
databases, relevant academic journals, and gray literature sources.
helping/intervening).
We also contacted leading authors and experts on bystander
3. Self‐efficacy with regard to bystander intervention (e.g., respon-
programs to identify any current/ongoing research that might be
dents’ confidence in their ability to intervene).
eligible for the review. Additionally, we screened the bibliographies
4. Intentions to intervene when witnessing instances or warning
of eligible studies and relevant reviews to identify additional
signs of sexual assault.
candidate studies. We conducted forward citation searches (searches
5. Actual intervention behavior when witnessing instances or
for reports citing eligible studies) using the website Google Scholar,
warning signs of sexual assault.
as this database produces similar results to other search engines (e.g.,
6. Perpetration of sexual assault (i.e., rates of perpetration among
Web of Science; Tanner‐Smith & Polanin, 2015) and is also more
individuals assigned to the treatment or comparison group of a
likely to locate existing gray literature. Our search was global in
study).
scope and attempted to identify studies of bystander programs
implemented in any country.
Depending on directionality, these outcomes capture both
beneficial and adverse effects (e.g., increases or decreases in victim
empathy, pro‐social bystander behavior, etc.) that are important to 8.2.2 | Electronic searches
adolescents, college students, and decision‐makers alike.
The prevention of sexual assault among college students and
Any outcome falling in these domains was eligible for inclusion.
adolescents is a topic that spans multiple disciplines (e.g., sociology,
This includes outcomes measured with any form of assessment (e.g.,
psychology, education, and public health). Thus, we searched a
self‐report, official/administrative report, observation, etc.) that
variety of databases that are relevant to these fields. Search terms
could be summarized by any type of quantitative score (e.g.,
varied by database, but generally included two blocks of terms and
percentage, continuous variable, count variable, categorical variable,
appropriate Boolean or proximity operators, when allowed: blocks
etc.). In the event that a particular study included multiple measures
included terms that address the intervention and outcomes. We
of a single construct category (e.g., two measures of rape myth
specifically searched the following electronic databases (hosts) in
acceptance or bystander intervention within a given study), we only
October 2016 and June 2017:
included one outcome per study for that construct. We selected the
most similar outcomes for synthesis within a construct category
• Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials (CENTRAL).
(described in detail below).
• Cochrane Database of Abstracts of Reviews of Effects (DARE).
• Education Resources Information Center (ERIC, via ProQuest).

8.1.5 | Duration of follow‐up • Education Database (via ProQuest).


• International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS, via
Studies reporting follow‐ups of any duration were eligible for ProQuest).
inclusion. When studies reported more than one follow‐up wave, • PsycINFO (via ProQuest).
each wave was coded and identified by its reported duration. As • PsycARTICLES (via ProQuest).
described in more detail below, follow‐ups of similar durations were • PubMed.
analyzed together. • Social Services Abstracts (via ProQuest).
• Sociological Abstracts (via ProQuest).

8.1.6 | Types of settings


The strategy for searching electronic databases involved the use
The review focused on studies that examine outcomes of bystander of search terms specific to the types of interventions and outcomes
programs and target sexual assault and are implemented with eligible for inclusion. Search terms for types of interventions included
adolescents and/or college students in educational settings. Eligible general terms for bystander programs as well as names of specific
educational settings included secondary schools (i.e., grades 7–12) bystander programs (e.g., MVP). Search terms for types of outcomes
and colleges or universities. Studies that assessed bystander included terms that are specific to measures of sexual violence (e.g.,
KETTREY ET AL. | 11 of 40

sexual assault) as well as more general terms that have the potential eligible studies, and reference lists of eligible studies. We also
to identify studies that measure physical and/or sexual violence. Due conducted forward citation searches of all eligible studies.
to the overwhelming focus of bystander programs on adolescents
and college students (aside from a few implementations with military 8.3 | Data collection and analysis
samples) search terms did not limit initial results by the age or
8.3.1 | Selection of studies
general characteristics of the target population.
The search terms and strategy for PsycINFO via ProQuest were Once candidate studies were identified in the literature search, each
as follows (terms were modified for other databases): reference was entered into the project database as a separate record.
(AB,TI(“bystander”)) AND (AB,TI(“education” OR “program” Two reviewers then independently screened each study title and
OR “training” OR “intervention” OR “behavior” OR “attitude” OR abstract and recorded their eligibility recommendation (i.e., ineligible
“intention” OR “efficacy” OR “prosocial” OR “pro‐social” OR or eligible for full‐text screening) into the pertinent database record.
“empowered” OR “Bringing in the Bystander” OR “Green Dot” Disagreements between reviewers were resolved by discussion and
OR “Step Up” OR “Mentors in Violence Prevention” OR “MVP” OR consensus, and the final abstract screening decision was recorded in
“Know Your Power” OR “Hollaback” OR “Circle of 6” OR “That’s Not the database. Potentially eligible studies were then retrieved in full
Cool” OR “Red Flag Campaign” OR “Where Do You Stand” OR “White text and these full texts were reviewed for eligibility, again using two
Ribbon Campaign” OR “Men Can Stop Rape” OR “The Men’s independent reviewers who recorded their eligibility recommenda-
Program” OR “The Women’s Program” OR “The Men’s Project” OR tion (and, when applicable, rationale for an ineligibility recommenda-
“Coaching Boys into Men” OR “Campus Violence Prevention tion). Disagreements between reviewers were again resolved via
Program” OR “Real Men Respect” OR “Speak Up Speak Out” OR discussion and consensus and the final eligibility decision was
“How to Help a Sexual Assault Survivor” OR “InterACT”)) AND (AB,TI recorded in the database. In cases where we could not determine
(“sexual assault” OR “rape” OR “violence” OR “victimization”)) eligibility due to missing information in a report, we contacted study
authors for this information.
Throughout the search and screening process we maintained a
document that included the number of unique candidate studies
8.2.3 | Searching other resources
identified through various sources (e.g., electronic database searches,
We searched the tables of contents of current issues of journals that academic journal searches, and gray literature searches). We used
publish research on sexual violence. This included the following the information in this record to create a PRISMA flow chart that
journals: Journal of Adolescent Health, Journal of Community Psychology, reports the screening process (Moher et al., 2015). Additionally, we
Journal of Family Violence, Journal of Interpersonal Violence, Psychology used the final screening decisions recorded in the meta‐analysis
of Violence, Violence Against Women, and Violence and Victims. We database to create a table that lists studies excluded during the full‐
searched these sources in October 2016 and June 2017. text screening phase along with the rationale for each exclusion
We also conducted gray literature searches to identify unpub- decision.
lished studies that met inclusion criteria. This included searching
electronic databases that catalog dissertations and theses, searching
8.3.2 | Data extraction and management
conference proceedings, and searching websites with content
relevant to sexual assault and/or violence against women. We Two reviewers independently double‐coded all included studies,
specifically searched the following gray literature sources in October using a piloted codebook (see Appendix). All coding was entered into
2016 and June 2017: an electronic database, with a separate record maintained for each
independent coding of each study. Coding disagreements were
• ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global. resolved via discussion and consensus with final coding decisions
• Clinical Trials Register (https://clinicaltrials.gov). maintained in a separate record. If data needed to calculate an effect
• End Violence Against Women International ‐ conference proceed- size were missing from a report, we contacted the primary study
ings (http://www.evawintl.org/conferences.aspx). authors for this information.
• National Sexual Violence Resource Center website (nsvrc.org). The primary categories for coding are as follows: participant
• National Online Resource Center on Violence Against Women demographics and characteristics (e.g., age, gender, education
website (VAWnet.org). level, race/ethnicity, athletic team membership, fraternity/sorority
• Us Department of Justice Office on Violence Against Women membership); intervention setting (e.g., state, country, secondary
website (www.justice.gov/ovw). or postsecondary institution, mixed‐ or single‐sex group); study
• Center for Changing our Campus Culture website (www. characteristics (e.g., attrition, duration of follow‐up, study design,
changingourcampusculture.org). participant dose, sample N); outcome construct (e.g., type,
description of measure); and outcome results (e.g., timing
Additionally, we searched reference lists of previous systematic at measurement, baseline and follow‐up means and standard
reviews and meta‐analyses, CVs and websites of primary authors of deviations or proportions).
12 of 40 | KETTREY ET AL.

8.3.3 | Assessment of risk of bias in included studies randomized into the intervention or comparison conditions at the
group level (e.g., entire schools were assigned to a single condition),
We assessed risk of bias in included studies using the Cochrane risk
and inferences were made at the individual level. To correct for these
of bias tools for randomized studies (Higgins & Green, 2011) and
unit of analysis errors, we followed the procedures outlined in the
nonrandomized studies (Sterne, Higgins, & Reeves, 2016). The
Cochrane Handbook (Higgins & Green, 2011) to inflate the standard
Cochrane risk of bias tool for randomized studies assesses risk of
errors of the effect sizes from these nine studies by multiplying them
bias in the following domains: random sequence generation,
by the design effect:
allocation concealment, blinding of participants and personnel,
blinding of outcome assessment, incomplete outcome data, selective 1 + (M − 1) ICC,
reporting, and other bias. These domains are each rated as low,
unclear, or high risk of bias. where M is the average cluster size for a given study and ICC is the
The Cochrane risk of bias tool for nonrandomized studies, intracluster correlation coefficient for a given outcome. In cases
ROBINS‐I, requires that at the protocol stage of the review, two where study authors did not report ICCs we used a liberal assumed
sets of items are determined: (a) confounding areas that are expected value of 0.10. This value has been used in a past Campbell review
to be relevant to all or most studies in the review and (b) that synthesizes attitude and behavior outcomes of dating/sexual
cointerventions that could be different between intervention groups violence prevention programs (De La Rue et al., 2014) and is
with the potential to differentially impact outcomes. In our protocol, supported by Hedges and Hedberg’s (2007) research on ICCs in
we anticipated the following confounding factors, which were coded cluster randomized trials conducted in educational settings.
in the risk of bias assessments for nonrandomized studies: gender,
fraternity/sorority membership, athletic team membership, preinter-
8.3.6 | Dealing with missing data
vention attitudes (e.g., victim empathy, rape myth acceptance),
preintervention bystander measures (e.g., efficacy, intentions, beha- When studies reported insufficient data to calculate effect sizes we
vior), and prior sexual assault victimization. Additionally, we contacted the primary authors to request the necessary information.
anticipated the following cointerventions to have a potential impact This author query method was highly successful; there were only two
on outcomes, and they were coded in the risk of bias assessments: cases in which we were unable to obtain sufficient data from study
general sexual assault prevention programs, dating violence preven- authors (see details below with search results).
tion programs, and general bystander programs (not explicitly
targeting sexual violence).
8.3.7 | Assessment of heterogeneity
We assessed and reported heterogeneity using the χ 2 statistic and its
8.3.4 | Measures of treatment effect corresponding p value, as well as the I2 and τ 2 statistics. We used the
restricted maximum likelihood estimator for τ 2. When at least 10
We extracted relevant summary statistics (e.g., means and standard
studies were included in any given meta‐analysis, we also used
deviations, proportions, observed sample sizes) to calculate effect
mixed‐effect meta‐regression models to conduct moderator analyses.
sizes. We then used Wilson’s (2013) online effect size calculator to
Moderators specified in the protocol included: (a) gendered content
calculate effect sizes. The overwhelming majority of studies reported
of the program, (b) mixed‐ or single‐sex group implementation,
continuous measures of treatment effects, so we used a SMD effect
(c) gender composition of the sample, (d) education level of the
size metric with a small sample correction (i.e., Hedges’ g). In the rare
sample (i.e., secondary school or college students), (e) mean age of
cases in which binary outcome measures were reported in included
the sample, (f) proportion of fraternity/sorority members in the
studies, we deemed these measures to represent the same under-
sample, and (g) and proportion of athletic team members in the
lying construct as continuous measures, as they typically relied on
sample.
the same measurement tools as relevant continuous measures. Thus,
we transformed any log odd ratio effect sizes available from binary
measures into SMD effect sizes by entering the observed proportions 8.3.8 | Assessment of reporting biases
and sample sizes into Wilson’s (2013) online effect size calculator. All
To assess the potential of small study/publication bias, we used the
SMD effect sizes were coded such that positive values (i.e., greater
contour enhanced funnel plot (Palmer, Peters, Sutton, & Moreno,
than 0) indicate a beneficial outcome for the intervention group.
2008), Egger’s regression test (Egger, Smith, Schneider, & Minder,
1997), and trim and fill analysis (Duval & Tweedie, 2000).

8.3.5 | Unit of analysis issues


8.3.9 | Data synthesis
The unit of analysis of interest for this review was the individual (i.e.,
individual‐level attitudes and behaviors). Nine of the included studies We conducted all statistical analyses with the metafor package in R.
used cluster randomized trial designs where participants were We conducted meta‐analyses using random‐effects inverse variance
KETTREY ET AL. | 13 of 40

weights and reported 95% confidence intervals along with all mean sensitivity analyses that removed any Winsorized effect sizes from
effect size estimates. We conducted and reported all meta‐analyses the meta‐analysis, to assess whether this method for handling
separately for RCTs and non‐RCTs. Additionally, we conducted all outliers may have substantively altered the review findings.
syntheses separately by outcome domain (i.e., attitudes toward
sexual assault and victims, prerequisite skills and knowledge to
intervene, bystander self‐efficacy, bystander intentions, bystander
8.3.12 | Interpretation of findings
behavior, perpetration) and follow‐up timing. For most outcomes, To provide substantive interpretations of statistically significant
follow‐up timing fell into the following categories: immediate mean effect sizes, we transformed the average SMD effect size back
post‐intervention, 1‐ to 4‐month post‐intervention, and 6 months into an unstandardized metric using commonly reported scales/
to 1‐year post‐intervention. We displayed each synthesis using forest measures in the respective outcome domains. Namely, we multiplied
plots. the average SMD effect size by the standard deviation of a scale/
To minimize any potential bias in the meta‐analysis results due to measure that was frequently used in our meta‐analytic sample to
effect size outliers, prior to conducting the meta‐analysis, we yield an unstandardized difference in means. We aimed to select
Winsorized any outlier effect sizes that fell more than two standard means and standard deviations from the most representative studies
deviations away from the mean of the effect size distribution (Lipsey in our sample (e.g., RCTs and/or studies with larger sample sizes), but
& Wilson, 2001). In such cases, we replaced the outlier effect size we recognize this is an imperfect process that requires generalization
with the value that fell exactly two standard deviations from the from a single study (for each significant outcome). We present these
mean of the distribution of effect sizes. transformations in our Discussion section, at the conclusion of this
As noted previously, each meta‐analysis synthesized a set of report. Readers should be mindful of the fact that these are
statistically independent effect sizes. To ensure the statistical extrapolations intended to provide meaningful context to our
independence of effect sizes synthesized within any given meta‐ findings, and that results are most accurately represented by the
analysis, we split all analyses by follow‐up timing and outcome Hedges’ g effect sizes.
domain. In the event that a particular study included multiple
measures within a single outcome domain (e.g., two measures of rape
myth acceptance within a given study), we only included one 9 | RESULTS
outcome per study for that construct. We selected the most similar
outcomes for synthesis within a construct category. We adopted this 9.1 | Description of studies
approach for synthesizing statistically independent effect sizes given
9.1.1 | Results of the search
the small number of included studies and effect sizes available for
synthesis. Other approaches that can be used to synthesize We conducted an initial literature search in October 2016 and an
dependent effect sizes, such as robust variance estimation, require updated search in June 2017. Figure 1 outlines the flow of studies
larger sample sizes for efficient parameter estimation than the through the search and screening process. Through our initial and
sample sizes we had available for analysis (Hedges, Tipton, & updated search we identified 797 reports. Of these reports, 738
Johnson, 2010; Tanner‐Smith & Tipton, 2014; Tipton, 2013). were identified from searches of electronic databases, 19 from
ClinicalTrials.gov, 1 from conference proceedings, 5 from website
searches, 20 from reference lists of review articles, 1 from tables of
8.3.10 | Subgroup analysis and investigation of contents searches, 3 from reference lists of eligible reports, 9 from
heterogeneity CVs and websites of primary authors of eligible studies, and 1 from
We performed sub‐group analyses based on study design, synthesiz- forward citation searching of eligible studies. After deleting duplicate
ing effect sizes (a) for RCTs alone, (b) for non‐RCTs alone, and (c) for reports and reports that were deemed ineligible through the abstract
RCTs and non‐RCTS combined. For each of these three subgroup screening process 154 reports were deemed eligible for full‐text
analyses we assessed and reported heterogeneity using the χ 2 screening. Three of these reports could not be located; thus, we
statistic and its corresponding p value, I2, and τ 2. When at least ten screened a total of 151 full‐text reports for eligibility.
studies were included in any given meta‐analysis, we used mixed‐
effect meta‐regression models to conduct moderator analyses.
9.1.2 | Included studies
Twenty‐seven independent studies summarized in 38 reports met
8.3.11 | Sensitivity analysis
inclusion criteria. One report (Jouriles, Rosenfield, Yule, Sargent, &
When at least 10 studies were included in a given meta‐analysis we McDonald, 2016b) presented findings from two independent studies.
conducted sensitivity analyses to examine whether study‐level We coded these two studies separately and identified them as
attrition and high risk of bias (for each domain assessed with the Jouriles et al. (2016a) and Jouriles et al. (2016b). Table 1 summarizes
risk of bias tools) were associated with effect size magnitude, using aggregate characteristics of all eligible studies. Twelve studies
mixed‐effects meta‐regression models. Additionally, we conducted utilized random assignment at the individual level, nine studies
14 of 40 | KETTREY ET AL.

FIGURE 1 PRISMA study flow diagram

utilized random assignment at the group level, and six studies utilized individual students composing the sample changed from wave to
nonrandom assignment but reported pretest equivalence measures wave—and many students entering the school in later waves did not
on at least one eligible outcome. More specific details of each receive direct intervention. Darlington (2014) only reported within‐
individual study are summarized in Table A1 (see Figures and Tables group pre‐post effect sizes. No between‐group data were reported
Appendix). The majority of studies were conducted in the United and our attempt to obtain these data from the study author was
States, with one study conducted in Canada and one conducted in unsuccessful. Mabry and Turner (2016) reported three‐way ANOVA
India. This geographic pattern was primarily due to a paucity of findings from a three‐armed study (i.e., one eligible intervention
research on bystander programs conducted outside of the United group, one ineligible intervention group, and a comparison group).
States and, to a lesser extent, to the failure of studies conducted Our attempts to obtain data for the eligible intervention and
outside the United States to meet the scope and methodological comparison groups from the study author were unsuccessful. We
criteria for this review. therefore summarized each of these three studies in the systematic
Three studies were eligible for the review by methodological review but were unable to include results from those studies in any
standards but did not report codable outcomes (Cook‐Craig et al. of the meta‐analyses.
2014; Darlington, 2014; Mabry & Turner, 2016). For example, in Two studies (Banyard et al., 2007; Jouriles et al., n.d.) included
Cook‐Craig et al.’s (2014) study the unit of analysis was the school, two eligible intervention arms. Banyard et al. (2007) randomly
not the individual. The study authors surveyed entire intervention assigned participants to one of three groups: (a) a no‐treatment
and comparison schools across several years. As a result, the control that only completed pre‐ and post‐intervention surveys,
KETTREY ET AL. | 15 of 40

T A B L E 1 Characteristics of included studies (N = 27) analyses in which we ran additional meta‐analyses that included
n % effect sizes that compared the alternative intervention arms with the
comparison arm.
Study design
Randomized – Individual 12 44.44
Randomized – Group 9 33.33
Nonrandomized 6 22.22
9.1.3 | Excluded studies
Comparison treatment As shown in Figure 1, 105 reports were deemed ineligible after full‐
Active/Sham treatment 9 33.33 text screening. These reports were ineligible because they either did
Inactive control 18 66.67 not present an evaluation of an eligible intervention (n = 25), did not
Peer reviewed 22 81.48 include an eligible comparison group (n = 58), did not involve an

Funded 18 66.67
eligible population or setting (n = 11), did not involve an eligible
research design (n =8), or did not measure any eligible outcomes (n =
Study report year
3). Table A2 provides a brief description of each of these studies as
2017 2 7.41
well as the specific reasons for exclusion (see Figures and Tables
2016 6 22.22
Appendix).
2015 4 14.81
2014 4 14.81
2012 1 3.70 9.2 | Risk of bias in included studies
2011 2 7.41
9.2.1 | Randomized studies
2010 1 3.70
Table 2 summarizes the risk of bias for the 21 randomized studies
2008 1 3.70
included in the systematic review. A large percentage of studies
2007 2 7.41
failed to report information that would permit assessment of risk of
2000 1 3.70
bias; these were coded as unclear risk. Among those studies
1998 1 3.70
reporting sufficient information, most exhibited low risk of bias in
1997 1 3.70 the domains of random sequence generation, allocation concealment,
Not reported 1 3.70 blinding of participants, and selective reporting. The majority of
Educational setting randomized studies (95.2%) exhibited high risk of bias in blinding of
College/University 22 81.48 outcome assessment; these studies relied on self‐report outcome
Secondary school 5 18.52 data. Although a slight majority of randomized studies (52.4%)
Country exhibited low risk of bias in handling incomplete outcome data, one‐

United States 25 92.59


third exhibited high risk of bias. These studies with high risk of bias
tended to exhibit uneven attrition between the intervention and
Canada 1 3.70
comparisons groups or handled missing data inappropriately (e.g.,
India 1 3.70
mean imputation). The majority of randomized studies (81.0%)
exhibited high risk of bias in the “other” domain; these studies
tended to be conducted by researchers who either developed the

(b) an intervention group that was assigned to complete one 90‐min


T A B L E 2 Risk of bias of randomized studies
bystander program session, or (c) an intervention group that was
Low risk High risk Unclear risk
assigned to complete three 90‐min bystander program sessions.
Jouriles et al. (n.d.) randomly assigned participants to one of three n % n % n %

groups: (a) a comparison “sham treatment” condition that presented Random sequence 8 38.1 0 0 13 61.9
generation
material on study skills, (b) a computer‐delivered bystander program
that participants completed independently, or (c) a computer‐ Allocation concealment 3 14.3 0 0 18 85.7

delivered bystander program that participants completed in a lab Blinding of participants 1 4.8 0 0 20 95.2
under supervision. We handled these multiple‐arm studies by Blinding of outcome 1 4.8 20 95.2 0 0
selecting the intervention groups that were most consistent with assessment

the intervention groups from the other studies in the sample (i.e., the Incomplete outcome 11 52.4 7 33.3 3 14.3
data
single‐session treatment for Banyard et al. and the unmonitored
treatment for Jouriles et al.). We contrasted these intervention Selective reporting 19 90.5 2 9.5 0 0

groups with their comparison groups and included the calculated Other bias 3 14.3 17 81.0 1 4.8
effect sizes in our main meta‐analyses. We then conducted sensitivity Note: N = 21 studies.
16 of 40 | KETTREY ET AL.

