You are on page 1of 53

LABELING EFFECTS OF FIRST JUVENILE ARRESTS:

SECONDARY DEVIANCE AND SECONDARY SANCTIONING*

Akiva M. Liberman

Justice Policy Center, Urban Institute

David S. Kirk

University of Texas at Austin

KiDeuk Kim

Justice Policy Center, Urban Institute

Feb 2014 Manuscript. In press, Criminology

*
This research was funded by the grant 2010-MU-FX-0613 from the Office of Juvenile Justice
and Delinquency Prevention, Office of Justice Programs, U.S. Department of Justice. The
opinions, findings, and conclusions expressed are those of the authors alone and do not
necessarily represent the official positions of the U.S. Department of Justice. We are grateful to
the Project on Human Development in Chicago Neighborhoods for providing the data necessary
to undertake this study.
First Juvenile Arrests 1

Labeling Effects of First Juvenile Arrests:

Secondary Deviance and Secondary Sanctioning

ABSTRACT

A growing literature suggests that juvenile arrests perpetuate offending and increase the

likelihood of future arrests. The effect on subsequent arrests is generally regarded to be a product

of the perpetuation of criminal offending. However, increased rearrest may also reflect

differential law enforcement behavior. Using longitudinal data from the Project on Human

Development in Chicago Neighborhoods (PHDCN) together with official arrest records, the

current study estimates the effects of first arrests on both reoffending and rearrest. Propensity

score methods were used to control differences between arrestees and non-arrestees and

minimize selection bias. Among 1,249 PHDCN youth, 58 were first arrested during the study

period; 43 of these arrestees were successfully matched to 126 control cases who were equivalent

on a broad set of individual, family, peer, and neighborhood factors. We find that first arrests

increased both the likelihood of subsequent offending and of subsequent arrest, through separate

processes. The effects on rearrest are substantially larger and largely independent of the effects

on reoffending, suggesting that labels trigger "secondary sanctioning" processes distinct from

secondary deviance processes. Attempts to ameliorate deleterious labeling effects should include

efforts to dampen their escalating punitive effects on societal responses.


First Juvenile Arrests 2

INTRODUCTION

The 1980s and the early 1990s were characterized by an “epidemic” of youth violence in

the United States, which peaked in 1993-1994 (Cook and Laub, 2002). Policy responses to the

epidemic included a shift from the traditional rehabilitative goal of juvenile justice toward more

retributive goals (e.g., Allen, 2000), under the mantra of “old enough to do the crime, old enough

to do the time.” The jurisdiction of the juvenile court was curtailed through lowered age of

criminal responsibility, legislative exclusion of various age-charge combinations from juvenile

court jurisdiction, and increased prosecutorial discretion to “direct file” cases in adult court (see

Fagan and Zimring, 2000). The wisdom of retaining a separate juvenile court system was also

debated (e.g., Ainsworth, 1995; Bishop, 2004; Butts and Mitchell, 2000; Dawson, 1990; Feld,

1998).

States have begun stepping away from the punitive philosophy of late 20th century

juvenile justice (National Conference of State Legislatures, 2012; U. S. Department of

Education, 2014), following the decline in youth crime and violence over the past two decades.

Juvenile violent offending rates are now at historic lows, with the latest arrest data from the U.S.

Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention (2012) down 55 percent from its mid-

1990s peak. However, the decline in violent crime among juveniles outpaced the decline in

arrests of violent juveniles through at least the early 2000s, so that the ratio of juvenile violent

crime arrests to violent victimizations by juveniles increased from about 0.72 in 1980, to about

1.0 in the early 1990s, and to about 1.45 by 2003 (Snyder and Sickmund, 2006, p. 64). Despite

massive declines in juvenile crime and violence over the past two decades, a convincing case can

be made that U.S. society is still very much “governed through crime,” with a youth control

complex that criminalizes juveniles at an extraordinary level (Rios, 2011; Simon, 2007).
First Juvenile Arrests 3

The current study examines the collateral consequences of this criminalization of youth,

and revisits a question that has captivated and challenged criminologists for some time: What is

the effect of arresting juveniles? Two theoretical perspectives provide opposing answers to this

question. Deterrence theory predicts that arrests will have the specific deterrent effect of reduced

offending (e.g., Smith and Gartin, 1989), while labeling perspectives predict that arrests will lead

to increased offending and criminal sanctioning (e.g., Lemert, 1951). A third perspective,

Gottfredson and Hirschi’s (1990) self-control theory, argues that a lack of self-control explains

any apparent relationship between system responses such as arrest and subsequent behavior, and

that the relationship between juvenile arrest and reoffending is spurious. To date the empirical

literature has revealed little support for specific-deterrence. The literature is largely split between

null findings, in accord with self-control theory, and findings that seem to show that arresting

juveniles is associated with more subsequent offending, in accord with labeling theory.

In view of the literature to date, a preliminary aim of this study is to test the replicability

of the labeling effects previously reported. Through the use of propensity score methods

combined with the necessary sensitivity analyses, this study aims to minimize the selection-bias

threats to validity that are common in non-experimental studies. The study draws upon the broad

data on youth, family, peer, and neighborhood characteristics collected as part of the Project on

Human Development in Chicago Neighborhoods (PHDCN). The comprehensiveness of the

PHDCN allows us to account for many confounding influences that distinguish arrestees from

non-arrestees in estimating the relationship between juvenile arrest and future offending.

The primary aim of this paper is to then distinguish between two types of potential

labeling effects: the effects of labels on delinquent behavior versus the effects of labeling on

societal responses to the label, particularly via future sanctioning. This broadens our exploration
First Juvenile Arrests 4

of the effects of labels on not just deviant behavior but also effects on societal response to

misbehavior.

Per these two aims, we ask the following research questions: Does the first arrest of a

juvenile increase the likelihood of future offending? Does it increase the likelihood of

subsequent arrest? Does juvenile arrest increase the likelihood of subsequent arrest even after

accounting for any increases in offending? Put differently, does a first juvenile arrest lead to

subsequent arrests even if the arrestee does not engage in more subsequent offending than a

similar non-arrestee?

LABELING EFFECTS ON DELINQUENT BEHAVIOR AND ON SYSTEM RESPONSE

Labeling theory generally predicts that an “official” response to delinquency promotes

future delinquency (e.g., Lemert, 1951). Labeling theory includes two different mechanisms by

which a “label” can lead to increased deviancy (Paternoster and Iovanni, 1980). In one strand of

labeling theory, the primary mechanism is that a delinquent label redirects a youth’s self-

conception or personal identity toward a deviant self-concept, which is then self-fulfilling (e.g.,

Matsueda, 1992). Edwin Lemert’s (1951) version of labeling theory is emblematic of this

process, particularly his depiction of the progression from “primary deviance” to “secondary

deviance.” Individuals come to internalize the deviant status stemming from societal reaction to

their behavior, and deviants’ come to organize their lives around this status (see also Becker,

1963; Schur, 1971). Labeled deviants may then associate with more deviant peers (Wiley

Slocum, and Esbensen, 2013), withdraw from conventional pursuits (Bernburg, 2009), and

ultimately engage in criminal offending at a higher rate than otherwise similar individuals who

have not been labeled “deviant.” With this higher rate of offending, stigmatized youth would

presumably also have more frequent interaction with the criminal justice system than non-
First Juvenile Arrests 5

deviants.

Another mechanism in labeling theory focuses more on external processes involving

social and societal responses to the label, including increased surveillance as well as reduced

social opportunities and interactions (e.g., Klein, 1986; Link et al., 1989; Paternoster and

Iovanni, 1989). Here, the mechanisms are not internal to the labeled individual, but rather the

external social and societal responses, per se. In a parallel to Lemert’s terms of primary versus

secondary deviance, we conceptualize the labeling event—here an arrest—as a “primary

sanction” and subsequent punitive societal responses resulting from the label as “secondary

sanctioning.” This terminology is intended to capture the idea that there may be two parallel

processes operating in reaction to a deviant label, one internal and one external.

Representative of this version of labeling theory, Sampson and Laub’s (1997) life-course

theory of cumulative disadvantage emphasizes that once an individual is labeled a deviant, a

variety of detachment processes are set in motion that promote the likelihood of further deviance.

The stigma of a criminal record undermines social control processes, whether or not the labeled

deviant internalizes the deviant status as in the Lemert framework. Sampson and Laub (1997, p.

147) note, “The theory specifically suggests a ‘snowball’ effect—that adolescent delinquency

and its negative consequences (e.g., arrest, official labeling, incarceration) increasingly

‘mortgage’ one’s future, especially later life chances molded by schooling and employment.”

Several recent studies show evidence of such secondary sanctioning processes. For

instance, Kirk and Sampson (2013) suggest that an arrest record officially marks a juvenile as a

“criminal” and changes the way educational institutions treat the student. Students with criminal

records are often pushed out of high school through exclusionary policies, and segregated into

specialized programs for problem youth. The result of the primary sanction (arrest) and the
First Juvenile Arrests 6

secondary sanction (school exclusionary policies and practices) is an increased likelihood of high

school dropout and diminished prospects for going to college (e.g., Bernburg and Krohn, 2003;

Hirschfield, 2009; Kirk and Sampson, 2013; Sweeten, 2006), thereby leading to a higher

likelihood of future criminality. Similarly, the stigma of a criminal record drastically influences

how former offenders are treated by potential employers, and the denial of employment

represents a form of secondary sanctioning (Laub and Sampson, 2003; Pager, 2003; Schwartz

and Skolnick, 1962).

Moreover, if labeling effects operate though differential social or societal responses to

those labeled as deviant, then a labeled individual may have more frequent interactions with the

criminal justice system even if his or her criminal offending does not increase following an arrest

(relative to otherwise similar “non-deviants” who avoided an arrest record). As Petrosino and

colleagues put it (2010, p. 9), “The same actions that resulted in police turning a blind eye to

misconduct may now result in an arrest.” Such secondary sanctioning processes fit broadly under

the realm of labeling theory, but offer slightly different predictions than classic versions of

labeling which stress identity internalization, or even Sampson and Laub’s (1997) version which

stresses a decline in social controls. The essential difference is that the stigmatized deviant may

not engage in crime at a higher rate following arrest relative to an otherwise similar individual

who managed to avoid arrest, but the stigmatized deviant would still be rearrested and sanctioned

more often because of the intensified gaze, or declining tolerance, of the criminal justice system.

