You are on page 1of 3

A R T IC L E S

child appears to result in more fear- 3. Lautch, H. Dental phobia. Br J Psychiatry 12. Sackett, G.P.; Stephenson, E.; and Ruppen-
119:151-158, 1971. thel, G.C. Digital data acquisition system s for ob­
related behaviors.
4. Kleinknecht, R.A.; Klepac, R.K., and A lexan­ serving behavior in laboratory and field settings.
This study has begun to specify be­ der, L.D. O rigins and characteristics of fear of Behav Res M ethods and Instrum entation 5:344-
havioral interactions during routine dentistry. JADA 86:842-848, 1973. 348, 1979.
treatm ent of young children. A d d i­ 5. B ernstein, D.A.; K leinknecht, R.A.; and 13. Sackett, G.P., and others. A fortran program
tional studies of other populations of Alexander, L.D.Antecedents of dental fear.J Pub­ for lag sequential analysis of contingency and
lic Health Dent 39(2):113-124, 1979. cyclicity in behavioral interaction data. Behav
children, such as school-age children, 6. W einstein, P. Identifying patterns of behav­ Res M ethods and Instrum entation, 11:366-378,
are needed. Moreover, the m ethodol­ ior during treatm ent of children. In Ingersoll, B., 1979.
ogy used in this study has the potential and M cKutcheon, W. eds. Proceedings of the Sec­ 14. Howitt, J.W., and Strieker, G. Child patient
to assess the effects of not only behav­ ond N ational Conference on Behavioral Dentistry: responses to various dental procedures. JADA
Clinical Research in Behavioral Dentistry, Mor­ 70:70-74, 1965.
iors, but also pharm acological m an­
gantow n, WVa Univ of West Virginia Press, 1980. 15. Langer, E.J.; Janis, I.L.; and Wolfer, J.A.Re­
agem ent of patients. W hat is learned 7. Getz, T.; W einstein, P.; and Domoto, P. The duction of psychological stress in surgical pa­
from these studies may be of consider­ effect of structural variables on child behavior in tients. J Exper Soc Psychol 11:155-165, 1975.
able benefit to both providers and den­ the operatory. Pediatr Dent, to be published. 16. Johnson, J.E.Effects of accurate expectations
8. Domoto, P.K.; W einstein, P.; and Getz, T. A about sensations on the sensory and d istress
tal students. CT'aT',
pilot study using remote broadcasting equipm ent com ponents of pain. J Pers Soc Psychol 27:261-
to provide instruction in pedodontics.J Dent Educ 275, 1973.
4 3 (llptl):599-601, 1979. 17. Averell, J.R.Personal control over aversive
This study was funded by grant no. 1 RO 1 DE 9. W urster, C.A.; W einstein, P.; and Cohen, A.J. stim uli and its relationship to stress. Psychol Bull
04770-01 from the N ational Institute for Dental Com m unication patterns in pedodontics. Percept 80:286-303, 1973.
Research. Mot Skills 48(1):159-166, 1979. 18. Maccoby, E.E. Social developm ent. New
10. Glennon, B., and Weisz, J.R. An observa­ York, Harcourt, Brace, Jovanovich, Inc, 1980.
1. Forgione, A.G., and Clark, R.E. Com ments on tional approach to the assessm ent of anxiety in 19. Bernstein, L.; Bernstein, R.S.; and Dana,
an em pirical study of the cause of dental fears. J children. J Consult Clin Psychol 46:1247-1257, R.H. Interview ing: a guide for health profes­
Dent Res 53:496, 1974. 1978. sionals. New York, A ppleton-C entury-C rofts,
2. Shoben, E.M., and Borland, L. An em pirical 11. Sackett, G.P. M easurem ent in observational 1974.
study of the etiology of dental fears. J Clin Psychol research. In Sackett, G.P., ed. Observing behavior, 20. Rogers, C.R. On becoming a person. Boston,
10:17-174, 1954. vol II. Baltimore, Md, University Park Press, 1978. Houghton Mifflin, 1961.