T A B L E 3 Risk of bias of nonrandomized studies Salazar et al. indicating a significant positive effect 6 months post‐
Even Uneven intervention.
between between
groups groups Not reported
n % n % n % 9.3.2 | Rape myth acceptance
Confounding variables
Twelve studies measured rape myth acceptance as an intervention
Gender 4 66.7 1 16.7 1 16.7 outcome. These studies most often operationalized rape myth
Fraternity/sorority 2 33.3 0 0 4 66.7 acceptance using the Illinois Rape Myth Acceptance Scale (Payne,
Athlete 1 16.7 0 0 5 83.3 Lonsway, & Fitzgerald, 1999), which asks respondents to rate their
Preintervention 3 50.0 0 0 3 50.0 agreement with statements such as, “rape happens when a man’s sex
attitudes drive gets out of control.” All but four of these studies (Amar,
Prior victimization 0 0 1 16.7 5 83.3 Tuccinardi, Heislein, & Simpson, 2015; Baker, Naai, Mitchell, &
Confounding 0 0 1 16.7 5 83.3 Trecker, 2014; Foubert & Marriott, 1997; Peterson et al., 2016)
interventions involved random assignment of participants to conditions. All but one
Note: N = 6 studies. study (Brokenshire, 2015) was published in a peer‐reviewed outlet;
this study was an unpublished master’s thesis. We collapsed
bystander efficacy effect sizes into three follow‐up intervals: (a)
intervention under evaluation or were closely affiliated with the
immediate posttest (i.e., 0 week to 1 week), (b) 1 month to 4 months
developer of the intervention under evaluation.
post‐intervention, and (c) 6 to 7 months post‐intervention.

9.2.2 | Nonrandomized studies Immediate post‐test effects


Seven studies reported rape myth acceptance as an immediate
Table 3 summarizes the risk of bias for the six nonrandomized posttest outcome. As shown in the forest plot in Figure 2, the
studies included in the systematic review. Among the studies that intervention effect for the two nonrandomized studies was
reported group differences in confounding variables, most reported significant and positive (g = 0.65, 95% CI [0.23, 1.08]) with minimal
congruence between groups. This was often a product of the between‐study heterogeneity (χ 2 = 1.07 [p = .30], I2 = 6.75%,
targeted population. For example, three studies targeted a specific τ 2 = 0.01). The intervention effect for the five randomized studies
gender (i.e., young men or young women) and two specifically was also significant and positive (g = 0.30, 95% CI [0.03, 0.57]), with
targeted fraternity or sorority members. Only one study reported significant between‐study heterogeneity (χ 2 = 12.35 [p = .01],
group differences between participants’ previous experiences with I2 = 71.90%, τ 2 = 0.06). When we synthesized the findings from the
co‐interventions. The remaining studies either failed to report this randomized and nonrandomized studies together, the average
information (n = 3) or reported it at the aggregate level (n = 2). intervention effect was significant and positive (g = 0.37, 95%
CI [0.13, 0.61]) with significant between‐study heterogeneity (χ 2 =
9.3 | Synthesis of results 15.73 [p = .02], I2 = 66.36%, τ 2 = 0.06). Thus, at immediate post‐
intervention, the results indicate that bystander programs have a
9.3.1 | Victim empathy positive (beneficial) effect on rape myth acceptance. However, the
Two studies measured victim empathy as an intervention outcome small sample size in this meta‐analysis (n < 10) precluded ad hoc
(Brokenshire, 2015; Salazar et al., 2014). Both studies operationa- analysis of moderators or small study/publication bias.
lized victim empathy using the Rape Empathy Scale (Deitz, Blackwell,
Rape Myth Acceptance: Immediate Post-test
Daley, & Bentley, 1982), which asks participants to rate their Study Hedges g [95% CI]
agreement with statements such as “In general, I feel that rape is an Nonrandom Assignment
Amar et al. (2015) 0.56 [ 0.15 , 0.97 ]
act that is not provoked by the rape victim.” Both of these studies Baker et al. (2014) 1.16 [ 0.10 , 2.22 ]
Nonrandom 0.65 [ 0.23 , 1.08 ]
involved random assignment of participants to conditions at the Random Assignment
individual level. Salazar et al. was published in a peer‐reviewed Banyard et al. (2007) 0.26 [ 0.01 , 0.51 ]
Brokenshire (2015) -0.21 [ -0.56 , 0.14 ]
journal and Brokenshire was an unpublished master’s thesis. Because Foubert et al. (2007) 0.43 [ 0.25 , 0.61 ]
Foubert & McEwen (1998) 0.51 [ -0.14 , 1.16 ]
the two studies measured the outcome at very different time points, Moynihan et al. (2010) 0.59 [ 0.18 , 1.00 ]
Random 0.30 [ 0.03 , 0.57 ]
we could not synthesize effect sizes. Brokenshire measured victim Total 0.37 [ 0.13 , 0.61 ]
empathy as an immediate posttest outcome (g = −0.44, 95% CI
-1.00 0.00 1.00 2.00
[−0.79, −0.09]) and Salazar et al. reported victim empathy 6 months
Favors Control Favors Treatment
post‐intervention (g = 0.68, 95% CI [0.41, 0.95]). Notably, effect sizes
from these two studies are divergent, with Brokenshire indicating a F I G U R E 2 Forest plot of bystander intervention effects on rape
significant negative intervention effect at immediate posttest and myth acceptance – immediate posttest
KETTREY ET AL. | 17 of 40

Rape Myth Acceptance: Immediate Post-test Rape Myth Acceptance: 1 to 4 Months Post-test
Study Hedges g [95% CI] Study Hedges g [95% CI]
Nonrandom Assignment Nonrandom Assignment
Amar et al. (2015) 0.56 [ 0.15 , 0.97 ]
Baker et al. (2014) 0.82 [ -0.20 , 1.84 ]
Baker et al. (2014) 1.16 [ 0.10 , 2.22 ]
Nonrandom 0.65 [ 0.23 , 1.08 ] Foubert & Marriott (1997) 0.28 [ -0.50 , 1.06 ]
Random Assignment Peterson et al. (2016) 0.71 [ 0.26 , 1.16 ]
Banyard et al. (2007) Alt Tx Arm 0.56 [ 0.31 , 0.81 ] Nonrandom 0.63 [ 0.27 , 1.00 ]
Brokenshire (2015) -0.21 [ -0.56 , 0.14 ] Random Assignment
Foubert et al. (2007) 0.43 [ 0.25 , 0.61 ] Banyard et al. (2007) Alt Tx Arm 0.25 [ -0.04 , 0.54 ]
Foubert & McEwen (1998) 0.51 [ -0.14 , 1.16 ]
Gidycz et al. (2011) 0.11 [ -0.34 , 0.56 ]
Moynihan et al. (2010) 0.59 [ 0.18 , 1.00 ]
Random 0.37 [ 0.07 , 0.67 ] Moynihan et al. (2010) 0.48 [ 0.07 , 0.89 ]
Total 0.43 [ 0.18 , 0.68 ] Random 0.28 [ 0.07 , 0.49 ]
Total 0.38 [ 0.17 , 0.58 ]
-1.00 0.00 1.00 2.00
-1.00 0.00 1.00 2.00

F I G U R E 3 Forest plot of bystander intervention effects on rape Favors Control Favors Treatment

myth acceptance—immediate posttest with alternative Tx arm for


Banyard et al. (2007) F I G U R E 5 Forest plot of bystander intervention effects on rape
myth acceptance—1 to 4 months post‐intervention—with alternative
Tx arm for Banyard et al. (2007)
Rape Myth Acceptance: 1 to 4 Months Post-test
Study Hedges g [95% CI]
Nonrandom Assignment
Baker et al. (2014) 0.82 [ -0.20 , 1.84 ]
Foubert & Marriott (1997) 0.28 [ -0.50 , 1.06 ] 4‐month post‐intervention, bystander programs have a positive
Peterson et al. (2016) 0.71 [ 0.26 , 1.16 ]
(beneficial) effect on rape myth acceptance. However, the small
Nonrandom 0.63 [ 0.27 , 1.00 ]
Random Assignment sample size in this meta‐analysis (n < 10) precluded ad hoc analysis of
Banyard et al. (2007) 0.19 [ -0.10 , 0.48 ]
Gidycz et al. (2011) 0.11 [ -0.34 , 0.56 ]
moderators or small study/publication bias.
Moynihan et al. (2010) 0.48 [ 0.07 , 0.89 ] Sensitivity analysis in which the Banyard et al. (2007) one‐session
Random 0.25 [ 0.04 , 0.46 ]
Total 0.36 [ 0.14 , 0.59 ] intervention was replaced by the three‐session intervention did not
substantively change these findings. As illustrated in Figure 5, the
-1.00 0.00 1.00 2.00
treatment effect for the five randomized studies (which included
Favors Control Favors Treatment
Banyard et al., 2007) was significant and positive (g = 0.28, 95% CI
F I G U R E 4 Forest plot of bystander intervention effects on rape [0.07, 0.49]) with nonsignificant heterogeneity (χ 2 = 1.49 [p = .47],
myth acceptance—1 to 4 months post‐intervention I2 = 0.00%, τ 2 = 0). Across all studies, the average intervention effect
(for randomized and nonrandomized studies) was significant and
Sensitivity analysis in which the Banyard et al. (2007) one‐session positive (g = 0.38, 95% CI [0.17, 0.58]) with nonsignificant
intervention was replaced by the three‐session intervention did not heterogeneity (χ 2 = 5.18 [p = .39], I2 = 12.40%, τ 2 = 0.01).
substantively change these findings. As illustrated in Figure 3, the
treatment effect for the five randomized studies (which included
Six to seven month post‐intervention effects
Banyard et al., 2007) was significant and positive (g = 0.37, 95% CI
Four studies reported rape myth acceptance outcomes at 6 to
[0.07, 0.67]) with significant between‐study heterogeneity (χ 2 =
7 months post‐intervention. Each of these studies involved random
14.04 [p = .01], I2 = 76.48%, τ 2 = 0.08). When synthesizing all studies
assignment of participants to conditions. As shown in the forest plot
together, the average intervention was significant and positive (g =
in Figure 6, the average intervention effect across these four studies
0.43, 95% CI [0.18, 0.68]) with significant between‐study hetero-
was significant and positive (g = 0.38, 95% CI [0.17, 0.58]) with
geneity (χ 2 = 16.47 [p = .01], I2 = 68.55%, τ 2 = 0.07).
nonsignificant between‐study heterogeneity (χ 2 = 5.18 [p = .39],

One to four month post‐intervention effects


Six studies reported rape myth acceptance 1 to 4 months Rape Myth Acceptance: 6 to 7 Months Post-test
Study Hedges g [95% CI]
post‐intervention. As depicted in the forest plot in Figure 4 the
intervention effect for the three nonrandomized studies Foubert et al. (2007) 0.01 [ -0.17 , 0.19 ]
was significant and positive (g = 0.63, 95% CI [0.27, 1.00]) with Foubert (2000) 0.23 [ -0.40 , 0.86 ]
nonsignificant heterogeneity (χ 2 = 1.02 [p = .60], I2 = 0.00%, τ 2 = 0).
Gidycz et al. (2011) 0.06 [ -0.39 , 0.51 ]
The intervention effect for the three randomized studies was
Salazar et al. (2014) 0.99 [ 0.70 , 1.28 ]
significant and positive (g = 0.25, 95% CI [0.04, 0.46]) with
Total 0.38 [ 0.17 , 0.58 ]
nonsignificant heterogeneity (χ 2 = 1.73 [p = .42], I2 = 0.00%,
-0.50 0.00 0.50 1.00 1.50
τ 2 = 0). Across all studies, the average intervention effect was
Favors Control Favors Treatment
significant and positive (g = 0.36, 95% CI [0.14, 0.59]) with
nonsignificant heterogeneity (χ 2 = 5.90 [p = .32], I2 = 23.73%, F I G U R E 6 Forest plot of bystander intervention effects on rape
τ 2 = 0.02). This average intervention effect indicates that, at 1‐ to myth acceptance—6 to 7 months post‐intervention
18 of 40 | KETTREY ET AL.

I2 = 12.40%, τ 2 = 0.01). These findings indicate that bystander Gender Attitudes: 6 Months to 1 Year Post-test
Study Hedges g [95% CI]
programs have a positive (beneficial) effect on rape myth acceptance
Nonrandom Assignment
6 to 7 months post‐intervention. However, the small sample size in
Miller et al. (2014) 0.05 [ -0.30 , 0.40 ]
this meta‐analysis (n < 10) precluded ad hoc analysis of moderators Nonrandom 0.05 [ -0.30 , 0.40 ]
Random Assignment
or small study/publication bias.
Gidycz et al. (2011) 0.05 [ -0.40 , 0.50 ]
Miller et al. (2013) -0.17 [ -0.46 , 0.12 ]
Salazar et al. (2014) 0.66 [ 0.39 , 0.93 ]
Random 0.19 [ -0.32 , 0.70 ]
9.3.3 | Gender attitudes
Total 0.16 [ -0.22 , 0.53 ]

Six studies measured gender attitudes as an intervention outcome. -0.50 0.00 0.50 1.00
These studies operationalized gender attitudes or gender‐role Favors Control Favors Treatment

ideology through multiple‐item scales consisting of statements such


F I G U R E 8 Forest plot of bystander intervention effects on
as “If men pay for a date, they deserve something in return.” gender attitudes—6 months to 1 year post‐intervention
All but two of these studies (Miller et al., 2014; Peterson et al.,
2016) involved random assignment of participants to conditions. All gender attitudes 2 to 4 months post‐intervention. Again, however,

but one study (Brokenshire, 2015) was published in a peer‐reviewed the small sample size in this meta‐analysis (n < 10) precluded ad hoc

outlet; this study was an unpublished master’s thesis. We collapsed analysis of moderators or small study/publication bias.

gender attitude effect sizes into three timing intervals: (a) immediate
posttest (i.e., 0 week to 1 week), (b) 2 to 4 months post‐intervention, Six month to one‐year intervention effects
and (c) 6 months to 1 year post‐intervention. Four studies reported gender attitudes 6 months to 1 year post‐
intervention. As shown in the forest plot in Figure 8, the intervention
effect for the one nonrandomized study was nonsignificant (g = 0.05,
Immediate post‐test effects
95% CI [−0.30, 0.40]). The intervention effect for the three
One study (Brokenshire, 2015) reported gender attitudes as an
randomized studies was nonsignificant (g = 0.19, 95% CI [−0.32,
immediate posttest. This study indicated that bystander programs
0.70]) with significant heterogeneity (χ 2 = 17.15 [p = .00],
had a nonsignificant but negative effect on gender attitudes at 2
I = 85.94%, τ 2
= 0.17). Across all studies (randomized and
immediate posttest (g = −0.41, 95% CI [−0.88, 0.06]).
nonrandomized), the average intervention effect was nonsignificant
(g = 0.16, 95% CI [−0.22, 0.53]) with significant heterogeneity
Two to four month intervention effects (χ 2 = 18.00 [p = .00], I2 = 80.07%, τ 2 = 0.12). Thus, there is no
Three studies reported gender attitudes 2 to 4 months post‐ evidence that bystander programs had an effect on gender attitude
intervention. As shown in the forest plot in Figure 7, the intervention outcomes 6 months to 1 year post‐intervention. The small sample
effect for the one nonrandomized study was significant and positive size in this meta‐analysis (n < 10) precluded ad hoc analysis of
(g = 0.79, 95% CI [0.32, 1.26]). The average effect for the two moderators or small study/publication bias.
randomized studies was nonsignificant (g = −0.10, 95% CI [−0.35,
0.75]) with nonsignificant heterogeneity (χ 2 = 0.43 [p = .51], I2 =
9.3.4 | Date rape attitudes
0.00%, τ 2 = 0). Across all studies (randomized and nonrandomized),
the average intervention effect was nonsignificant (g = 0.20, 95% CI Four studies measured data rape attitudes. These studies operatio-
[−0.35, 0.75]) with significant between‐study heterogeneity (χ 2 = nalized date rape attitudes with multi‐item scales such as the College
12.02 [p = .00], I2 = 84.17%, τ 2 = 0.20). Thus, these findings indicate Date Rate Attitude Survey (Lanier & Elliot, 1997) and the Rape
that there is no evidence that bystander programs have an effect on Attitudes and Beliefs Scale (Burgess, 2007). Such scales asked
respondents to rate their approval of statements such as, “Many
Gender Attitudes: 2 to 4 Months Post-test
women pretend they don’t want to have sex because they don’t want
Study Hedges g [95% CI]
to appear ‘easy.’” All four of these studies involved random
Nonrandom Assignment
assignment of participants to conditions. Two of these studies were
Peterson et al. (2016) 0.79 [ 0.32 , 1.26 ]
Nonrandom 0.79 [ 0.32 , 1.26 ]
published in peer‐reviewed outlets (Banyard et al., 2007; Salazar
Random Assignment et al., 2014) and two were unpublished theses/dissertations
Gidycz et al. (2011) 0.03 [ -0.42 , 0.48 ] (Brokenshire, 2015; Chiriboga, 2016). More than one study reported
Miller et al. (2013) -0.14 [ -0.38 , 0.10 ] immediate posttest effects, so we synthesized these together. The
Random -0.10 [ -0.31 , 0.10 ]
timing of follow‐up waves was too disparate between studies to
Total 0.20 [ -0.35 , 0.75 ]
permit synthesis; thus, we report these results individually.
-0.50 0.00 0.50 1.00 1.50
Favors Control Favors Treatment
Immediate post‐test effects
F I G U R E 7 Forest plot of bystander intervention effects on Three studies reported date rape attitudes as an immediate posttest
gender attitudes—2 to 4 months post‐intervention outcome. As shown in Figure 9, there was no evidence that bystander
KETTREY ET AL. | 19 of 40

Noticing Sexual Assault: Immediate Post-test


Study Hedges g [95% CI]

Nonrandom Assignment
Date Rape Attitudes: Immediate Post-test
Senn & Forrest (2016) 0.26 [ 0.06 , 0.46 ]
Study Hedges g [95% CI] Nonrandom 0.26 [ 0.06 , 0.46 ]
Random Assignment
Banyard et al. (2007) 0.19 [ -0.06 , 0.44 ] Brokenshire (2015) -0.14 [ -0.81 , 0.53 ]
Random -0.14 [ -0.81 , 0.53 ]
Brokenshire (2015) -0.30 [ -0.65 , 0.05 ]
Total 0.19 [ -0.10 , 0.49 ]
Chiriboga (2016) 0.25 [ -0.20 , 0.70 ]
-1.00 -0.50 0.00 0.50 1.00
Total 0.04 [ -0.29 , 0.38 ]
Favors Control Favors Treatment
-1.00 -0.50 0.00 0.50 1.00
Favors Control Favors Treatment F I G U R E 1 1 Forest plot of bystander intervention effects on
noticing sexual assault —immediate posttest
F I G U R E 9 Forest plot of bystander intervention effects on date
rape attitudes at immediate posttest

interventions had an effect on date rape attitudes at immediate 9.3.5 | Noticing a sexual assault or its warning signs
follow‐up (g = 0.04, 95% CI [−0.29, 0.38]; χ 2
= 5.68 [p = .06], Four studies reported a measure of whether participants noticed
I2 = 64.81%, τ 2 = 0.06). The small sample size in this meta‐analysis
a sexual assault occurring. Studies operationalized this outcome
(n < 10) precluded ad hoc analysis of moderators or small study/ in a number of ways, including a single item developed by Burn
publication bias. (2009): “At a party or bar, I am probably too busy to be aware of
Sensitivity analysis in which we replaced the Banyard et al. (2007) whether someone is at risk for sexually assaulting someone.” All
one‐session intervention with the three‐session intervention did not but one of these studies (Senn & Forrest, 2016) involved random
substantively change these findings (g = 0.07, 95% CI [−0.30, 0.45])
assignment of participants to conditions and all but one
and indicated significant between‐study heterogeneity in effects (Brokenshire, 2015) were published in a peer‐reviewed outlet.
(χ 2 = 7.07 [p = .03], I2 = 70.93%, τ 2 = 0.08). See Figure 10.
This specific report was an unpublished master’s thesis. We
collapsed noticing sexual assault effect sizes into three timing
intervals: (a) immediate posttest (i.e., 0 week to 1 week),
Follow‐up effects (b) 1 month to 4 months, and (c) 1 year.
Two studies reported a date rape attitude outcome beyond the
immediate posttest. The timing of these measures was too disparate
Immediate post‐test effects
to permit synthesis of effect sizes. Banyard et al. (2007) reported
Two studies reported noticing sexual assault as an immediate posttest
date rape attitudes 2 months post‐intervention for both the (focal)
outcome (i.e., 0 week to 1 week post intervention). As shown in the
one‐session intervention (g = 0.23, 95% CI [−0.06, 0.52]) and the
forest plot in Figure 11, the intervention effect for the one
(alternative) three‐session intervention (g = 0.21, 95% CI [−0.06,
nonrandomized study was significant and positive (g = 0.26, 95% CI
0.48]). Neither of these intervention effects was statistically
[0.06, 0.46]). Conversely, the effect for the one randomized
significant. Salazar et al. (2014) reported date rape attitudes 6
study was nonsignificant (g = −0.14, 95% CI [−0.81, 0.53]). Across the
months post‐intervention and reported a positive and statistically
two studies, the average intervention effect was nonsignificant
significant intervention effect (g = 0.91, 95% CI [0.64, 1.18]).
(g = 0.19, 95% CI [−0.10, 0.45]) with nonsignificant heterogeneity
(χ 2 = 1.27 [p = .26], I2 = 21.50%, τ2 = 0.02). Thus, at immediate post‐
Date Rape Attitudes: Immediate Post-test
intervention, there was no evidence that bystander programs have an
Study Hedges g [95% CI] effect on participants’ reports of noticing a sexual assault. The small
sample size in this meta‐analysis (n < 10) precluded ad hoc analysis of
Banyard et al. (2007) - Alt Tx Arm 0.27 [ 0.02 , 0.52 ] moderators or small study/publication bias.