PRIOR RESEARCH ON THE LABELING EFFECTS OF JUVENILE ARRESTS

Few studies of the effects of arrest, whether in experimental or observational studies,

have simultaneously examined both secondary deviance (subsequent delinquency) and secondary

sanctioning (subsequent justice-system responses). Most experimental studies have relied solely
First Juvenile Arrests 7

on administrative outcome data, and have generally taken official data (arrests) as an indicator of

offending behavior per se, without distinguishing between effects on offending behavior

(secondary deviance) versus effects on later system response (secondary sanctioning). Petrosino

et al. (2010) recently conducted a meta-analysis of the effects of formal responses to juvenile

delinquency, limited to studies with random assignment (or quasi-random assignment) of

juveniles to either traditional processing versus release or some form of diversion. Overall, the

meta-analysis found that formal sanctioning was associated with more reoffending, across self-

report and official measures. Of the studies reviewed, 13 address the question of immediate

interest here by comparing juveniles who received traditional processing – beginning with a

formal arrest – to juveniles who were “released” or “counseled and released” without additional

programming (see Petrosino, Turpin-Petrosino, and Guckenburg, 2010, Table 8.6).1 All but one

of these 13 experimental studies used official arrest measures.

One of the few experimental studies to measure both self-reported offending (SRO) and

official arrests was conducted by Klein (1986). Youth identified by police were randomly

assigned to be counseled and released, petitioned, or referred to one of two diversion conditions.

Nine-months later, no effects were found on youths’ SRO or their agreement with descriptions of

themselves that “encapsulated” a delinquent label, but formally-petitioned youth were more

likely to have been rearrested.

In contrast to the experimental studies, most longitudinal studies have relied upon SRO

outcomes, and have not explored system responses. In a recent review, Huizinga and Henry

(2008) identified 19 longitudinal studies of the effects of arrest with reasonable attempts to

control for selection; most relied solely upon SRO. About half found no effect of arrest; the other

half seemed to find labeling effects on SRO.


First Juvenile Arrests 8

Longitudinal studies can allow confident establishment of temporal ordering. However,

selection bias remains a persistent challenge to non-experimental labeling findings, even in

longitudinal designs. In addition to lower self-control (Gottfredson and Hirschi, 1987), arrested

youth typically differ from non-arrestees in many ways that predispose them to greater

offending, including individual-level risk factors, as well as family, peer, and neighborhood risk.

To control this selection bias, about half of the studies reviewed by Huizinga and Henry included

predisposing factors to arrest as control variables in regression models, and about half used

matching strategies.

One of the most extensive matched longitudinal studies was conducted by Huizinga,

Schumann, Ehret, and Elliott (2003). Arrest during adolescence was examined in two

longitudinal samples, from Denver and from Bremmen (Germany). Each arrestee was matched to

a non-arrestee who was most similar on gender, age, minority status, annual delinquency since

age 14 up to the age of arrest, history of prior arrest, and annual history of delinquent peer

involvement.2 With this matching, adolescent arrest showed little effect on subsequent SRO in

either site, nor did sanctions beyond arrest show much effect on subsequent SRO in Denver,3 but

sanctions as an adolescent was related to increased unemployment as a young adult.

Propensity score methods have recently been used to better control selection bias, by

allowing matching on many risk variables simultaneously (Rosenbaum, 2002). Rather than

matching on a few select variables, one matches on a summary measure (i.e., the “propensity” of

arrest) that is computed from many variables simultaneously. This addresses one limitation of

traditional (exact) matching, where one typically can only match on a few variables

simultaneously. Three recent longitudinal studies have applied propensity-score methods to the

question of the labeling effect of arrest. Wiley and Esbensen (in press; also Wiley, Slocum, and
First Juvenile Arrests 9

Esbensen, 2013) used student survey data from the second national evaluation (2006-2013) of

the Gang Resistance Education And Training (GREAT) program. Controlling for seventeen pre-

arrest covariates, they find that youth who report having been arrested subsequently self-report

significantly more offending.

McAra and McVie (2007) used data from the Edinburgh Study of Youth Transitions and

Crime to explore the effects of three stages of formal processing after police contact (charging by

police, “referral to Reporter,” and being brought to a hearing). Charging and referral did not

affect SRO, but being brought to a hearing was associated with significantly more SRO. Thus, in

a rare study where arrest and prosecution could be disentangled, the study found no effect of

arrest per se, but a labeling effect of prosecution.

Labeling effects should theoretically be strongest for the first labeling event and each

repeated labeling event should have a smaller marginal effect. The difference between having an

arrest history or not should be larger than the difference between having three versus four prior

arrests. Conversely, deterrence should also be strongest for the first arrest. Because most

empirical studies fail to distinguish those newly labeled from those being labeled repeatedly,

they may have inadvertently conducted weak tests of the marginal effects of additional arrests,

(Paternoster and Iovanni, 1989). This may be one contributing factor to the many null findings in

the literature.

One of the few studies to isolate first arrests is Morris and Piquero’s (2013) analysis of

National Youth Study data. Selection was controlled by using propensity score matching within

groups with similar prior-offending trajectories (see Haviland et al., 2008). First arrests reported

at wave 5 (1980) were found to increase SRO at wave 6 (1983). This effect was most substantial

with chronic delinquents, and negligible for youth with very little prior offending.
First Juvenile Arrests 10

In sum, the prior literature generally finds that arrest increases subsequent offending, as

predicted by labeling theory, or finds no effect at all. Experimental studies generally show

labeling effects on rearrest, but do not distinguish offending from sanctioning. Longitudinal

studies show effects on SRO. Some recent studies reaffirm the basic labeling effect using

propensity score methods to control for selection bias, but none of these recent studies have

examined secondary sanctioning.

THE CURRENT STUDY

The prior literature does not clearly delineate whether the effect of arrest on rearrest is

primarily the product of the indirect effect of arrest through subsequent offending, or whether the

first juvenile arrest independently increases rearrest beyond any effect through future offending.

This paper aims to distinguish labeling effects, if any, on reoffending (secondary deviance) from

labeling effects on rearrest (secondary sanctioning).

To study the effects of juvenile first arrests on both subsequent SRO and rearrest, we use

data from the PHDCN, linked to official arrest data from the Chicago Police Department (CPD)

and the Illinois State Police (ISP). Using the PHDCN data, we construct propensity scores for

matching arrestees and non-arrestees using a rich set of covariates, including measures of prior

offending, temperament, family circumstances, demographics, education, peer influences, and

neighborhood characteristics. In contrast to most prior studies, we restrict the present study to

first arrests.

We test three hypotheses: (1) A first juvenile arrest has an independent positive effect on

subsequent delinquency and criminal offending above and beyond the influence of individual,

family, peer, and neighborhood, and school correlates. (2) Arrested adolescents are more likely

to be arrested in the future than otherwise similar youth without arrest records. (3) The effect of
First Juvenile Arrests 11

first arrest on future arrests is independent of the effect of a first arrest on criminal offending.

That is, adolescents with a previous arrest are more likely to be arrested in the future than

comparable non-arrestees even if they engage in similar levels of future delinquency.

DATA AND METHODS

SAMPLE

Our sample comes from the PHDCN’s longitudinal cohort study, which involved three

waves of data collection from seven cohorts of youth, at three-year age intervals (i.e., ages 0, 3,

6, 9, 12, 15, and 18 at wave 1). The first wave of interviews was conducted in 1995 through

1997, and subsequent waves were separated by about 2.5 years, with the third interviews

occurring approximately five years later (from January 2000 to January 2002).

For the longitudinal study, the PHDCN selected a sample of 80 neighborhood clusters,

stratified by racial/ethnic composition (seven categories) and socioeconomic status (high,

medium, and low), from a total of 343 neighborhood clusters in Chicago (Sampson, Raudenbush,

and Earls, 1997). Within these 80 neighborhood clusters, a simple random sample of households

yielded a total sample of 1,517 youth in the 12-year-old and 15-year-old cohorts. We specifically

focus on these two cohorts because our study examines the effect of arrest as a juvenile.

For rearrest outcomes, our sample consists of 1,249 youth, 58 arrestees (the treatment

group) and 1,191 non-arrestees, who completed the Wave 1 SRO questionnaire and consented to

the official records search. This excludes 34 youth who failed to respond to the Wave 1 SRO

questionnaire, as well as 234 youth who did not to consent to the official records search. Prior

research comparing PHDCN respondents who did or did not consent to the records search found

no systematic difference on a measure of self-reported arrest (Kirk, 2008).

Although Wave 3 participation was not necessary for our rearrest outcomes, for SRO
First Juvenile Arrests 12

outcomes Wave 3 attrition slightly reduced the sample to 53 arrestees and 951 non-arrestees.

Wave 3 attrition was lower among arrestees (8.6%; 5 youth) than non-arrestees (20.2%; 240

youth). The sample of arrestees was distributed across 39 of the 80 neighborhood clusters; only

one neighborhood cluster contained more than three arrestees.

DESIGN

Reliably establishing the temporal order of pre-treatment propensities, treatment, and

outcomes is a key requirement for a methodologically strong quasi-experimental study of the

causal effects of arrests. We capitalize on the three-wave structure of the PHDCN longitudinal

study and dates in the official arrest data to insure that measures used to predict our treatment

condition—first arrest—were indeed measured prior to treatment, and that the re-offending and

re-arrest outcomes follow the treatment condition. Pretreatment characteristics used in propensity

models were measured at Wave 1, the treatment was restricted to a window between the Waves 1

and 3, and our self-report outcome is measured at Wave 3. Arrest outcomes are contemporaneous

with the Wave 3 SRO period.

TREATMENT VARIABLE

Administrative data on arrests from the CPD and the ISP were obtained and merged with

the PHDCN data. These data span 1995 to 2001, and include both juvenile and adult arrest data

throughout Illinois. Identifying information used in matching the data sets includes social

security number, name, birth date, county and zip code, race and ethnicity, and gender. To

construct our treatment variable, we determined whether each given PHDCN survey respondent

was officially arrested as a juvenile (per the CPD and ISP data) for the first time sometime

between their Wave 1 interview date and 15 months preceding the Wave 3 PHDCN interview.

This allows a 3 month buffer against recall error before the beginning of the 12-month recall
First Juvenile Arrests 13

period used for the SRO outcomes in Wave 3 (i.e., at the Wave 3 interview, respondents were

asked about offending at any point during the prior 12 month period). With approximately five

years between the first and third wave of data collection, this arrest window was approximately

45 months (60 months – 15).