Dentists’ responses to fear- and nonfear-related


behaviors in children
P. Weinstein, PhD
T. Getz, MS
P. Ratener, M S
P. Domoto, DDS, M PH

supposition that the process of provid­


D entists m a y respond differen tly to fear- an d nonfear-related ing care is at least as im portant as the
behaviors in children, an d those responses m ay influence treatm ent outcome.
the ch ildren’s further behavior. This study exam ines that Most research on the m anagem ent of
interaction. children has been based on the tacit
assum ption that the child is the prob­
lem and that it is his or her behavior
that m ust be altered. This assum ption
r has led to studies designed to assess
h ild re n ’s behavior in the opera­ experienced by the child was posi­ the effect of interventions to m odify
tory has alw ays been of concern in tively related to the degree of d isru p ­ the c h ild ’s behavior. Few studies have
dentistry. Children m ust cooperate or tive behavior displayed during a re­ hypothesized that m anagem ent prob­
m ust at least passively accept treat­ storative appointm ent. Moreover, re­ lems may have an iatrogenic com po­
m ent so that the dentist can com plete search2'4 suggests that traum atic den­ n e n t, and few have a tte m p te d to
technical procedures. M elam ed and tal experiences lead to the develop­ exam ine how the dentist’s behaviors
associates1 found that the level of fear m ent of fear, providing support for the influence treatm ent outcome.
38 ■ JADA, Vol. 104, January 1982
ARTICLES

that dentists w ould use reassurance,


ignoring I denying, or putdow ns in­
Dentists most frequently respond to the child’s creased significantly after the ch ild ’s
fear-related behavior with those behaviors that are fear-related behaviors; w hereas the
counterproductive and ineffective in reducing fear: pro b ab ility th a t the d en tist w ould
question for feeling decreased. Fol­
rules, coercion, coaxing, reassurances, putdowns. lowing the ch ild ’s nonfear-related be­
haviors, questioning for feelings in­
creased, w hile ignoring I denying de­
creased. Differences betw een the den­
A pilot study by Wurster and as­ data analysis and results are available tist’s responses to fear and nonfear be­
sociates5 examined communication from the authors. haviors are statistically significant for
patterns during pedodontic appoint­ all dentist behaviors except reassur­
ments in a dental school setting. Six­ ance. At lag 2, the probabilities for
Results
teen dental students were videotaped questioning for feelings, reassurance,
during a restorative appointment with The dentists’ responses in the guid­ ignoring I denying, and putdow ns in­
a child. Markov analysis techniques ance dim en sio n , im m ediately after crease following fear-related behav­
were u sed to ca lcu la te the prob­ fear-related behaviors by the ch ild iors by the child. Following nonfear-
abilities for the occurrence of specific (first lag), were less likely to be direc­ relate d beh av io rs, th e co n d itio n a l
behaviors by the child after certain be­ tion and reinforcement; whereas after probabilities for all dentist behaviors
haviors by the dentist. The prob­ nonfear-related behaviors, the dentist did not vary from the unconditional
abilities for the occurrence of specific was more likely to use direction, and probabilities. Differences betw een the
dentist behaviors after certain child reinforcement. The difference in the dentist’s responses to fear and nonfear
behaviors were also calculated so that d e n tis ts ’ re sp o n ses after fear- and behaviors at lag 2 are significant for