Brokenshire (2015) -0.30 [ -0.65 , 0.05 ]

Chiriboga (2016) 0.25 [ -0.20 , 0.70 ] One to four‐month follow‐up effects


Total 0.07 [ -0.30 , 0.45 ]
Three studies reported noticing sexual assault as an outcome 1 to
4‐month post‐intervention. As shown in the forest plot in Figure 12,
-1.00 -0.50 0.00 0.50 1.00 the intervention effect for the one nonrandomized study was
Favors Control Favors Treatment
nonsignificant (g = 0.11, 95% CI [−0.09, 0.31]). The average effect

F I G U R E 1 0 Forest plot of bystander intervention effects on date for the two randomized studies was also nonsignificant (g = −0.04,
rape attitudes at immediate posttest—sensitivity analysis with 95% CI [−0.26, 0.17]) with nonsignificant heterogeneity (χ 2 = 0.60
alternative treatment arm for Banyard et al. (2007) [p = .44], I2 = 0.00%, τ 2 = 0). Across all studies (randomized and
20 of 40 | KETTREY ET AL.

Noticing Sexual Assault: 1 to 4 Months Post-test Identifying Situation: Immediate Post-test


Study Hedges g [95% CI] Study Hedges g [95% CI]
Nonrandom Assignment Nonrandom Assignment
Senn & Forrest (2016) 0.11 [ -0.09 , 0.31 ] Amar et al. (2015) -0.30 [ -0.71 , 0.11 ]
Baker et al. (2014) 1.40 [ 0.30 , 2.50 ]
Nonrandom 0.11 [ -0.09 , 0.31 ]
Senn & Forrest (2016) 0.52 [ 0.32 , 0.72 ]
Random Assignment Nonrandom 0.43 [ -0.43 , 1.30 ]
Miller et al. (2013) -0.01 [ -0.25 , 0.23 ] Random Assignment
Moynihan et al. (2011) -0.26 [ -0.85 , 0.33 ] Addison (2015) 0.95 [ 0.56 , 1.34 ]
Random -0.04 [ -0.26 , 0.17 ] Banyard et al. (2007) 0.65 [ 0.40 , 0.90 ]
Random 0.76 [ 0.48 , 1.05 ]
Total 0.04 [ -0.10 , 0.19 ]
Total 0.57 [ 0.08 , 1.05 ]
-1.00 -0.50 0.00 0.50
-1.00 0.00 1.00 2.00 3.00
Favors Control Favors Treatment
Favors Control Favors Treatment

F I G U R E 1 2 Forest plot of bystander intervention effects on


F I G U R E 1 3 Forest plot of bystander intervention effects on
noticing sexual assault—1 to 4 months post‐intervention
identifying a situation as appropriate for intervention—immediate
posttest

nonrandomized), the average intervention effect was nonsignificant heterogeneity across studies (χ 2 = 15.74 [p = .00], I2 = 91.24%,
(g = 0.04, 95% CI [−0.10, 0.19]) with nonsignificant heterogeneity τ 2 = 0.49). The average intervention effect for the randomized
(χ 2 = 1.66 [p = .44], I2 = 0.00%, τ 2 = 0). Thus, there was no evidence studies was significant and positive (g = 0.76, 95% CI [0.48, 1.05]),
that bystander programs have an effect on participants’ reports of however, with nonsignificant heterogeneity (χ 2 = 1.58 [p = .21],
noticing a sexual assault 1 to 4‐month post‐intervention. The small I2 = 36.78%, τ 2 = 0.02). Across all studies (randomized and
sample size in this meta‐analysis (n < 10) precluded ad hoc analysis of nonrandomized), the average intervention effect was significant
moderators or small study/publication bias. and positive (g = 0.57, 95% CI [0.08, 1.05]) with significant
heterogeneity across studies (χ 2 = 23.31 [p = .00], I2 = 89.71%,
One year follow‐up effects τ 2 = 0.25). Thus, at immediate post‐intervention, bystander programs
One study reported noticing sexual assault as an intervention have a positive (beneficial) and significant beneficial effect on
outcome 1 year post‐intervention. The effect size from this study participants’ identification of a situation as appropriate for interven-
(Miller et al., 2013) was nonsignificant (g = −0.06, 95% CI [−0.13, tion. The small sample size in this meta‐analysis (n < 10) precluded ad
0.41]), thus providing no evidence that the program in this hoc analysis of moderators or small study/publication bias.
study affected respondents’ noticing a sexual assault 1 year We conducted a sensitivity analysis in which we replaced the
post‐intervention. Banyard et al. (2007) one‐session intervention arm with the three‐
session intervention arm. As shown in Figure 14, findings were
substantively similar to those from the main analysis. The interven-
9.3.6 | Identifying a situation as appropriate for tion effect for the two randomized studies (which included Banyard
intervention et al.) was still significant and positive (g = 1.23, 95% CI [0.71, 1.75];
Six studies reported a measure of participants’ identification of a χ 2 = 4.49 [p = .03], I2 = 77.75%, τ 2 = 0.011). The average intervention
situation as appropriate for intervention. Studies frequently oper- effect across all studies was also still significant and positive (g = 0.77,
ationalized this concept using some adaptation of Burn’s (2009) 95% CI [0.12, 1.42]; χ 2 = 56.05 [p = .00], I2 = 93.82%, τ 2 = 0.47).
Failure to Identify Situation as High Risk scale (e.g., “In a party or bar Importantly, the replacement of Banyard et al.’s one‐session
situation, I think I might be uncertain as to whether someone is at‐ intervention arm with their three‐session intervention arm increased
risk for being sexually assaulted”). All but three of these studies
(Amar et al., 2015; Baker et al., 2014; Senn & Forrest, 2016) involved Identifying Situation: Immediate Post-test
random assignment of participants to conditions and all but one Study Hedges g [95% CI]
Nonrandom Assignment
(Addison, 2015) was published in a peer‐reviewed outlet. This Amar et al. (2015) -0.30 [ -0.71 , 0.11 ]
specific report was an unpublished doctoral dissertation. We Baker et al. (2014) 1.40 [ 0.30 , 2.50 ]
Senn & Forrest (2016) 0.52 [ 0.32 , 0.72 ]
collapsed noticing sexual assault effect sizes into three timing Nonrandom 0.43 [ -0.43 , 1.30 ]
intervals: (a) immediate posttest (i.e., 0 week to 1 week), (b) 1 month Random Assignment
Addison (2015) 0.95 [ 0.56 , 1.34 ]
to 4 months post‐intervention, and (c) 6 months post‐intervention.
Banyard et al. (2007) - Alt Tx Arm 1.48 [ 1.19 , 1.77 ]
Random 1.23 [ 0.71 , 1.75 ]
Total 0.77 [ 0.12 , 1.42 ]
Immediate post‐test effects
Five studies reported identification of a situation as appropriate for -1.00 0.00 1.00 2.00 3.00

intervention as an immediate posttest outcome (i.e., 0 week to 1 Favors Control Favors Treatment

week post‐intervention). As shown in the forest plot in Figure 13. The F I G U R E 1 4 Forest plot of bystander intervention effects on
average intervention effect for the nonrandomized studies was identifying a situation as appropriate for intervention—immediate
nonsignificant (g = 0.43, 95% CI [−0.43, 1.30]) with significant posttest with alternative Tx arm for Banyard et al. (2007)
KETTREY ET AL. | 21 of 40

the magnitude of the average intervention effect for the randomized Identifying Situation: 1 to 4 Months Post-test
Study Hedges g [95% CI]
studies (0.76 to 1.23) as well as for the total sample (0.57 to 0.77). Nonrandom Assignment
Baker et al. (2014) 1.17 [ 0.09 , 2.25 ]
Senn & Forrest (2016) 0.26 [ 0.06 , 0.46 ]
Nonrandom 0.55 [ -0.28 , 1.39 ]
One to four‐month follow‐up effects Random Assignment
Three studies reported identification of a situation as appropriate for Banyard et al. (2007) - Alt Tx Arm 1.07 [ 0.78 , 1.36 ]
Random 1.07 [ 0.78 , 1.36 ]
intervention 1‐ to 4‐month post‐intervention. The effect size for one Total 0.75 [ 0.13 , 1.37 ]
of these studies (Senn & Forrest, 2016) fell more than two standard
0.00 1.00 2.00
deviations below the mean of the distribution for the total sample
Favors Control Favors Treatment
(g = 0.26) so we Winsorized it by replacing it with the value that fell
exactly two standard deviations below the mean (g = 0.33). As shown F I G U R E 1 6 Forest plot of bystander intervention effects on
in the forest plot in Figure 15, the average intervention effect for the identifying a situation as appropriate for intervention – 1‐ to
4‐month follow‐up with alternative treatment arm for Banyard et al.
two nonrandomized studies was nonsignificant (g = 0.58, 95% CI
(2007)
[−0.17, 1.33]) with nonsignificant heterogeneity (χ 2 = 2.26 [p = .13],
I2 = 55.71%, τ 2 = 0.20). The intervention effect for the one revealed that there were no outliers in the total sample containing
randomized study was significant and positive (g = 0.46, 95% Banyard et al.’s three‐session intervention arm. As indicated in
CI [0.17, 0.75]). Across all studies (randomized and nonrandomized), Figure 16, findings were substantively similar to those from the main
the average intervention effect was significant and positive (g = 0.39, analysis. The intervention effect for Banyard et al. (2007), the one
95% CI [0.23, 0.55]) with nonsignificant heterogeneity across studies randomized study in the sample, remained significant and positive
(χ 2 = 2.59 [p = .27], I2 = 0.01%, τ 2 = 0). Thus, the overall mean effect (g = 1.07, 95% CI [0.78, 1.36]). The overall average effect for
size indicates that bystander programs have a significant beneficial randomized and nonrandomized studies) also remained significant
effect on participants’ identification of a situation as appropriate for and positive (g = 0.75, 95% CI [0.13, 1.37]) with significant
intervention 1‐ to 4‐month post‐intervention. The small sample size heterogeneity across studies (χ 2 = 21.60 [p = .00], I2 = 88.54%,
in this meta‐analysis (n < 10) precluded ad hoc analysis of moderators τ 2 = 0.23).
or small study/publication bias.
We conducted a sensitivity analysis to determine whether
Winsorizing the effect size for Senn and Forrest (2016) affected Six month follow‐up effects
our overall findings. Running the meta‐analysis with the original One study reported participants’ identification of a situation as
effect size for this study produced nearly identical findings to those appropriate for intervention as an outcome 6 months post‐interven-
from the main analysis for both the two nonrandomized studies (g = tion. The effect size from this study (Salazar et al., 2014) was
0.55, 95% CI [−0.28, 1.39]) and the total sample containing both significant and positive (g = 1.56, 95% CI [1.25, 1.87]).
randomized and nonrandomized studies (g = 0.37, 95% CI [0.15,
0.59]). Tests for heterogeneity revealed nonsignificant heterogeneity,
9.3.7 | Taking responsibility for acting/intervening
although specific values were slightly different than those from the
main analysis for both the nonrandomized studies (χ 2 = 2.65 [p = Four studies reported a measure of participants’ taking responsibility
.10], I2 = 62.26%, τ 2 = 0.26) and the total sample (χ 2 = 3.56 [p = .17], for acting/intervening when witnessing violence or its warning signs.
I2 = 26.48%, τ 2 = 0.01). Studies operationalized this outcome using either the full or an
We also conducted a sensitivity analysis in which we replaced the adapted version of the Failure to Take Intervention Responsibility
Banyard et al. (2007) one‐session intervention arm with the three‐ scale developed by Burn (2009), which consisted of items such as “I
session intervention arm. Inspection of the distribution of effect sizes am less likely to intervene to reduce a person’s risk of sexual assault
if I think she/he made choices that increased their risk.” Two studies
Identifying Situation: 1 to 4 Months Post-test (Banyard et al., 2007; Moynihan, Banyard, Arnold, Eckstein, &
Study Hedges g [95% CI]
Nonrandom Assignment Stapleton, 2011) involved random assignment of participants to
Baker et al. (2014) 1.17 [ 0.09 , 2.25 ] conditions and two (Amar et al., 2015; Senn & Forrest, 2016) did not.
Senn & Forrest (2016) 0.33 [ 0.13 , 0.53 ]
Nonrandom 0.58 [ -0.17 , 1.33 ] All four studies were published in a peer‐reviewed outlet. We
Random Assignment collapsed noticing sexual assault effect sizes into two timing
Banyard et al. (2007) 0.46 [ 0.17 , 0.75 ]
Random 0.46 [ 0.17 , 0.75 ] intervals: (a) immediate posttest (i.e., 0 week to 1 week) and (b) 1
Total 0.39 [ 0.23 , 0.55 ] month to 4 months post‐intervention.
0.00 1.00 2.00
Favors Control Favors Treatment
Immediate post‐test effects
F I G U R E 1 5 Forest plot of bystander intervention effects on Three studies reported taking responsibility for acting/intervening as
identifying a situation as appropriate for intervention – 1‐ to an immediate posttest outcome (i.e., 0 week to 1 week post‐
4‐month follow‐up intervention). As shown in the forest plot in Figure 17, the
22 of 40 | KETTREY ET AL.

Taking Responsibility: Immediate Post-test Taking Responsibility: 1 to 4 Months Post-test


Study Hedges g [95% CI] Study Hedges g [95% CI]

Nonrandom Assignment Nonrandom Assignment


Amar et al. (2015) -0.50 [ -0.91 , -0.09 ] Senn & Forrest (2016) 0.16 [ -0.04 , 0.36 ]
Senn & Forrest (2016) 0.37 [ 0.17 , 0.57 ]
Nonrandom 0.16 [ -0.04 , 0.36 ]
Nonrandom -0.05 [ -0.90 , 0.81 ]
Random Assignment Random Assignment
Banyard et al. (2007) 0.96 [ 0.69 , 1.23 ] Banyard et al. (2007) 0.55 [ 0.26 , 0.84 ]
Random 0.96 [ 0.69 , 1.23 ] Moynihan et al. (2011) 0.29 [ -0.30 , 0.88 ]
Total 0.29 [ -0.53 , 1.11 ]
Random 0.50 [ 0.24 , 0.76 ]
Total 0.32 [ 0.04 , 0.61 ]
-1.00 0.00 1.00
Favors Control Favors Treatment -0.50 0.00 0.50 1.00
Favors Control Favors Treatment
F I G U R E 1 7 Forest plot of bystander intervention effects on
taking responsibility to act/intervene – immediate posttest F I G U R E 1 9 Forest plot of bystander intervention effects on
taking responsibility to act/intervene – 1 to 4 months posttest

intervention effect for the two nonrandomized studies was non-


significant (g = −0.05, 95% CI [−0.90, 0.81]) with significant One to four‐month follow‐up effects
heterogeneity across studies (χ 2 = 14.00 [p = .00], I2 = 92.85%, Three studies reported taking responsibility for acting/intervening
τ = 0.35). The intervention effect for the randomized study was
2
1‐ to 4‐month post‐intervention. As shown in the forest plot in
significant and positive (g = 0.96, 95% CI [0.69, 1.23]). The overall Figure 19, the intervention effect for the one nonrandomized study
average intervention effect (for the randomized and nonrandomized was nonsignificant (g = 0.16, 95% CI [−0.04, 0.36]). The average
studies) was nonsignificant (g = 0.29, 95% CI [−0.53, 1.11]) with intervention effect for the two randomized studies was significant
significant heterogeneity (χ = 34.30 [p = .00], I = 96.07%, τ = 0.50).
2 2 2
and positive (g = 0.50, 95% CI [0.24, 0.76]) with nonsignificant
The overall average intervention effect indicates that, at immediate heterogeneity (χ 2 = 0.60 [p = .44], I2 = 0.00%, τ 2 = 0). Overall, the
post‐intervention, there is no evidence that bystander programs have average intervention effect (for the randomized and nonrandomized
an effect on participants’ taking responsibility for acting/intervening studies) was significant and positive (g = 0.32, 95% CI [0.04, 0.61])
when witnessing violence or its warning signs. The small sample size with nonsignificant heterogeneity (χ 2 = 4.68 [p = .10], I2 = 56.86%,
in this meta‐analysis (n < 10) precluded ad hoc analysis of moderators τ 2 = 0.03). Thus, the results indicate that bystander programs have a
or small study/publication bias. significant positive (beneficial) effect on participants’ taking respon-
We conducted a sensitivity analysis in which we replaced the sibility for acting/intervening when witnessing violence or its warning
Banyard et al. (2007) one‐session intervention arm with the three‐ signs 1‐ to 4‐month post‐intervention. The small sample size in this
session intervention arm. As indicated in Figure 18, findings were meta‐analysis (n < 10) precluded ad hoc analysis of moderators or
substantively similar to those from the main analysis. The interven- small study/publication bias.
tion effect for Banyard et al. (2007), the single randomized study, was We conducted a sensitivity analysis in which we replaced the
significant and positive (g = 1.51, 95% CI [1.22, 1.80]). The overall Banyard et al. (2007) one‐session intervention arm with the three‐
average intervention effect (for randomized and nonrandomized session intervention arm. As indicated in Figure 20, substituting this
studies) was nonsignificant (g = 0.47, 95% CI [−0.67, 1.60]) with intervention arm resulted in substantively different findings from
significant heterogeneity across studies (χ 2 = 69.20 [p = .00], I2 = those in the main analysis. The intervention effect for the two
97.83%, τ 2 = 0.98). randomized studies (which included Banyard et al., 2007) remained
significant and positive (g = 0.67, 95% CI [0.04, 1.30]) with

Taking Responsibility: Immediate Post-test


Study Hedges g [95% CI] Taking Responsibility: 1 to 4 Months Post-test
Study Hedges g [95% CI]
Nonrandom Assignment
Nonrandom Assignment
Amar et al. (2015) -0.50 [ -0.91 , -0.09 ]
Senn & Forrest (2016) 0.16 [ -0.04 , 0.36 ]
Senn & Forrest (2016) 0.37 [ 0.17 , 0.57 ]
Nonrandom 0.16 [ -0.04 , 0.36 ]
Nonrandom -0.05 [ -0.90 , 0.81 ]
Random Assignment
Random Assignment
Banyard et al. (2007) - Alt Tx Arm 0.94 [ 0.65 , 1.23 ]
Banyard et al. (2007) - Alt Tx Arm 1.51 [ 1.22 , 1.80 ]
Moynihan et al. (2011) 0.29 [ -0.30 , 0.88 ]
Random 1.51 [ 1.22 , 1.80 ]
Random 0.67 [ 0.04 , 1.30 ]
Total 0.47 [ -0.67 , 1.60 ]
Total 0.47 [ -0.04 , 0.98 ]

-1.00 0.00 1.00 2.00 -0.50 0.00 0.50 1.00 1.50


Favors Control Favors Treatment Favors Control Favors Treatment

F I G U R E 1 8 Forest plot of bystander intervention effects on F I G U R E 2 0 Forest plot of bystander intervention effects on
taking responsibility to act/intervene – immediate posttest with taking responsibility to act/intervene – 1‐ to 4‐month posttest with
alternative Tx arm for Banyard et al. (2007) alternative Tx arm for Banyard et al. (2007)
KETTREY ET AL. | 23 of 40

nonsignificant heterogeneity (χ 2 = 3.76 [p = .05], I2 = 73.37%, meta‐analysis (n < 10) precluded ad hoc analysis of moderators or
τ 2
= 0.16). However, the overall average intervention effect (for small study/publication bias.
randomized and nonrandomized studies) was nonsignificant (g = 0.47,
95% CI [−0.04, 0.98]) with significant heterogeneity across studies Four‐month follow‐up effects
(χ 2 = 18.84 [p = .00], I2 = 86.59%, τ 2 = 0.17). One study (Senn & Forrest, 2016) reported knowing strategies for
helping/intervening 4 months post‐intervention. The effect size for
this study was significant and positive (g = 0.63, 95% CI [0.43, 0.83]).
9.3.8 | Knowing strategies for helping/intervening
Four studies reported a measure of participants’ knowledge of
9.3.9 | Bystander efficacy
strategies for helping/intervening. Studies operationalized this
concept using Burn’s (2009) two‐item Failure to Intervene Due to Eleven studies reported a measure of bystander efficacy as an
Skills Deficit scale, which asked respondents to rate their agreement outcome. These studies operationalized bystander efficacy using a
with statements such as “Although I would like to intervene when a modified or adapted version of the Bystander Efficacy Scale (Banyard
guy’s sexual conduct is questionable, I am not sure I would know et al., 2007). This scale asked participants to rate their confidence in
what to say or do.” Two studies (Brokenshire, 2015; Potter, their ability to perform behaviors such as “try and stop or discourage
Stapleton, & Moynihan, 2008) involved random assignment of someone who is spreading rumors online about another person’s
participants to conditions and two (Amar et al., 2015, Senn & body or sexual behavior” or “do something to help a very drunk
Forrest, 2016) did not. All but one study (Brokenshire, 2015) was person who is being brought upstairs to a bedroom by a group of
published in a peer‐reviewed outlet. This specific report was an people at a party.” All but three of these studies (Baker et al., 2014;
unpublished master’s thesis. We collapsed knowing strategies for Peterson et al., 2016; Senn & Forrest, 2016) involved random
helping/intervening into two timing intervals: (a) immediate posttest assignment of participants to conditions and all but one (Addison,
(i.e., 0 week to 2 weeks) and (b) 4 months post‐intervention. 2015) was published in a peer‐reviewed outlet. This specific report
was an unpublished dissertation. We collapsed bystander efficacy
Immediate post‐test effects effect sizes into three timing intervals: (a) immediate posttest (i.e.,
Four studies reported knowing strategies for helping/intervening as 0 week to 1 week), (b) 1 month to 4 months post‐intervention, and (3)
an immediate posttest outcome (i.e., 0 week to 2 weeks post‐ 6 months post‐intervention.
intervention). As shown in the forest plot in Figure 21, the average
intervention effect for the two nonrandomized studies was non- Immediate post‐test effects
significant (g = 0.33, 95% CI [−0.70, 1.36]) with significant Eight studies reported bystander efficacy as an immediate posttest
heterogeneity across studies (χ 2 = 20.38 [p = .00], I2 = 95.10%, outcome (i.e., 0 week to 1 week post‐intervention). As shown in the
τ 2 = 0.52). The treatment effect for the randomized studies was forest plot in Figure 22, the average intervention effect for the two
nonsignificant (g = 0.83, 95% CI [−0.03, 1.69]) with nonsignificant nonrandomized studies was significant and positive (g = 0.23, 95% CI
heterogeneity (χ 2 = 2.29 [p = .13], I2 = 56.32%, τ 2 = 0.22). Overall, [0.04, 0.43]) with nonsignificant heterogeneity (χ 2 = 0.53 [p = .46],
the average intervention effect (for the randomized and nonrando- I2 = 0.00%, τ 2 = 0). The average intervention effect for the six
mized studies) was nonsignificant (g = 0.54, 95% CI [−0.09, 1.17]) randomized studies was significant and positive (g = 0.49, 95% CI
with significant heterogeneity (χ 2 = 23.50 [p = .00], I2 = 86.46%, τ 2 = [0.27, 0.72]) with significant heterogeneity across individual studies
0.33). Thus, at immediate post‐intervention, there is no evidence that (χ 2 = 18.48 [p = .00], I2 = 70.51%, τ 2 = 0.05). Across all studies
bystander programs have an effect on participants’ knowledge of (randomized and nonrandomized), the average intervention effect
strategies for helping/intervening. The small sample size in this
Bystander Efficacy: Immediate Post-test
Study Hedges g [95% CI]
Knowing Strategies: Immediate Post-test
Study Hedges g [95% CI] Nonrandom Assignment
Baker et al. (2014) 0.60 [ -0.40 , 1.60 ]
Nonrandom Assignment Senn & Forrest (2016) 0.22 [ 0.02 , 0.42 ]
Amar et al. (2015) -0.21 [ -0.62 , 0.20 ] Nonrandom 0.23 [ 0.04 , 0.43 ]
Senn & Forrest (2016) 0.84 [ 0.64 , 1.04 ] Random Assignment
Addison (2015) 0.89 [ 0.65 , 1.13 ]
Nonrandom 0.33 [ -0.70 , 1.36 ]
Banyard et al. (2007) 0.51 [ 0.26 , 0.76 ]
Random Assignment Jouriles et al. (2016a) 0.41 [ 0.14 , 0.68 ]
Brokenshire (2015) 1.21 [ 0.54 , 1.88 ] Jouriles et al. (2016b) 0.10 [ -0.19 , 0.39 ]
Kleinsasser et al. (2015) 0.40 [ -0.01 , 0.81 ]
Potter et al. (2008) 0.32 [ -0.62 , 1.26 ]
Moynihan et al. (2010) 0.61 [ 0.20 , 1.02 ]
Random 0.83 [ -0.03 , 1.69 ] Random 0.49 [ 0.27 , 0.72 ]
Total 0.54 [ -0.09 , 1.17 ] Total 0.45 [ 0.25 , 0.65 ]

-1.00 0.00 1.00 2.00 -0.50 0.50 1.00 1.50 2.00


Favors Control Favors Treatment Favors Control Favors Treatment

F I G U R E 2 1 Forest plot of bystander intervention effects on F I G U R E 2 2 Forest plot of bystander intervention effects on
knowing strategies for helping/intervening‐ immediate posttest bystander efficacy – immediate posttest
24 of 40 | KETTREY ET AL.