OUTCOME VARIABLES

One of our offending outcome variables is a general measure of SRO over the past year.

Subjects were asked at the Wave 3 interview whether they had engaged in each of 22 behaviors

during the preceding 12 months, and if so, how many times. As our general offending measure,

we calculate the variety of offending across the 22 items, which counts the number of different

types of criminal acts in which the person engaged in at least once. The 22 items consist of 6

violent offenses, 8 property offenses, 3 drug-selling offenses, 3 public-order offenses, and 2

status offenses.4 In addition to the offending variety score, we also use as outcome variables

separate measures of the prevalence of violent, property, and drug-sales offending.5

Official arrest records were used as the secondary sanctioning outcome, using the same

arrest data from CPD and ISP described above. We constructed a binary prevalence measure

indicating whether each individual was arrested at any point from 12 months prior to their Wave

3 interview date and the end of 2001, which was the last available extract of data on arrests from

CPD and ISP.

PROPENSITY SCORE MATCHING

Most adolescents commit some delinquent acts (Porterfield, 1943; Short and Nye, 1957;

1958; Wallerstein and Wyle, 1947), but, as Tannenbaum observes (1938, p. 19), “Only some of

the children are caught.” Only a minority of delinquent acts are detected and fewer lead to

arrests. Law enforcement officers exercise considerable discretion regarding whether and when
First Juvenile Arrests 14

to initiate a formal arrest. In their classic study, Black and Reiss (1970) found that only 15

percent of police contacts with juveniles resulted in an arrest. The arrest decision to a large

extent, then, lies with the police and is based on a host of external factors beyond the criminal

behavior of the arrested individual. In this sense, juvenile arrest has a random component,

making it likely that for each arrested individual in the PHDCN sample, there are one or more

equivalent non-arrestees, in terms of criminal offending and other pre-treatment covariates, who

were fortunate enough to avoid arrest following the commission of their crime or crimes. We

exploit this randomness in juvenile arrest via propensity score matching, to determine if arrest is

causally related to subsequent offending and rearrest.

Using the PHDCN’s extensive data on pre-treatment covariates, we used propensity score

matching to identify comparison youth who were otherwise similar to treated (i.e., arrested)

youth by modeling the probability of arrest. Propensity score methods allow the creation of

balanced treatment and control groups who are equivalent on all measured covariates

(Rosenbaum and Rubin, 1983). Given a set of covariates that account for the features associated

with selection into the treatment condition, this approximates an experimental design (Morgan

and Winship, 2007; Rosenbaum, 2002). Importantly, however, whether the set of covariates is

sufficient to account for selection into treatment cannot be empirically determined. Instead, as

with other regression or matching approaches, judgments of the sufficiency of the control

variables must be assessed on a priori grounds based on theory and understanding of the

treatment under consideration. In addition, sensitivity tests allow one to examine the sensitivity

of the results to possible omitted variables. Use of sensitivity analyses is critical when employing

propensity score methods, particularly in analyses where relatively few covariates are used to

estimate the propensity score, but even in our analysis that draws upon a vast array of
First Juvenile Arrests 15

pretreatment covariates. We use Rosenbaum’s (2002, 2010) bounding approach for this purpose.

Propensity scores were constructed from 79 Wave 1 covariates. SRO variables from the

first wave of interviews, constructed identically to the SRO outcome variables from Wave 3

interviews, were included as pre-treatment covariates predicting the probability of arrest. We

also included similarly constructed Wave 1 SRO variables concerning status and public order

offending in constructing the propensity score. In addition, pre-treatment variables included drug

use, temperament, household composition, parenting characteristics, socioeconomic status

(SES), demographics, education, and peer influences. Neighborhood characteristics were also

included, from the 1995 PHDCN Community Survey, U.S. Census data, and reported crime data

(Sampson, Raudenbush, and Earls, 1997). To estimate the effect of a first juvenile arrest on

future offending and arrest, we statistically match and then compare arrested and non-arrested

sample members who are otherwise similar to one another in their frequency of criminal

offending and all the pretreatment characteristics.

Before creating propensity scores, multiple imputation procedures were used to impute

missing values for Wave 1 pre-treatment variables. Approximately 24 percent of the cases had at

least one missing value. Multiple-chain imputation was implemented through Stata 12’s MI

procedure, to create five imputed data sets. We followed Hill’s (2004, p. 13) multiple-imputation

matching strategy and calculated a propensity score for each observation in each of the imputed

data sets, using the mi estimate and mi predict commands in Stata 12. We then averaged the

propensity scores for each respondent across the five imputed data sets.

Propensity score matching was done using nearest neighbor 3:1 matching, with

replacement, with a caliper set at 0.02. That is, each arrested youth was matched with up to three

otherwise similar non-arrested youth who had a propensity of arrest (i.e., a predicted probability
First Juvenile Arrests 16

of arrest) within .02 of the arrested youth. Matching was accomplished via Stata’s psmatch2

routine. The resulting matched samples for arrest outcomes consisted of 43 arrestees and 126

nonarrestees; because we matched with replacement, the 126 control matches include 103 unique

control cases. For SRO outcomes, the matched samples consisted of 38 arrestees and 111 non-

arrestees; these control matches include 80 unique control cases.6

Linear models of the effect of first arrest on offending variety were estimated directly by

psmatch2. With binary outcomes (the prevalence of offending as well as rearrest), psmatch2 was

used to identify matches; then the matched samples were analyzed via logit models while

accounting for matching. That is, each treatment observation and its corresponding control

matches represent a cluster, and logit models were estimated while accounting for this

clustering.7

RESULTS

PRE-TREATMENT DIFFERENCES

A key early question concerns the similarity or difference among youth who are and are

not arrested. Arrested youth significantly differed from non-arrestees on 34 of 79 covariates

examined. Table 1 shows differences in individual-level factors in the left panel, and Tables 2

and 3 display differences among family, peer, and neighborhood-level factors.

Prevalence of offending at Wave 1 among arrested youth was significantly greater across

all offending categories (violence, 60%; property, 22%; drug-sales, 14%; public–order, 21%;

status, 64%; mean offending variety = 4.28) than among control youth (violence, 15%; property,

9%; drug-sales, 2%; public–order, 11%; status, 12%; mean offending variety = 1.80). In

addition, arrestees differed significantly on variables in other domains. Arrested youth were

significantly more likely to smoke cigarettes, with a marginally significant difference in


First Juvenile Arrests 17

marijuana use as well. In terms of temperament, arrested youth had lower inhibitory control,

more sensation seeking, and were quicker to make decisions (i.e., a form of impulsivity). As

noted by Gottfredson and Hirschi (1990), these aspects of self-control are likely confounders

related to both the likelihood of first arrest and future behavior. Accounting for these aspects of

control and temperament are essential when attempting to estimate the unbiased effects of arrest.

Among demographic variables, arrested youth were significantly more likely to be male,

older, and African-American, and were less likely to be Mexican or second-generation

immigrants (compared to third or later generations). First-generation immigrants were also

descriptively less likely to be arrested than third generation or later, although the difference was

not statistically significant.

(Tables 1, 2 and 3 about here)

Table 2 shows differences in family and parent variables. Arrested youth were less likely

to have married parents, had lower levels of parental supervision, and came from families with

more parent-child and family conflict as well as less developmental stimulation and parental

warmth.

Table 3 shows peer characteristics and neighborhood differences. Arrested youth reported

significantly more peer deviance, more peer pressure, and less peer attachment. At the

neighborhood level, arrested youth lived in neighborhoods with proportionally more African

Americans residents, fewer Hispanics, and fewer foreign-born residents, as well as higher

concentrations of poverty and higher violent crime rates. The neighborhoods had more social and

physical disorder, and residents had significantly more cynicism toward the law.

PROPENSITY SCORES AND BALANCE

The maximum propensity score, expressed as the probability of being arrested, found for
First Juvenile Arrests 18

the non-arrestees was 0.738 while the lowest propensity for the treatment group was 0.003 (see

Figure 1). Following recommendations of Ho, Imai, King and Stuart (2007) and Stuart (2010),

we restricted our analyses to individuals with propensities in the ranges found in both groups,

known as the region of common support. Thus, we excluded four arrestees with propensity

scores greater that any controls (i.e., greater than 0.738). We do not extrapolate our results to

individuals with the highest propensities to be arrested, leading us to estimate only the "Average

effect of the Treatment on the Treated" (ATT; the effect of treatment for those subjects who

actually received the treatment). These effects may not generalize to individuals with very high

probabilities of arrest.

Once matched to arrestees, the resulting sample of non-arrestees did not differ from the

arrestees on any of the covariates, as shown in the right half of Tables 1-3. The post-match t-

statistics and corresponding p-values on the right side of Tables 1-3 reveal that among the 79

covariates used to estimate the propensity of arrest, none showed a significant difference

between the treated and controls in our final matched sample. In addition, matching on

propensity score reduced absolute bias across all covariates by 63%, from a mean of 30.3 down

to 11.5 (median bias reduced from 25.8 to 11.1).8 9

(Figure 1 about here)

EFFECT OF ARREST ON OFFENDING AND RE-ARREST

Having established the effectiveness of our propensity score matching to produce

equivalent samples of arrestees and non-arrestees, we turn now to the results of the effect of first

juvenile arrest on self-reported reoffending from the third wave of PHDCN subject interviews.

As expected, in the absence of matching, the prevalence of offending at Wave 3 among

arrested youth was considerably greater (violence, 57%; property, 30%; drug-sales 17%) than
First Juvenile Arrests 19

among control youth (violence, 23%; property, 19%; drug-sales, 6%); offending variety too, was

considerably greater for arrestees (2.08 vs. 0.90). And those already arrested were much more

likely to be arrested later (55% vs. 9%). These differences, all highly significant, reflect both the

preexisting differences in propensity between those who had and had not been arrested (i.e.,

selection effects) as well as any effects of being arrested.

