You are on page 1of 7

Body Image 22 (2017) 6–12

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Body Image
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/bodyimage

The importance of body image concerns in overweight and normal


weight individuals with binge eating disorder
Angelina Yiu, Susan M. Murray, Jean M. Arlt, Kalina T. Eneva, Eunice Y. Chen ∗
Temple Eating Disorders Program, Department of Psychology, Temple University, 1701 North 13th Street, Philadelphia, PA 19122, USA

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Body image concerns in binge eating disorder (BED) have been examined almost exclusively in overweight
Received 1 November 2016 individuals with BED. The current study extends past research by including overweight and normal weight
Received in revised form 11 April 2017 BED and non-BED groups to assess the multifactorial construct of body image using subscales of the
Accepted 27 April 2017
Eating Disorder Examination 16.0 (EDE-16.0) and a Body Comparison Task. Independent of weight status
and when controlling for age and race, women with BED are distinguished from those without BED by
Keywords:
significantly greater overvaluation of shape and weight on the EDE-16.0 and significantly reduced weight
Body image
satisfaction after a Body Comparison Task. Both BED diagnosis and weight status were independently
Body comparison
Binge Eating Disorder
associated with Weight Concern and Shape Concern subscales on the EDE-16.0. Taken together, these
data provide further support for the consideration of body image concerns in the diagnostic criteria for
BED.
© 2017 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction weight individuals with and without BED (Goldschmidt et al., 2010;
Grilo, Masheb, & White, 2010; Grilo et al., 2009; Grilo, White,
Binge eating disorder (BED) is found in 2–3.5% of individuals Gueorguieva, Wilson, & Masheb, 2013; Harrison, Mond, Rieger, Hay,
worldwide (Kessler et al., 2013). BED involves repeated objective & Rodgers, 2015). In BED, overvaluation of shape and weight is
overeating with a sense of loss of control. Likely due to the asso- strongly related to severity of eating disorder (ED) psychopathol-
ciation between regular binge eating and weight gain over time ogy and depressive symptomatology and is also predictive of poor
(Fairburn, Cooper, Doll, Norman, & O’Connor, 2000), individuals treatment response (Grilo, Ivezaj, & White, 2015; Grilo et al., 2013;
with BED tend to be overweight, making it challenging to deter- Hilbert, Tuschen-Caffier, & Vögele, 2002).
mine whether body image concerns are a feature of the psychology Overvaluation of shape and weight is one way to assess the
of BED or a function of overweight status. In the current Diagnos- multifactorial construct of body image concerns, and is related to,
tic and Statistical Manual for Mental Disorders-5 (DSM-5; American but distinct from, body dissatisfaction. Overvaluation of shape and
Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013), the diagnosis of BED does not weight refers to a stable influence of shape and weight on one’s
include a criterion regarding body image concerns (Ahrberg, Trojca, self-concept. In comparison, body dissatisfaction refers to negative
Nasrawi, & Vocks, 2011). This is in contrast to the diagnostic cri- attitudes, judgments and evaluations of one’s body that tend to vary
teria for both anorexia nervosa (AN) and bulimia nervosa (BN), based on mood or shape or weight changes (Cash, Counts, & Huffine,
which include a requirement for individuals to place excess value 1990; Mond & Hay, 2011). Body dissatisfaction affects up to 63% of
on their perceived shape and weight when considering their overall women (Frederick, Peplau, & Lever, 2006), and self-report studies
self-concept, i.e., overvaluation of shape and weight (Mond & Hay, show that overweight women exhibit elevated levels of body dis-
2011). satisfaction compared to normal weight women (Annis, Cash, &
Although not included in DSM-5 (APA, 2013) diagnostic crite- Hrabosky, 2004; Hill & Williams, 1998; Neighbors & Sobal, 2007;
rion, body image concerns are clearly relevant for individuals with Weinberger, Kersting, Riedel-Heller, & Luck-Sikorski, 2016). Indi-
BED. Studies using self-report and interview measures have shown viduals with BED may also experience greater body dissatisfaction
that overvaluation of shape and weight reliably distinguishes over- than individuals without BED (Eldredge & Stewart, 1996; Svaldi,
Caffier, Blechert, & Tuschen-Caffier, 2009), though findings are not
consistent (Fichter, Quadflieg, & Brandl, 1993).
In addition to self-report measures, body image concerns have
∗ Corresponding author. also been explored using body comparison paradigms. Compari-
E-mail address: Eunice.Chen@temple.edu (E.Y. Chen).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2017.04.005
1740-1445/© 2017 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
A. Yiu et al. / Body Image 22 (2017) 6–12 7

