You are on page 1of 12

European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry

10:67±78 (2001) Ó Steinkopff Verlag 2001 ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION

D. A. Allen Autistic disorder versus other pervasive


M. Steinberg
M. Dunn developmental disorders in young children:
D. Fein
C. Feinstein same or different?
L. Waterhouse
I. Rapin

Accepted: 30 August 2000


j Abstract Eighteen preschool ``high'' and ``low'' cognitive sub-
children diagnosed according to groups of AD, but not of PDD-
the Diagnostic and Statistical NOS, differed substantially on
D. A. Allen Manual of Mental Disorders most measures, with the children
Division of Child Psychiatry Third Edition Revised (DSM III- with lower cognitive scores sig-
Department of Psychiatry R) as having Pervasive Develop- ni®cantly more impaired on all
Albert Einstein College of Medicine mental Disorder-Not Otherwise measures. Similarity of PDD-NOS
Bronx NY, USA
Speci®ed (PDD-NOS) were com- children to AD children in mal-
M. Steinberg pared to 176 children with DSM adaptive behaviors and an inter-
Department of Psychology III-R Autistic Disorder (AD), and mediate position between autistic
Marymount College, Tarrytown NY, USA
to 311 non-autistic children with and non-autistic groups on vir-
M. Dunn á I. Rapin (&) developmental language disorders tually all measures explains the
Department of Neurology (DLD) (N = 201) or low IQ dif®culty clinicians encounter in
Rose F. Kennedy Center for Research (N = 110). All children were par- classifying children with PDD and
in Mental Retardation and Human
Development titioned into ``high'' and ``low'' raises questions about the speci-
Albert Einstein College of Medicine cognitive subgroups at a nonver- ®city of these diagnostic subtypes
1300 Morris Park Avenue bal IQ of 80. Within cognitive of the autistic spectrum.
Bronx NY 10461, USA subgroups, the 18 PDD-NOS
D. Fein children did not differ signi®-
Department of Psychology cantly from either the DLD or the
University of Connecticut AD children in verbal and adap-
Storrs CT, USA
tive skills and obtained scores
C. Feinstein intermediate between those of
Division of Child Psychiatry these groups. The PDD-NOS did
Department of Psychiatry
Stanford University Medical School not differ from the AD children
Stanford CA, USA in maladaptive behaviors. Both
the PDD-NOS and AD children
L. Waterhouse
Child Behavior Study
had many more of these behav- j Key words Autism ± sub-
The College of New Jersey iors than the non-autistic com- groups ± diagnosis ± speci®city ±
Trenton NJ, USA parison groups. Children in the cognition

DSM IV The fourth (1994) edition of the DSM


Abbreviations AD Autistic disorder according to the DSM
DSM Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of III-R or DSM IV criteria
Mental Disorders of the American Psy- PDD Pervasive developmental disorder. Refers
chiatric Association to the entire spectrum of disorders with
ECAP 0213

DSM III The third (1980) edition of the DSM autistic symptomatology
DSM III-R The revision (1987) of DSM III
68 European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, Vol. 10, No. 1 (2001)
Ó Steinkopff Verlag 2001