The key question is whether these differences persist once the selection effects are

reduced through propensity score matching. We find that arrestees continued to report

significantly more offending variety at Wave 3 than matched non-arrestees (2.03 vs. 1.04;

SE=0.40; t=2.44, p=.016). In addition, as shown in Figure 2, arrestees were more likely to report

committing violent, property, and drug distribution offending than matched non-arrestees. These

prevalence differences were significant in logit models for violent offending (OR = 3.23;

SE=1.45; z = 2.61, p = .009) and marginally significant for property offending (OR = 2.17;

SE=0.98; z = 1.72, p = .086). In addition, as shown in the figure, arrestees were much more

likely to be rearrested than equivalent non-arrestees (OR = 5.20; SE=1.85; z = 4.63, p < .001).

(Figure 2 about here)

SENSITIVITY AND ROBUSTNESS

Sensitivity. We explored the sensitivity of our results to possible unobserved variables.

The rich set of covariates used in our propensity score analyses suggests that our matched results

control for pre-treatment differences between the treatment and control groups. Nonetheless, the

potential for hidden biases in our estimation of the effect of arrest remains. We used

Rosenbaum’s (2002) bounding strategy, which explores how large the bias of an omitted variable

would need to be to substantively affect our results (see Appendix A for methodological details).

Γ in Table 4 refers to the assumed increase in the odds of treatment (first arrest) due to
First Juvenile Arrests 20

hypothetical unobservable factors.

We begin with the effect on future arrest. At Γ= 1, we assume there are no hidden biases,

and would conclude that arrest has a significant positive effect on future arrest (Q+ = 4.320, p <

.001). Hypothetical unobserved variables that would bias the results in the direction of the

observed effect are explored with values of Γ larger than 1. At Γ= 1.25, we assume there is an

unobserved variable that increases the odds of being arrested (receiving the treatment in the

current study) by an additional 25 percent after accounting for the propensity score. Under this

scenario, we still find a significant positive effect of arrest on future arrest (Q+ = 3.720, p <

.001). At Γ= 2.00, we assume an unobserved variable that doubles the odds of being arrested, and

we still find a significant positive effect of arrest on future arrest (Q+ = 2.496, p < .01). To

render the effect on future arrest insignificant would require a Γ value of over 2.75. As an

example, increasing the probability of arrest from .50 (odds=1.0) to .7333 (odds=2.75) would

produce a Γ of 2.75.

For the effect on future violent offending, we find that at Γ= 1.25, the effect persists (Q+

= 1.992, p = .023). To render the treatment effect of arrest on violent offending no longer

significant, at p < .05, would require a Γ value of nearly 1.5. As an example, increasing the

probability of arrest from .50 (odds=1.0) to .60 (odds=1.5) would produce a Γ of 1.5.

As a comparison, we find that Wave 1 violent offending increases the odds of first arrest

by an additional 20 percent after controlling for a propensity score that excludes this factor.

Thus, in order to spuriously produce the effect on future violent offending, an unobserved factor

would need to be related to first arrests somewhat more strongly than is prior violent offending.

To spuriously produce the effect on future arrest, an unobserved factor would need to be related

to first arrests much more strongly than is prior violent offending. Given that we already control
First Juvenile Arrests 21

for a full range of offending behavior and substance use in developing the propensity score, it is

challenging to conceive of an omitted factor that would yield such a sizable increase in the

probability of arrest, particularly for the effect of arrest on future arrest.

Robustness. We also explored the robustness of the effects found in the matching

approach already described (3:1 nearest neighbor matching, with caliper = 0.02), through four

other propensity-score-matching specifications. We widened the caliper for 3:1 matching to 0.04,

used 1:1 matching, and used kernel matching with bandwidths set at 0.06 or 0.10. The alternative

matching approaches and their results are presented in Appendix A.

The matching approach already described, which we term our “primary” specification,

was the most efficient in removing bias.. This primary specification was more stringent than

most of the other specifications on matching, so that 43 of the 58 arrestees were matched (a.k.a.,

"on support"), and 38 of the 53 arrestees with SRO data. The other specifications were somewhat

less efficient in reducing bias, but were able to match more treated cases to controls.

The estimates of the effect on future arrest were highly significant across all

specifications, and effect sizes (ORs) were larger under the alternative specifications than with

our primary specification. However, for the effects on future offending, the picture was more

mixed. The effects on violent offending were largest under our primary specification, and were

reasonably robust, although the ORs and significance were somewhat reduced under other

specifications. The results on property offending and on offending variety score were less robust,

and effects were considerably diminished and no longer significant using kernel matching rather

than nearest neighbor matching.

In sum, we find that the effects on future arrests effects are robust and are not sensitive to

omitted variable bias. The effects on future offending, however, were both less robust and
First Juvenile Arrests 22

somewhat more sensitive. The largest offending effect, on violent offending, is reasonably robust

and reasonably insensitive to omitted variable bias.

(Table 4 about here)

THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN SECONDARY DEVIANCE AND SECONDARY

SANCTIONING

To this point, we have evidence in support of our first two hypotheses: arrest has an

effect on offending, particularly violence, and also on rearrest. We now consider whether the

effect of arrest on rearrest is independent of its effect on offending. Put differently, are

individuals with an arrest record more likely to be arrested in the future even if they engage in

comparable rates of offending as non-arrestees? Or, are prior arrestees more likely to be

rearrested in the future primarily because of greater offending?

That the difference in rearrest reported in Figure 2 is considerably larger than the

differences in SRO suggests that the two effects are distinct. To explore these questions more

formally, we conducted an analysis of rearrest while controlling for the SRO at Wave 3. For this

purpose we use our most robust effect of offending, violent offending, as a mediating variable.

We weight cases by the inverse of their propensity score, and estimate the effect of arrest on

subsequent arrest, net of criminal offending, in a logistic regression model (see Appendix A for

methodological details).

Results are shown in Table 5. Model 1 replicates our earlier propensity score matching

results with propensity weighting, and confirms that first arrests significantly increase the

likelihood of later arrest (OR = 4.97).10 Model 2 then explores the extent to which this effect is

accounted for by increased SRO.11

Consistent with our third hypothesis, the effect of first arrest on future arrest is essentially
First Juvenile Arrests 23

independent of the effect on reoffending. That is, offending at Wave 3 is not predictive of

rearrest (with a nonsignificant negative coefficient), once common influences are controlled

through propensity scores. The clear implication is that the secondary sanctioning effect of

increased rearrest is distinct from the secondary deviance effect on reoffending.

(Table 5 about here)

Finally, Model 3 examines whether the system response effect (i.e., the significant effect

of arrest on subsequent arrest) is concentrated among the more active offenders, by interacting

arrest with the level of offending. Focusing on the interaction term, we find no evidence that

arrest is any more or less consequential for high rate versus low rate offenders.12

DISCUSSION

This study explored the effects of arresting juveniles on subsequent offending and on

rearrest, and tested two types of labeling effects. Supporting our first hypothesis, we found that

arrest led to a greater likelihood of offending, consistent with labeling theory. Supporting our

second hypothesis, first arrests increased the likelihood of rearrest, and this effect was

considerably larger than the effects on subsequent offending. Supporting our third hypothesis, we

found that the increased likelihood of subsequent arrest was not due to the increase in offending.

Rather, a first juvenile arrest appears to increase subsequent law enforcement responses to those

youth compared to other youth who are offending at a comparable level but managed to evade a

first arrest. This could result from increased scrutiny of the individual’s future behavior, by

police as well as other actors such as teachers and school staff, as well as through reduced

tolerance by police and other actors of an arrestee’s future transgressions.

These findings extend prior labeling research in several important ways. The increases in

SRO following arrest confirm findings from studies with other longitudinal data. Using
First Juvenile Arrests 24

propensity score methods with the PHDCN’s extensive set of covariates from the individual,

family, peer, and neighborhood domains provides grounds for believing that we have

substantially reduced the threat to validity from selection bias that is a concern in many labeling

studies. Varied specifications and sensitivity tests confirm that our results are reasonably robust,

especially the effect on violent offending, and relatively insensitive to bias from unobservables.

These findings are also consistent with two other recent propensity score studies of SRO

outcomes, Morris and Piquero’s (2013) study of arrests circa 1980, and Wiley and Esbensen’s (in

press) study of arrests in the 2000s, both of which support the conclusion that arrests lead to

secondary deviance.

The present study also confirms earlier findings, mostly from experimental studies, that

an arrest tends to generate more subsequent arrests. This finding is robust across model

specifications and quite insensitive to the possibility of omitted variable bias. Perhaps our most

important finding concerns the relationship between the effect on SRO and the effect on rearrest.

It is commonly assumed that rearrest is largely a product of the perpetuation of offending

associated with secondary deviance. Our findings cast doubt on this common interpretation.

Instead, we find a considerably larger effect on arrest than on SRO, consistent with Klein

(1986).13 Moreover, we find that the arrest effect is not diminished after accounting for the

potential mediating effect of Wave 3 SRO, leading us to conclude that the effects of secondary

deviance and secondary sanctioning are essentially independent.

One limitation of the present study is that it excludes the highest rate offenders, for whom

no matched non-arrestees were found with a comparable propensity to be arrested. Hence, we

have no empirical basis to estimate how such extremely high-propensity youth would fare if not

arrested, because all such youth in the PHDCN were arrested. Possibly, the highest propensity
First Juvenile Arrests 25

youth are already so firmly on a deviant trajectory that a formal arrest has little effect either on

their behavior or on societal responses. Or perhaps cumulative disadvantage makes first arrests

especially likely to result in secondary deviance and/or secondary sanctioning for such high

propensity youth.

IMPLICATIONS

Our results suggest that the large labeling effect found on rearrest truly reflects secondary

sanctioning—that is, differential societal response to a youth with an “arrestee” or “delinquent”

label—and that this societal response is not mediated by differential offending behavior of the

juvenile. This process of secondary sanctioning, in which initial arrests beget further arrests,

suggests a cascading effect of deepening involvement in the justice system.

How does this secondary sanctioning come about? Does it reflect only differential

behavior of law enforcement or might some differential youth behavior also be implicated? One

limitation from applying data collected for broader purposes to these labelling questions is that

we have limited information about the arrests, their particular circumstances or what offending

behavior may have prompted those arrests, let alone how the arrestee interacted with law

enforcement. Thus, although the secondary sanctioning effects were not mediated by differential

offending, we cannot confidently assume that no other behavioral differences between youth

with and without prior arrest experiences played a role in their differential subsequent arrests.