son to slender bodies results in greater self-defeating perceptions targeting BED participants. Recruitment yielded a total of 1445
of one’s body (Corning, Krumm, & Smitham, 2006) when compared telephone screens to determine preliminary eligibility. Of the 189
to images of average-size models, plus-size models, and inanimate potential participants invited to participate in the current study,
objects (Groesz, Levine, & Murnen, 2002), especially among women 32 participants were excluded due to meeting criteria for an eat-
with elevated ED symptoms. During a body exposure task, over- ing disorder other than BED or endorsement of binge eating that
weight women with BED expressed a comparable frequency of did not meet DSM-5 BED criteria (APA, 2013). Of the remaining
negative cognitions about their shape and weight as normal weight participants, 59 were not interested or did not respond.
women with BN and significantly more negative cognitions than The final sample consisted of 30 overweight women with BED,
normal weight non-BED women (Hilbert & Tuschen-Caffier, 2005), 28 overweight non-BED women, 21 normal weight women with
suggesting an effect of ED pathology independent of weight. BED, and 19 normal weight non-BED women. Previous research
To date, the examination of body image concerns in BED has suggests that the effect size for overvaluation of shape and weight,
occurred almost exclusively in overweight groups. To our knowl- shape concern, and weight concern are large for comparisons
edge, only one study has taken into account different weight between BED and non-BED groups (Grilo et al., 2008). Power anal-
categories among BED; results of this study indicated that a normal yses conducted using GPower (Faul, Erdfelder, Lang, & Buchner,
weight BED group had significantly lower weight concerns relative 2007) revealed that a total sample size of 68, i.e., 17 per group
to a very obese BED group (Dingemans & van Furth, 2012). Thus, the (power = .90 for f = .40, given ˛ = .05), was sufficient to detect a large
current study aims to disentangle the contributions of BED diagnos- effect size for each outcome measure.
tic status and weight status on overvaluation of shape and weight, The mean age of participants was 31.10 years and the sam-
shape and weight concerns more generally, and weight dissatisfac- ple was predominately non-Hispanic/Latino (96.9%). Nearly half of
tion following a Body Comparison Task in a sample of overweight the sample self-identified as Caucasian (n = 48; 49.0%), followed by
and normal weight BED and non-BED groups. African American (n = 39, 39.8%), Asian (n = 8; 8.2%) and Hispanic
As overvaluation of shape and weight distinguishes BED from (n = 3, 3.1%). See Table 1 for a summary of demographic information.
non-BED (Grilo et al., 2008), we hypothesized that (a) the BED
group would report significantly greater overvaluation of shape and 2.2. Procedure
weight relative to the non-BED group. We also hypothesized that
(b) overweight women with BED would report the greatest weight The present study was reviewed and approved by the univer-
and shape concerns, followed by (in order) normal weight women sity institutional review board and took place over two separate
with BED, overweight women without BED, and normal weight testing sessions as part of a larger study. The two testing ses-
women without BED. Finally, we hypothesized that (c) women in sions were conducted within four weeks of one another. After
BED groups would show greater decreases in weight satisfaction screening for eligibility and obtaining consent, participants com-
in response to the Body Comparison Task relative to women in the pleted an online battery of self-report questionnaires. In the first
non-BED groups. session, eating pathology, mood, and personality disorders were
assessed by doctoral-level clinicians using the measures described
2. Method below. Diagnoses were confirmed at a weekly best-estimate meet-
ing with a licensed clinical psychologist (Klein, Ouimette, Kelly,
2.1. Participants Ferro, & Riso, 1994; Kosten & Rounsaville, 1992). Height and weight
were measured to calculate BMI. During the second session, partic-
Female participants were recruited into one of four groups: ipants completed a 10-min Body Comparison Task administered by
overweight BED, overweight non-BED, normal weight BED and nor- doctoral-level clinicians.
mal weight non-BED. For the purposes of categorizing individuals
within the current study, body mass index (BMI) guidelines from 2.3. Measures
the World Health Organization (WHO, 1995) were used to clas-
sify participants as overweight (BMI ≥ 25.0) or normal weight (BMI 2.3.1. Demographic data survey. An abbreviated version of
between 18.5 and 24.9). It should be noted that BMI is not indicative the Demographic Data Scale (DDS; Linehan, 1982), a self-report
of levels of body fat (WHO, 1995) and is imperfect at categorizing questionnaire, was used to collect demographic information, such
diverse samples. However, information on BMI is easy to collect and as age and sex.
calculate and is an anthropometric measurement that is common
among large, population based survey datasets (e.g., the Behavioral 2.3.2. Psychiatric disorders. The Structured Clinical Interview
Risk Factor Surveillance System, WHO Mental Health Surveys). for the DSM IV-Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR) Axis-I disorders (SCID-I;
One hundred and eighty-nine BED and non-BED participants First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 2002) was conducted with all
were invited to participate in the current study from ongoing participants by a team of doctoral-level clinicians.
studies within the laboratory. General study recruitment involved
online advertisements and flyer advertisements in the commu- 2.3.3. Eating Disorder Examination-16.0. The EDE-16.0
nity. Advertisements for BED and non-BED were identical with the (Fairburn, Cooper, & O’Connor, 2008) is an interviewer based,
exception of the question “Do you binge eat?” on advertisements semi-structured interview that assesses behavioral features of

Table 1
Demographic information for overweight and normal weight women with and without BED.