PDD-NOS Pervasive developmental disorder-not which corresponds quite closely to DSM IV), used
otherwise speci®ed. Refers to children number and distribution of behavioral descriptors as
with PDD who do not ful®ll DSM criteria criteria for discriminating classical autism (autistic
for one of the other disorders on the disorder-AD) from less typically affected individuals.
spectrum The present study examines the validity of the
distinction between DSM III-R AD and Pervasive
Developmental Disorder-Not Otherwise Speci®ed
Non-standard abbreviations (PDD-NOS), the only other disorder on the autistic
spectrum retained in DSM III-R. In a previous
DLD Developmental language disorder, a investigation of the same children in the present
group of children (N = 201) selected study, we showed that the number of children
according to uniform criteria captured by the descriptors in the subsequent DSM
H-PDD-NOS High PDD, a group of 9 PDD-NOS editions varied greatly, a sign of their overlapping
children according to DSM III-R criteria boundaries (37). Biological considerations regarding
who had nonverbal IQs of 80 or more the variable behavioral phenotypes to be expected in
HAD High AD, a group of AD children largely genetically caused complex developmental
(N = 51) selected according to DSM disorders (18) legitimizes the fuzzy boundaries found
III-R criteria who had nonverbal IQs of in that study.
80 or more The term PDD-NOS has been retained in DSM IV
L-PDD-NOS Low PDD, a group of 9 PDD-NOS and ICD 10, without speci®ed diagnostic criteria, for
children according to DSM III-R who individuals who do not ®t other subtypes of PDD.
had nonverbal IQs below 80 There has been widespread discussion about the
LAD Low AD, a group of AD children meaningfulness of diagnostic boundaries between
(N = 125) selected according to DSM Autistic Disorder and other so-called ``non-autistic
III-R criteria who had nonverbal IQs PDDs'' (36), but few systematic comparative studies
below 80 of the similarities and differences between DSM III-R
NALIQ Non-autistic low IQ, a group of non- AD and PDD-NOS in well-de®ned populations (21,
autistic children (N = 110) whose non- 26). The purpose of the present study was to
verbal IQ was below 80 determine whether preschool children captured by a
NVIQ Nonverbal IQ equivalent (see text) uniform instrument, the Wing Autistic Disorder
WADIC Wing Autistic Disorder Interview Interview Checklist (WADIC) ((38) in (24), Appendix
Checklist 1), and interviewed by experienced psychiatrists using
DSM III-R criteria (DSM IV was not available when
the study was carried out) as having AD differed in
Introduction kind from those with PDD-NOS when the groups were
matched for nonverbal IQ. Comparisons were also
Autism, a behaviorally-de®ned disorder with many made with two non-autistic contrast groups screened
biologic causes, is quite possibly the most widely for an autistic spectrum disorder with the WADIC:
investigated developmental disorder in this age group. children with developmental language disorders and
It is indexed in infancy or very early childhood by those with low IQ uncomplicated by autism. We
severe impairments in three behavioral domains: hypothesized that there would be substantial overlaps
sociability; language, communication and play; and between PDD-NOS and AD subgroups matched for
range of interests and activities. Despite extensive nonverbal IQ and that both of them would differ
research and clinical interest in autism, there is still a substantially from their non-autistic nonverbal IQ-
lack of agreement as to its diagnostic boundaries and matched counterparts.
its relationships with other disorders manifest in early
childhood.
The third edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Methods
Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM III) (2) of the
American Psychiatric Association introduced the j Subjects
umbrella term Pervasive Developmental Disorder
(PDD) to encompass the broad range of individuals The children in the present study were originally
with de®cits in these three behavioral domains. This recruited between 1985 and 1988 for a large multi-
and subsequent editions (DSM III-R (3) and DSM IV center program project, Nosology of Higher Cerebral
(4), as well as in the International Classi®cation of Function Disorders in Children (24). The purpose of
Diseases, the most recent tenth edition (ICD 10 (39) of the parent project was to devise an internally valid
D. A. Allen et al. 69
Autistic disorder versus other pervasive