Nonetheless, the major expected behavioral contributor of youth would seem to be offending

differences. The absence of any indication that offending behavior mediates the increase in arrest

suggests, therefore, that secondary sanctioning effects are due in considerable part to differential

societal responses. Whether this reflects police actively scrutinizing and/or monitoring "the usual

suspects," being more likely to take formal action rather than issue a warning to youth with a
First Juvenile Arrests 26

prior arrest, or some other changed response remains a question for future research.

Labelling effects on youth behavior and on police behavior may also be interdependent.

Secondary sanctioning processes may be partly co-produced in the interaction between two

actors whose expectations are conditioned by the earlier arrest, with police having higher

expectations that labeled youth will offend, while youth have higher expectations that police will

carry out arrests. Long-standing experimental work finds that experimentally induced

expectations of students' academic performance influence teachers’ and students’ interactions in

ways that are expectancy-confirming (Jussim, 2005; Rosenthal and Jacobson, 1968), especially

when teachers do not yet have much direct experience with the student (Raudenbush, 1984).

Similar expectancy processes may operate in how labels affect the interactions between labelled

offenders and justice system actors.

The present study thus suggests that understanding labeling processes will require future

work on mechanisms of secondary sanctioning processes as well as on secondary deviance. This

will require broadening the labeling perspective to include studying the behavioral response of

societal actors to labels.

The policy implications of labeling findings are twofold. The most obvious implication

for curtailing the destructive effects of labeling is to restrict formal law enforcement responses to

serious delinquency and to resist the temptation to criminalize minor misbehavior, such as school

discipline problems (e.g., Kupchik, 2010). While few U.S. policy-makers have been willing to go

as far as Schur (1973) in considering “radical non-intervention” by law enforcement, there are

some promising signs in the school domain. Following the lead of several school districts,

notably in Broward County, Florida (see Stucki, 2013), the U.S. Department of Education (2014)

has recently issued a set of guiding principles with respect to school discipline that marks a
First Juvenile Arrests 27

fundamental shift away from the tough-on-crime school policies that led to the criminalization of

so many minor school infractions over the past two decades.

The second type of policy implication concerns how to decrease the detrimental effects of

labeling. This typically focuses on how to ameliorate secondary deviance effects (e.g., Burnberg,

Krohn, and Rivera, 2006; Wiley, Slocum, and Esbensen, 2013). The current study, however,

suggests that in the relatively short term, the effects of arrest through secondary deviance may be

dwarfed by the detrimental effects that operate though secondary sanctioning. Ameliorative

policy efforts that address the secondary societal responses may be as important as those that try

to ease the deviance-amplifying effect of the primary sanction. In the law enforcement realm,

these would involve efforts to prevent the compounding effect of increasingly punitive law

enforcement responses to equivalent misbehavior, especially minor misbehavior. These results

also highlight the importance of policies and practices to maintain the confidentiality of juvenile

records, and expunge and remove such records after an appropriate period of redemption

(Blumstein and Nakamura, 2009).

One way for an offender to lessen the risk of secondary sanctioning is to simply move

residences or change schools. There may be some benefit to putting physical distance between an

arrestee seeking to reform his or her behavior and the increasingly watchful eye of the

authorities. Recent research on prisoner reentry has found that residential relocation can

potentially provide a turning point for ex-prisoners by helping sever ties to former peers and

neighborhoods, thereby lessening some of the risk factors which propel individuals toward crime

(Kirk, 2009; 2012). Residential change may also lower the risk of rearrest by separating

individuals from the watchful gaze of local police who might have a decreasing level of

tolerance for a given person’s transgressions. In accord with this line of reasoning, Keels (2008)
First Juvenile Arrests 28

finds that male youths from families participating in the Gautreaux housing mobility program in

Chicago who moved to the suburbs were significantly less likely to be arrested for drug, theft,

and violent offenses than male youths who moved internally within Chicago. A move to the

suburbs means moving outside of the jurisdiction and surveillance of the Chicago Police

Department, thereby providing more of an opportunity for a fresh start.

In conclusion, among otherwise equivalent youth with similar levels of criminal

offending, those youth unlucky enough to become ensnared by the criminal justice system face a

daunting task of steering clear from future interaction with the system. Not only is the likelihood

of future offending increased for a host of reasons, but the likelihood of future sanctioning

increases even if criminal behavior does not escalate relative to non-arrested counterparts.

Therefore, policy solutions to the detrimental consequences of a delinquent label must address

not only ways to reduce secondary deviance, but also ways to reduce secondary sanctions. Of

course two ways to avoid the necessity of countering the consequences of criminal stigma are to

reduce primary deviance and primary sanctioning. Fortunately, the federal government has

recently recognized the far reaching consequences of a criminal stigma, and has taken recent

steps to reduce the criminalization of youth, particularly misbehavior in schools (U.S.

Department of Education, 2014). The U.S. Department of Education’s new guiding principles are

a refreshing alternative to the culture of control that has characterized the U.S criminal justice

and education systems for too long.


First Juvenile Arrests 29

FOOTNOTES

1
The extent of formal processing varies among these studies. “Because the system

processing condition is usually the control group in the experiments, it often is not described

further” (Petrosino, Turpin-Petrosino, and Guckenburg, 2010:13), and was described just as

“processing” in nearly two thirds of the studies reviewed (p. 22). Among studies in which the

control group was “released” or “counseled and released,” the commonality among the

traditional processing conditions seems to be a formal arrest. This is similar to studies of “arrest”

per se, in which system penetration following arrest is generally unknown.


2
Neighborhood type was also used as a matching variable in the Denver sample.
3
In Bremmen, too few youth were sanctioned to support analyses.
4
Violent Offenses: carried a hidden weapon, hit someone not lived with, attacked

someone with a weapon, used force to rob, threw objects at people, was in a gang fight. Property

Offenses: damaged property, set a fire, broke into a building to steal, stole from a store, stole

from household member, snatched a purse, stole from a car, bought/sold stolen goods. Drug-

Selling Offenses: sold marijuana, sold cocaine/crack, sold heroin. Public-Order Offenses: caused

trouble in public, paid for sex, got a driving ticket. Status Offenses: ran away from home

overnight, drove without a license.


5
We also explored the frequency of offending in these three items. Results from analysis

of offending frequency were substantively similar to the prevalence results.


6
Reestimating the rearrest model using the smaller SRO sample generated equivalent

results to those presented below (available upon request).


First Juvenile Arrests 30

7
Stata code for the logit models is as follows: logit outcome_var treatment_var

[pweight=controlweight], vce( cluster clusterid). Each treated case and its matched control cases

have a common, unique cluster identification (clusterid); vce(cluster) accounts for this

clustering. Control cases are down-weighted if more than 1 case match a given treated case;

controlweight =1 for 1 match, .5 for 2, and .333 for 3 matches. Treated cases' weight = 1.
8
Bias represents the mean differences across groups as a percentage of the square root of

the average of the sample variances: 100 * ( x T  x C ) /( s T  s C )


2 2 1/ 2
, where x T and xC are the sample

means in the treated group and the control group respectively, and sT
2
and 2
sC are the respective

sample variances (Rosenbaum and Rubin, 1985). Absolute bias is unsigned, and facilitates

comparison across variables.


9
Tables 1-3 matching results are based on the sample of youth with Wave 3 SRO data on

violent offending. With the slightly larger sample of youth with official (re)arrest data, 3:1

matching reduced mean absolute bias by 66%, from 29.1 to 6.9, and no significant covariate

differences remained.
10
This OR is slightly different than presented earlier because of differences in estimation

method. Propensity score weighting utilizes the full sample under common support in estimating

the effect of arrest, including control and treated cases not used in the matched samples.
11
Our regression estimation of the indirect path to rearrest via increased offending does

not control for possible alternative indirect pathways. Its identification is therefore weaker than

for our PSM estimation of the primary effects. However, because this mediating path is the

critical pathway suggested by theories of secondary deviance, it warrants particular scrutiny. By


First Juvenile Arrests 31

failing to control alternative pathways, we likely overestimate the magnitude of this pathway. In

which case, this produces a conservative estimate of the residual effect that we take as evidence

for a secondary sanctioning path.


12
Similar results were founds in equivalent propensity-weighted analyses using the

variety score as the control variable, or using offending from Wave 1 (rather than Wave 3).
13
Although Klein found no effect on secondary deviance, we find such an effect on

offending, perhaps because we restrict our study to first arrests, which should theoretically show

the largest effects on secondary deviance.


First Juvenile Arrests 32

REFERENCES

Ainsworth, Janet E. 1995. Youth justice in a unified court: Response to critics of juvenile court

abolition. Boston College Law Review 36:927–51.

Allen, Francis A. 2000. Foreword. In The Changing Borders of Juvenile Justice: Transfer of

Adolescents to the Criminal Court, eds. Jeffrey Fagan and Franklin Zimring Chicago, IL:

University of Chicago Press.

Becker, Howard S. 1963. Outsiders: Studies in the Sociology of Deviance. New York: Free

Press.

Becker, Sascha O., and Marco Caliendo. 2007. Sensitivity analysis for average treatment effects.

Stata Journal 7:71–83.

Bernburg, John Gunnar. 2009. Labeling theory. In Handbook of Crime and Deviance, eds.

Marvin D. Krohn, Alan J. Lizotte, and Gina Penly Hall. New York: Springer.

Bernburg, Jon Gunnar, and Marvin D. Krohn. 2003. Labeling, life chances, and adult crime: The

direct and indirect effects of official intervention in adolescence on crime in early

adulthood. Criminology 41:1287–318.

Bernburg, Jon Gunnar, Marvin D. Krohn, and Craig J. Rivera. 2006. Official labeling, criminal

embeddedness, and subsequent delinquency: A longitudinal test of labeling theory.

Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency 43:67–88.

Bishop, Donna M. 2004. Injustice and irrationality in contemporary youth policy. Criminology &

Public Policy 3:633–44.

Black, Donald J., and Albert J. Reiss, Jr. 1970. Police Control of Juveniles. American

Sociological Review 35:63–77.

Blumstein, Alfred, and Kiminori Nakamura. 2009. Redemption in the presence of widespread
First Juvenile Arrests 33

criminal background checks. Criminology 47:327–359.

Butts, Jeffrey A., and Ojmarrh Mitchell. 2000. Brick by brick: Dismantling the border between

juvenile and adult justice. In Boundary Changes in Criminal Justice Organizations, ed.

Charles M. Friel. Washington, DC: National Institute of Justice.