Bystander Efficacy: Immediate Post-test Bystander Efficacy: 1 to 4 Months Post-test


Study Hedges g [95% CI] Study Hedges g [95% CI]
Nonrandom Assignment Nonrandom Assignment
Baker et al. (2014) 0.60 [ -0.40 , 1.60 ] Baker et al. (2014) 0.42 [ -0.58 , 1.42 ]
Senn & Forrest (2016) 0.22 [ 0.02 , 0.42 ] Peterson et al. (2016) 0.97 [ 0.50 , 1.44 ]
Nonrandom 0.23 [ 0.04 , 0.43 ] Senn & Forrest (2016) 0.15 [ -0.05 , 0.35 ]
Random Assignment Nonrandom 0.50 [ -0.07 , 1.06 ]
Addison (2015) 0.89 [ 0.65 , 1.13 ] Random Assignment
Banyard et al. (2007) Alt Tx Arm 0.76 [ 0.51 , 1.01 ] Banyard et al. (2007) Alt Tx Arm 0.56 [ 0.27 , 0.85 ]
Jouriles et al. (2016a) 0.41 [ 0.14 , 0.68 ] Jouriles et al. (2016a) 0.70 [ 0.43 , 0.97 ]
Jouriles et al. (2016b) 0.10 [ -0.19 , 0.39 ] Jouriles et al. (2016b) 0.31 [ 0.02 , 0.60 ]
Kleinsasser et al. (2015) 0.40 [ -0.01 , 0.81 ] Kleinsasser et al. (2015) 0.31 [ -0.10 , 0.72 ]
Moynihan et al. (2010) 0.61 [ 0.20 , 1.02 ] Moynihan et al. (2011) 0.63 [ 0.02 , 1.24 ]
Random 0.54 [ 0.30 , 0.78 ] Moynihan et al. (2010) 0.72 [ 0.31 , 1.13 ]
Total 0.49 [ 0.27 , 0.71 ] Random 0.53 [ 0.37 , 0.69 ]
Total 0.50 [ 0.31 , 0.69 ]
-0.50 0.50 1.00 1.50 2.00
Favors Control Favors Treatment -1.00 0.00 1.00
Favors Control Favors Treatment
F I G U R E 2 3 Forest plot of bystander intervention effects on
bystander efficacy – immediate posttest with alternative Tx arm for F I G U R E 2 5 Forest plot of bystander intervention effects on
Banyard et al. (2007) bystander efficacy – 1‐ to 4‐month posttest with alternative Tx arm
for Banyard et al. (2007)

was significant and positive (g = 0.45, 95% CI [0.25, 0.65]) with for the three nonrandomized studies was nonsignificant (g = 0.50,
significant heterogeneity across studies (χ 2 = 25.17 [p = .00], I2 = 95% CI [−0.07, 1.06]) with significant heterogeneity (χ 2 = 10.03 [p =
69.78%, τ 2 = 0.05). Thus, at immediate post‐intervention, bystander .01], I2 = 76.18%, τ 2 = 0.18). The average intervention effect for the
programs had a significant and positive effect on bystander efficacy. six randomized studies was significant and positive (g = 0.52, 95% CI
The small sample size in this meta‐analysis (n < 10) precluded ad hoc [0.36, 0.68]) with nonsignificant heterogeneity (χ 2 = 5.65 [p = .34],
analysis of moderators or small study/publication bias. I2 = 20.50%, τ 2 = 0.01). Across all studies (randomized and
We conducted a sensitivity analysis in which we replaced the nonrandomized), the average intervention effect was significant
Banyard et al. (2007) one‐session intervention arm with the three‐ and positive (g = 0.50, 95% CI [0.31, 0.68]) but with significant
session intervention arm. As indicated in Figure 23, findings were heterogeneity (χ 2 = 20.19 [p = .01], I2 = 58.56%, τ 2 = 0.04). Thus,
substantively similar to those from the main analysis. The average bystander programs have a significant positive (beneficial) effect on
intervention effect for the six randomized studies (which included bystander efficacy at 1‐ to 4‐month post‐intervention. The small
Banyard et al.) was significant and positive (g = 0.54, 95% CI [0.30, sample size in this meta‐analysis (n < 10) precluded ad hoc analysis of
0.78]) with significant heterogeneity (χ 2 = 21.05 [p = .00], I2 = moderators or small study/publication bias.
74.41%, τ 2 = 0.07). The overall average intervention effect (for We conducted a sensitivity analysis in which we replaced the
randomized and nonrandomized studies) was also significant and Banyard et al. (2007) one‐session intervention arm with the three‐
positive (g = 0.49, 95% CI [0.27, 0.71]) with significant heterogeneity session intervention arm. As indicated in Figure 25, findings were
across studies (χ 2 = 30.32 [p = .00], I2 = 74.30%, τ 2 = 0.07). substantively similar to those from the main analysis. The average
intervention effect for the six randomized studies (which included
Banyard et al.) was significant and positive (g = 0.53, 95% CI [0.37, 0.69])
One to four‐month follow‐up effects with nonsignificant heterogeneity (χ 2 = 5.68 [p = .34], I2 = 20.90%, τ 2 =
Nine studies reported bystander efficacy 1 to 4 months post‐ 0.01). Across all studies (randomized and nonrandomized), the average
intervention. As shown in Figure 24, the average intervention effect intervention effect was also significant and positive (g = 0.50, 95% CI
[0.31, 0.69]) but with significant heterogeneity across studies
Bystander Efficacy: 1 to 4 Months Post-test (χ 2 = 20.66 [p = .01], I2 = 59.10%, τ 2 = 0.04).
Study Hedges g [95% CI]
Nonrandom Assignment
Baker et al. (2014) 0.42 [ -0.58 , 1.42 ] Six month follow‐up effects
Peterson et al. (2016) 0.97 [ 0.50 , 1.44 ]
Senn & Forrest (2016) 0.15 [ -0.05 , 0.35 ] One study (Salazar et al., 2014) reported bystander efficacy as an
Nonrandom 0.50 [ -0.07 , 1.06 ]
Random Assignment outcome 6 months post‐intervention. Since this study measured
Banyard et al. (2007) 0.51 [ 0.22 , 0.80 ]
Jouriles et al. (2016a) 0.70 [ 0.43 , 0.97 ]
bystander efficacy 2 to 6 months later than all other studies
Jouriles et al. (2016b) 0.31 [ 0.02 , 0.60 ] reporting this outcome, we elected to report it separately. Findings
Kleinsasser et al. (2015) 0.31 [ -0.10 , 0.72 ]
Moynihan et al. (2011) 0.63 [ 0.02 , 1.24 ] from this particular study indicated a nonsignificant program effect
Moynihan et al. (2010) 0.72 [ 0.31 , 1.13 ]
Random 0.52 [ 0.36 , 0.68 ] (g = 0.13, 95% CI [−0.14, 0.40]) 6 months post‐intervention.
Total 0.50 [ 0.31 , 0.68 ]

-1.00 0.00 1.00


Favors Control Favors Treatment 9.3.10 | Bystander intentions

F I G U R E 2 4 Forest plot of bystander intervention effects on Eleven studies reported a measure of bystander intentions as an
bystander efficacy – 1‐ to 4‐month posttest outcome. Studies operationalized this outcome using the full or
KETTREY ET AL. | 25 of 40

adapted Bystander Intent to Help Scale (Banyard, Moynihan, Cares, Bystander Intentions: Immediate Post-test
Study Hedges g [95% CI]
& Warner, 2014), which asks participants their likelihood to engage
Nonrandom Assignment
in bystander behavior (e.g., “I stop and check in on someone who Amar et al. (2015) -0.62 [ -1.03 , -0.21 ]
Senn & Forrest (2016) 0.29 [ 0.09 , 0.49 ]
looks intoxicated when they are being taken upstairs at a party.”). All Nonrandom -0.15 [ -1.04 , 0.74 ]
Random Assignment
but four of these studies (Amar et al., 2015; Miller et al., 2014; Banyard et al. (2007) Alt Tx Arm 0.77 [ 0.52 , 1.02 ]
Brokenshire (2015) -0.12 [ -0.51 , 0.27 ]
Peterson et al., 2016; Senn & Forrest, 2016) involved random Mabry & Turner (2016) 0.24 [ -0.03 , 0.51 ]
assignment of participants to conditions and all but one (Brokenshire, Moynihan et al. (2010) 0.50 [ 0.09 , 0.91 ]
Random 0.36 [ -0.01 , 0.73 ]
2015) was published in a peer‐reviewed outlet. This specific report Total 0.19 [ -0.19 , 0.57 ]

was an unpublished master’s thesis. We collapsed bystander efficacy -1.50 -0.50 0.50 1.50
effect sizes into three timing intervals: (a) immediate posttest (i.e., 0 Favors Control Favors Treatment

week to 1 week), (b) 1 month to 4 months post‐intervention, and (c) 6


F I G U R E 2 7 Forest plot of bystander intervention effects on
months to 1 year post‐intervention.
bystander intentions – immediate posttest with alternative Tx arm
for Banyard et al. (2007)

Immediate post‐test effects


Six studies reported bystander intentions as an immediate posttest significant and positive (g = 0.32, 95% CI [0.08, 0.56]) with significant
outcome (i.e., 0 week to 1 week post‐intervention). As shown in the heterogeneity (χ 2 = 11.26 [p = .02], I2 = 67.19%, τ 2 = 0.04).
forest plot in Figure 26, the average intervention effect for the two We conducted a sensitivity analysis in which we replaced the
nonrandomized studies was nonsignificant (g = −.15, 95% CI [−1.04, Banyard et al. (2007) one‐session intervention arm with the three‐
0.74]) but with significant heterogeneity across studies (χ 2 = 15.31 [p session intervention arm. As indicated in Figure 27, findings were
= .00], I = 93.47%, τ = 0.39). The average intervention effect for the
2 2 substantively similar to those from the main analysis. The average
four randomized studies was significant and positive (g = 0.32, 95% intervention effect for the six randomized studies (which included
2
CI [0.01, 0.64]) with significant heterogeneity (χ = 10.94 [p = .01], I 2 Banyard et al.) was nonsignificant (g = 0.36, 95% CI [−0.01, 0.73])
= 73.17%, τ = 0.07). Across all studies (randomized and nonrando-
2 with significant heterogeneity across studies (χ 2 = 16.34 [p = .00], I2
mized), the average intervention effect was nonsignificant (g = 0.17, = 81.16%, τ 2 = 0.12). Across all studies (randomized and nonrando-
95% CI [−0.18, 0.52]) but with significant heterogeneity (χ = 30.25 2 mized), the average intervention effect was nonsignificant (g = 0.19,
[p = .00], I = 87.57%, τ = 0.16). The small sample size in this meta‐
2 2 95% CI [−0.19, 0.57]) but with significant heterogeneity across
analysis (n < 10) precluded ad hoc analysis of moderators or small individual study effect sizes (χ 2 = 37.74 [p = .00], I2 = 89.59%,
study/publication bias. τ 2 = 0.20).
Interestingly, the average intervention effect for the randomized
studies was significant and positive, but when synthesized with the One to four‐month follow‐up effects
two nonrandomized studies, the average intervention effect was Six studies reported a measure of bystander intentions 1 to 4 months
nonsignificant. This attenuation appears to be driven by the extreme post‐intervention. As shown in Figure 28, the average intervention
(negative) effect size reported by Amar et al. (2015). However, effect for the nonrandomized studies was nonsignificant (g = 0.51,
preliminary inspection of the distribution of effect sizes indicated this 95% CI [−0.40, 1.42) with significant heterogeneity across individual
was not an outlier; thus, we did not Winsorize it in our main analysis. effect sizes (χ 2 = 13.16 [p = .00], I2 = 92.40%, τ 2 = 0.40). The average
Nevertheless, in light of these finding we ran a sensitivity analysis in intervention effect for the randomized studies was significant and
which we dropped Amar et al. (2015) from the sample. When positive (g = 0.44, 95% CI [0.24, 0.63]) with nonsignificant
removing Amar et al. (2015), the overall average effect size was
Bystander Intentions: 1 to 4 Month Post-test
Study Hedges g [95% CI]
Bystander Intentions: Immediate Post-test
Nonrandom Assignment
Stu d y Hedges g [95% CI]
Peterson et al. (2016) 1.00 [ 0.53 , 1.47 ]
Nonrandom Assignment
Senn & Forrest (2016) 0.07 [ -0.11 , 0.25 ]
Amar et al. (2015) -0.62 [ -1.03 , -0.21 ]
Senn & Forrest (2016) 0.29 [ 0.09 , 0.49 ] Nonrandom 0.51 [ -0.40 , 1.42 ]
Nonrandom -0.15 [ -1.04 , 0.74 ] Random Assignment
Random Assignment Banyard et al. (2007) 0.51 [ 0.22 , 0.80 ]
Banyard et al. (2007) 0.62 [ 0.37 , 0.87 ] Miller et al. (2013) 0.28 [ -0.15 , 0.71 ]
Brokenshire (2015) -0.12 [ -0.51 , 0.27 ] Moynihan et al. (2010) 0.56 [ 0.15 , 0.97 ]
Mabry & Turner (2016) 0.24 [ -0.03 , 0.51 ]
Moynihan et al. (2011) 0.19 [ -0.40 , 0.78 ]
Moynihan et al. (2010) 0.50 [ 0.09 , 0.91 ]
Random 0.32 [ 0.01 , 0.64 ] Random 0.44 [ 0.24 , 0.63 ]
Total 0.17 [ -0.18 , 0.52 ] Total 0.41 [ 0.15 , 0.68 ]

-1.50 -0.50 0.50 1.00 -0.50 0.00 0.50 1.00 1.50


Favors Control Favors Treatment Favors Control Favors Treatment

F I G U R E 2 6 Forest plot of bystander intervention effects on F I G U R E 2 8 Forest plot of bystander intervention effects on
bystander intentions – immediate posttest bystander intentions – 1 to 4 months post‐intervention
26 of 40 | KETTREY ET AL.

Bystander Intentions: 1 to 4 Month Post-test 0.46). The average intervention effect for the two randomized
Study Hedges g [95% CI]
Nonrandom Assignment
studies was significant and positive (g = 0.29, 95% CI [0.05, 0.53])
Peterson et al. (2016) 1.00 [ 0.53 , 1.47 ] with nonsignificant heterogeneity across individual effect sizes (χ 2 =
Senn & Forrest (2016) 0.07 [ -0.11 , 0.25 ]
Nonrandom 0.51 [ -0.40 , 1.42 ] 0.03 [p = .87], I2 = 0.00%, τ 2 = 0). Across all studies (randomized and
Random Assignment
Banyard et al. (2007) Alt Tx Arm 0.45 [ 0.16 , 0.74 ] nonrandomized), the average intervention effect was significant and
Miller et al. (2013) 0.28 [ -0.15 , 0.71 ]
Moynihan et al. (2010) 0.56 [ 0.15 , 0.97 ] positive (g = 0.23, 95% CI [0.03, 0.43]) with nonsignificant
Moynihan et al. (2011) 0.19 [ -0.40 , 0.78 ]
Random 0.41 [ 0.21 , 0.61 ] heterogeneity (χ 2 = 0.70 [p = .70], I2 = 0.00%, τ 2 = 0). Overall, these
Total 0.40 [ 0.14 , 0.67 ]
findings indicate bystander programs had a significant positive
-0.50 0.50 1.00 1.50 (beneficial) effect on bystander intentions 6 months to 1 year post‐
Favors Control Favors Treatment intervention.

F I G U R E 2 9 Forest plot of bystander intervention effects on


bystander intentions – 1 to 4 months post‐intervention with
9.3.11 | Bystander intervention
alternative Tx arm for Banyard et al. (2007)
Thirteen studies reported a measure of bystander intervention as an
heterogeneity (χ 2 = 1.77 [p = .62], I2 = 0.00%, τ 2 = 0). Across all outcome. All studies used a form of the Bystander Behaviors Scale
studies (randomized and nonrandomized), the average intervention (Banyard et al., 2007), which asked participants to indicate the extent
effect was significant and positive (g = 0.41, 95% CI [0.15, 0.68]) but to which they have engaged in bystander behavior (e.g., “walked a
with significant heterogeneity across studies (χ 2 = 18.64 [p = .00], I2 friend home from a party who has had too much to drink”). All but
= 70.16%, τ 2 = 0.07). Overall, these findings indicate bystander three of these studies (Miller et al., 2014; Peterson et al., 2016; Senn
programs have a significant positive (beneficial) effect on bystander & Forrest, 2016) involved random assignment of participants to
intentions 1‐ to 4‐month post‐intervention. The small sample size in conditions and all but one (Jouriles et al., n.d.) was published in a
this meta‐analysis (n < 10) precluded ad hoc analysis of moderators peer‐reviewed outlet. This specific report was a draft of a manuscript
or small study/publication bias. under review for publication at the time of this review.
We conducted a sensitivity analysis in which we replaced the No studies measured bystander intervention at immediate post‐
Banyard et al. (2007) one‐session intervention arm with the three‐ intervention. We thus collapsed bystander intervention effect sizes
session treatment arm. As indicated in Figure 29, findings were into two timing intervals: (a) 1 to 4 months post‐intervention and (b)
substantively similar to those from the main analysis. The average 6 months to 1 year post‐intervention.
intervention effect for the four randomized studies (which included
Banyard et al.) was significant and positive (g = 0.41, 95% CI [0.21, One to four‐month follow‐up effects
0.61]) with nonsignificant heterogeneity effect sizes (χ 2 = 1.47 [p = Eleven studies reported bystander behavior 1‐ to 4‐month post‐
.69], I2 = 0.00%, τ 2 = 0). Across all studies, the average intervention intervention. Inspection of the distribution of effect sizes revealed
effect was significant and positive (g = 0.40, 95% CI [0.14, 0.67]) with that the effect size of one of the nonrandomized studies (Peterson
significant heterogeneity (χ 2 = 17.60 [p = .00], I2 = 69.26%, τ 2 = 0.07). et al., 2016) fell more than two standard deviations above the mean
of the distribution. We therefore Winsorized this effect size (g =
0.63) by replacing it with the value that fell exactly two standard
Six month to one year follow‐up effects deviations above the mean (g = 0.60). As depicted in the forest plot in
Three studies reported bystander intentions 6 months to 1 year post‐ Figure 31 the average intervention effect for the two nonrandomized
intervention. As shown in Figure 30, the program effect for the one studies was significant and positive (g = 0.43, 95% CI [0.25, 0.61])
nonrandomized study was nonsignificant (g = 0.11, 95% CI [−0.24, with nonsignificant heterogeneity (χ 2 = 0.59 [p = .44], I2 = 0.00%, τ 2 =
0). The average intervention effect for the nine randomized studies
Bystander Intentions: 6 Months to 1 Year Post-test
Study Hedges g [95% CI] was also significant and positive (g = 0.23, 95% CI [0.13, 0.33]) with
nonsignificant heterogeneity (χ 2 = 5.44 [p = .71], I2 = 0.00%, τ 2 = 0).
Nonrandom Assignment
Across all studies (randomized and nonrandomized), the average
Miller et al. (2014) 0.11 [ -0.24 , 0.46 ]
Nonrandom 0.11 [ -0.24 , 0.46 ] intervention effect was significant and positive (g = 0.27, 95% CI
Random Assignment [0.19, 0.36]) with nonsignificant heterogeneity (χ 2 = 9.70 [p = .47], I2
Miller et al. (2013) 0.25 [ -0.28 , 0.78 ] = 2.16%, τ 2 = 0). These findings indicate that, at 1‐ to 4‐month post‐
Salazar et al. (2014) 0.30 [ 0.03 , 0.57 ]
intervention bystander programs have a significant positive (bene-
Random 0.29 [ 0.05 , 0.53 ]
ficial) effect on bystander intervention.
Total 0.23 [ 0.03 , 0.43 ]
We conducted mixed‐effects meta‐regression to examine
-0.40 0.00 0.40 0.80
whether the effect of bystander programs on bystander intervention
Favors Control Favors Treatment
behavior varied based on the following: (a) gendered content of the
F I G U R E 3 0 Forest plot of bystander intervention effects on program (i.e., sex of perpetrators and victims); (b) mixed‐ or single‐
bystander intentions – 6 months to 1 year post‐intervention sex group implementation; (c) gender composition of the sample (i.e.,
KETTREY ET AL. | 27 of 40