classi®cation system for developmentally handicapped children with two or more checks on the WADIC were
preschool children uniformly selected and assessed. interviewed by an experienced child psychiatrist at
The study included 194 children diagnosed as having each site who had been trained to reliability at the
DSM III-R PDD, and a comparison cohort of 311 non- inception of the study, using both live children and
autistic developmentally disordered children (201 videotapes, to apply DSM III and DSM III-R criteria to
children with developmental language disorders endorse or exclude a diagnosis of PDD. These psychi-
[DLD] and 110 non-autistic children with a low IQ atrists also determined whether the children ful®lled
[NALIQ]). Methodological details of the parent project DSM III criteria for Infantile Autism or DSM III-R
are described in Rapin (24). The study was not criteria for AD, or another PDD diagnosis. Doubtful
designed as an epidemiological study, as children cases were brought up at periodic meetings of the
were selected speci®cally to meet certain inclusionary investigators to resolve uncertain diagnoses.
criteria, in particular criteria for DSM III-R AD. Of the 194 referred to the project 176 children
Children who did not meet inclusionary criteria were (91%) (147 boys [83.5%] and 29 girls [16.5%]) were
either reclassi®ed if they met inclusionary criteria for classi®ed by a child psychiatrist as having AD
another group or were excluded from the study (see according to DSM III-R criteria. The other 18 (14
(24) for a detailed discussion of these issues). boys [77.8%] and four girls [22.2%]) failed to meet
Children were recruited 1) by clinical referral to the DSM III-R criteria for AD, but did meet DSM III-R
clinicians/researchers among the investigators, and 2) criteria for PDD-NOS. None of the 18 PDD-NOS
by solicited participation of schools and programs for children met DSM III criteria for Infantile Autism but
special needs children. Recruitment occurred at ®ve were classi®ed as having some other form of PDD (see
different sites where the investigators' institutions were Table 1). These 18 children were excluded from the
located: Boston, Massachusetts; Bronx, New York; parent study but are the subjects of the present study.
Manhasset, New York; Cleveland, Ohio; and Trenton, In order to match the nonverbal IQs of children
New Jersey. with PDD to those of the DLD and NALIQ contrast
Exclusionary criteria for all of the children in the groups, the 176 subjects in the AD group were divided
study were a) a hearing threshold worse than 25 dB at into two sub-groups: 1) AD children with a nonverbal
any frequency in either ear; b) a family in which IQ equivalent of 80 or more were called the high
English was not the primary language; c) a major autistic disorder (HAD) group (N = 51, 29%); 2) AD
neurologic problem or structurally de®ned brain children with a nonverbal IQ equivalent below 80 were
lesion resulting in a gross motor de®cit or frequent called the low autistic disorder LAD group (N = 125,
seizures requiring high doses of anticonvulsants, or 79%).1 The contrast groups consisted of 311 non-
an identi®ed condition, such as tuberous sclerosis or autistic children: 201 children with developmental
Rett syndrome; d) a craniofacial anomaly; or e) high language disorders (DLD), all of whom had nonverbal
doses of behavior-altering medications (less that 15% IQ equivalents of 80 or above, and 110 non-autistic
of children in any group were taking behavior mentally de®cient (NALIQ) children, all of whom had
modifying drugs at the time of testing, methylphen- nonverbal IQ equivalents of less than 80. None of the
idate in virtually all cases). Inclusionary criteria for DLD or NALIQ children ful®lled criteria for AD or
the contrast groups are fully described in Rapin (24). PDD-NOS, either because they had less than two
behaviors endorsed on the WADIC or because the
psychiatrist determined that they did not ful®ll DSM
Inclusionary criteria for the PDD cohort
III-R criteria for these disorders.
The children screened for inclusion in the PDD cohort For purposes of comparisons with the subjects
had been identi®ed by a clinical or educational from the parent project, the 18 PDD-NOS subjects in
professional as exhibiting de®cits in socialization and the present study were likewise divided into the same
behavior of the sort described in DSM III-R or other two nonverbal IQ groups. The PDD-NOS group was
published autism descriptors. The parents of all the comprised of nine children with NVIQ 80 or above
children referred for possible inclusion in the study, (called the H-PDD-NOS subgroup) who were com-
whether potentially ful®lling criteria for AD, DLD, or pared with the HAD and DLD contrast groups. Nine
NALIQ, ®lled out the 21-item WADIC, a predecessor of with NVIQ below 80 (called the L-PDD-NOS sub-
DSM III-R criteria for AD. It is divided into three areas: group) were compared with the LAD and NALIQ
A) reciprocal social interaction, B) communication, contrast groups.
verbal and nonverbal language, and symbolic develop-
ment, and C) restricted repertoire of behaviors during 1
The somewhat higher number of children in the LAD group than
self-selected activities. To be potentially netted into the the traditionally quoted 2/3 of children with autism and mental
AD cohort, children's parents had to endorse at least de®ciency is attributable to the cut at NVIQ 80 rather than 70 in
three items, one in each area, or two items in area A. All this study.
70 European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, Vol. 10, No. 1 (2001)
Ó Steinkopff Verlag 2001