Caliendo, Macro, and Sabine Kopeinig. 2008. Some practical guidance for the implementation of

propensity score matching. Journal of Economic Surveys 22:31–72.

Cook, Philip J., and John H. Laub. 2002. After the epidemic: Recent trends in youth violence in

the United States. Crime and Justice 29:1–37.

Dawson, Robert O. 1990. The future of juvenile justice: Is it time to abolish the system? Journal

of Criminal Law and Criminology 81:136–55.

DiPrete, Thomas A., and Markus Gangl. 2004. Assessing bias in the estimation of causal effects:

Rosenbaum bounds on matching estimators and instrumental variables estimation with

imperfect instruments. Sociological Methodology 34:271–310.

Fagan, Jeffrey, and Franklin Zimring (Eds.). 2000. The Changing Borders of Juvenile Justice:

Transfer of Adolescents to the Criminal Court. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Feld, Barry C. 1998. Abolish the juvenile court: Youthfulness, criminal responsibility, and

sentencing policy. Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology 88:68–136.

Gottfredson, Michael R., and Travis Hirschi. 1990. A General Theory of Crime. Stanford, CA:

Stanford University Press.

Haviland, Amelia, Daniel S. Nagin, Paul R. Rosenbaum, and Richard E. Tremblay. 2008.

Combining group-based trajectory modeling and propensity score matching for causal

inferences in nonexperimental longitudinal data. Developmental Psychology 44:422–436.

Hill, Jennifer. 2004. Reducing Bias in Treatment Effect Estimation in Observational Studies
First Juvenile Arrests 34

Suffering from Missing Data. Columbia University Institute for Social and Economic

Research and Policy (ISERP) working paper 04-01. Available at:

http://academiccommons.columbia.edu/catalog/ac:129151.

Hirano, Keisuke, and Guido W. Imbens. 2001. Estimating of causal effects using propensity

score weighting: An application to data on right heart catheterization. Health Services

and Outcomes Research Methodology 2:259–278.

Hirano, Keisuke, Guido Imbens, and Geert Ridder. 2003. Efficient estimation of average

treatment effects using the estimated propensity score. Econometrica 71:1161–89.

Hirschfield, Paul J. 2009. Another way out: The impact of juvenile arrests on high school

dropout. Sociology of Education 82:368–93.

Ho, Daniel E., Kosuke Imai, Gary King, and Elizabeth A. Stuart. 2007. Matching as

nonparametric preprocessing for reducing model dependence in parametric causal

inference. Political Analysis 15:199–236.

Huizinga, David, and Kim L. Henry. 2008. The effect of arrest and justice system sanctions on

subsequent behavior: Findings from longitudinal and other studies. In The Long View of

Crime. ed. A.M. Liberman. New York, NY: Springer Science+Business Media, LLC.

Huizinga, David, Karl Schumann, Beate Ehret, and Amanda Elliott. 2003. The effect of juvenile

justice system processing on subsequent delinquent and criminal behavior: A cross-

national study. Final report to The National Institute of Justice, Grant Number

199IJCX0037.

Jussim, Lee, and Kent D. Harber. 2005. Teacher expectations and self-fulfilling prophecies:

Knowns and unknowns, resolved and unresolved controversies. Personality and Social

Psychology Review 9: 131–155.


First Juvenile Arrests 35

Keels, Micere. 2008. Second-generation effects of Chicago’s Gautreaux residential mobility

program on children’s participation in crime. Journal of Research on Adolescence 18:305–

52.

Kirk, David S. 2008. The neighborhood context of racial and ethnic disparities in arrest.

Demography 45:55–77.

Kirk, David S. 2009. A natural experiment on residential change and recidivism: Lessons from

Hurricane Katrina. American Sociological Review 74:484–505.

Kirk, David S. 2012. Residential change as a turning point in the life course of crime: Desistance

or temporary cessation? Criminology 50:329–58.

Kirk, David S., and Robert J. Sampson. 2013. Juvenile arrest and collateral educational damage

in the transition to adulthood. Sociology of Education 86:36–62.

Klein, Malcolm W. 1986. Labeling theory and delinquency policy: An experimental test.

Criminal Justice and Behavior 13:47–79.

Kupchik, Aaron. 2010. Homeroom Security: School Discipline in an Age of Fear. New York:

New York University.

Laub, John H., and Robert J. Sampson. 2003. Shared Beginnings, Divergent Lives: Delinquent

Boys to Age 70. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Lemert, Edwin M. 1951. Social Pathology: A Systematic Approach to the Theory of Sociopathic

Behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill.

Link, Bruce, Francis Cullen, Elmer Struening, Patrick Shrout, and Bruce Dohrenwend. 1989. A

modified labeling approach to mental disorders: An empirical assessment. American

Sociological Review 54:400–23.

Lopes, Giza, Marvin D. Krohn, Alan J. Lizotte, Nicole M. Schmidt, Bob E. Vasquez, and Jon
First Juvenile Arrests 36

Gunnar Bernberg. 2012. Labeling and cumulative disadvantage: The impact of formal

police intervention on life chance and crime during emerging adulthood. Crime and

Delinquency 58:456–88.

Mantel, Nathan, and William Haenszel. 1959. Statistical aspects of the analysis of data from

retrospective studies of disease. Journal of the National Cancer Institute 22:719–48.

Matsueda, Ross L. 1992. Reflected appraisal, parental labeling, and delinquency: Specifying a

symbolic interactionist theory. American Journal of Sociology 97:1577–611.

McAra, Lesley, and Susan McVie. 2007. Youth justice? The impact of system contact on

patterns of desistance. European Journal of Criminology 4:315–45.

Morgan, Stephen L., and Christopher Winship. 2007. Counterfactuals and Causal Inference:

Methods and Principles for Social Research. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Morris, Robert G., and Alex R. Piquero. 2013. For whom do sanctions deter and label? Justice

Quarterly 30:837–68.

National Conference of State Legislatures. 2012. Trends in Juvenile Justice State Legislation:

2001-2011. Washington, DC: National Conference of State Legislatures

Pager, Devah. 2003. The mark of a criminal record. American Journal of Sociology 108:937–75.

Paternoster, Raymond, Robert Brame, Paul Mazerolle, and Alex Piquero. 1998. Using the correct

statistical rest for the equality of regression coefficients. Criminology 36:859–866.

Paternoster, Raymond, and Leeann Iovanni. 1989. The labeling perspective and delinquency: An

elaboration of the theory and assessment of the evidence. Justice Quarterly 6:359–94.

Petrosino, A., C. Turpin-Petrosino, and S. Guckenburg. 2010. Formal system processing of

juveniles: Effects on delinquency. Campbell Systematic Reviews 2010:1.

Porterfield, Austin L. 1943. Delinquency and outcome in court and college. American Journal of
First Juvenile Arrests 37

Sociology 49:199–208.

Raudenbush, Stephen W. 1984. Magnitude of teacher expectancy effects on pupil IQ as a

function of the credibility of expectancy induction: A synthesis of findings from 18

experiments. Journal of Educational Psychology 76: 85–97.

Rios, Victor. 2011. Punished: Policing the Lives of Black and Latino Boys. New York: NYU

Press.

Rosenbaum, Paul R. 2002. Observational Studies. New York: Springer.

Rosenbaum, Paul R. 2010. Design of Observational Studies. New York: Springer.

Rosenbaum, Paul R., and Donald B. Rubin.1983. The central role of the propensity score in

observational studies for causal effects. Biometrika 70:41–55.

Rosenthal, Robert, and Lenore Jacobson. 1968. Pygmalion in the classroom. New York: Holt,

Rinehart & Winston.

Sampson, Robert J., and John H. Laub. 1997. A Life-Course Theory of Cumulative

Disadvantage and the Stability of Delinquency. In Developmental Theories of Crime and

Delinquency, Advances in Criminological Theory, Volume 7, ed. T. P. Thornberry. New

Brunswick, NJ: Transaction.

Sampson, Robert J., Stephen W. Raudenbush, and Felton Earls. 1997. Neighborhoods and

violent crime: A multilevel study of collective efficacy. Science 277:918–24.

Schur, Edwin W. 1973. Radical Non-intervention: Rethinking the Delinquency Problem.

Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Schwartz, Richard, and Jerome Skolnick. 1962. Two studies of legal stigma. Social Problems

10:133–42.

Short, James F. Jr. 1958. Extent of unrecorded juvenile delinquency: Tentative conclusions.
First Juvenile Arrests 38

Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology 49:296–302.

Short, James F. Jr., and F. Ivan Nye. 1957. Reported behavior as a criterion of deviant behavior.

Social Problems 5:207–13.

Simon, Jonathan. 2007. Governing through Crime: How the War on Crime Transformed

American Democracy and Created a Culture of Fear. New York: Oxford University

Press.

Smith, Douglas A., and Patrick R. Gartin. 1989. Specifying specific deterrence: The influence of

arrest on future criminal activity. American Sociological Review 54:94–106.

Snyder, Howard N., and Melissa Sickmund. 2006. Juvenile Offenders and Victims: 2006

National Report. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice, Office of Justice

Programs, Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention.

Stuart, Elizabeth A. 2010. Matching methods for causal inference: A review and a look forward.

Statistical Science 25:1–21.

Stucki, Bryce. 2013. Reversing Broward County’s School-to-Prison Pipeline. American

Prospect, 4 December. Available: http://prospect.org/article/reversing-broward-countys-

school-prison-pipeline

Sweeten, Gary. 2006. Who will graduate? Disruption of high school education by arrest and

court involvement. Justice Quarterly 23:462–80.

Tannenbaum, Frank. 1938. Crime and the Community. New York and London: Columbia

University Press.

Uggen, Christopher. 2000. Work as a turning point in the life course of criminals: A duration

model of age, employment, and recidivism. American Sociological Review 65:529–46.

U.S. Department of Education. 2014. Guiding Principles: A Resource Guide for Improving
First Juvenile Arrests 39

School Climate and Discipline. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education.

Available: http://www2.ed.gov/policy/gen/guid/school-discipline/guiding-principles.pdf

U.S. Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention. 2012. OJJDP Statistical Briefing

Book. Online. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice, Office of Justice Programs,

Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention. Available:

http://www.ojjdp.gov/ojstatbb/crime/JAR_Display.asp?ID=qa05201.

van Buuren, S., J. P. L. Brand, C. G. M. Groothuis-Oudshoorn, and D. B. Rubin. 2006. Fully

conditional specification in multivariate imputation. Journal of Statistical Computation

and Simulation 76: 1049–1064.