Bystander Intervention: 1 to 4 Month Follow-Up Bystander Intervention: 1 to 4 Month Follow-Up


Study Hedges g [95% CI]

0
Nonrandom Assignment
Peterson et al. (2016) 0.60 [ 0.13 , 1.07 ]
Senn & Forrest (2016) 0.40 [ 0.20 , 0.60 ]

0.06
Nonrandom 0.43 [ 0.25 , 0.61 ]

Standard Error
Random Assignment
Banyard et al. (2007) 0.42 [ 0.13 , 0.71 ]

0.12
Gidycz et al. (2011) 0.16 [ -0.29 , 0.61 ]
Jouriles et al. (n.d.) 0.26 [ 0.04 , 0.48 ]
Jouriles et al. (2016a) 0.33 [ 0.06 , 0.60 ]
Jouriles et al. (2016b) 0.30 [ 0.01 , 0.59 ]

0.18
Kleinsasser et al. (2015) 0.02 [ -0.39 , 0.43 ]
Miller et al. (2013) 0.20 [ -0.23 , 0.63 ]
Moynihan et al. (2010) 0.01 [ -0.40 , 0.42 ]

0.24
Sargent et al. (2017) 0.13 [ -0.09 , 0.35 ]
Random 0.23 [ 0.13 , 0.33 ]
Total 0.27 [ 0.19 , 0.36 ] -0.5 0 0.5 1

-0.50 0.00 0.50 1.00 1.50 Hedges G


Favors Control Favors Treatment
F I G U R E 3 2 Contour enhanced funnel plot of bystander
F I G U R E 3 1 Forest plot of bystander intervention effects on intervention 1 to 4 months post‐intervention
bystander intervention – 1 to 4 months post‐intervention

proportion males); (d) education level of the sample (i.e., secondary and nonsignificance, the results from this contour‐enhanced funnel
school or college students); and (e) mean age of the sample. Although plot indicate a potential risk of publication bias.
we had also planned to examine proportion of fraternity/sorority To further investigate the possibility of bias, we conducted an
members in the sample and proportion of athletic team members as Egger test for funnel plot asymmetry. The results provided no
potential effect size moderators, these measures were rarely or significant evidence of small study effects (bias coefficient: 0.36; t:
never reported in the included studies and thus we were unable to −0.60, p = .56). Finally, we also conducted a trim and fill analysis.
examine them. Finally, due to the small number of included studies in With one study trimmed and filled, the resulting mean effect size was
this synthesis, we conducted separate bivariate regressions for each similar to that observed in the main analysis (trim‐and‐fill mean
potential moderator, meaning that we estimated separate meta‐ g = 0.28, 95% CI [0.20, 0.37]). With these collective findings, we
regression models for each potential moderator. As summarized in therefore conclude that the meta‐analysis results shown in Figure 31
Table 4, none of these moderators exhibited a statistically significant are likely robust to any small study/publication bias.
bivariate association with effect size magnitude. The results from We also conducted meta‐regression to examine whether the effect of
these moderator analyses should be interpreted cautiously, however, bystander programs on bystander intervention outcomes varied based on
given the small number of included studies and our inability to the following characteristics: (a) attrition at first follow‐up (i.e., 1 to
estimate more comprehensive, multivariable meta‐regression 4 months post‐intervention), (b) random sequence ROB, (c) blinding of
models. participants ROB, (d) incomplete data ROB, and (e) selective reporting
To examine small study and publication bias we created a ROB. We had also planned to examine blinding of assessment ROB, but
contour‐enhanced funnel plot of the 11 effect sizes plotted against the lack of variation in this measure precluded such analysis (i.e., all
their standard errors (see Figure 32). Visual inspection of the funnel studies in this meta‐analysis were coded as high ROB for blinding of
plot reveals an absence of adverse intervention effects. Given the assessment). Due to the small sample size, we conducted bivariate
absence of negative effects in the regions of statistical significance regressions for each potential moderator.
As shown in Table 5, there was no evidence that attrition,
blinding of participants, incomplete data, or selective reporting risk
T A B L E 4 Unstandardized bivariate meta‐regression coefficients of bias assessments were associated with effect size magnitude.
for potential moderators of program effects on bystander There was, however, evidence that studies rated as unclear risk of
intervention behavior – 1 to 4 months post‐intervention bias due to random sequence generation reported significantly
Moderator B SE 95% CI smaller effect sizes than those at low risk of bias. There was no
Sex of perpetrators & victims 0.10 0.17 [−0.23, 0.43] evidence that studies rated as high risk of bias due to random
(gender neutral) sequence generation reported significantly different effect sizes from
Mixed sex implementation (Yes) 0.13 0.17 [−0.21, 0.46] those at low risk of bias.
Proportion males in sample −0.00 0.00 [−0.01, 0.00] We conducted a sensitivity analysis to determine whether

Education level (secondary school) −0.16 0.11 [−0.38, 0.05] Winsorizing the effect size for Peterson et al. (2016) affected our
overall findings. Running the meta‐analysis with the original effect
Age 0.04 0.02 [−0.01, 0.08]
size for this study produced identical findings to those from the main
Notes: The reference group for sex of perpetrators and victims is male
analysis for both the two non‐randomized studies (g = 0.43, 95% CI
perpetrators/female victims. The reference group for education level is
college. Coefficients were estimated from separate bivariate regressions [0.25, 0.61]) and the total sample containing both randomized and
for each moderator. non‐randomized studies (g = 0.27, 95% CI [0.19, 0.36]). Tests for
28 of 40 | KETTREY ET AL.

T A B L E 5 Unstandardized bivariate meta‐regression coefficients Bystander Intervention: 1 to 4 Month Follow-Up


Study Hedges g [95% CI]
for attrition and risk of bias moderators of the relationship between
Nonrandom Assignment
bystander programs and bystander intervention 1‐ to 4‐month Peterson et al. (2016) 0.60 [ 0.13 , 1.07 ]
post‐Tx Senn & Forrest (2016) 0.40 [ 0.20 , 0.60 ]
Nonrandom 0.43 [ 0.25 , 0.61 ]
Random Assignment
Moderator B SE 95% CI Banyard et al. (2007) Alt Tx Arm 0.17 [ -0.08 , 0.42 ]
Gidycz et al. (2011) 0.16 [ -0.29 , 0.61 ]
Attrition at first follow‐up −0.39 0.38 [−1.14, 0.35] Jouriles et al. (n.d.) 0.26 [ 0.04 , 0.48 ]
Jouriles et al. (2016a) 0.33 [ 0.06 , 0.60 ]
Random sequence bias Jouriles et al. (2016b) 0.30 [ 0.01 , 0.59 ]
Kleinsasser et al. (2015) 0.02 [ -0.39 , 0.43 ]
High 0.13 0.11 [−0.09, 0.35] Miller et al. (2013) 0.20 [ -0.23 , 0.63 ]
Moynihan et al. (2010) 0.01 [ -0.40 , 0.42 ]
Unclear −0.21* 0.10 [−0.41, −0.00] Sargent et al. (2017) 0.13 [ -0.09 , 0.35 ]
Random 0.20 [ 0.10 , 0.30 ]
Blinding of participants Total 0.25 [ 0.16 , 0.34 ]

Unclear −0.16 0.11 [−0.37, 0.06]


-0.50 0.00 0.50 1.00 1.50
Incomplete data Favors Control Favors Treatment

High 0.20+ 0.11 [−0.00, 0.41]


F I G U R E 3 3 Forest plot of bystander intervention effects on
Unclear −0.03 0.23 [−0.47, 0.41] bystander intervention – 1 to 4 months post‐intervention with
Selective reporting alternative Tx arm for Banyard et al. (2007)
High −0.12 0.24 [−0.58, 0.35]
Notes: *p < .05; +p < .10. Low is the reference group for all risk of bias program effect for the single nonrandomized study in this subsample
predictors. For blinding of participants no studies were coded as high risk. was nonsignificant (g = −0.06, 95% CI [−0.41, 0.29]) and the average
For selective reporting no studies were coded as unclear risk. Coefficients intervention effect for the three randomized studies was nonsigni-
were estimated from separate bivariate regressions for each moderator.
ficant (g = 0.20, 95% CI [−0.08, 0.32]) with nonsignificant hetero-
geneity (χ 2 = 1.08 [p = .58], I2 = 0.00%, τ 2 = 0). Across all studies
heterogeneity revealed non‐significant heterogeneity, although spe- (randomized and nonrandomized), the average intervention effect
cific values were slightly different than those from the main analysis was also nonsignificant (g = 0.12, 95% CI [−0.08, 0.32]) with
2
for both the nonrandomized studies (χ = 0.72 [p = .40], I = 0.00%, 2 nonsignificant heterogeneity (χ 2 = 2.60 [p = .46], I2 = 14.23%,
τ = 0) and the total sample (χ = 9.93 [p = .45], I = 2.63%, τ = 0).
2 2 2 2 τ 2 = .01). Thus, these findings indicate that, at 6 months to 1 year
We also conducted two sensitivity analyses in which we replaced (a) post‐intervention, bystander programs have a significant positive
the Banyard et al. (2007) one‐session intervention arm with the (beneficial) effect on bystander intervention. The small sample size in
three‐session treatment arm and (b) the Jouriles et al. (n.d.) independent this meta‐analysis (n < 10) precluded ad hoc analysis of moderators
intervention arm with the monitored intervention arm (i.e., completed in or small study/publication bias.
a lab with supervision). As indicated in Figure 33, findings from the
Banyard et al. sensitivity analysis were substantively similar to those from
the main analysis. The average intervention effect for the nine 9.3.12 | Sexual assault perpetration
randomized studies (which included Banyard et al.) was significant and
positive (g = 0.20, 95% CI [0.10, 0.30]) with non‐significant heterogeneity Six studies reported sexual assault perpetration as an outcome,
(χ = 3.64 [p = .89], I = 0.00%, τ = 0). The average intervention effect
2 2 2 asking participants to self‐report their perpetration of sexual
(for randomized and nonrandomized studies) was also significant and coercion or sexual abuse. All but one of these studies (Miller et al.,
positive (g = 0.25, 95% CI [0.16, 0.34]) with nonsignificant heterogeneity
(χ 2 = 9.10 [p = .52], I2 = 0.93%, τ 2 = 0). Bystander Intervention: 1 to 4 Month Follow-Up
Study Hedges g [95% CI]
Similarly, as indicated in Figure 34, findings from the Jouriles Nonrandom Assignment
Peterson et al. (2016) 0.60 [ 0.13 , 1.07 ]
et al. (n.d.) sensitivity analysis were substantively similar to those Senn & Forrest (2016) 0.40 [ 0.20 , 0.60 ]
Nonrandom 0.43 [ 0.25 , 0.61 ]
from the main analysis. The average intervention effect for the Random Assignment
Banyard et al. (2007) 0.42 [ 0.13 , 0.71 ]
nine randomized studies (which included Jouriles et al. (n.d.)) Gidycz et al. (2011) 0.16 [ -0.29 , 0.61 ]
Jouriles et al. (n.d.) Alt Tx Arm 0.16 [ -0.04 , 0.36 ]
was significant and positive (g = 0.20, 95% CI [0.11, 0.30]) with Jouriles et al. (2016a) 0.33 [ 0.06 , 0.60 ]
Jouriles et al. (2016b) 0.30 [ 0.01 , 0.59 ]
nonsignificant heterogeneity (χ 2 = 5.60 [p = .69], I2 = 0.00%, τ 2 = 0). Kleinsasser et al. (2015) 0.02 [ -0.39 , 0.43 ]
Miller et al. (2013) 0.20 [ -0.23 , 0.63 ]
The overall average effect (for randomized and nonrandomized Moynihan et al. (2010) 0.01 [ -0.40 , 0.42 ]
Sargent et al. (2017) 0.13 [ -0.09 , 0.35 ]
studies) was also significant and positive (g = 0.25, 95% CI [0.16, Random 0.20 [ 0.11 , 0.30 ]
Total 0.25 [ 0.16 , 0.35 ]
0.35]) with nonsignificant heterogeneity across individual study
effect sizes (χ 2 = 10.80 [p = .37], I2 = 12.69%, τ 2 = 0). -0.50 0.00 0.50 1.00 1.50
Favors Control Favors Treatment

Six month to one‐year follow‐up effects F I G U R E 3 4 Forest plot of bystander intervention effects on
Four studies reported bystander behavior 6 months to 1 year bystander intervention – 1 to 4 months post‐intervention with
post‐intervention. As shown in the forest plot in Figure 35, the alternative Tx arm for Jouriles et al. (n.d.)
KETTREY ET AL. | 29 of 40

Bystander Intervention: 6 Month to 1 Year Follow-Up Sexual Assault: 6 Month to 1 Year Post-test
Study Hedges g [95% CI] Study Hedges g [95% CI]

Nonrandom Assignment
Nonrandom Assignment Miller et al. (2014) 0.11 [ -0.75 , 0.97 ]
Miller et al. (2014) -0.06 [ -0.41 , 0.29 ] Nonrandom 0.11 [ -0.75 , 0.97 ]
Nonrandom -0.06 [ -0.41 , 0.29 ] Random Assignment
Random Assignment Foubert et al. (2007) 0.01 [ -0.17 , 0.19 ]
Gidycz et al. (2011) 0.07 [ -0.38 , 0.52 ] Foubert (2000) -0.06 [ -0.86 , 0.74 ]
Miller et al. (2013) 0.04 [ -0.49 , 0.57 ] Salazar et al. (2014) 0.27 [ 0.00 , 0.54 ]
Salazar et al. (2014) 0.29 [ 0.02 , 0.56 ] Random 0.10 [ -0.12 , 0.32 ]
Random 0.20 [ -0.02 , 0.41 ] Total 0.10 [ -0.10 , 0.30 ]
Total 0.12 [ -0.08 , 0.32 ]
-1.00 -0.50 0.00 0.50 1.00
-0.60 -0.20 0.20 0.60 Favors Control Favors Treatment
Favors Control Favors Treatment
F I G U R E 3 7 Forest plot of bystander intervention effects on
F I G U R E 3 5 Forest plot of bystander intervention effects on sexual assault perpetration – 6 months to 1‐year post‐intervention
bystander intervention ‐ 6 months to 1 year post‐intervention

2014) involved random assignment of participants to conditions. All average intervention was nonsignificant (g = 0.10, 95% CI [−0.10, 0.30])
six studies were published in a peer‐reviewed outlet. with nonsignificant heterogeneity (χ 2 = 2.57 [p = .46], I2 = 23.85%, τ 2 =
No studies measured sexual assault perpetration at immediate 0.01). These findings provide no evidence that bystander programs have
post‐intervention. We thus collapsed bystander intervention effect an effect on sexual assault perpetration 6 months to 1 year post‐
sizes into two timing intervals: (a) 3 to 4 months post‐intervention intervention. The small sample size in this meta‐analysis (n < 10)
and (2) 6 months to 1 year post‐intervention. precluded ad hoc analysis of moderators or small study/publication bias.

Three to four‐month follow‐up effects


Two studies measured sexual assault perpetration 3 or 4 months 10 | D I S C U S S I O N
post‐intervention. As shown in the forest plot in Figure 36, the
average intervention effect across these two studies was nonsigni- 10.1 | Summary of main results
ficant (g = 0.33, 95% CI [−0.70, 1.36]) with nonsignificant hetero-
10.1.1 | Objective 1: Bystander intervention effects
geneity across individual study effect sizes (χ 2 = 0.45 [p = .50],
on knowledge, attitudes, and behavior
I2 = 0.00%, τ 2 = 0). These findings provide no evidence that bystander
programs have an effect on sexual assault perpetration at 3‐ to Knowledge/attitudes
4‐months post‐intervention. Bystander intervention effects on knowledge/attitude outcomes
varied widely across constructs. The most pronounced effect in this
domain was on rape myth acceptance. The average program effect
Six month to one‐year follow‐up effects
for this outcome was immediate and sustained across all reported
Four studies measured sexual assault perpetration 6 months to 1 year
follow‐up waves (i.e., from immediate posttest to 6‐ to 7‐month post‐
post‐intervention. As shown in the forest plot in Figure 37, the program
intervention). At each of these follow‐up waves rape myth
effect for the one nonrandomized study was nonsignificant (g = 0.11, 95%
acceptance scores were approximately two‐fifths of a standard
CI [−0.75, 0.97]). The average intervention effect across the three
deviation lower for young people who participated in a bystander
randomized studies was nonsignificant (g = 0.10, 95% CI [−0.12, 0.32])
program relative to scores among the comparison group. To put this
with nonsignificant heterogeneity (χ 2 = 2.56 [p = .28], I2 = 35.66%,
in context, one study in our meta‐analytic sample (Salazar et al.,
τ 2 = 0.01). Across all studies (randomized and nonrandomized), the
2014) reported a preintervention mean score of 36.69 (SD = 10.29)
on the Rape Myth Acceptance Scale, with a total possible score of 85
Sexual Assault: 3 to 4 Month Post-test
and higher scores indicating greater rape myth endorsement.
Study Hedges g [95% CI]
Considering that the average effect size for rape myth acceptance
Gidycz et al. (2011) 0.71 [ -0.80 , 2.22 ] at immediate posttest in our sample was 0.37, extrapolation suggests

Miller et al. (2013) 0.00 [ -1.41 , 1.41 ]


that participating in a bystander program decreased the mean rape
myth acceptance score from 36.69 to 32.88 (i.e., (36.69 − [0.37 ×
Total 0.33 [ -0.70 , 1.36 ]
10.29]) = 32.88), a desirable change of approximately 4 points on an
85‐point scale.
-2.00 0.00 1.00 2.00 3.00
Program effects on bystander efficacy were also fairly pronounced,
Favors Control Favors Treatment
with a significant effect observed at both immediate posttest and 1‐ to
F I G U R E 3 6 Forest plot of bystander intervention effects on 4‐month post‐intervention. At both of these follow‐up waves measures
sexual assault perpetration – 3 to 4 months post‐intervention of bystander efficacy were approximately one‐half of a standard
30 of 40 | KETTREY ET AL.

deviation higher for bystander program participants than for the deviation higher). To put these findings into context, one study in
comparison group. Again, for illustrative purposes, one study in our our meta‐analytic sample (Salazar et al., 2014) reported a preinter-
meta‐analytic sample (Senn & Forrest, 2016) reported a preintervention vention mean score of 52.09 (SD = 12.78) on the Bystander Intention
mean score of 62.47 (SD = 20.58) on the Bystander Efficacy Scale, with Scale, with a total possible score of 75 and higher scores indicating
a total possible score of 100 and lower scores representing greater greater intentions. Considering that the average effect size for this
bystander efficacy. Considering that the average effect size for outcome was 0.41 at the one‐to‐four‐months follow‐up wave,
bystander efficacy at immediate posttest in our sample was 0.45, extrapolation suggests that participating in a bystander program
extrapolation suggests that participating in a bystander program increased the mean score on this outcome from 52.09 to 57.33 (i.e.,
decreased the mean bystander efficacy score from 62.47 to 53.21 52.09 + [0.41 × 12.78]) = 57.33), a desirable change of approximately
(i.e., (62.47 − [0.45 × 20.58]) = 53.21), a desirable change of 5 points on a 75‐point scale. Significant effects for this outcome were
approximately 9 points on a 100‐point scale. also observed at the 6 months to 1‐year follow‐up wave (approxi-
Program effects on identifying a situation as appropriate for mately one‐fourth a standard deviation). Considering the effect size
intervention were significant but were smaller at later follow‐up waves. at this follow‐up wave was 0.23, participation in a bystander program
At immediate posttest measures of this outcome were almost six‐tenths increased the mean bystander intention score from 52.09 to 55.03
of a standard deviation higher for bystander program participants than between preintervention and the 6‐month to 1 year follow‐up (i.e.,
for the comparison group. By 1‐ to 4‐month follow‐up this difference 52.09 + [0.23 × 12.78]) = 55.03), a change of approximately 3 points
was four‐tenths. Only one study measured identifying a situation as on a 75‐point scale. Additionally, the mean score on bystander
appropriate for intervention past 6 months follow‐up. To put the intentions decreased from 57.33 to 55.03 between one‐to‐four‐
immediate posttest findings into context, one study in our meta‐analytic month follow‐up and 6 months to 1‐year follow‐up.
sample (Addison, 2015) reported a preintervention mean rating of 5.94 The fact that significant effects were delayed until 1‐ to 4‐month
(SD = 1.11) regarding responses to a vignette depicting a scenario in follow‐up for three attitude/knowledge outcomes may indicate that
which a young woman was too inebriated to consent to sexual activity. participants’ knowledge and attitudes shifted over time or that the
The total possible score was 7, with higher scores indicating stronger programs required a bit of rumination and reflection to take effect.
agreement that the scenario depicted in the vignette warranted We found limited or no evidence of intervention effects on gender
intervention. Considering that the average effect size for this outcome attitudes, victim empathy, date rape attitudes, and noticing sexual
was 0.57 at immediate posttest, extrapolation suggests that participat- assault.
ing in a bystander program increased the mean rating from 5.94 to 6.57
(i.e., (5.94 + [0.57 * 1.11]) = 6.57), a change of approximately two‐thirds Behavior
a point on a 7‐point scale. Our results indicated that bystander programs have a desirable effect
For three of the attitude/knowledge outcomes, effects were on bystander intervention. At 1‐ to 4‐month follow‐up measures of
nonsignificant at immediate posttest, but significant at the 1‐ to 4‐ bystander intervention were approximately one‐fourth a standard
month post‐intervention follow‐up period. This was the case for taking deviation higher for bystander program participants than for the
responsibility to intervene, knowing strategies for intervening, and comparison group. To contextualize, one study in our meta‐analytic
intentions to intervene. At 1‐ to 4‐month follow‐up, relative to the sample (Jouriles et al., 2016a) reported a pre‐intervention mean score
comparison group, bystander program participants reported greater of 27.95 (SD = 19.02) on the Bystander Intervention Scale, with a total
responsibility to intervene (difference of approximately one‐third a possible score of 49 and higher scores indicating greater intervention
standard deviation). To put these findings into context, one study in our behavior over the past month. Considering that the average effect size
meta‐analytic sample (Senn & Forrest, 2016) reported a preintervention for bystander intervention at 1‐ to 4‐month follow‐up was 0.27, then
mean score of 3.51 (SD = 1.13) on a measure of responsibility to extrapolation suggests that participation in a bystander intervention
intervene, with a total possible score of 7 and lower scores indicating program increased bystander intervention scores from 27.95 to 33.09
greater responsibility. Considering that the average effect size for this (i.e., (27.95 + [0.27 * 19.02]) = 33.09), indicating that participants
outcome was 0.32, extrapolation suggests that participating in a engaged in approximately five additional acts of intervention in the past
bystander program decreased the mean score on this outcome from month (relative to pretest behavior). However, this effect was
3.51 to 3.15 (i.e., (3.51 + [0.32 * 1.13]) = 3.15), a desirable change of nonsignificant at 6 months post‐intervention.
approximately four‐tenths a point on a 7‐point scale. It is noteworthy that bystander programs did not have a
At one‐to‐four‐month follow‐up, relative to the comparison significant effect on sexual assault perpetration. This finding seems
group, bystander program participants reported higher measures of to belie the original intent of some of the earliest bystander
knowing strategies for intervening (approximately six‐tenths of a programs, which aimed to reduce sexual violence perpetration by
standard deviation higher). However, this effect was only based on approaching young people (young men especially) as allies in
one study. preventing violence against women, rather than as potential
At one‐ to‐four‐month follow‐up, relative to the comparison perpetrators of violence. This nonthreatening approach was antici-
group, bystander program participants reported higher measures of pated to be more effective at reducing violence against women than
intentions to intervene (approximately four‐tenths a standard traditional victim/perpetrator‐focused models, which may risk young
KETTREY ET AL. | 31 of 40