Table 1 DSM III-R PDD-NOS


Subjects Subject number Sex Age NVIQ WADIC# DSMIIIDx
(mos.) of checks

H-PDD
1 M 54 131 12 APDD
2 M 43 100 6 APDD
3 F 75 99 16 COS
4 M 45 91 12 APDD
5 M 80 88 6 APDD
6 M 50 86 12 APDD
7 M 59 86 8 COPDD
8 M 43 84 8 APDD
9 M 55 82 12 APDD
L-PDD
10 M 61 79 10 APDD
11 M 65 66 11 COPDD
12 M 41 54 10 APDD
13 M 89 51 17 APDD
14 F 79 51 16 APDD
15 M 74 43 14 COPDD
16 M 74 41 8 COPDD
residual state
17 F 45 40 17 APDD
18 F 62 16 12 APDD

Abbreviations: APDD = atypical pervasive developmental disorder; COPDD = childhood-onset PDD; COS = childhood-onset
schizophrenia; DSM-III = Diagnostic and Statistical Manual, 3rd Edition, (APA,1980); DSM-III-R = DSM-III Revised (1987); Dx
= diagnosis; F = female; M = male; mos. = months; NVIQ = nonverbal IQ equivalent (see text); PDD-NOS = pervasive
developmental disorder-not-otherwise-specified; # = number; WADIC = Wing Autistic Disorder Interview Checklist

j Behavioral and cognitive measures the Stanford-Binet, we administered the Bayley Scales
of Infant Development ± Mental Scale (6) and used
We administered to all subjects in the parent project a the Kent scoring of visual-spatial items to derive a
broad battery which included a comprehensive de- nonverbal mental age equivalent. We divided the
velopmental and family history, behavioral ratings, nonverbal mental age from either the Stanford-Binet
standardized neurological and psychiatric examina- or the Bayley by the child's chronological age to
tions, standardized neuropsychological and language derive a nonverbal intelligence ratio score (NVIQ
tests, and assessments of spontaneous language and equivalent).
play abilities (see Rapin (24) for a complete descrip- Verbal comprehension was evaluated with the
tion of these measures). The behavioral measures Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised (11) and
retained for the present study included the scores verbal cognition with the Verbal Reasoning subtest
obtained from the following instruments: 1) the DSM standard scores of the Stanford-Binet 4th Edition.
III and DSM III-R diagnoses assigned by the psychi- Analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were run to
atrists who interviewed the children, 2) the number of compare differences of IQ and behavioral scores of
checks parents assigned on the WADIC (out of a children in each of the six subgroups. Separate Tukey
potential total of 21 checks), 3) standard scores on post-tests were performed to determine the signi®-
three domains (Communication, Daily Living, and cance of differences among means. The relevant
Socialization) as well as the composite score on the comparisons are those between subgroups within
Revised Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales (29), and the ``high'' (NVIQ ³ 80) and low (NVIQ < 80)
4) the total scores assigned on the Social Abnormality cognitive groups, although some comparisons among
Scales I and II which were developed for the parent subgroups across these two cognitive levels are also
project (Allen in (4) Appendix 2). presented.
The cognitive measures retained for the present
study included evaluation of performance and verbal
abilities. The children's level of nonverbal competence Results
was assessed with the Stanford-Binet 4th Edition (31)
Abstract Visual Reasoning standard score. This j Behavioral classification
instrument was selected because it spans ages two
years to adulthood and we planned to study our The gender, age, nonverbal IQ, and psychiatric DSM
sample longitudinally. For 47 children untestable with III classi®cations of each of the 18 DSM III-R PDD-
D. A. Allen et al. 71
Autistic disorder versus other pervasive

NOS children are shown in Table 1. Subject

Differences within NVIQ groups: (ANOVA with Tukey post-test pairwise comparisons): *** = p<.001; all other comparisons not statistically significant. Abbreviations: ANOVA = analysios of variance; DLD =
developmental language disorder; H-PDD-NOS = high pervasive developmental disorder-not otherwise specified; HAD = high autistic disorder (AD); L-PDD-NOS = low PDD-NOS; LAD = low AD; NALIQ = non-
numbers 1±9 constitute the ``high'' (NVIQ ³ 80)
H-PDD-NOS subgroup; subject numbers 10±18