Wallerstein, J. S., and C. J. Wylie. 1947. Our law-abiding law-breakers. Probation 25:107–12.

Wiley, Stephanie A., and Finn-Aage Esbensen. In press. The effect of police contact: Does

official intervention result in deviance amplification? Crime and Delinquency.

Wiley, Stephanie A., Lee Ann Slocum, and Finn-Aage Esbensen. 2013. The unintended

consequences of being stopped and arrested: An exploration of the labeling mechanisms

through which police contact leads to subsequent delinquency. Criminology 51:927–66.


First Juvenile Arrests 40

Table 1. Differences between Arrestees and Non-arrestees, Before and after Matching: Individual-level Factors.
UNMATCHED MATCHED
Not % Not %
Wave 1 Variable: Arrested Arrested
Arrested absolute Arrested absolute
Individual Factors Mean Mean
Mean t-test sig bias Mean t-test sig bias
SRO & Aggression
Violent offending 0.60 0.15 9.30 * 84.9 0.45 0.42 0.17 4.5
Property offending 0.22 0.09 4.54 * 45.3 0.15 0.16 -0.15 3.4
Drug selling 0.14 0.02 5.78 * 41.4 0.07 0.06 0.10 2.0
Public order offending 0.21 0.11 2.68 * 32.2 0.18 0.26 -0.94 26.0
Status offending 0.64 0.12 10.13 * 97.9 0.42 0.47 -0.38 9.1
Variety score 4.28 1.80 7.42 * 82.5 3.50 3.40 0.13 3.2
Delinquent score (Achenbach) 4.98 3.24 4.34 * 56.1 4.34 3.97 0.53 12.1
Aggressive score (Achenbach) 9.83 8.83 1.16 15.0 9.61 9.74 -0.08 2.0
Temperament
Inhibitory control 2.80 2.41 2.94 * 39.1 2.68 2.68 0.01 0.1
Impulsivity 3.19 2.98 1.87 25.8 3.06 3.01 0.25 6.0
Sensation seeking 3.07 2.71 3.33 * 47.7 2.93 2.84 0.52 12.5
Activeness 3.80 3.57 1.77 26.2 3.70 3.47 1.12 26.5
Emotionality 2.81 2.67 0.93 12.9 2.74 2.58 0.64 14.5
Sociability 3.69 3.64 0.48 7.3 3.66 3.61 0.28 6.6
Shyness 2.25 2.48 -1.91 28.5 2.17 2.47 -1.64 35.6
School & Education
Ever repeated grade 0.22 0.15 1.30 18.3 0.25 0.18 0.64 16.8
Ever remediation class 0.33 0.29 0.58 8.6 0.31 0.32 -0.03 0.8
School truancy 0.50 0.16 4.37 * 50.1 0.30 0.45 -0.81 22.1
IQ (standardized) 96.89 100.78 -1.83 28.8 97.86 98.94 -0.34 8.0
school mobility 3.13 2.48 3.69 * 49.9 2.82 2.74 0.27 5.7
Drug Use
days marij. last month 1.30 1.11 2.05 * 23.4 1.21 1.12 0.74 10.8
First Juvenile Arrests 41

days cigarettes last month 1.64 1.22 3.07 * 33.5 1.58 1.43 0.47 11.7
days alcohol last month 1.25 1.12 1.88 25.3 1.24 1.12 0.93 23.2
Demographics
Gender 0.72 0.45 3.75 * 55.1 0.66 0.63 0.28 6.4
Age 0.51 -0.55 5.00 * 75.1 0.27 0.07 0.58 13.9
African American 0.64 0.34 4.57 * 63.7 0.63 0.66 -0.24 5.5
Mexican 0.17 0.32 -2.32 * 35.7 0.18 0.18 0.00 0.0
Puerto Rican or other Hispanic 0.08 0.14 -1.28 19.9 0.08 0.10 -0.27 5.7
Other race 0.06 0.03 0.89 11.0 0.03 0.02 0.26 4.2
1st generation immigrant 0.09 0.15 -1.07 16.2 0.11 0.07 0.53 10.8
2nd generation immigrant 0.15 0.31 -2.42 * 37.7 0.13 0.10 0.48 8.5
N 53 951 38 111
* significant at p<.05.
First Juvenile Arrests 42

Table 2. Differences between Arrestees and Non-arrestees, Before and after Matching: Family Factors.
UNMATCHED MATCHED
Not Not %
Wave 1 Variable: Arrested Arrested
Arrested Arrested absolute
Family Factors Mean Mean
Mean t-test Mean t-test bias
Household Composition
Parents married 0.23 0.54 -4.45 * 67.3 0.24 0.24 0.00 0.0
Extended family in household 0.26 0.19 1.28 17.1 0.21 0.31 -0.95 22.9
# children 3.55 3.36 0.77 10.0 3.29 3.24 0.10 2.5
Single parent 0.45 0.31 2.17 * 29.5 0.47 0.54 -0.53 12.7
SES & Residence
Years current address (subject) 5.78 5.87 -0.14 1.8 5.22 6.88 -1.37 33.5
Years current address (primary
caregiver) 7.78 6.98 0.76 9.8 6.64 8.67 -1.09 24.9
Caregiver occupational status (SEI) 41.42 42.29 -0.35 5.2 41.26 39.23 0.56 12.1
Caregiver education 3.08 3.04 0.21 2.9 3.03 2.92 0.37 8.1
Household income 3.79 4.25 -1.73 25.7 3.82 3.85 -0.09 2.0
SES composite -0.22 -0.06 -0.81 11.9 -0.24 -0.34 0.34 7.2
Home Interior physical envir. -0.25 0.05 -1.14 16.0 -0.24 -0.36 0.30 6.8
Home exterior physical envir. -0.28 0.04 -1.72 25.4 -0.21 -0.52 1.04 24.3
Parent Risk
Father criminal involv. 0.09 0.11 -0.45 6.6 0.11 0.14 -0.46 11.4
Father substance use 0.21 0.14 1.29 16.9 0.24 0.26 -0.26 6.9
Mother substance use 0.08 0.05 0.88 11.2 0.05 0.11 -0.96 25.4
Mother depression 0.19 0.14 0.94 12.5 0.16 0.09 0.92 18.8
Family and Parenting Processes
Family supervision -0.45 -0.05 -3.67 * 47.6 -0.29 -0.13 -0.86 18.3
Parent-child conflict 0.27 -0.07 3.21 * 41.2 0.19 0.25 -0.29 7.2
Family conflict 51.08 47.48 2.49 * 32.5 49.24 50.00 -0.32 6.9
Disciplined child-rearing 58.36 58.31 0.04 0.5 58.26 58.78 -0.29 6.1
First Juvenile Arrests 43

Religiosity score 62.77 60.44 2.18 * 35.8 62.94 62.22 0.53 11.2
Family support -0.12 0.01 -1.14 15.7 0.08 -0.06 0.70 16.0
Lack of hostility (primary caregiver) -0.15 0.34 -0.79 11.9 -0.07 -0.26 0.22 4.5
Developmental Environment
Access to reading -0.38 -0.03 -1.32 17.9 -0.22 -0.07 -0.35 8.0
Developmental stimulation -0.48 -0.07 -2.87 * 36.2 -0.38 -0.03 -1.46 31.5
Family outings -0.10 -0.03 -0.59 7.9 -0.03 -0.09 0.35 7.8
Parental verbal ability -0.26 0.06 -1.28 16.6 -0.01 0.03 -0.11 2.2
Parental warmth -0.55 0.04 -2.53 * 33.8 -0.48 -0.50 0.04 0.8
N 38 951 38 111
* significant at p< .05.
First Juvenile Arrests 44

Table 3. Differences between Arrestees and Non-arrestees, Before and after Matching: Peer and Neighborhood Factors.
UNMATCHED MATCHED
Not Not %
Arrested Arrested
Wave 1 Variable Arrested Arrested absolute
Mean Mean
Mean t-test Mean t-test bias
Peer Factors
Peer pressure 0.47 -0.06 3.55 * 46.6 0.47 0.50 -0.10 2.5
Peer attachment to school 0.11 -0.01 1.98 * 25.7 0.04 0.00 0.37 7.6
Friend support 0.06 0.06 0.04 0.6 0.07 0.02 0.39 9.3
Deviance of peers 0.55 -0.05 5.33 * 72.0 0.42 0.35 0.37 8.3
Peer attachment -0.18 0.07 -2.55 * 34.3 -0.09 -0.21 0.68 16.9
Neighborhood Factors
Legal cynicism 2.55 2.50 2.73 * 33.3 2.53 2.55 -0.71 17.3
Tolerance of deviance 4.24 4.24 0.14 2.1 4.24 4.25 -0.25 5.6
Perceived social disorder 2.13 2.00 2.54 * 36.2 2.09 2.13 -0.46 10.4
Perceived physical disorder 1.74 1.65 2.34 * 31.4 1.71 1.75 -0.56 12.8
Neighborhood organizations -0.29 -0.41 1.53 22.1 -0.30 -0.25 -0.44 9.9
Services for youth -1.63 -1.74 1.08 14.7 -1.68 -1.55 -0.78 18.0
Collective efficacy 3.84 3.89 -1.49 20.4 3.87 3.84 0.47 10.6
Residential stability 0.06 -0.01 0.46 6.3 0.13 0.03 0.43 10.2
Resident victimization last 6 mos. 0.39 0.42 -1.25 18.1 0.36 0.37 -0.17 3.6
% foreign born 13.44 21.28 -3.66 * 52.4 13.03 12.37 0.20 4.4
Concentrated poverty 0.28 -0.14 4.12 * 52.0 0.22 0.44 -0.97 26.7
Concentrated affluence -0.28 -0.21 -0.81 11.7 -0.26 -0.32 0.35 7.8
1995 official violent crime 9.20 8.85 3.98 * 57.3 9.11 9.27 -1.05 25.2
% black 55.61 31.37 4.60 * 62.4 53.95 57.79 -0.40 9.9
% Latino 22.13 32.11 -2.41 * 34.3 20.91 20.91 0.00 0.0
N 53 951 38 111
* significant at p< .05.
First Juvenile Arrests 45

Table 4. Rosenbaum Bounds, Effect of First Arrest


Future Arrest Violent Offending
Γ Q+ p-value Q+ p-value

1.00 4.320 <.001 2.550 0.005


1.25 3.720 <.001 1.992 0.023
1.50 3.236 0.001 1.533 0.063
1.75 2.836 0.002 1.148 0.125
2.00 2.496 0.006 0.817 0.207
2.25 2.200 0.014 0.526 0.299
2.50 1.938 0.026 0.266 0.395
2.75 1.703 0.044 0.031 0.488
3.00 1.490 0.068 -0.183 0.573

Note: Γ refers to the odds ratio of the effect of unobserved variables on the likelihood of first

arrest for youths who were arrested versus youths who were not arrested.
First Juvenile Arrests 46

Table 5. Effects of First Arrest on Rearrest


Model 1 Model 2 Model 3
Coef SE OR t sig Coef SE OR t sig Coef SE OR t sig
First Arrest 1.60 0.41 4.97 3.92 *** 1.63 0.41 5.10 3.98 *** 1.40 0.55 4.06 2.55 *
Wave 3 Violent
Offending -0.21 0.44 0.81 -0.48 -0.58 0.54 0.56 -1.07
Arrest x Offending 0.55 0.81 1.73 0.68
Constant -1.33 0.27 0.26 -4.87 *** -1.24 0.32 0.29 -3.86 *** -1.11 0.33 0.33 -3.41 ***
N 581 581 581

ABBREVIATIONS: Coeff = b coefficient; SE = standard error; OR = odds ratio; sig = significance.