the pattern of behavioral interactions nonfear-related behaviors is statisti­ reassurance, ignoring I denying, and
would be clearer. Their results suggest cally significant. putdow ns.
that child behavior is systematically The probability that the dentist w ill In the first lag of the verbalization
related to the communication style of use rules, coercion, and coaxing was dim ension, im m ediately following the
the practitioner, and that the behavior g re a te r im m e d ia te ly after a c h ild child’s fear-related behaviors, the den­
of the dentist is influenced by the showed fear-related behaviors and de­ tist’s dental and nondental verbaliza­
child’s behavior. creased after nonfear-related behav­ tion to the assistant decreased signifi­
A more thorough understanding of iors. The difference in the probability cantly, w hile dental and nondental
dentist-child interactional patterns is that the dentist w ill use rules and coax­ verbalizations to the child increased.
necessary. In a field study of private ing is significant. Follow ing nonfear-related behaviors
p ra ctitio n ers, W e in ste in and a s­ In the second lag after fear-related by the child, the dentist’s silence and
sociates6 found that the incidence of behaviors, the probability for the den­ verbalization to the assistant increased
fear -and distress-related behaviors by tists’s use of reinforcement, rules, and while verbalization to the child de­
the child varied significantly after dif­ coercion was sim ilar to that of the first creased. The dentist’s responses after
ferent behaviors by the dentist. This lag. However, the only significant dif­ the c h ild ’s fear-related behaviors in
paper describes the probabilities for fe re n c e b e tw e e n th e d e n tis t’s re ­ the verb alizatio n d im en sio n differ
the occurrence of specific dentist be­ sponses after fear- and nonfear-related significantly from the verbalization re­
haviors after fear-related and nonfear- behaviors occurred for reinforcement; sponses after nonfear-related behav­
related child behaviors. again, the dentist was more likely to iors for the first lag. At lag 2, following
use reinforcem ent after nonfear behav­ child’s fear-related behaviors, the den­
Method iors th an after fear behaviors. The tist’s silence or nondental verbaliza­
probability that the dentist w ould use tion to the assistant decreased while
The m ethods used in this study are the explanations, a frequent dentist behav­ dental and nondental verbalizations to
same as those described in the preced­ ior, and rhetorical questions rem ained the child increased. The differences in
ing paper. unchanged after the c h ild ’s fear- and the dentists’s responses to the c h ild ’s
In this study, the conditional prob­ n o n fear-related beh av io rs for both fear- and nonfear-related behaviors are
abilities for specific dentist behaviors lags. significant for dentist silence and den­
in each of the four dimensions follow­ For the first lag in the em pathy di­ tal and nondental verbalization to the
ing fear- and nonfear-related child be­ mension, the conditional probability child.
haviors were compared. The signifi­
cance of the differences between den­
tist’s responses after the child’s fear
and nonfear behaviors was determined Providing explanations, use of rhetorical questions,
by transforming the data into 2 scores
and determining the probability that
and patting appear to be more a function of the
the differences were due to chance. dentist’s interpersonal style rather than a response to
Seventy-two videotapes have been the child’s behavior.
analyzed, representing two sequential
appointments for 36 children. Detailed