L-PDD-NOS=NALIQ
LAD < NALIQ***
L-PDD-NOS=LAD
Between ``Low''
constitute the ``low'' (NVIQ < 80) L-PDD-NOS sub-

L-PDD-NOS=
Comparisons
group. The range of NVIQ (16 to 131) across the 18

LAD=NALIQ
Subgroups
DSM III-R PDD-NOS clinical diagnoses belies the
commonly-held notion that PDD-NOS is a ``milder''
variant of the autistic condition, at least with regard
to cognition, as the psychiatrists applied this
diagnosis to signi®cantly mentally de®cient children

55.5(18.0)
55.9(13.3)
as well as to children with normal to superior

(N=110)
NALIQ
intelligence. The psychiatrists' DSM III diagnoses
span both cognitive groups as well. Also shown in
Table 1 are each child's number of checks on the
WADIC. Although the mean number checks on the

45.9(19.4)
59.6(16.4)
WADIC, an index of the severity of autism, was

(N=125)
somewhat lower in the H-PDD-NOS (10.2) than in

LAD
``Low'' Group (NVIQ < 80)
the L-PDD-NOS (12.8) children, this difference
failed to discriminate (p = 0.68) between the two
cognitive subgroups.

L-PDD-NOS

49.0(17.6)
66.0(15.5)
Table 2 Pairwise comparisons within nonverbal IQ groups: age and nonverbal IQ equivalent (means and standard deviations)
j Age and NVIQ

(N=9)
In view of clinical and diagnostic overlaps between
children on the autistic spectrum with children with
language disorders and mental de®ciency, we

H-PDD-NOS= HAD=DLD
present comparisons between these three groups,
strati®ed by NVIQ, in Tables 2±5. Table 2 summa-

H-PDD-NOS=HAD
H-PDD-NOS=DLD
Between ``High''

HAD > DLD***


rizes the ages and NVIQs of all the children in the
Comparisons

study. The signi®cantly lower age of the DLD


Subgroups

children is due to their enrollment between ages


3.0 and 5.11, whereas, to enhance the number of
AD and NALIQ testable children, age was extended
from 3.0 to 7.11 years. None of the NVIQ compar-
isons among the ``high'' cognitive subgroups was
102.4(17.1)
49.0(10.9)
(N=201)

signi®cant, all being in the average range. In the

autistic low IQ; N = number; NVIQ = nonverbal IQ equivalent (see text)


DLD

``low'' functioning group, all subgroups had signif-


icantly sub-normal mean NVIQ scores, with the
LAD, but not the L-PDD-NOS, signi®cantly lower
102.5(23.1)

(p < 0.01) than the NALIQ subgroup. NVIQ differ-


57.8(15.2)
(N=51)

ences between subgroups of the ``high'' and ``low''


HAD
``High'' Group (NVIQ ³ 80)

groups were predictably highly signi®cant


(p < 0.001).
H-PDD-NOS

j Communication skills
94.1(15.2)
56.0(13.3)
(N=9)

Verbal measures are presented in Fig. 1, the most


relevant comparisons being those within the ``high''
and ``low'' cognitive groups. The question was
whether the PDD-NOS subgroups were most similar
Age (months)

Nonverbal IQ

Binet/Bayley)

to the non-autistic children or to autistic children


Equivalent
Measures

(Stanford

with similar nonverbal skills. Compared to normative


scores, all subgroups were impaired on all verbal
measures, with the exception of the DLD and H-PDD-
72

Table 3 Pairwise comparisons within nonverbal IQ groups: verbal Measures (means and standard deviations)

Language measures ``High'' Group (NVIQ ³ 80) ``Low'' Group (NVIQ < 80)

H-PDD-NOS HAD DLD Comparisons L-PDD-NOS LAD NALIQ Comparisons


(N = 9) (N = 51) (N = 201) Between ``High'' (N = 9) (N = 125) (N = 110) Between ``Low''
Subgroups Subgroups