Significance: *** p < .001; ** p <.01; * p < .05; ~ p<.10 (two-tailed tests).
First Juvenile Arrests 47

Table A-1. Alternative Specifications


Means Tests Absolute Bias
Treatment
Diff N On
Specification Arrestees Controls or OR SE t or z Sig Support Mean Median
Arrest (logit)
NN, 3:1, caliper 0.02 0.49 0.16 5.20 1.12 4.63 *** 43/58 6.9 4.8
NN, 1:1, caliper 0.02 0.48 0.14 5.45 1.74 3.14 ** 42/58 11.8 9.5
NN, 3:1, caliper 0.04 0.48 0.14 5.41 1.11 4.86 *** 46/58 7.5 6.8
KM, BW = .06 0.51 0.13 7.18 1.33 5.41 *** 49/58 8.7 6.8
KM, BW = .10 0.51 0.12 7.45 1.31 5.68 *** 49/58 12.1 10.7
Variety (continuous)
NN, 3:1, caliper 0.02 2.03 1.04 0.98 0.40 2.44 * 38/53 11.1 8.5
NN, 1:1, caliper 0.02 1.89 0.94 0.94 0.44 2.13 * 36/53 13.7 11.6
NN, 3:1, caliper 0.04 1.88 1.18 0.70 0.39 1.79 + 41/53 11.5 10.0
KM, BW = .06 1.84 1.62 0.22 0.37 0.58 44/53 12.0 10.5
KM, BW = .10 1.84 1.41 0.43 0.35 1.21 44/53 14.1 12.6
Violence (logit)
NN, 3:1, caliper 0.02 0.58 0.30 3.24 1.24 2.61 ** 38/53 11.1 8.5
NN, 1:1, caliper 0.02 0.56 0.28 3.25 1.53 2.12 * 36/53 13.7 11.6
NN, 3:1, caliper 0.04 0.54 0.34 2.23 1.28 1.75 + 41/53 11.5 10.0
KM, BW = .06 0.52 0.37 1.86 1.21 1.53 44/53 12.0 10.5
KM, BW = .10 0.52 0.33 2.26 1.04 2.17 * 44/53 14.1 12.6
Property (logit)
NN, 3:1, caliper 0.02 0.29 0.16 2.17 1.26 1.72 + 38/53 11.1 8.5
NN, 1:1, caliper 0.02 0.28 0.11 3.08 1.65 1.87 + 36/53 13.7 11.6
NN, 3:1, caliper 0.04 0.27 0.15 2.14 1.24 1.72 + 41/53 11.5 10.0
KM, BW = .06 0.27 0.19 1.63 1.37 1.19 44/53 12.0 10.5
KM, BW = .10 0.27 0.18 1.67 1.24 1.34 44/53 14.1 12.6
Drug Selling (logit)
NN, 3:1, caliper 0.02 0.13 0.09 1.49 2.53 0.59 38/53 11.1 8.5
NN, 1:1, caliper 0.02 0.11 0.08 1.38 3.72 0.37 36/53 13.7 11.6
NN, 3:1, caliper 0.04 0.12 0.11 1.18 4.90 0.24 41/53 11.5 10.0
KM, BW = .06 0.11 0.16 0.66 -1.10 -0.60 44/53 12.0 10.5
KM, BW = .10 0.11 0.12 0.91 -6.06 -0.15 44/53 14.1 12.6
NOTES: Matching for SRO outcomes utilizes 53 arrestees and 951 nonarrestees; matching with
arrest outcomes utilizes 58 arrestees and 1191 nonarrestees. Binary outcomes estimated via logit
models with a Z-test of significance; continuous outcomes estimated via psmatch2 with a t-test
of significance.
ABBREVIATIONS: NN = Nearest Neighbor; KM = Kernel Matching; BW = bandwidth; sig =
significance.
+p < .10; * p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p < .001 (two-tailed tests).
-- 47 --
First Juvenile Arrests 48

Figure 1. The Distribution of Propensity Scores of First Arrest, By Treatment Status.


Frequency

0 .2 .4 .6 .8 1
Propensity Score

Nonarrestee Arrestee: Matched Arrestee: Unmatched

-- 48 --
First Juvenile Arrests 49

Figure 2: The Effect of First Arrest of the Probability of Subsequent Offending and Rearrest,
Individually Matched Arrested and Non-Arrested Youths

-- 49 --
First Juvenile Arrests 50

APPENDIX A: METHODOLOGY

BOUNDS FOR THE EFFECT OF ARREST ON RE-ARREST

We use Rosenbaum’s (2002) bounding approach to examine the sensitivity of our

propensity-matched inferences to hidden biases (see also Becker and Caliendo, 2007; DiPrete

and Gangl, 2004). This approach allows us to determine how strongly an omitted confounding

variable must influence selection into treatment to undermine our inferences about the causal

effect of arrest. If there is hidden bias, then two individuals with the same observed

characteristics will have differing likelihoods of being arrested because of unobserved factors.

The odds that an individual will receive treatment is given by the following:

Pr( Arrest  1)
 exp(    X   U )
1  Pr( Arrest  1) ,

where X represents observed variables and U represents unobserved variables. In this

case, the variable U increases the probability of arrest by a factor equal to γ. For a pair of

individuals i and j matched on propensity score (i.e., the same observed covariates X), where i is

ultimately arrested and j is not, the ratio of odds of receiving treatment is given by:

Pi
1  Pi exp(    X  U i )

i

Pj exp(    X j
 U j )
1  Pj

Because i and j have the same set of observed covariates, X cancels out:

exp(  U i )

 exp  U i
U j

exp(  U j )

If there are no differences in unobserved variables (Ui = Uj for all matched pairs) or if

unobserved variables have no influence on the probability of arrest (γ=0), then there is no hidden

-- 50 --
First Juvenile Arrests 51

bias. Since we do not have direct information on unobservables, we use a sensitivity analysis to

evaluate whether our statistical inferences pertaining to the effect of arrest on subsequent arrest

would change under different values of γ. Per Rosenbaum (2002), the bounds on the odds ratio

that either of the two matched individuals will receive treatment is given by:

1 Pi 1  P j  
  e
P j 1  Pi 

e
,

where Γ=exp(γ). Use of this bounding approach is suitable if pairwise matching is done

without replacement (Becker and Caliendo, 2007).

We use the mhbounds routine in Stata to implement our sensitivity analysis. The

mhbounds command uses the Mantel and Haenszel (MH; 1959) test statistic. The Q+ test-

statistic adjusts the MH statistic downward in the event of positive unobserved selection. Positive

selection occurs when arrested individuals are more likely to be arrested again in the future for

reasons other than their prior arrest. In this case, we would overestimate the treatment effect of

arrest.

PROPENSITY SCORE WEIGHTS

Rather than using propensity scores for matching, for our analysis of the effect of arrest

on future arrest net of offending, we use propensity scores as inverse weights in order to estimate

the average treatment effect on the treated (Hirano and Imbens, 2001; Hirano, Imbens, and

Ridder, 2003; Stuart, 2010). In equation form, the weight equals:

eˆ i
w i  T i  (1  T i )
1  eˆ i
,

ê i
where is the estimated propensity score for individual i. Per this formula, treated individuals

(i.e., arrestees) receive a propensity weight equal to one. Control individuals with greater

-- 51 --
First Juvenile Arrests 52

propensity scores receive a larger weight. One particular advantage of using propensity score

weights is that we can use the weights in a regression model that includes covariate adjustment.

In our case, per Hypothesis 3, we are interested in estimating the effect of arrest on subsequent

arrest after adjusting for the extent of criminal offending at Wave 3 of the PHDCN.

ALTERNATIVE PROPENSITY SCORE SPECIFICATIONS

Along with the 3:1 nearest neighbor (NN) matching approach already described, we

estimated effects through four other propensity-score-matching specifications. We used two

alternative specifications with NN matching: an expanded caliper (0.04) in 3:1 matching, and 1:1

matching with caliper=0.02. We also explored two kernel matching (KM) specifications. In

contrast to NN, which uses just a few observations from a potential control group to create a

counterfactual, KM uses a weighted average of all non-arrestees to construct the counterfactual

outcome (Caliendo and Kopeinig, 2008; Stuart, 2010). One advantage of kernel matching is

reduced variance, because more observations and therefore more information are used to

construct the counterfactual. The tradeoff for reduced variance is an increase in bias. KM has

similarities with the inverse probability of treatment weighting previously described, with the

main distinction being the type of weighting employed (i.e., kernel weighting versus an inverse

of the probability).

Results are shown in Table A-1. The approach already described (3:1 nearest neighbor

matching, caliper = 0.02) was the most efficient in removing bias. At the same time it was more

stringent than most of the other specifications on matching, so that 38 of 53 arrestees with SRO

data were matched (a.k.a., "on support"), and 43 of 58 with future arrest data. The other

specifications were somewhat less efficient in reducing bias, but were more liberal in retaining

matching controls to arrestees.

-- 52 --

You might also like