Overweight BED Normal weight Overweight Normal weight


(n = 30) BED (n = 21) non-BED (n = 28) non-BED (n = 19)

Caucasian % 33.30% 66.70% 32.10% 78.90%


African American % 63.30% 9.50% 64.30% 0%
Asian % 0% 23.80% .00% 15.80%
Hispanic/Latino % 3.30% 0% 3.60% 5.30%
Age M (SD) 36.98 (11.36) 23.14 (3.37) 36.11 (12.54) 23.19 (7.39)
BMI M (SD) 33.99 (5.37) 22.67 (1.82) 30.28 (4.01) 21.51 (1.93)
8 A. Yiu et al. / Body Image 22 (2017) 6–12

EDs (e.g., frequency of binge eating episodes and compensatory dent variables.1 A repeated measures Time × BED × Weight Status
behaviors) as well as cognitions associated with EDs (e.g., have ANCOVA was conducted with weight satisfaction at baseline and
you criticized yourself as a person for being the shape or weight after the Body Comparison Task as dependent variables. If inter-
that you are?). The EDE-16.0 was used to diagnose BED, as defined actions were significant, simple effects analyses were conducted
by the DSM-5 (APA, 2013), as well as to examine the individual to probe interactions. To correct for multiple comparisons, a
subscales with reference to body image. In a study that examined Bonferroni-adjusted p-value of .012 was used to determine signifi-
the interrater and test-retest reliability of the EDE in a sample of cance. Partial 2 was used to report effect sizes for ANCOVAs, with
adult patients with BED, scores on the EDE-16.0 show both good the following cut-off conventions: small (.01), medium (.06) and
interrater and test-retest reliability for the EDE total score (Grilo, large (.14; Cohen, 1988), and confidence intervals were calculated
Masheb, Lozano-blanco, & Barry, 2004). In the current study, the based on Wuensch (2012). To explore whether the inclusion of age
EDE total score demonstrated an internal consistency of ˛ = .93. and race as covariates influenced the effect of BED and Weight Sta-
Shape and weight overvaluation, or the importance of shape tus on study variables, post-hoc analyses were conducted without
and weight on one’s self-evaluation, was assessed using an average the inclusion of covariates. Finally, exploratory, post-hoc Pearson
score of two questions that assess shape overvaluation and weight correlations were conducted to clarify associations between study
overvaluation from the Weight Concern and Shape Concern sub- variables.
scales of the EDE-16.0 (Fairburn et al., 2008), as in previous studies
(Goldschmidt et al., 2010; Grilo et al., 2008). In the current study, 3. Results
the correlation between the importance of shape and importance
of weight questions was r = .93. 3.1. Preliminary analyses
Most items on the Weight Concern and Shape Concern sub-
scales from the EDE-16.0 (Fairburn et al., 2008) were used to assess First, age and race were examined as potential covariates. A 2 × 2
body dissatisfaction. The Weight Concern subscale consists of five ANOVA indicated that overweight individuals were older than nor-
items that assess preoccupation with weight or shape, desire to lose mal weight individuals, F(1, 94) = 43.18, p < .001. A chi-square test
weight, overvaluation of weight, emotional reaction to prescribed indicated that there was a greater proportion of African Americans
weighing, and dissatisfaction with weight. The Shape Concern sub- in the overweight than normal weight groups, 2 (3, N = 98) = 39.87,
scale consists of eight items that assess a desire for a flat stomach, p < .001, relative to individuals who identified as Caucasian, Asian,
preoccupation with weight or shape, fear of weight gain, feelings or Hispanic. Therefore, age and race were controlled for in all sub-
of fatness, overvaluation of shape, dissatisfaction with shape, dis- sequent analyses.
comfort viewing body, and discomfort with exposing one’s body
to scrutiny. For the present study, the overvaluation of shape and 3.2. Clinical characteristics
weight items were excluded from the subscale scores, as these
items were analyzed separately to estimate the shape and weight Participants with BED reported an average of 2.82 (SD = 4.05)
overvaluation variable mentioned above (Grilo et al., 2008). In the episodes of binge eating per week for the preceding three months.
current study, the internal consistency of the Weight Concern sub- There were no significant differences in binge frequencies between
scale was ˛ = .76 and the internal consistency of the Shape Concern normal weight and overweight BED groups (p = .252). Of the par-
subscale was ˛ = .91. ticipants with BED, 23 had co-occurring psychiatric disorders: 10
with a lifetime history of mood disorders, five with current (past
2.4. Body Comparison Task month) mood disorders, two with a lifetime history of anxiety dis-
orders, nine with current anxiety disorders, and two with a lifetime
The Body Comparison Task was presented on a laptop using E- history of substance abuse. Participants within the non-BED com-
Prime 2.0 software (Psychology Software Tools, 2012). Immediately parison group reported no episodes of binge eating and no lifetime
prior to the Body Comparison Task, participants were asked to rate history of psychiatric disorders.
“How satisfied do you feel about your weight right now?” on a visual
analogue scale that ranged from Not at all satisfied (0) to Completely 3.3. Eating Disorder Examination
satisfied (460). Body comparison was induced using the presenta-
tion of 20 images of slim female bodies dressed in clothing that 3.3.1. Shape and weight overvaluation. On the composite
revealed their shape (Friederich et al., 2007). Previous research has measure of shape and weight overvaluation from the EDE-16.0
found these images to be associated with decreased weight satis- (Fairburn et al., 2008), a 2 × 2 ANCOVA revealed a significant main
faction as well as increased shape and weight concern in non-BED, effect of BED for overvaluation of shape and weight (see Table 2),
normal weight women (Friederich et al., 2007). While viewing each F(1, 88) = 25.18, p < .001, partial 2 = .22, 90% CI (.11, .34). Regardless
image, participants listened to a recording that instructed: “When of weight status, the BED groups exhibited greater overvaluation
you look at this picture, please compare your body to the body of of shape and weight relative to the non-BED groups. There was no
this model.” After viewing each image, participants were asked to main effect of weight status or interaction between BED and weight
rate weight satisfaction using the same scale as at baseline. Rat- status.
ings obtained after each of the 20 images were averaged to create
a composite score. 3.3.2. Weight Concern and Shape Concern subscales. On the
EDE-16.0 (Fairburn et al., 2008) Weight Concern and Shape Con-
2.5. Statistical treatment cern subscales, 2 × 2 ANCOVAs revealed significant main effects of
BED and weight status for Weight Concern (BED: F(1, 88) = 52.57,
Age and race were considered as covariates. For these analyses, p < .001, partial 2 = .37, 90% CI [.24, .48]; Weight Status: F(1,
race was dummy coded using four variables (African–American 1/0 88) = 12.46, p = .001, partial 2 = .12, 90% CI [.04, .23]) and Shape
[Yes/No], Asian 1/0, Hispanic 1/0, Caucasian 1/0) (Field, 2009).
For all analyses, independent variables were BED (BED or non-
BED) and Weight Status (overweight or normal weight). Three 1
Data were missing for one participant in the normal weight BED group for the
separate 2 × 2 ANCOVAs were conducted with overvaluation of Body Comparison Task and for one participant in the overweight BED group and one
shape and weight, weight concern, and shape concern as depen- participant in the overweight non-BED group for EDE-16 scores.
A. Yiu et al. / Body Image 22 (2017) 6–12 9