men’s defensiveness or backlash (Katz, 1995; Messner, 2015). research employed a variety of modalities, program curricula, and
Instead, evidence from our review indicates that, although bystander delivery methods. In order to be included in this systematic review
programs may have a meaningful, short‐term effect on bystander and meta‐analysis, all programs had to include a bystander interven-
intervention behavior, there is no evidence that the programs have a tion component, but there were no other restrictions or limitations
similar effect on sexual assault perpetration. Thus, it appears that on programs. Readers should therefore consider that our syntheses
bystander program participants interpret program information from include different types of programs. Although this does not
the perspective of potential allies rather than potential perpetrators. necessarily limit the applicability or generalizability of the systematic
review and meta‐analysis, readers should be cognizant that a variety
of bystander intervention programs may be included in each
10.1.2 | Objective 2: Effects for different
quantitative synthesis.
participant profiles
Although our systematic literature search identified 27 eligible
We had planned to conduct moderator analyses to assess any studies, only 13 reported bystander behavior and only six reported
differential effects of bystander programs on outcomes based on the sexual assault. Thus, results for these behavioral outcomes may be
following participant characteristics: mean age of the sample, considered tentative, as an updated review that includes more
education level of the sample, proportion of males/females in the studies may produce different findings. Additionally, included studies
sample, proportion of fraternity/sorority members in the sample, and rarely reported some of the moderators that we identified in our
proportion of athletic team members in the sample. Our review only protocol (i.e., fraternity/sorority membership and athletic team
produced a sufficient number of studies (n ≥ 10) to conduct such membership) and the vast majority (i.e., 25 of 27) were conducted
moderator analyses for the bystander intervention behavior out- in the United States. Thus, we do not know how well fraternity/
come. Those results provided no evidence that mean age, education sorority members and athletic team members were represented in
level, or proportion of males/female were associated with the the sample and we do know that participants from the United States
magnitude of program effects on bystander intervention behavior. were over‐represented.
We were unable to conduct moderator analyses for proportion of This paucity of international research highlights a clear need for
fraternity/sorority members in the sample and proportion of athletes further study of bystander programs in a global context and serves to
in the sample, as these variables were rarely or never reported. underscore the necessity of forming a broader evidence base.
Readers should be cautioned that the findings of this review may
not generalize to all contexts, especially those that are demographi-
10.1.3 | Objective 3: Effects based on gendered
cally different from the United States.
content/implementation
Finally, no eligible studies examined sexual assault in lesbian, gay,
We conducted moderator analyses to assess any differential effects bisexual, or queer relationships or interactions, and programs
of bystander programs on measured outcomes based on (a) the reported in the literature did not make clear that they mentioned
gender of perpetrators and victims in specific bystander programs or addressed potential sexual assault in nonheterosexual relation-
and (b) whether programs were implemented in mixed‐ or single‐sex ships or interactions. Further, eligible studies did not indicate that
settings. Our review only produced a sufficient number of studies (n they provided any particular or tailored strategies for being a
≥ 10) to conduct such moderator analyses for the bystander bystander in nonheterosexual situations.
intervention behavior outcome. The results provided no evidence
that these measures were associated with the magnitude of program
10.3 | Quality of the evidence
effects on bystander intervention behavior.
Although we deemed a large number of studies to be ineligible based
on methodological quality, the large body of extant research on
10.2 | Overall completeness and applicability of
bystander program permitted the identification of 27 independent
evidence
studies that met all inclusion criteria for this review. Of these 27
The thorough nature of our systematic literature search process studies, 21 were RCTs (i.e., 12 randomized at the individual level and
(e.g., searching electronic databases, ClinicalTrials.gov, conference 9 randomized at the group level) and 6 were high‐quality quasi‐
proceedings, organization websites, reference lists of review articles experimental studies.
and eligible reports, tables of contents of relevant journals, CVs and Risk of bias for the RCTs in this review was relatively low;
websites of primary authors of eligible studies, and forward citation however, there were some issues that must be noted. First, we coded
searching of eligible studies) helped ensure that all relevant studies approximately 95% of the sample of RCTs as exhibiting high risk of
were identified for consideration in this review. Nonetheless, there bias pertaining to blinding of outcome assessments. This was due to
were three rogue reports that we were unable to locate. Readers the frequent reporting of self‐report outcomes. Second, we coded
should be mindful of this when interpreting findings from this review. approximately 80% of the sample of RCTs as exhibiting high risk of
Readers should also be mindful that, because bystander sexual bias in some “other” domain. Such designation was typically the result
assault prevention programs are not standardized, the summarized of study authors evaluating programs that they designed themselves.
32 of 40 | KETTREY ET AL.

Risk of bias for the nine non‐RCT studies was typically low or unclear. programs implemented with college students, our analysis focused on
The latter designation was typically a result of study authors’ failure programs implemented with both college students and adolescents.
to report our prespecified confounding variables broken down by Given that our moderator analyses indicated age and education level
treatment and comparison groups. were not significant moderators of program effects on bystander
intervention, our findings for this outcome may be representative of
both college students and adolescents. However, findings from these
10.4 | Limitations and potential biases in the
moderator analyses should be interpreted as preliminary, given the
review process
small number of studies reporting bystander intervention.
As a whole, the existing evidence base for this review (N = 27) was Second, whereas Katz and Moore did not evaluate program
fairly strong methodologically, but the relatively small number of content/implementation as a moderator of program effects, we were
eligible studies in each meta‐analysis precluded our analysis of able to examine the influence of such variables on an important
important sources of heterogeneity. That is, only one meta‐analysis outcome. We found that (a) sex of perpetrators/victims in bystander
contained sufficient studies (n ≥ 10) to conduct moderator analyses programs and (b) whether programs were implemented in mixed‐ or
and explore issues stemming from risk of bias or publication/small single‐group settings were not significant moderators of program
study bias. Future accumulation of high‐quality research should effects on bystander intervention. Again, however, findings from
permit such analyses in updated reviews. Additionally, the dearth of these moderator analyses should be interpreted as preliminary given
non‐US research is a limitation to the generalizability of the findings. the small number of studies reporting bystander intervention.

10.5 | Agreements and disagreements with other


studies or reviews 11 | A U T H O R S ’ C O NC LU SION S
To date, there is only one existing meta‐analysis examining the
11.1 | Implications for practice and policy
effects of bystander programs. In what they called an “initial” meta‐
analysis of experimental and quasi‐experimental studies (published The United States 2013 Campus SaVE Act requires postsecondary
through 2011), Katz & Moore (2013) found moderate effects of educational institutions participating in Title IX financial aid
bystander programs on participants’ self‐efficacy and intentions to programs to provide incoming college students with primary
intervene, and small (but significant) effects on bystander behavior, prevention and awareness programs addressing sexual violence.
rape‐supportive attitudes, and rape proclivity (but not perpetration). The Campus SaVE Act mandates that these programs include a
Effects were generally stronger among younger samples and samples component on bystander intervention. Currently, there is no
containing a higher percentage of males. comparable legislation regarding sexual assault among adolescents
The main findings from the current review are consistent with (e.g., mandating bystander programs in secondary schools). This is an
those reported by Katz and Moore. Specifically, like Katz and unfortunate oversight, as adolescents who experience sexual assault
Moore’s analysis, our review findings indicated that bystander are at an increased risk of repeated victimization in young adulthood
programs have beneficial effects on bystander efficacy and inten- (Cui et al., 2013). Thus, the implementation of bystander programs in
tions. Additionally, we found beneficial effects on bystander behavior secondary schools not only has the potential to reduce sexual assault
(i.e., bystander intervention) and rape supportive attitudes (i.e., rape among adolescents, but may also have the long‐term potential to
myth acceptance). Unlike Katz and Moore, however, we did not reduce sexual assault on college campuses.
analyze rape proclivity outcomes; but similar to Katz and Moore, we Findings from this review indicate that bystander programs have
found no evidence of an effect on sexual assault perpetration. significant beneficial effects on bystander intervention behavior. This
Katz and Moore also conducted moderator analyses to assess provides important evidence of the effectiveness of mandated
differential effects of bystander programs on (a) bystander efficacy programs on college campuses. Additionally, the fact that our
and (b) bystander intentions. They reported that program effects on (preliminary) moderator analyses found program effects on bystan-
bystander efficacy were stronger among younger participants and der intervention to be similar for adolescents and college students
that effects on bystander intentions were stronger among samples suggests early implementation of bystander programs (i.e., in
containing a higher proportion of males. Our moderator analyses secondary schools with adolescents) may be warranted.
provided no evidence that participant age and proportion of males Importantly, although we found that bystander programs had a
were predictors of program effects. However, we were unable to significant beneficial effect on bystander intervention behavior, we
explore these moderators in depth given the small number of studies found no evidence that these programs had an effect on participants’
included in each meta‐analysis. sexual assault perpetration. Bystander programs may therefore be
Although findings from our meta‐analysis are largely similar to appropriate for targeting bystander behavior, but may not be
those of Katz and Moore, our analysis advances our understanding of appropriate for targeting the behavior of potential perpetrators.
the evidence base for bystander programs in two important ways. Additionally, effects of bystander programs on bystander interven-
First, whereas Katz and Moore’s analysis focused on bystander tion behavior diminished by 6‐month post‐intervention. Thus,
KETTREY ET AL. | 33 of 40

programs effects may be prolonged by the implementation of booster CON F LI CT OF IN TE RES T S


sessions conducted prior to 6 months post‐intervention.
The authors declare that there is no conflict of interests.

11.2 | Implications for research AU TH OR CO NTRIB UTION S


Findings from this review suggest there is a fairly strong body of
Members of the research team responsible for core areas of the review
research assessing the effects of bystander programs on attitudes
are as follows: Content: H. H. K.; Systematic review methods: H. H. K., R.
and behaviors. However, there are a couple of important questions
A. M., and E. E. T.‐S.; Statistical analysis: H. H. K.; Information retrieval: H.
worth further exploration.
H. K. and R. A. M. Dr Kettrey, the lead review author, coordinated the
First, according to one prominent logical model, bystander
review team and assumed responsibility for the implementation of
programs promote bystander intervention by fostering prerequisite
the project throughout its duration. Specific tasks included compiling the
knowledge and attitudes (Burn, 2009). Our meta‐analysis provides
sample of research reports, creating the database, coding studies,
inconsistent evidence of the effects of bystander programs on
analyzing data, and preparing the Campbell Review. Dr Marx, the second
knowledge and attitudes, but promising evidence of short‐term
review author, collaborated closely with Dr Kettrey to compile the
effects on bystander intervention. This casts uncertainty around the
sample of research reports, code studies, and make methodological
proposed relationship between knowledge/attitudes and bystander
decisions throughout the duration of the project. Dr Tanner‐Smith, the
behavior. Although we were unable to assess these issues in the
third review author, provided methodological guidance and mentorship
current review, this will be an important direction for future
to Dr Kettrey and Dr Marx throughout all phases of data collection and
research. Our understanding of the causal mechanisms of program
analysis.
effects on bystander behavior would benefit from further analysis
(e.g., path analysis mapping relationships between specific knowl-
edge/attitude effects and bystander intervention).
Second, bystander programs exhibit a great deal of content R E F E R E N CE S

variability, most notably in framing sexual assault as a gendered or References to included studies
gender‐neutral problem. That is, bystander programs tend to adopt
one of two main approaches to addressing sexual assault: (a) Addison, W. B. (2015). Embedding sexual assault bystander awareness
education in a criminal justice course (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved
presenting sexual assault as a gendered problem (overwhelmingly
from ProQuest Dissertations Publishing. (3741449)
affecting women) or (b) presenting sexual assault as a gender‐neutral Amar, A. F., Tuccinardi, N., Heislein, J., & Simpson, S. (2015). Friends
problem (affecting women and men alike). Differential effects of Helping Friends: A nonrandomized control trial of a peer‐based
these two types of programs remain largely unexamined. Our response to dating violence. Nurse Outlook, 63(4), 496–503. https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.outlook.2015.01.004
analysis indicated that (a) the sex of victims/perpetrators (i.e.,
Baker, C. K., Naai, R., Mitchell, J., & Trecker, C. (2014). Utilizing a train‐
portrayed in programs as gender neutral or male perpetrator and the‐trainer model for sexual violence prevention: Findings from a pilot
female victim) and (b) whether programs were implemented in study with high school students of Asian and Pacific Islander descent
mixed‐ or single‐sex settings were not significant moderators of in Hawai‘i. Asian American Journal of Psychology, 5(2), 106–115.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034670
program effects on bystander intervention. However, these findings
Brokenshire, S. J. (2015). Evaluating bystander intervention training: Creating
are limited to a single outcome and they should be considered awareness on college campuses (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from
preliminary, as they are based on a small sample (n = 11). Our ProQuest Dissertations Publishing. (1593998).
understanding of the differential effects of gendered versus gender Chiriboga, X. (2016). A text nessage intervention to increase awareness of
neutral programs would benefit from the design and implementation sexual assault on campus (Masters thesis). Retrieved from DigitalCom-
mons. (Paper 901)
of high‐quality primary studies that make direct comparisons
Cook‐Craig, P. G., Coker, A. L., Clear, E. R., Garcia, L. S., Bush, H. M.,
between these two types of programs (e.g., RCTs comparing the Brancato, C. J., … Fisher, B. S. (2014). Challenge and opportunity in
effects of two active treatment arms that differ in their gendered evaluating a diffusion‐based active bystanding prevention program:
approach). Green Dot in high schools. Violence Against Women, 20(10),
1179–1202. https://doi.org/10.1177/1077801214551288
Finally, our systematic review and meta‐analysis demonstrate the
Cook‐Craig, P. G., Millspaugh, P. H., Recktenwald, E. A., Kelly, N. C., Hegge,
lack of global evidence concerning bystander program effectiveness. L. M., Coker, A. L., & Pletcher, T. S. (2014). From empower to Green
Our understanding of bystander programs’ generalizability to non‐US Dot: Successful strategies and lessons learned in developing
contexts would be greatly enhanced by high quality research comprehensive sexual violence primary prevention programming.
Violence Against Women, 20(10), 1162–1178. https://doi.org/10.
conducted across the world.
1177/1077801214551286
Darlington, E. M. (2014). Decreasing misperceptions of sexual violence to
increase bystander intervention: A social norms intervention (Doctoral
F UN D ING INF O RM AT IO N dissertation). Retrieved from University of Oregon Libraries. (18489).
Foubert, J. D. (2000). The longitudinal effects of a rape‐prevention
This review was supported by a grant from Campbell (CSR1.60). program on fraternity men's attitudes, behavioral intent, and
34 of 40 | KETTREY ET AL.

behavior. Journal of American College Health, 48, 158–163. https:// Potter, S. J., Stapleton, J. G., & Moynihan, M. M. (2008). Designing,
doi.org/10.1080/07448480009595691 implementing, and evaluating a media campaign illustrating the
Edwards, K. M., Rodenhizer‐Stämpfli, R. A., & Eckstein, R. P. (2015). bystander role. Journal of Prevention & Intervention in the Commu-
Bystander action in situations of dating and sexual aggression: A nity, 36(1‐2), 39–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/10852350802022308
mixed methodological study of high school youth. Journal of Youth Salazar, L. F., Vivolo‐Kantor, A., Hardin, J., & Berkowitz, A. (2014). A web‐
and Adolescence, 44(12), 2321–2336. https://doi.org/10.1007/ based sexual violence bystander intervention for male college
s10964‐015‐0307‐z students: Randomized controlled trial. Journal of Medical Internet
Foubert, J. D., & Marriott, K. A. (1997). Effects of a sexual assault peer Research, 16(9), 3–24.
education program on men’s belief in rape myths. Sex Roles, 36, Sargent, K. S., Jouriles, E. N., Rosenfield, D., & McDonald, R. (2017). A high
259–268. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02766271 school‐based evaluation of TakeCARE, a video bystander program to
Foubert, J. D., & McEwen, M. K. (1998). An all‐male rape‐prevention peer prevent adolescent relationship violence. Journal of Youth and Adoles-
education program: Decreasing fraternity men’s behavioral intent to cence, 46(3), 633–643. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964‐016‐0622‐z
rape. Journal of College Student Development, 39, 548–556. Senn, C. Y., & Forrest, A. (2016). “And then one night when I went to
Foubert, J. D., Newberry, J. T., & Tatum, J. L. (2007). Behavior differences class.”: The impact of sexual assault bystander intervention work-
seven months later: Effects of a rape prevention program. NACPA shops incorporated in academic courses. Psychology of Violence, 6(4),
Journal, 44, 728–749. 607–618. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039660
Gidycz, C. A., Orchowski, L. M., & Berkowitz, A. D. (2011). Preventing
sexual aggression among college men: An evaluation of a social norms
and bystander intervention program. Violence Against Women, 17(6), References to excluded studies
720–742. https://doi.org/10.1177/1077801211409727
Jouriles, E., et al. (n.d.). TakeCARE, a video to promote bystander behavior on Ahrens, C. E., Rich, M. D., & Ullman, J. B. (2011). Rehearsing for real life:
college campuses: Replication and extension. Unpublished manuscript. The impact of the InterACT Sexual Assault Prevention Program on
Jouriles, E. N., McDonald, R., Rosenfield, D., Levy, N., Sargent, K., Caiozzo, self‐reported likelihood of engaging in bystander interventions.
C., & Grych, J. H. (2016a). TakeCARE, a video bystander program to Violence Against Women, 17(6), 760–776. https://doi.org/10.1177/
help prevent sexual violence on college campuses: Results of two 1077801211410212
randomized, controlled trials. Psychology of Violence, 6(3), 410–420. Alegría‐Flores, K., Raker, K., Pleasants, R. K., Weaver, M. A., &
https://doi.org/10.1037/vio0000016. Weinberger, M. (2017). Preventing interpersonal violence on college
Kleinsasser, A., Jouriles, E. N., McDonald, R., & Rosenfield, D. (2015). An campuses: The effect of one act training on bystander intervention.
online bystander intervention program for the prevention of sexual Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 32(7), 1103–1126.
violence. Psychology of Violence, 5(3), 227–235. https://doi.org/10. Amar, A. F., Sutherland, M., & Kesler, E. (2012). Evaluation of a bystander
1037/a0037393 education program. Issues in Mental Health Nursing, 33(12),
Mabry, A., & Turner, M. M. (2016). Do sexual assault bystander 851–857. https://doi.org/10.3109/01612840.2012.709915
interventions change men's intentions? Applying the theory of Baiocchi, M. (2016). A cluster‐randomized trial to assess a sexual assault
normative social behavior to predicting bystander outcomes. Journal prevention intervention in adolescents in Nairobi, Kenya. Retrived
of Health Communication, 21(3), 276–292. https://doi.org/10.1080/ from ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT02771132.
10810730.2015.1058437 Baldwin‐White, A., Thompson, M. S., & Gray, A. (2016). Pre‐ and
McCauley, H. L., Tancredi, D. J., Silverman, J. G., Decker, M. R., Austin, S. postintervention factor analysis of the Illinois rape myth acceptance
B., McCormick, M. C., … Miller E., M. C. (2013). Gender‐equitable scale. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma, 25(6), 636–651.
attitudes, bystander behavior, and recent abuse perpetration against https://doi.org/10.1080/10926771.2015.1107173
heterosexual dating partners of male high school athletes. American Bannon, R. S. (2014). The bystander approach to sexual assault risk reduction:
Journal of Public Health, 103(10), 1882–1887. https://doi.org/10. Effects on risk recognition, perceived self‐efficacy, and protective behavior
2105/AJPH.2013.301443 (Doctoral dissertation). Retrived from Oklahoma State University.
Miller, E., Das, M., Tancredi, D. J., McCauley, H. L., Virata, M. C. D., Bannon, R. S. P., & Foubert, J. D. P. (2017). The bystander approach to
Nettiksimmons, J., … Verma, R. (2014). Evaluation of a gender‐based sexual assault risk reduction: Effects on risk recognition, perceived
violence prevention program for student athletes in Mumbai, India. self‐efficacy, and protective behavior. Violence and Victims, 32(1),
Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 29(4), 758–778. https://doi.org/10. 46–59. https://doi.org/10.1891/0886‐6708.VV‐D‐15‐00057
1177/0886260513505205 Banyard, V. L. (2015). Toward the next generation of bystander
Miller, E., Tancredi, D. J., McCauley, H. L., Decker, M. R., Virata, M. C. D., prevention of sexual and relationship violence: Action coils to engage
Anderson, H. A., … Silverman, J. G. (2013). One‐year follow‐up of a communities, New York, NY: Springer Science + Business Media.
coach‐delivered dating violence prevention program: A cluster Banyard, V. L., Moynihan, M. M., & Crossman, M. T. (2009). Reducing
randomized controlled trial. American Journal of Preventive Medi- sexual violence on campus: The role of student leaders as empowered
cine, 45(1), 108–112. bystanders. Journal of College Student Development, 50(4), 446–457.
Moynihan, M. M., Banyard, V. L., Arnold, J. S., Eckstein, R. P., & Stapleton, J. Barnes, C. (2015). The effect of a class‐wide training on prosocial bystander
G. (2011). Sisterhood may be powerful for reducing sexual and intimate behaviors (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from ProQuest Disserta-
partner violence: An evaluation of the Bringing in the Bystander in‐ tions. (3688030).
person program with sorority members. Violence Against Women, Barone, R. P., Wolgemuth, J. R., & Linder, C. (2007). Preventing sexual assault
17(6), 703–719. https://doi.org/10.1177/1077801211409726 through engaging college men. Journal of College Student Development,
Moynihan, M. M., Banyard, V. L., Arnold, J. S., Eckstein, R. P., & Stapleton, 48(5), 585–594. https://doi.org/10.1353/csd.2007.0045
J. G. (2010). Engaging intercollegiate athletes in preventing and Beardall, N. G. (2008). A program evaluation research study on the
intervening in sexual and intimate partner violence. Journal of implementation of the mentors in violence prevention program in a public
American College Health, 59(3), 197–204. high school (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from ProQuest Dis-
Peterson, K., Sharps, P., Banyard, V., Powers, R. A., Kaukinen, C., Gross, sertations. (3305287)
D., … Campbell, J. (2016). An evaluation of two dating violence Bluth, S. J. (2014). Breaking the culture of silence in the sisterhood: Using
prevention programs on a college campus. Journal of Interpersonal bystander intervention (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from Pro-
Violence, https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260516636069 Quest Dissertations. (3581896)
KETTREY ET AL. | 35 of 40