Verbal reasoning 87.6 81.4 91.4 H-PDD-NOS = HAD 73.3 65.8 74.8 L-PDD-NOS = LAD
(Stanford Binet) (18.3) (13.0) (12.4) H-PDD-NOS = DLD (14.9) (14.6) (14.1) L-PDD-NOS = NALIQ
HAD < DLD*** LAD<NALIQ**
Verbal 85.3 73.2 86.3 H-PDD-NOS = HAD 60.4 50.9 63.5 L-PDD-NOS = LAD
comprehension (15.9) (19.0) (17.9) H-PDD-NOS = DLD (15.5) (17.1) (18.9) L-PDD-NOS = NALIQ
(PPVT) HAD < DLD*** LAD < NALIQ***
Vineland 82.3 79.5 79.0 H-PDD-NOS = 59.2 53.2 63.2 L-PDD-NOS = LAD
Communication SS (11.6) (20.2) (12.2) HAD = DLD (8.0) (13.9) (12.3) L-PDD-NOS = NALIQ
LAD <
NALIQ***

Differences within NVIQ groups: (ANOVA with Tukey post-test pairwise comparisons): ** = p<.01; *** = p<.001; all other comparisons not statistically significant. Abbreviations: ANOVA + analysis of variance;
DLD = developmental language disorder; H-PDD-NOS = high pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified; HAD = high autistic disorder (AD); L-PDD-NOS = low PDD-NOS; LAD = low AD;
NALIQ = non-autistic low IQ; N = number; NVIQ = nonverbal IQ equivalent (see text); PPVT = Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised; SS = scaled score
Ó Steinkopff Verlag 2001

Table 4 Pairwise comparisons within nonverbal IQ groups: vineland scores (means and standard deviations)

Vineland domain ``High'' Group (NVIQ ³ 80) ``Low'' Group (NVIQ < 80)

H-PDD-NOS HAD DLD Comparisons L-PDD-NOS LAD NALIQ Comparisons


(N = 9) (N = 51) (N = 201) between ``High'' (N = 9) (N = 125) (N = 110) between ``Low''
subgroups subgroups

Communication SS 82.3 79.5 79.0 H-PDD-NOS = 59.2 53.2 63.2 L-PDD-NOS = LAD
(11.6) (20.2) (12.2) HAD = DLD (8.0) (13.9) (12.3) L-PDD-NOS = NALIQ
European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, Vol. 10, No. 1 (2001)

LAD < NALIQ***

Daily Living SS 72.3 70.1 84.8 H-PDD-NOS = HAD 64.6 51.8 64.3 L-PDD-NOS = LAD
(7.7) (15.0) (14.7) H-PDD-NOS = DLD (14.1) (15.9) (15.1) L-PDD-NOS = NALIQ
HAD < DLD*** LAD < NALIQ***
Socialization SS 75.9 69.8 85.0 H-PDD-NOS = HAD 60.6 56.0 68.9 L-PDD-NOS = LAD
(12.2) (13.4) (13.0) H-PDD-NOS = DLD (5.0) (9.4) (11.7) L-PDD-NOS = NALIQ
HAD < DLD*** LAD < NALIQ***
Adaptive SS 73.0 68.9 78.9 H-PDD-NOS = HAD 58.4 49.6 59.5 L-PDD-NOS = LAD
(composite) (7.9) (13.8) (12.8) H-PDD-NOS = DLD (7.6) (11.1) (10.6) L-PDD-NOS = NALIQ
HAD < DLD*** LAD < NALIQ***

Differences within NVIQ groups: (ANOVA with Tukey post-test pairwise comparisons): ** = p<.01; *** = p<.001; all other comparisons not statistically significant.Abbreviations: ANOVA = analysis of variance;
DLD = developmental language disorder; H-PDD-NOS = high pervasive developmental disorder-not otherwise specified; HAD = high autistic disorder (AD); L-PDD-NOS = low PDD-NOS; LAD = low AD;
NALIQ = non-autistic low IQ; N = number; NVIQ = nonverbal IQ equivalent (see text); SS = scaled score
D. A. Allen et al. 73
Autistic disorder versus other pervasive