Weinstein-Others : DENTISTS’ RESPONSES TO FEAR- AND NONFEAR-RELATED BEHAVIORS IN CHILDREN ■ 39


AR TICLES

dentist behaviors in affecting child


fear-related behaviors, present mean­
Direction, reinforcement, and questioning for ingful patterns of dentist and child be­
feelings have the highest probabilities of reducing havior in private practice. It appears
that at least two problems exist. First,
fear-related behaviors by the child. practitioners may not have the appro­
priate child management skills in their
repertoire. In a recent survey of Wash­
In the physical contact dimension As would be expected, when the ington State private practitioners,
after fear-related behaviors, the condi­ c h ild b e g in s to sh o w fear- and Domoto and associates7 reported that
tional probability that the dentist w ill distress-related behaviors, the den­ only 48% of the sample received some
be in physical contact with the child tist’s verbal and physical interactions form of instruction in child manage­
increased for the first two lags. In the w ith th e c h ild in c r e a se d . C on­ ment when in dental school. Second,
first lag, working contact, holding, and sequently, dentist silence, noncontact, though the practitioners may have the
the use of restraint increased following and verbalization to the assistant de­ necessary skills, they may not use
fear-related behaviors. Assistance in creased. Following nonfear behaviors, th e se s k ills w h en they are m ost
p o sitio n in g decreased. F ollow in g verbalizations to the child decrease, needed—w hen the child responds
nonfear-related behaviors, the use of w hile verbalization to the assistant with fear-related behaviors. Instruc­
restraint decreased, while assistance and the amount of silence by the den­ tion, especially training w ith vid ­
increased. In lag 2, the probability of tist increase. eotape or remote broadcasting equip­
the use of restraint increased following ment,8 may be extremely useful in add­
fear-related behaviors. Conclusions ing new skills or altering how existing
skills are used. ft
Discussion In all, these data suggest that dentists
most frequently respond to the child’s
Dentists were less likely to use direc­ fear-related behavior with behaviors T his study was funded by grant no. 1 RO l DE
04770-01 from the National Institute for Dental
tion or reinforcement (guidance di­ that are counterproductive or ineffec­
Research.
mension) after the child’s fear-related tive in reducing fear: rules, coercion,
behavior. However, learning theory coaxing, reassurances, and putdowns. Dr. W einstein is associate professor and acting
suggests that ignoring or failing to Dentists less frequently respond with chairperson, Mr. Getz is lecturer, and Mr. Ratener
reinforce cooperation tends to lead to behaviors such as direction, rein­ is biostatistician, departm ent of com m unity den­
an increase in uncooperative behav­ forcement, and questioning for feel­ tistry, and Dr. Domoto is associate professor and
chairperson, departm ent of pedodontics, U niver­
iors. This is supported by the results of ings, which have the highest prob­
sity of W ashington, Seattle, 98195. Address re­
W einstein and associates that the abilities of reducing fear-related be­ print requests to Dr. Weinstein.
probabilities of child fear behaviors haviors by the child.
decreased after the dentist uses direc­ Some dentist’s behaviors did not 1. Melam ed, B.G., and others. Effects of film
tion and reinforcement.6 Dentists were vary under fear or nonfear conditions. m odeling on the reduction of anxiety-related be­
The behaviors that were invariant haviors in individuals varying in level or previ­
more likely to use rules, coercion, and
ous experience in the stress situation. J Consult
coaxing after child fear-related behav­ were providing explanations, use of Clin Psychol 46(6):1357-1367, 1978.
iors than after nonfear behaviors. rhetorical questions, and patting. 2. Lautch, H. Dental phobia. Br J Psychiatry,
These behaviors were found in the These appear to be more a function of 119:151-158, 1971.
Weinstein study to be ineffective in the dentist’s interpersonal style rather 3. Kleinknecht, R.A.; Klepac, R.K.; and Alexan­
der, L.D. Origins and characteristics, of fear of
reducing the child’s fear behavior. than a management strategy or a re­ dentistry. JADA 86:842-848, 1973.
The probability for use of question­ sp o n s e to th e c h ild ’s b e h a v io r . 4. M elam ed, B.G., and others. Reduction of
ing for feelings, a behavior subcate­ Analysis of the dentist’s responses fear-related dental m anagem ent problem s w ith
gory of the empathy dimension, de­ clearly illustrated distinct differences use of film ed m odeling. JADA 90(4):822-826,
creased immediately following fear- in interactional styles. Some dentists, 1975.
5. W urster, C.A.; W einstein, P.; and Cohen, A.J.
related behaviors by the child, even for exam ple, used em pathetic re­ Com m unication patterns in pedodontics. Percept
though W ein stein and asso cia tes sponses far more frequently than did Mot Skils 48(1], 159-166, 1979.
found that questioning for feelings is others. It may be that the range of a 6. W einstein, P., and others. The effect of den­
relatively effective in reducing fear- particular dentist’s style varies only tist variables on fear-related behaviors of young
children. J Dent Res 60(Special Issue A): 193,
related behaviors.6 Use of the other be­ slightly, irrespective of the behavior of 1981.
haviors in the empathy dimension in­ the child. Further investigations of 7. Domoto, P.; W einstein, P.; and Getz, T. A
creased following fear-related behav­ appointments with high and low fear pilot study using remote broadcasting equipm ent
ior, but it appears, as reported previ­ children treated by the same dentist to provide instruction in pedodontics. J Dent Ed
43(11 pt 1): 599-601, 1979.
ously, that these forms of attention to w ill be conducted to test this hypothe­
8. Domoto, P.; W einstein, P.; and Getz, T. Dif­
distress have little or no effect in re­ sis. ficult children: experiences of 145 W ashington
ducing the probabilities of fear behav­ These findings, in conjunction with State private practitioners. Pediatr Dent, to be
iors by the child. our study of the efficacy of specific published.

40 ■ JADA, Vol. 104, January 1982

You might also like