Borsky, A. E. (2014). Promoting prosocial behaviors to prevent dating violence Australian Football League. Violence Against Women, 22(3),
among college students: Evaluation of a bystander intervention (Doctoral 324–324.
dissertation). Retrived from ERIC. (ED567503) Dempsey, A. G. (2009). Aggression and victimization in middle school: A
Borsky, A. E., McDonnell, K. P., Rimal, R. N. P., & Turner, M. P. (2016). mixed methods analysis of the process and effectiveness of implementing a
Assessing bystander behavior intentions toward friends to prevent prevention program (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from ProQuest
dating violence: Development of the bystander behavior intentions‐ Dissertations. (3467697)
friends scale through concept mapping and exploratory factor Dick, A. S. (2009). Transforming witnesses to actors: 100+ men against
analysis. . Violence and Victims, 31(2), 215–234. https://doi.org/10. domestic violence (Masters thesis). Retrieved from Rutgers University‐
1891/0886‐6708.VV‐D‐13‐00196 New Brunswick.
Borsky, A. E., McDonnell, K., Turner, M. M., & Rimal, R. (2016). Raising a El‐Khoury, J. R., & Shafer, A. (2016). Narrative exemplars and the celebrity
red flag on dating violence: Evaluation of a low‐resource, College‐ spokesperson in lebanese anti‐domestic violence public service
Based Bystander Behavior Intervention Program. Journal of Inter- announcements. Journal of Health Communication, 21(8), 935–943.
personal Violence, https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260516635322 Elias‐Lambert, N. (2013). Bystander sexual violence prevention program:
Bush, H. (2016). ConnectEd: A randomized controlled trial connecting Implementation and evaluation with high‐risk university males (Doctoral
through educational training (ConnectEd). ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: dissertation). Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations. (3568843)
NCT02786472. Elias‐Lambert, N., & Black, B. M. (2016). Bystander sexual violence
Bush, H. (2015). A pilot of bystander training programming in sexual prevention program: Outcomes for high‐ and low‐risk university men.
violence and substance Abuse (P_ConnectED). ClinicalTrials.gov Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 31(19), 3211–3235. https://doi.
Identifier: NCT02591212. org/10.1177/0886260515584346
Cambron, A. P. (2014). Pilot study of safe sisters: A sexual assault prevention Espelage, D. (2013). Multi‐Site Evaluation of Second Step (SSTP).
program for sorority women (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT01792167.
ProQuest Dissertations & Theses Global. (1660186740). Foubert, J. D., & Masin, R. C. (2012). Effects of the Men's Program on U.S.
Cares, A. C., Banyard, V. L., Moynihan, M. M., Williams, L. M., Potter, S. J., Army Soldiers' intentions to commit and willingness to intervene to
& Stapleton, J. G. (2015). Changing attitudes about being a bystander prevent rape: A Pretest Posttest Study. Violence and Victims, 27(6),
to violence: Translating an in‐person sexual violence prevention 911–921. https://doi.org/10.1891/0886‐6708.27.6.911
program to a new campus. Violence Against Women, 21(2), 165–187. Foubert, J. D., & Newberry, J. T. (2006). Effects of two versions of an
https://doi.org/10.1177/1077801214564681 Empathy‐Based Rape Prevention Program on fraternity men's
Caver, K. A. (2012). Masculine ideology and college men's reactions to a survivor empathy, attitudes, and behavioral intent to commit rape
sexual assault prevention program (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved or sexual assault. Journal of College Student Development, 133–148.
from ProQuest Dissertations. (3532120) https://doi.org/10.1353/csd.2006.0016
Childers, E. M. (2011). An evaluation of a peer led bystander intervention Foubert, J. D., & Perry, B. C. (2007). Creating lasting attitude and behavior
program to reduce sexual assault violence among college students change in fraternity members and male student athletes: The
(Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from ScholarWorks@UARK. (253) qualitative impact of an Empathy‐Based Rape Prevention Program.
Cissner, A. B. (2009). Evaluating the mentors in violence prevention Violence Against Women, 13, 70–86. https://doi.org/10.1177/
program: Preventing gender violence on a college campus. Unpub- 1077801206295125
lished manuscript, Center for Court Innovation, New York, NY. Foubert, J. D., Godin, E. E., & Tatum, J. L. (2009). In their own words:
Coker, A. (2016). Multi‐college bystander efficacy evaluation. Clinical- Sophomore college men describe attitude and behavior changes
Trials.gov Identifier: NCT02659423. resulting from a rape prevention program 2 years after their
Clinton‐Sherrod, A. M., Morgan‐Lopez, A. A., Gibbs, D., Hawkins, S. R., participation. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 25, 2237–2257.
Hart, L., Ball, B., … Littler, N. (2009). Factors contributing to the https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260509354881
effectiveness of four school‐based sexual violence interventions. Foubert, J. D., Langhinrichsen‐Rohling, J., Brasfield, H., & Hill, B. (2010).
Health Promotion Practice, 10(1), 19S. https://doi.org/10.1177/ Effects of a rape awareness program on college women: Increasing
1524839908319593 bystander efficacy and willingness to intervene. Journal of Commu-
Coker, A. L., & Clear, E. R. (2015). New approaches to violence prevention nity Psychology, 38(7), 813–827.
through bystander intervention. Critical issues on violence against Foubert, J. D., Tatum, J., & Donahue, G. A. (2006). Reactions of first‐year
women: International perspectives and promising strategies 221–232. men to Rape Prevention Program: Attitude and predicted behavior
New York, NY: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group. changes. NASPA Journal, 43(3), 578–598.
Coker, A. L., Bush, H. M., Fisher, B. S., Swan, S. C., Williams, C. M., Clear, E. Garman, A. M. (2013). Increasing the effectiveness of sexual harassment
R., & DeGue, S. (2016). Multi‐college bystander intervention evalua- prevention through learner engagement (Doctoral dissertation). Re-
tion for violence prevention. American Journal of Preventive trieved from ProQuest Dissertations. (1523174).
Medicine, 50(3), 295–302. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2015. Gedney, C. R. (2016). Sexual assault prevention: A randomized controlled trial
08.034 of a military intervention (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from
Coker, A. L., Cook‐Craig, P. G., Williams, C. M., Fisher, B. S., Clear, E. R., ProQuest Dissertations. (10158652).
Garcia, L. S., & Hegge, L. M. (2011). Evaluation of Green Dot: An active Glass, N. (2014). Effectiveness of a safety app to respond to dating
bystander intervention to reduce sexual violence on college cam- violence for college women and their friends. ClinicalTrials.gov
puses. Violence Against Women, 17(6), 777–796. https://doi.org/10. Identifier: NCT02236663.
1177/1077801211410264 Guerra, N. G., & Williams, K. R. (2010). Implementing bullying prevention
Coker, A. L., Fisher, B. S., Bush, H. M., Swan, S. C., Williams, C. M., Clear, E. in diverse settings: Geographic, economic, and cultural influences,
R., & DeGue, S. (2015). Evaluation of the Green Dot bystander Preventing and treating bullying and victimization (pp. 319–336).
intervention to reduce interpersonal violence among college students New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
across three campuses. Violence Against Women, 21(12), 1507–1527. Hahn, C. K., Morris, J. M., & Jacobs, G. A. (2017). Predictors of bystander
https://doi.org/10.1177/1077801214545284 behaviors and sexual assertiveness among college women attending a
Corboz, J., Flood, M., & Dyson, S. (2016). Challenges of bystander sexual assault prevention program. Journal of Community Psychology,
intervention in male‐dominated professional sport: Lessons from the 45(5), 672–677.
36 of 40 | KETTREY ET AL.

Hawkins, S. R. (2005). Evaluation findings: Men can stop rape, men of Lynch, A., & Fleming, M. W. (2005). Bystander approaches: Empowering
strength club 2004‐2005. Retrieved from http://www. students to model ethical sexual behavior. Journal of Family and
mencanstoprape.org/images/stories/Images__Logos/ Consumer Sciences, 97(3), 27–33.
Henriksen, C. B., Mattick, K. L., & Fisher, B. S. (2016). Mandatory McMahon, S. (2014). Experimentally testing the effectiveness of a
bystander intervention training: Is the SaVE Act requirement the campus‐based bystander intervention. ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier:
“right” program to reduce violence among college students? The crisis NCT02083302.
of campus sexual violence: Critical perspectives on prevention and McMahon, S. (2009). Hearing from student‐athlete peer educators: A
response 169–183. New York, NY: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group. qualitative study of the impact of participation in an interpersonal
Hines, D. A., & Palm Reed, K. M. (2015). Predicting improvement after a violence peer education group. Academic Athletic Journal, 20,
bystander program for the prevention of sexual and dating violence. 1–28.
Health promotion practice, 16(4), 550–559. https://doi.org/10.1177/ Mcmahon, S., & Dick, A. (2011). "Being in a room with like‐minded men":
1524839914557031 An exploratory study of men's participation in a bystander interven-
Hines, D. A., & Reed, K. M. P. (2015). An experimental evaluation of peer tion program to prevent intimate partner violence. The Journal of
versus professional educators of a bystander program for the Men's Studies, 19(1), 3–18. https://doi.org/10.3149/jms.1901.3
prevention of sexual and dating violence among college students. McMahon, S., Hoffman, M. L., McMahon, S. M., Zucker, S., & Koenick, R. A.
Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma, 24(3), 279–298. (2013). What would you do? Strategies for bystander intervention to
https://doi.org/10.1080/10926771.2015.1009601 prevent sexual violence by college students. Journal of College and
Hollingsworth, C., Ramey, K. J., & Hadley, J. A. (2011). Bystander Character, 14(2), 141–151.
intervention pilot: Final report. Retrieved from http://www.ncdsv. McMahon, S., Postmus, J. L., Warrener, C., & Koenick, R. A. (2014).
org/images/USNavy_BystanderInterventionPilot_4‐2011.pdf Utilizing peer education theater for the primary prevention of sexual
Jaime, M. C. D., McCauley, H. L., Tancredi, D. J., Nettiksimmons, J., Decker, violence on college campuses. Journal of College Student Develop-
M. R., Silverman, J. G., … Miller, E. (2015). Athletic coaches as violence ment, 55(1), 78–85. https://doi.org/10.1353/csd.2014.0001.
prevention advocates. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 30(7), McMahon, S., Winter, S. C., Palmer, J. E., Postmus, J. L., Peterson, A. N.,
1090–1111. https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260514539847 Zucker, S., & Koenick, R. (2015). A randomized controlled trial of a
Johnson‐Quay, N. L. (2015). Measuring rape culture with college students and multi‐dose bystander intervention program using peer education
its role in bystander intervention (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from theater. Health Education Research, 30(4), 554–568.
OhioLINK. (1406555995) Miller, E. (2015). Engendering healthy masculinities to prevent sexual
Jouriles, E. N., Rosenfield, D., Yule, K., Sargent, K. S., & McDonald, R. (2016b). violence. ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT02427061.
Predicting high‐school students' bystander behavior in simulated dating Mitchell, K. S., & Freitag, J. L. (2011). Forum theatre for bystanders: A new
violence situations. Journal of Adolescent Health, 58(3), 345–351. model for gender violence prevention. Violence Against Women,
Katz, J. (1995). Reconstructing masculinity in the locker room: The 17(8), 990–1013. https://doi.org/10.1177/1077801211417152
Mentors in Violence Prevention Project. Harvard Educational Review, Moynihan, M., & Banyard, V. (2008). Community responsibility for
65(2), 163–174. preventing sexual violence: A pilot study with campus Greeks and
Katz, J., Heisterkamp, H. A., & Fleming, W. M. (2011). The social justice intercollegiate athletes. Journal of Prevention and Intervention in the
roots of the mentors in violence prevention model and its application Community, 36(1/2), 23–38.
in a high school setting. Violence Against Women, 17, 684–702. Moynihan, M. M., Banyard, V. L., Cares, A. C., Potter, S. J., Williams, L. M.,
https://doi.org/10.1177/1077801211409725 & Stapleton, J. G. (2015). Encouraging responses in sexual and
Katz, J., Olin, R., Herman, C., & DuBois, M. (2013). Spotting the signs: relationship violence prevention: What program effects remain 1 year
First‐year college students' responses to bystander‐themed rape later? Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 30(1), 110–132. https://doi.
prevention posters. Journal of Community Psychology, 41(4), org/10.1177/0886260514532719
523–529. https://doi.org/10.1002/jcop.21552 Nabity, J. (2010). The bystander approach to sexual harassment training:
Keller, J., Mboya, B. O., Sinclair, J., Githua, O. W., Mulinge, M., Bergholz, L., Considering a new perspective (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from
… Kapphahn, C. (2017). A 6‐week school curriculum improves boys’ ProQuest Dissertations & Theses Global. (860136402).
attitudes and behaviors related to gender‐based violence in Kenya. Nitzel, C. L. (2016). The process of engagement with the green dot bystander
Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 32(4), 535–557. intervention program among higher education staff/faculty (Doctoral
Kervin, D., & Obinna, J. (2010). Youth action strategies in the primary dissertation). Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations. (10102750).
prevention of teen Dating violence. Journal of Family Social Work, O’Donohue, W., Yeater, E. A., & Fanetti, M. (2003). Rape prevention with
13(4), 362–374. college males: The roles of rape myth acceptance, victim empathy, and
Kostouros, P., Warthe, G. D., Carter‐Snell, C., & Burnett, C. (2016). outcome expectancies. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 18(5),
"Stepping up": A focus on facilitator development. Journal of Student 513–531. https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260503251070
Affairs Research and Practice, 53(2), 218–229. Palm Reed, K. M., Hines, D. A., Armstrong, J. L., & Cameron, A. Y. (2015).
Lamb, S., & Randazzo, R. (2016). An examination of the effectiveness of a Experimental evaluation of a bystander prevention program for
sexual ethics curriculum. Journal of Moral Education, 45(1), 16–30. sexual assault and dating violence. Psychology of Violence, 5(1),
https://doi.org/10.1080/03057240.2016.1156520 95–102. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037557
Lawson, S. L., Munoz‐Rojas, D., Gutman, L. S., & Siman, M. N. (2012). Pfetsch, J., Steffgen, G., Gollwitzer, M., & Ittel, A. (2011). Prevention of
Changing attitudes and perceptions of Hispanic men ages 18 to 25 aggression in schools through a bystander intervention training.
about rape and rape prevention. Issues in Mental Health Nursing, International Journal of Developmental Science, 5(1‐2), 139–149.
33(12), 864–870. https://doi.org/10.3109/01612840.2012.728279 Potter, S. J. (2012). Using a multimedia social marketing campaign to
Leacock, J. (2017). Sexual violence: Helping men become allies. A sexual increase active bystanders on the college campus. Journal of
violence bystander intervention program for collegiate males (Doctoral American College Health, 60(4), 282–295.
dissertation). Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations. (10263666) Potter, S. J., & Moynihan, M. M. (2011). Bringing in the bystander in‐
Liu, E. H. (2010). A study of a university‐based men‐only prevention program (men person prevention program to a U.S. military installation: Results from
care): Effect on attitudes and behaviors related to sexual violence (Doctoral a pilot study. Military Medicine, 176(8), 870–875. https://doi.org/10.
dissertation). Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations. (3418108). 7205/MILMED‐D‐10‐00483
KETTREY ET AL. | 37 of 40

Potter, S. J., & Stapelton, J. G. (2013). Assessing the efficacy of a experimental evaluation. Violence and Victims, 25, 202–223. https://
bystander social marketing campaign four weeks following the doi.org/10.1891/0886‐6708.25.2.202
campaign administration. Sexual Assault Report, 16, 65–80. Taylor, B. G., Mumford, E. A., Liu, W., & Stein, N. D. (2017). The effects of
Potter, S. J., & Stapleton, J. G. (2012). Translating sexual assault different saturation levels of the Shifting Boundaries intervention on
prevention from a college campus to a United States military preventing adolescent relationship abuse and sexual harassment.
installation: Piloting the Know‐Your‐Power Bystander Social Market- Journal of Experimental Criminology, 13(1), 79–100. https://doi.org/
ing Campaign. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 27(8), 1593–1621. 10.1007/s11121‐012‐0293‐2
Potter, S. J., Moynihan, M. M., & Stapleton, J. G. (2011). Using social self‐ Twemlow, S. W., Fonagy, P., & Sacco, F. C. (2004). The Role of the Bystander in
identification in social marketing materials aimed at reducing violence the Social Architecture of Bullying and Violence in Schools and
against women on campus. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 26(5), Communities, Scientific Approaches to Youth Violence Prevention (pp.
971–990. 215–232). New York, NY: New York Academy of Sciences.
Potter, S. J., Moynihan, M. M., Stapleton, J. G., & Banyard, V. L. (2009). Vadovic, R. J. (2013). Implementing a bystander awareness program on a
Empowering bystanders to prevent campus violence against women: A university campus (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from ProQuest
preliminary evaluation of a poster campaign. Violence Against Women, Dissertations. (3590174).
15(1), 106–121. https://doi.org/10.1177/1077801208327482 Wang, C., Couch, L., Rodriguez, G. R., & Lee, C. (2015). The bullying
Rich, M. (2010). The interACT model: Considering rape prevention literature project: Using children's literature to promote prosocial
from a performance activism and social justice perspective. Feminism behavior and social‐emotional outcomes among elementary school
and Psychology, 20(4), 511–528. https://doi.org/10.1177/ students. Contemporary School Psychology, 19(4), 320–329.
0959353510371366 Ward, K. J. (2001). Mentors in Violence Prevention Program Evaluation
Schumitsch‐Jewell, T. (2016). Stand up and speak up: Effectiveness of 1999‐2000. http://www.mvpnational.org/wp‐content/uploads/2011/
web‐based bystander intervention on a college campus. Oshkosh 12/MVP‐HS‐Eval‐Report‐1999‐2000.pdf
Scholar, 11, 8–20. Warthe, D. G., & Kostouros, P. (2015). Stepping up: Reducing dating violence
Schweer, J. L., Heather, K., Kay, K., Stewart, L. K., & Kovach, L. (2012). R.U. on post‐secondary campuses. Annual Conference ‐ End Violence
Ready?: Peer education and bystander intervention sexual assault Against Women International.
programming. NASPA Journal About Women in Higher Education, Weitzman, A., Cowan, S., & Walsh, K. (2017). Bystander interventions on
5(1), 96–98. behalf of sexual assault and intimate partner violence victims. Journal of
Shiflet, J. H. (2013). Action research evaluation of bystander intervention Interpersonal Violence, https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260517696873
training created by Munche, Stern, and O'Brien (Doctoral dissertation). Williams, D. J., & Neville, F. G. (2017). Qualitative evaluation of the
Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations. (3598482). mentors in violence prevention pilot in Scottish high schools.
Slaby, R., Wilson‐Brewer, R., & DeVos, H. (1994). Final report for Psychology of Violence, 7(2), 213.
aggressors, victims, and bystanders project ‐ final report of grant Winegust, A. K. (2015). Pass it on: An evaluation of a sexualized violence
No. R490CCR 103559. Unpublished manuscript, Centers for Disease prevention program for middle school and high school students (Masters
Control and Prevention, Atlanta, GA. thesis). Retrived from ProQuest Dissertations Publishing. (1605889).
Sparks, K. B. (2015). Initiating a bystander awareness program at a state Woglom, L., & Pennington, K. (2010). The bystander's dilemma: How can we
university (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from eRepository@Seton turn our students into upstanders? Social Education, 74(5), 254–258.
Hall. (6). Wuthrich, C. K. (2009). Reflections on personal responsibility: Sorority
Stephens, M. R. (2013). Helping survivors of sexual assault: The role of general members at risk for interpersonal violence. NASPA Journal, 46(2),
and event‐specific empathy (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from 228–257.
ProQuest Dissertations. (1527418). Yoshihama, M., & Tolman, R. M. (2015). Using interactive theater to create
Stewart, A. L. (2014). The Men’s Project: A sexual assault prevention socioculturally relevant community‐based intimate partner violence
program targeting college men. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, prevention. American Journal of Community Psychology, 55(1‐2),
15(4), 481–485. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033947. 136–147. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10464‐014‐9700‐0
Taylor, B. G., Stein, N. D., & Burden, F. F. (2010). Exploring gender
differences in dating violence/harassment prevention programming in
middle schools: Results from a randomized experiment. Journal of References to studies awaiting classification
Experimental Criminology, 4(6), 419–445. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s11292‐010‐9103‐7 Banyard, V. L., Potter, S. J., Cares, A. C., Williams, L. M., Moynihan, M. M.,
Taylor, B., & Stein, N. (n.d.). Evaluation of gender violence and harassment & Stapelton, J. S. (n. d.). Bystander interventions to prevent sexual
prevention programs in Middle Schools in Cleveland, Ohio, 2006‐ violence on diverse campuses: A test of multiple prevention tools.
2007 [United States]: Codebook. Unpublished manuscript, ICPSR Unpublished Manuscript.
22660, Ann Arbor, Michigan. Orchowski, L. M., Barnett, N., Borsari, B., Wood, M., Zlotnick, C., &
Taylor, B., et al. (2011). Shifting boundaries: Final report on an Oesterle, D. (2015). Integrated alcohol and sexual assault prevention
experimental evaluation of a youth dating violence prevention for heavy drinking collegemen: Short term findings from a randomized
program in New York City middle schools, Washington, DC: National pilot trial. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 39, 279A.
Institute of Justice. Plante, E. G., Banyard, V. L., & Moynihan, M. M. (2002). Rape prevention
Taylor, B. G., Mumford, E. A., Liu, W., & Stein, N. D. (2016). Assessing through bystander education: Bringing a broader community per-
different levels and dosages of the shifting boundaries intervention to spective to prevention. Paper presented at Victimization of Children
prevent youth dating violence in New York City Middle Schools: A and Youth: An International Research Conference.
randomized control trial. Unpublished manuscript.
Taylor, B., Stain, N., Woods, D., & Mumford, E. (n.d.). Experimental
evaluation of a youth dating violence prevention program in New References to ongoing studies
York City Middle Schools, 2009‐2010: Codebook. Unpublished
manuscript, ICPSR 32901. Gilliam, M. In‐school evaluation of bystander: A game‐based intervention
Taylor, B., Stein, N., & Burden, F. (2010). The effects of gender violence/ for sexual violence prevention. ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier:
harassment prevention programming in middle schools: A randomized NCT02919098.
38 of 40 | KETTREY ET AL.