Table 2
Main effects of binge eating disorder (BED) diagnosis and weight status on shape concern, weight concern, and shape and weight overvaluation.

Measure Main effect of BED

BED (n = 50) non-BED (n = 46) F Partial 2 90% CI


M (SD) M (SD)

Shape concern 3.42 (1.69) .74 (1.13) 87.93** .50 [.38, .59]
Weight concern 2.88 (1.50) .95 (1.14) 52.57** .37 [.24, .48]
Shape and weight overvaluation 3.04 (1.81) 1.05 (2.03) 25.18** .22 [.11, .34]

Measure Main effect of weight status

Overweight (n = 56) Normal weight (n = 40) F Partial 2 90% CI


M (SD) M (SD)

Shape concern 2.61 (2.00) 1.48 (1.75) 13.54** .13 [.04, .24]
Weight concern 2.33 (1.66) 1.43 (1.50) 12.46** .12 [.04, .23]
Shape and weight overvaluation 2.10 (2.08) 2.07 (2.29) .74 .01 [.00, .07]

**
p < .01.

Concern (BED: F(1, 88) = 87.93, p < .001, partial 2 = .50, 90% CI [.38, shape concern, and overvaluation of shape and weight in a positive
.59]; Weight Status: F(1, 88) = 13.54, p < .001, partial 2 = .13, 90% direction (ps < .001–.008). When examining correlations between
CI [.04, .24]; see Table 2). Specifically, the BED groups were associ- measures among the non-BED groups, overvaluation of shape and
ated with greater weight concern and shape concern, regardless of weight, weight concern, and shape concern were significantly asso-
weight status, relative to the non-BED groups. Furthermore, being ciated with each other in a positive direction (ps < .001–.003), but
overweight was associated with greater weight concern and shape not with decreased weight satisfaction during the Body Comparison
concern, regardless of BED status, relative to normal weight. The Task (ps = .110–.691).
interaction of BED and Weight Status was not significant for weight
concern or shape concern.
4. Discussion