Griffin, K. W. Primary prevention of sexual violence among college students. Bennett, S., Banyard, V. L., & Garnhart, L. (2014). To act or not to act, that
ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT03037866. is the question? Barriers and facilitators of bystander intervention.
Jouries, E. Evaluating a Video Bystander Program for first‐year college Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 29, 476–496. https://doi.org/10.
students. ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT03056560. 1177/0886260513505210
Miller, E. College health center‐based alcohol and sexual violence Bjorklund, K., Hakkanen‐Nyholm, H., Huttunen, T., & Kunttu, K. (2010).
intervention (GIFTSS). ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT02355470. Violence victimization among Finnish university students: Relevance,
Miller, E. Trial of a middle school coach gender violence prevention symptoms and healthcare usage. Social Science in Medicine, 70(9),
program. ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT02331238. 1416–22.
Orchowski, L. Preventing sexual aggression among high school boys. Black, B. M., Tolman, R. M., Callahan, M., Saunders, D. G., & Weisz, A. N.
ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT02506088 (2008). When will adolescents tell someone about dating violence
Shorey, R. Interventions for Sanctioned Ohio University Students. victimization? Violence Against Women, 14, 741–758. https://doi.org/
ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT02603978. 10.1177/1077801208320248
Shorey, R. Group alcohol interventions for college men. ClinicalTrials.gov Bogle, K. (2007). The shift from dating to hooking up in college: What
Identifier: NCT02983864. scholars have missed. Sociology Compass, 1/2, 775–788. https://doi.
org/10.1111/j.1751‐9020.2007.00031.x
Bogle, K. (2008). Hooking up: Sex, dating, and relationships on campus.
Additional references New York: New York University Press.
Burgess, G. H. (2007). Assessment of rape‐supportive attitudes and beliefs
Adelman, M., & Kil, S. H. (2007). Dating conflicts: Rethinking dating in college men: Development, reliability, and validity of the Rape
violence and youth conflict. Violence Against Women, 13, Attitudes and Beliefs Scale. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 22,
1296–1318. https://doi.org/10.1177/1077801207310800. 973–993. https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260507302993
Anderson, L. A., & Whiston, S. C. (2005). Sexual assault education Burn, S. M. (2009). A situational model of sexual assault prevention
programs: A meta‐analytic examination of their effectiveness. through bystander intervention. Sex Roles, 60, 779–792. https://doi.
Psychology of Women Quarterly, 29, 374–388. org/10.1007/s11199‐008‐9581‐5
Arriaga, X. B., & Foshee, V. A. (2004). Adolescent dating violence: Do Casey, E. A., & Ohler, K. (2012). Being a positive bystander: Male
adolescents follow in their friends’, or their parents’, footsteps? antiviolence allies’ experiences of ‘Stepping Up’. Journal of Inter-
Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 19, 162–184. https://doi.org/10. personal Violence, 27, 62–83. https://doi.org/10.1177/
1177/0886260503260247 0886260511416479
Ashley, O. S., & Foshee, V. A. (2005). Adolescent help‐seeking for dating Cornelius, T. L., & Resseguie, N. (2007). Primary and secondary prevention
violence: Prevalence, sociodemographic correlates, and sources of programs for dating violence: A review of the literature. Aggression
help. Journal of Adolescent Health, 36, 25–31. https://doi.org/10. and Violent Behavior, 12, 364–375. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.
1016/j.jadohealth.2003.12.014 2006.09.006
Australian Human Rights Commission (2017). Change the course: Cui, M., Ueno, K., Gordon, M., & Fincham, F. D. (2013). The continuation of
National report on sexual assault and sexual harassment at Australian intimate partner violence from adolescence to young adulthood.
Universities. Australian Human Rights Commission. Journal of Marriage and Family, 75, 300–313. https://doi.org/10.
Banyard, V. L. (2008). Measurement and correlates of prosocial bystander 1111/jomf.12016
behavior: The case of interpersonal violence. Violence and Victims, Darley, J. M., & Latane, B. (1968). Bystander intervention in emergencies:
23, 83–97. https://doi.org/10.1891/0886‐6708.23.1.83 Diffusion of responsibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
Banyard, V. L., Moynihan, M. M., & Plante, E. G. (2007). Sexual violence ogy, 4, 377–383http://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/h0025589
prevention through bystander education: An experimental evaluation. DeGue, S., Valle, L. A., Holt, M. K., Massetti, G. M., Matjasko, J. L., & Tharp,
Journal of Community Psychology, 35(4), 463–481. A. T. (2014). A systematic review of primary prevention strategies for
Banyard, V. L. (2011). Who will help prevent sexual violence? Creating an sexual violence perpetration. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 19,
ecological model of bystander intervention. Psychology of Violence, 1, 346–362. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2014.05.004
216–229. http://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/a0023739 Deitz, S. R., Blackwell, K. T., Daley, P. C., & Bentley, B. J. (1982).
Banyard, V. L., Moynihan, M. M., Cares, A. C., & Warner, R. (2014). How do Measurement of empathy toward rape victims and rapists. Journal of
we know if It works? Measuring outcomes in bystander‐focused abuse Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 372–384.
prevention on campuses. Psychology of Violence, 4, 101–115. https:// De Koker, P., Mathews, C., Zuch, M., Bastien, S., & Mason‐Jones, A. J.
doi.org/10.1037/a0033470 (2014). A systematic review of interventions for preventing adoles-
Banyard, V. L., Moynihan, M. M., & Crossman, M. T. (2009). Reducing cent intimate partner violence. Journal of Adolescent Health, 54,
sexual violence on campus: The role of student leaders as empowered 3–13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2013.08.008
bystanders. Journal of College Student Development, 50, 446–457. De La Rue, L., Polanin, J. R., Espelage, D. L., & Pigott, T. D. (2014). School‐
https://doi.org/10.1353/csd.0.0083 based interventions to reduce dating and sexual violence: A
Banyard, V. L., Moynihan, M. M., & Plante, E. G. (2007). Sexual violence systematic review. Campbell Systematic Reviews, 10(7).
prevention through bystander education: An experimental evaluation. Duval, S., & Tweedie, R. (2000). Trim and fill: a simple funnel‐plot–based
Journal of Community Psychology, 35, 463–481. https://doi.org/10. method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta‐analysis.
1002/jcop.20159. Biometrics, 56(2), 455–463. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0006‐341X.
Banyard, V. L., Plante, E. G., & Moynihan, M. M. (2004). Bystander 2000.00455.x
education: Bringing a broader community perspective to sexual Egger, M., Smith, G. D., Schneider, M., & Minder, C. (1997). Bias in meta‐
violence prevention. Journal of Community Psychology, 32, 61–79. analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. British Medical Journal,
https://doi.org/10.1002/jcop.10078 315, 629–634. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629
Barreto, M., & Ellemers., N. (2005). The perils of political correctness: England, P., & Ronen, S. (2015). Hooking up and casual sex, International
Men’s and women’s responses to old‐fashioned and modern sexist Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences: Second Edition,
views. Social Psychology Quarterly, 68, 75–88. https://doi.org/10. 192–196. Elsevier Inc. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978‐0‐08‐097086‐8.
1177/019027250506800106 35010‐3
KETTREY ET AL. | 39 of 40

Exner, D., & Cummings, N. (2011). Implications for sexual assault Katz, J. [Jennifer] (2015). Effects of group status and victim sex on male
prevention: College students as prosocial bystanders. Journal of bystanders’ responses to a potential party rape. Journal of Aggres-
American College Health, 59, 655–657. https://doi.org/10.1080/ sion, Maltreatment, & Trauma, 24, 588–602. https://doi.org/10.1080/
07448481.2010.515633 10926771.2015.1029184
Exner‐Cortens, D., Eckenrode, J., & Rothman, E. (2013). Longitudinal Katz, J. [Jennifer], Colbert, S., & Colangelo, L. (2015). Effects of group
associations between teen dating violence victimization and adverse status and victim sex on female bystanders’ responses to a potential
health outcomes. Pediatrics, 131(1), 71–78. https://doi.org/10.1542/ party rape. Violence and Victims, 30, 265–278. https://doi.org/10.
peds.2012‐1029 1891/0886‐6708.VV‐D‐13‐00099
Fellmeth, G. L. T., Heffernan, C., Nurse, J., Habibula, S., & Sethi, D. (2013). Katz, J. [Jennifer], & Moore, J. (2013). Bystander education training for campus
Educational and skills‐based interventions for preventing relationahip sexual assault prevention: An initial meta‐analysis. Violence and Victims,
and dating violence in adolescents and young adults (Review). 28, 1054–1067. https://doi.org/10.1891/0886‐6708.VV‐D‐12‐00113
Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, Issue 6, https://doi.org/ Kettrey, H. H. (2016). What’s gender got to do with it? Sexual double
10.1002/14651858.CD004534.pub3. Art. No.: CD004534. standards and power in heterosexual college hookups. The Journal of
Foshee, V. A. (1996). Gender differences in adolescent dating abuse Sex Research, 53, 754–765. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2016.
prevalence, types, and injuries. Health Education Research, 11, 1145181
275–286. https://doi.org/10.1093/her/11.3.275 Koss, M., & Gidycz, C. A. (1985). Sexual Experiences Survey: Reliability and
Foshee, V. A., Benefield, T. S., Ennett, S. T., Bauman, K. E., & Suchindran, C. validity. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53(3), 422–423.
(2004). Longitudinal predictors of serious physical and sexual dating Krebs, C. P., Lindquist, C. H., Warner, T. D., Fisher, B. S., & Martin, S. L.
violence victimization during adolescence. Preventive Medicine, 39, (2009). College women’s experiences with physically forced, alcohol‐
1007–1016. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2004.04.014 or other drug‐enabled, and drug‐facilitated sexual assault before and
Foshee, V. A., Linder, F., MacDougall, J. E., & Bangdiwala, S. (2001). since entering college. Journal of American College Health, 57,
Gender differences in the longitudinal predictors of adolescent dating 639–649. https://doi.org/10.3200/JACH.57.6.639‐649
violence. Preventive Medicine, 32, 128–141. https://doi.org/10.1006/ Lanier, C. A., & Elliot, M. N. (1997). A new instrument for the evaluation of
pmed.2000.0793 a date rape prevention program. Journal of College Student
Foshee, V. A., Reyes, H. L. M., & Ennett, S. T. (2010). Examination of sex Development, 38, 673–676.
and race differences in longitudinal predictors of the initiation of Lanier, C. A., & Green, B. A. (2006). Principal Component Analysis of the
adolescent dating violence perpetration. Journal of Aggression, College Date Rape Attitude Survey (CDRAS). Journal of Aggression,
Maltreatment, and Trauma, 19, 492–516. https://doi.org/10.1080/ Maltreatment, & Trauma, 13, 79–93.
10926771.2010.495032 Latane, B., & Darley, J. N. (1969). Bystander ‘apathy’. American Scientist,
Giordano, P. C. (2003). Relationships in adolescence. Annual Review of 57, 244–268. http://www.jstor.org/stable/27828530
Sociology, 29, 257–281. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.soc.29. Lehrer, J. A., Lehrer, E. L., & Koss, M. P. (2013). Sexual and dating violence
010202.100047 among adolescents and young adults in Chile: a review of findings
Gressard, L. A., Swahn, M. H., & Tharp, A. T. (2015). A first look at gender from a survey of university students. Culture, Health & Sexuality,
inequality as a societal risk factor for dating violence. American 15(1), 1–14.
Journal of Preventive Medicine, 49, 448–457. https://doi.org/10. Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical meta‐analyses. Thousand
1016/j.amepre.2015.05.017 Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.
Halpern, C. T., Spriggs, A. L., Martin, S. L., & Kupper, L. L. (2009). Patterns of McCauley, H. L., Tancredi, D. J., Silverman, J. G., Decker, M. R., Austin, S. B.,
intimate partner violence victimization from adolescence to young McCormick, M. C., … Miller, E. (2013). Gender‐equitable attitudes,
adulthood in a nationally representative sample. Journal of Adolescent bystander behavior, and recent abuse perpetration against heterosexual
Health, 45, 508–516. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.03.011 dating partners of male high school athletes. American Journal of Public
Harned, M. S. (2001). Abused women or abused men? An examination of Health, 103(10), 1882–1887. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2013.301443
the context and outcomes of dating violence. Violence and Victims, McMahon, S. (2010). Rape myth beliefs and bystander attitudes among
16, 269–285. incoming college students. Journal of American College Health, 59,
Hedges, L. V., & Hedberg, E. C. (2007). Intraclass correlation values for 3–11. https://doi.org/10.1080/07448481.2010.483715
planning group‐randomized trials in education. Educational Evaluation McMahon, S., & Banyard, V. L. (2012). When can I help? A conceptual
and Policy Analysis, 29, 60–87. framework for the prevention of sexual violence through bystander
Hedges, L. V., Tipton, E., & Johnson, M. C. (2010). Robust variance intervention. Trauma, Violence & Abuse, 13, 3–14. https://doi.org/10.
estimation in meta‐regression with dependent effect size estimates. 1177/1524838011426015
Research Synthesis Methods, 1, 39–65. https://doi.org/10.1002/jrsm. Messner, M. A. (2015). Bad men, good men, bystanders: Who is the rapist?
5 Gender & Society, https://doi.org/10.1177/0891243215608781. Ad-
Higgins, J. P. T., & Green, S. (Eds) (2011). Cochrane handbook for vance online publication.
systematic reviews of interventions Version 5.1.0 [updated March Moher, D., Shamseer, L., Clarke, M., Ghersi, D., Liberati, A., & Petticrew,
2011]. The Cochrane Collaboration. Available from www.cochrane‐ M., PRISMA‐P Group. (2015). Preferred reporting items for systema-
handbook.org tic review and meta‐analysis protocols (PRISMA‐P) 2015 statement.
Howard, D. E., Wang, M. Q., & Yan, F. (2007). Prevalence and psychosocial Systematic Reviews, 4(1), 1. https://doi.org/10.1186/2046‐4053‐4‐1
correlates of forced sexual intercourse among U.S. high school Molidor, C., & Tolman, R. M. (1998). Gender and contextual factors in
adolescents. Adolescence, 42, 629–643. adolescent dating violence. Violence Against Women, 4, 180–194.
Katz, J. [Jackson] (1995). Reconstructing masculinity in the locker room: https://doi.org/10.1177/1077801298004002004
The Mentors in Violence Prevention project. Harvard Educational Noonan, R. K., & Charles, D. (2009). Developing teen dating violence
Review, 65, 163–174. https://doi.org/10.17763/haer.65.2. prevention strategies: Formative research with middle school youth.
55533188520136u1 Violence Against Women, 15, 1087–1105. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Katz, J. [Jackson], Heisterkamp, H. A., & Fleming, W. M. (2011). The social 1077801209340761
justice roots of the Mentors in Violence prevention model and its Palmer, T., Peters, J., Sutton, A. J., & Moreno, S. (2008). Contour enhanced
application in a high school setting. Violence Against Women, 17, funnel plots for meta‐analysis. The Stata Journal, 8, 242–254. https://
684–702. https://doi.org/10.1177/1077801211409725 doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2007.11.010.
40 of 40 | KETTREY ET AL.

Payne, D. L., Lonsway, K. A., & Fitzgerald, L. F. (1999). Rape myth References to included studies
acceptance: Exploration of its structure and its measurement using
the Illinois Rape Myth Acceptance Scale. Journal of Research in Banyard, V. L., Plante, E. G., & Moynihan, M. M. (2005). Rape prevention
Personality, 33, 27–68. through bystander education: Final report to NIJ for grant 2002‐WG‐
Pengpid, S., & Peltzer, K. (2016). Intimate partner violence victimization BX‐009. Retrieved from https://www.ncjrs.gov/pdffiles1/nij/grants/
and associated factors among male and female university students in 208701.pdf
22 countries in Africa, Asia and the Americas. African Journal of Coker, A. (2013). Evaluation of Green Dot a primary prevention
Reproductive Health, 20(1), 29–39. intervention. Retrieved from ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier:
Sterne, J. A. C, Higgins, J. P. T., & Reeves, B. C. On behalf of the NCT01878097.
development group for ROBINS‐I. (2016, March). A tool for assessing Coker, A. L., Bush, H. M., Cook‐Craig, P. G., DeGue, S., Clear, E. R.,
Risk of Bias in Non‐randomized Studies of Interventions, Version 7. Brancato, C. J., … Davenport, M. (2017). Randomized controlled trial
Available from http://www.riskofbias.info (Accessed July 29) 2016. testing bystander effectiveness to reduce violence. American Journal
Storer, H. L., Casey, E., & Herrenkohl, T. (2015). Efficacy of bystander of Preventive Medicine, 52(5), 566–578.
programs to prevent dating abuse among youth and young adults: A Das, M., Ghosh, S., Miller, E., O’Connor, B., & Verma, R (2012). Engaging
review of the literature. Trauma, Violence, & Abuse, https://doi.org/ coaches and athletes in fostering gender equity: Findings from the
10.1177/1524838015584361. Advance online publication. Parivartan program in Mumbai, India (Unpublished manuscript). Inter-
Straus, M. A., Hamby, S. L., Boney‐McCoy, S., & Sugarman, D. B. (1996). national Center for Research on women (ICRW) & Futures Without
The Revised Conflict Tactics Scales (CTS2): Development and Violence, New Delhi, India.
preliminary psychometric data. Journal of Family Issues, 17, 283–316. Gidycz, C. A., Orchowski, L. M., Probst, D. R., Edwards, K. M., Murphy, M.,
Su, P. Y., Hao, J. H., Huang, Z. H., Xiao, L. M., & Tao, F. B. (2011). An & Tansill, E. (2015). Concurrent administration of sexual assault
investigation on the epidemic situation of intimate partner violence in prevention and risk reduction programming: Outcomes for women.
2,575 college students. Zhonghua Liuxingbingxue Zazhi, 32(4), 346–351. Violence Against Women, 21(6), 780–800. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Swim, J. K., Aikin, K. J., Hall, W. S., & Hunter, B. A. (1995). Sexism and 1077801215576579
racism: Old‐fashioned and modern prejudices. Journal of Personality Miller, E. (2011). A randomized controlled trial of an Adolescent
and Social Psychology, 68, 199–214. http://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10. Relationship Abuse Prevention Program entitled “coaching boys into
1037/0022‐3514.68.2.199 men”. ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT01367704.
Tanner‐Smith, E. E., & Polanin, J. R. (2015). A retrospective analysis of Miller, E., Tancredi, D. J., McCauley, H. J., Decker, M. R., Virata, M. C. D.,
dissemination biases in the brief alcohol intervention literature. Anderson, H. A., … Silverman, J. G. (2012). “Coaching boys into men”:
Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 29(1), 49–62. https://doi.org/10. A cluster randomized controlled trial of a dating violence prevention
1037/adb0000014 program. Journal of Adolescent Health, 51(5), 431–438. https://doi.
Tanner‐Smith, E. E., & Tipton, E. (2014). Robust variance estimation with org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.01.018
dependent effect sizes: Practical considerations including a software Moynihan, M. M., & Banyard, V. (2011). Educating bystanders helps
tutorial in Stata and SPSS. Research Synthesis Methods, 5, 13–30. prevent sexual violence and reduce backlash. Family & Intimate
https://doi.org/10.1002/jrsm.1091 Partner Violence Quarterly, 3, 293–304.
Tipton, E. (2013). Robust variance estimation in meta‐regression with Salazar, & L. A (2013). Web‐based Bystander Education program (RealCon-
binary dependent effects. Research Synthesis Methods, 4, 169–187. sent). ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT01903876.
https://doi.org/10.1002/jrsm.1070. Shaw, J., & Janulis, P. (2016). Re‐evaluating sexual violence prevention
Tomaszewska, P., & Krahé, B. (2018). Sexual aggression victimization and through bystander education: A latent growth curve approach.
perpetration among female and male university students in Poland. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 31(16), 2729–2750. https://doi.
Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 33(4), 571–594. org/10.1177/0886260515580365
Van Decraen, E., Michielsen, K., Herbots, S., Van Rossem, R., & Temmer-
man, M. (2012). Sexual coercion among in‐school adolescents in
SUPPO RTING IN F ORMATION
Rwanda: Prevalence and correlates of victimization and normative
acceptance. African Journal of Reproductive Health, 16(3), 139–154.
Additional supporting information may be found online in the
Vázquez, F. L., Torres, A., & Otero, P. (2012). Gender‐based violence and
mental disorders in female college students. Social Psychiatry and Supporting Information section.
Psychiatric Epidemiology, 47(10), 1657–1667.
Wade, L. (2017). American hookup: The new culture of sex on campus.
New York, NY, US: W W Norton & Co.
How to cite this article: Kettrey HH, Marx RA, Tanner‐Smith
Weisz, A. N., Tolman, R. M., Callahan, M. R., Saunders, D. G., & Black, B. M.
(2007). Informal helpers’ responses when adolescents tell them about
EE. Effects of bystander programs on the prevention of sexual
dating violence or romantic relationship problems. Journal of Adolescence, assault among adolescents and college students: A systematic
30, 853–868. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2006.09.004 review. Campbell Systematic Reviews. 2019;15:e1013.
Wilson, D. B. (2013). Practical meta‐analysis effect size calculator. https://doi.org/10.4073/csr.2019.1
Retrieved from. http://gemini.gmu.edu/cebcp/EffectSizeCalculator/
index.html

You might also like