3.3.3. Body Comparison Task. At baseline for the Body Com-


The finding that overvaluation of shape and weight was signifi-
parison Task, a 2 × 2 ANCOVA revealed significant main effects of
cantly greater among participants with BED relative to participants
BED, F(1, 90) = 17.49, p < .001, partial 2 = .16, 90% CI (.06, .27), and
without BED replicates past research in overweight women with
weight status ( F(1, 90)= 7.99, p = .006, partial 2 = .08, 90% CI [.01,
BED (Goldschmidt et al., 2010; Grilo et al., 2008, 2009, 2010;
.18]) for weight satisfaction. Specifically, the BED groups had lower
Harrison et al., 2015) and extends this finding to normal weight
baseline weight satisfaction, regardless of weight status, relative
women with BED. The inclusion of normal weight women with BED
to the non-BED groups. Furthermore, being overweight was asso-
in the present study provides further evidence of the specificity of
ciated with lower baseline weight satisfaction, regardless of BED
overvaluation of shape and weight to BED, and EDs more generally,
diagnosis, relative to normal weight. The interaction of BED and
rather than weight status (Goldschmidt et al., 2010; Grilo et al.,
weight status was not significant for baseline weight satisfaction.
2009, 2010, 2013). Research suggests that overvaluation of shape
In response to the Body Comparison Task, a Time × BED ×
and weight is a part of the core psychopathology of AN and BN
Weight Status repeated measures ANCOVA revealed a significant
(Cooper & Fairburn, 1993), and our results suggest that it may also
Time × BED interaction, F(1, 89) = 9.19, p = .003, partial 2 = .09, 90%
be applicable to BED.
CI (.02, .20). Specifically, women with BED, regardless of weight
Consistent with our hypotheses, decreases in weight satisfac-
status, exhibited decreased weight satisfaction from baseline to
tion in response to a Body Comparison Task and overvaluation of
after the Body Comparison Task relative to the non-BED groups.
shape and weight were greatest among women with BED relative
The interactions for Time × Weight Status, BED × Weight Status,
to women without BED, regardless of weight status. Effect sizes
and Time × BED × Weight Status were not significant for the Body
for the Body Comparison Task and interview measures were large,
Comparison Task.
even after correction for multiple comparisons. The finding that
body comparison led to significantly decreased weight satisfaction
3.4. Post-hoc analyses in participants with BED, but not participants without BED, is con-
sistent with past research in women with BN (Blechert, Nickert,
Analyses were re-run without age and race as covariates to Caffier, & Tuschen-Caffier, 2009) and AN (Friederich et al., 2010).
determine whether the inclusion of these covariates influenced the Collectively, the findings of greater overvaluation of shape and
effect of BED and weight status on the study variables. The pattern weight and decreased weight satisfaction as a result of body com-
of findings remained unchanged when age and race were excluded parison among those with BED suggests a unique contribution of
as covariates. BED diagnostic status, rather than overweight status, to body image
See Table 3 for means and standard deviations of study variables concerns (Blechert et al., 2009; Corning et al., 2006). Further sup-
for the overweight BED, normal weight BED, overweight non-BED, port for this idea comes from the finding that decreased weight
and normal weight non-BED groups. As the BED groups reported satisfaction after the Body Comparison Task was significantly cor-
greater overvaluation of shape and weight and decreased weight related with weight concern, shape concern, and shape and weight
satisfaction in response to the Body Comparison Task regardless overvaluation in participants with BED, but not in participants
of weight group, post-hoc correlations between study measures without BED. The specificity of BED diagnostic status on overval-
were conducted separately for the BED and non-BED groups (see uation of shape and weight is consistent with the transdiagnostic
Table 4). Among those with BED, all study measures were cor- cognitive behavioral theory of EDs (Fairburn, Cooper, & Shafran,
related, such that decreased weight satisfaction during the Body 2003), which suggests that binge eating may be caused and main-
Comparison Task was significantly associated with weight concern, tained by overvaluation of shape and weight. Past research suggests
10 A. Yiu et al. / Body Image 22 (2017) 6–12

Table 3
Descriptive statistics on study measures for overweight and normal weight women with and without BED.

Measure Overweight BED (n = 29) Normal weight BED (n = 21) Overweight non-BED (n = 27) Normal weight non-BED (n = 19)
M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)

Eating Disorder Examination


Shape and weight overvaluation 2.90 (1.84) 3.25 (1.78) 1.25 (2.00) 0.76 (2.10)
Weight concern 3.95 (1.55) 2.68 (1.63) 1.16 (1.30) .14 (.32)
Shape concern 3.25 (1.39) 2.38 (1.53) 1.34 (1.35) 0.38 (0.27)

Body Comparison Task


Weight satisfaction at baseline 177.00 (86.35) 203.33 (74.05) 220.71 (87.72) 299.47 (81.75)
Weight satisfaction at body comparison 94.07 (91.44) 159.24 (95.71) 223.76 (125.19) 308.80 (93.09)

Table 4
Correlations between overvaluation of shape and weight, weight concern, shape concern and change in weight satisfaction for women with BED without BED.

Binge Eating Disorder non-Binge Eating Disorder

Shape and Weight Shape Change in weight Shape and Weight concern Shape concern Change in weight
weight concern concern satisfaction weight satisfaction
overvaluation overvaluation

Shape and weight overvaluation 1 .41** .58** −.46** 1 .59** .43** .24
Weight concern – 1 .85** −.41** – 1 .88** .06
Shape concern – – 1 −.38** – – 1 −.07
Change in weight satisfaction – – – 1 – – – 1
**
p < .01.

that overvaluation of shape and weight is predictive of lower rates groups, relative to the non-BED groups. As we did not include a
of remission from binge eating at post-treatment and higher fre- psychiatric comparison group, we cannot conclude that the find-
quency of binge eating at 12-month follow-up (Grilo et al., 2013). ings are above and beyond the effects of a psychiatric condition.
Currently, there is limited research that exclusively assesses the However, research suggests that the rate of lifetime psychiatric co-
effectiveness of body image interventions in BED (Hilbert et al., morbidities in the current sample of BED is consistent with that
2002; Hilbert & Tuschen-Caffier, 2004), although body image inter- found in a large sample of those with BED. For example, Kessler
ventions are typically included as part of cognitive behavioral et al. (2013) found that of individuals with a lifetime history of
treatment of EDs (Fairburn, 2008). Findings from the current study BED, 46.1% also had a lifetime history of mood disorders and 56.1%
suggest that, regardless of weight status, women with BED may had a lifetime history of anxiety disorders. As this is the first study
benefit from treatment that specifically addresses overvaluation of to address body image concerns using four groups of overweight
shape and weight. Future studies that assess the outcome of treat- and normal weight individuals with and without BED, future stud-
ments that incorporate specific interventions for overvaluation of ies could examine body image concerns using a BED sample and a
shape and weight may yield important advances in the treatment non-BED psychiatric comparison group. Finally, the assessment of
of BED. overvaluation of shape and weight was conducted with only two
The current findings also provide insight into the relationship questions, making it difficult to assess the validity of this measure.
between BED and weight status on more general measures of However, the finding of a strong relationship between these ques-
body dissatisfaction. Unlike overvaluation of shape and weight and tions and the Body Comparison Task adds to the validity of this
changes in weight satisfaction due to body comparison, which measure. Although the Body Comparison Task used in this study
appear specific to BED, BED and weight status independently did not include a control condition (e.g., average size models or
accounted for more global aspects of body dissatisfaction (i.e., inanimate objects), past research suggests that body comparison
weight and shape concern; Cash et al., 1990; Frederick et al., 2006; with slender bodies increases body dissatisfaction, as opposed to
Mond & Hay, 2011). Women who are overweight with BED experi- inanimate objects, average size models, or plus-size models (Groesz
enced the greatest weight and shape concerns, while normal weight et al., 2002; Halliwell & Dittmar, 2004).
women without BED experienced the lowest weight and shape Strengths of the current study include the use of both over-
concerns, with normal weight women with BED and overweight weight and normal weight women with BED to extend the current
women without BED falling in between these two extremes. Thus, literature on body dissatisfaction and overvaluation of shape and
there appears to be an additive contribution with both BED and weight. The current study assessed body image concerns using
overweight status conferring the greatest risk for weight and shape multiple measures: an interview measure of ED psychopathology
concerns, whereas the presence of either BED or overweight status (Fairburn et al., 2008) and a Body Comparison Task that has been
each confers similar risk. found to induce body dissatisfaction (Friederich et al., 2007, 2010).
The findings of the current study were limited by the use of Further examination of the utility of the Body Comparison Task
a female-only sample with age and race differences between the to distinguish EDs from weight disorders and to examine body
overweight and normal weight groups, a lack of a psychiatric com- image concerns across EDs is needed. Future studies may incor-
parison group, and the measures used. Older age is associated with porate other aspects of body image concerns, such as perceptual
weight gain, and the age difference observed between the over- distortions and shape dissatisfaction.
weight and normal weight groups may reflect this trend (Flegal, The present study found a specific relationship between BED
Carroll, Kit, & Ogden, 2012). As the current pattern of findings diagnostic status with overvaluation of shape and weight and
remained the same with and without age and race as covariates, decreased weight satisfaction in response to a Body Comparison
it is unlikely that these covariates attenuated the effect of weight Task, regardless of weight status. Furthermore, the current find-
status on the primary outcomes assessed. Furthermore, psychi- ings show that both BED diagnostic status and overweight status,
atric comorbidity in the present study was higher among the BED independent of one another, conferred risk for more general body
A. Yiu et al. / Body Image 22 (2017) 6–12 11

dissatisfaction. The current data add to our knowledge of the rela- Flegal, K. M., Carroll, M. D., Kit, B. K., & Ogden, C. L. (2012). Prevalence of obesity
tionship between BED diagnosis and weight on various dimensions and trends in the distribution of body mass index among US adults, 1999-2010.
JAMA, 307, 491–497. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.2012.39
of body image concern and provide further support for the rec- Frederick, D. A., Peplau, L. A., & Lever, J. (2006). The swimsuit issue: Correlates of
ommendation (Grilo et al., 2008) that overvaluation of shape and body image in a sample of 52,677 heterosexual adults. Body Image, 3, 413–419.
weight be included as a diagnostic specifier of BED. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2006.08.002
Friederich, H. C., Brooks, S., Uher, R., Campbell, I. C., Giampietro, V., Brammer,
M., . . . & Treasure, J. (2010). Neural correlates of body dissatisfaction in
anorexia nervosa. Neuropsychologia, 48, 2878–2885. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
Funding j.neuropsychologia.2010.04.036
Friederich, H. C., Uher, R., Brooks, S., Giampietro, V., Brammer, M., Williams, S. C.
R21MH093932-01A1 from NIMH and NIDDK to Eunice Chen, R., . . . & Campbell, I. C. (2007). I’m not as slim as that girl: Neural bases of body
shape self-comparison to media images. Neuroimage, 37, 674–681. http://dx.doi.
Ph.D. org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.05.039
Goldschmidt, A. B., Hilbert, A., Manwaring, J. L., Wilfley, D. E., Pike, K. M., Fairburn, C.
G., & Striegel-Moore, R. H. (2010). The significance of overvaluation of shape and
Acknowledgements weight in binge eating disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48, 187–193.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2009.10.008
Grilo, C. M., Crosby, R. D., Masheb, R. M., White, M. A., Peterson, C. B., Wonderlich,
The authors thank Hans-Christoph Friederich, M.D., Rudolf Uher,
S. A., . . . & Mitchell, J. E. (2009). Overvaluation of shape and weight in binge
Ph.D., Samantha Brooks, Ph.D., Vincent Giampietro, Ph.D., Michael eating disorder, bulimia nervosa, and sub-threshold bulimia nervosa. Behaviour
Brammer, Ph.D., Steve C.R. Williams, Ph.D., Wolfgang Herzog, Ph.D., Research and Therapy, 47, 692–696. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2009.05.001
M.D., Janet Treasure, M.D., Ph.D., and Iain C. Campbell, Ph.D. for Grilo, C. M., Hrabosky, J. I., White, M. A., Allison, K. C., Stunkard, A. J., & Masheb,
R. M. (2008). Overvaluation of shape and weight in binge eating disorder and
supplying the materials for the Body Comparison Task. overweight controls: Refinement of a diagnostic construct. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 117, 414–419. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.117.2.414
Grilo, C. M., Ivezaj, V., & White, M. A. (2015). Evaluation of the DSM-5 severity indi-
References cator for binge eating disorder in a community sample. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 66, 72–76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2015.01.004
Ahrberg, M., Trojca, D., Nasrawi, N., & Vocks, S. (2011). Body image disturbance in Grilo, C. M., Masheb, R. M., Lozano-blanco, C., & Barry, D. T. (2004). Reliability of the
binge eating disorder: A review. European Eating Disorders Review, 19, 375–381. Eating Disorder Examination in patients with binge eating disorder. International
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/erv.1100 Journal of Eating Disorders, 35, 80–85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/eat.10238
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental Grilo, C., Masheb, R., & White, M. (2010). Significance of overvaluation of
disorders (DSM-5). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association. http://dx. shape/weight in binge-eating disorder: Comparative study with overweight and
doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596 bulimia nervosa. Obesity, 18, 499–504. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/oby.2009.280
Annis, N. M., Cash, T. F., & Hrabosky, J. I. (2004). Body image and psychosocial Grilo, C. M., White, M. A., Gueorguieva, R., Wilson, G. T., & Masheb, R. M. (2013).
differences among stable average weight, currently overweight, and formerly Predictive significance of the overvaluation of shape/weight in obese patients
overweight women: The role of stigmatizing experiences. Body Image, 1, with binge eating disorder: Findings from a randomized controlled trial with
155–167. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2003.12.001 12-month follow-up. Psychological Medicine, 43, 1335–1344. http://dx.doi.org/
Blechert, J., Nickert, T., Caffier, D., & Tuschen-Caffier, B. (2009). Social comparison 10.1017/s0033291712002097
and its relation to body dissatisfaction in bulimia nervosa: Evidence from eye Groesz, L. M., Levine, M. P., & Murnen, S. K. (2002). The effect of experimental pre-
movements. Psychosomatic Medicine, 71, 907–912. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/ sentation of thin media images on body satisfaction: A meta-analytic review.
PSY.0b013e3181b4434d International Journal of Eating Disorders, 31, 1–16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/eat.
Cash, T. F., Counts, B., & Huffine, C. E. (1990). Current and vestigial effects of over- 10005
weight among women: Fear of fat, attitudinal body image, and eating behaviors. Halliwell, E., & Dittmar, H. (2004). Does size matter? The impact of model’s body
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 12, 157–167. http://dx.doi. size on women’s body-focused anxiety and advertising effectiveness. Journal of
org/10.1007/bf00960765 Social and Clinical Psychology, 23, 104–122. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.23.1.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: 104.26989
Lawrence Earlbaum Associates. http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9780203771587 Harrison, C., Mond, J., Rieger, E., Hay, P., & Rodgers, B. (2015). Correlates of binge
Cooper, P. J., & Fairburn, C. G. (1993). Confusion over the core psy- eating with and without overvaluation of weight or shape: Further evidence
chopathology of bulimia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, from a general population sample of women. Advances in Eating Disorders: The-
13, 385–389. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/1098-108x(199305)13:4%3C385:aid- ory, Research and Practice, 3, 20–33. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/21662630.2014.
eat2260130406%3E3.0.co;2-w 948468
Corning, A. F., Krumm, A. J., & Smitham, L. A. (2006). Differential social comparison Hilbert, A., & Tuschen-Caffier, B. (2004). Body image interventions in
processes in women with and without eating disorder symptoms. Journal of cognitive–behavioural therapy of binge-eating disorder: A component
Counseling Psychology, 53, 338–349. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.53.3. analysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 1325–1339. http://dx.doi.org/10.
338 1016/j.brat.2003.09.001
Dingemans, A. E., & van Furth, E. F. (2012). Binge eating disorder psychopathology Hilbert, A., & Tuschen-Caffier, B. (2005). Body-related cognitions in binge eating
in normal weight and obese individuals. International Journal of Eating Disorders, disorder and bulimia nervosa. Journal of Social & Clinical Psychology, 24, 561–579.
45, 135–138. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/eat.20905 http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2005.24.4.561
Eldredge, K. L., & Stewart, A. W. (1996). Weight and shape overconcern and emo- Hilbert, A., Tuschen-Caffier, B., & Vögele, C. (2002). Effects of prolonged and repeated
tional eating in binge eating disorder. International Journal of Eating Disorders, body image exposure in binge-eating disorder. Journal of Psychosomatic Research,
19, 73–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(sici)1098-108x(199601)19:1%3C73:aid- 52, 13–144. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0022-3999(01)00314-2
eat9%3E3.3.co;2-x Hill, A. J., & Williams, J. (1998). Psychological health in a non-clinical sample of obese
Fairburn, C. G. (2008). Cognitive behavior therapy and eating disorders. New York: women. International Journal of Obesity and Related Metabolic Disorders: Journal
Guilford Press. of the International Association for the Study of Obesity, 22, 578–583. http://dx.
Fairburn, C. G., Cooper, Z., Doll, H. A., Norman, P., & O’Connor, M. (2000). The natural doi.org/10.1038/sj.ijo.0800631
course of bulimia nervosa and binge eating disorder in young women. Archives Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P. A., Chiu, W. T., Deitz, A. C., Hudson, J. I., Shahly, V., . . . &
of General Psychiatry, 57, 659–665. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.57.7.659 Xavier, M. (2013). The prevalence and correlates of binge eating disorder in the
Fairburn, C. G., Cooper, Z., & O’Connor, M. (2008). Eating Disorder Examination (Edi- World Health Organization world mental health surveys. Biological Psychiatry,
tion 16.0D). In C. G. Fairburn (Ed.), Cognitive behavior therapy and eating disorders 73, 904–914. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.11.020
(pp. 309–314). New York: Guilford Press. Klein, D. N., Ouimette, P. C., Kelly, H. S., Ferro, T., & Riso, L. P. (1994). Test-retest
Fairburn, C. G., Cooper, Z., & Shafran, R. (2003). Cognitive behaviour therapy for reliability of team consensus best-estimate diagnoses of Axis I and II disorders
eating disorders: A “transdiagnostic” theory and treatment. Behaviour Research in a family study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 1043–1047. http://dx.doi.
and Therapy, 41, 509–528. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0005-7967(02)00088-8 org/10.1176/ajp.151.7.1043
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). GPower 3: A flexible statisti- Kosten, T. A., & Rounsaville, B. J. (1992). Sensitivity of psychiatric diagnosis based
cal power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. on the best estimate procedure. American Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 1225–1227.
Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03193146 http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/ajp.149.9.1225
Fichter, M. M., Quadflieg, N., & Brandl, B. (1993). Recurrent overeating: An empirical Linehan, M. (1982). Demographic data survey (DDS). Seattle, WA: University of Wash-
comparison of binge eating disorder, bulimia nervosa, and obesity. Interna- ington.
tional Journal of Eating Disorders, 14, 1–16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/1098- Mond, J. M., & Hay, P. J. (2011). Dissatisfaction versus over-evaluation in a gen-
108x(199307)14:1%3C1::aid-eat2260140102%3E3.0.co;2-3 eral population sample of women. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 44,
Field, A. (2009). Discovering statistics using SPSS. London: Sage Publications. 721–726. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/eat.20878
First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W. (2002). Structured clinical Neighbors, L. A., & Sobal, J. (2007). Prevalence and magnitude of body weight and
interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I disorders, research version, patient edition. New shape dissatisfaction among university students. Eating Behaviors, 8, 429–439.
York: Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2007.03.003
12 A. Yiu et al. / Body Image 22 (2017) 6–12

Psychology Software Tools, Inc. (2012). E-Prime 2.0].. Retrieved from. http://www. World Health Organization. (1995). Physical status: The use and interpretation of
pstnet.com anthropometry. Report of a WHO Expert Committee. World Health Organization
Svaldi, J., Caffier, D., Blechert, J., & Tuschen-Caffier, B. (2009). Body-related film clip Technical Report Series, 854, 1–452.
triggers desire to binge in women with binge eating disorder. Behaviour Research Wuensch, K. (2012). Construct confidence intervals.. Retrieved from. http://core.ecu.
and Therapy, 47, 790–796. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2009.06.005 edu/psyc/wuenschk/SPSS/CI-R2-SPSS.zip
Weinberger, N.-A., Kersting, A., Riedel-Heller, S. G., & Luck-Sikorski, C. (2016). Body
dissatisfaction in individuals with obesity compared to normal-weight individ-
uals: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Obesity Facts, 9, 424–441. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1159/000454837

You might also like