NOS children on Verbal Reasoning (Table 3). None of

ANOVA = analysis of variance; DLD = developmental language disorder; H-PDD = high pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified (PDD-NOS); HAD = high autistic disorder (AD); L-PDD = low
Differences between NVIQ subgroups: (ANOVA with Tukey post-test pairwise comparisons): (*) p = <0.54; * = p<.05; ** = p<.01; *** = p<.001; all other comparisons not statistically significant. Abbreviations:
these measures discriminated between the AD and
PDD-NOS subgroups with equivalent NVIQs. With
Comparisons Between
only one exception, the AD subgroups were more

L-PDD > NALIQ***

L-PDD > NALIQ***


L-PDD > NALIQ**

L-PDD > NALIQ**


``Low'' Subgroups
verbally impaired than their non-AD comparison

LAD > NALIQ***

LAD > NALIQ***

LAD > NALIQ***

LAD > NALIQ***


L-PDD = LAD

L-PDD = LAD

L-PDD = LAD

L-PDD = LAD
subgroups, with the PDD-NOS subgroups obtaining
intermediate scores. The cognitive and language
scores in this study emphasize the blurred distinction
between ``high'' PDD-NOS and DLD. This fuzziness
explains why clinicians may confuse PDD-NOS with
DLD unless they are aware of the children's aberrant

PDD-NOS; LAD = low AD; NALIQ = non-autistic low IQ; N = number; NVIQ = nonverbal IQ equivalent (see text); WADIC = Wing Autistic Diagnostic Interview Checklist
behaviors.
NALIQ

(2.6)

(1.5)

(1.2)

(4.5)
1.9

1.2

0.8

3.9

j Adaptive skills

The Vineland is a well standardized instrument for


``Low'' Group (NVIQ < 80)

(1.8)

(1.2)

(1.6)

(3.7)
13.8
LAD

assessing the competence of children in everyday


7.5

3.4

2.9

life. Figure 2 depicts the mean standard scores of the


children on the Daily Living Skills and Socialization
domains of the Vineland, as well as their Composite
L-PDD-NOS

Vineland Adaptive mean scores. Mean scores of all


six subgroups were at least 15 points below norms
(1.8)

(1.0)

(1.6)

(3.3)
12.8
6.6

3.8

2.4

(Table 4). The pattern of standard scores assessing


developmental level of Daily Living Skills and
Socialization is similar to that of the language
Table 5 Pairwise comparisons within nonverbal IQ groups: behavioral scores (means and standard deviations)

measures: scores of the PDD-NOS subgroups fell


Comparisons Between

between those of the AD and their non-autistic


``High'' Subgroups

H-PDD > DLD***

H-PDD > DLD***

H-PDD > DLD***

controls and did not differ signi®cantly from either


H-PDD < HAD(*)

H-PDD < HAD*


H-PDD > DLD*
HAD > DLD***

HAD > DLD***

HAD > DLD***

HAD > DLD***


H-PDD = HAD

H-PDD = HAD

group. Children meeting AD criteria (both HAD and


LAD) had lower scores in development of Daily
Living skills and Socialization than non-autistic
controls.

j Maladaptive behaviors
(1.7)

(1.2)

(0.9)

(3.2)
DLD

0.8

0.7

0.3

1.8

Maladaptive social and communication behaviors, as


well as restricted and repetitive interests were as-
sessed with the WADIC (Fig. 3). Not surprisingly, the
subgroups of children meeting AD criteria had
(1.7)

(1.0)

(1.7)

(3.1)
HAD

14.2
''High'' Group (NVIQ ³ 80)

7.3

4.1

2.8

signi®cantly more maladaptive behaviors and restrict-


ed and repetitive interests than the IQ matched
non-autistic controls (Table 5). Importantly, the
PDD-NOS children also had more maladaptive and
H-PDD-NOS

aberrant behaviors than the controls. In the area of


social and communicative behaviors, neither of the
(2.2)

(1.5)

(1.1)

(3.4)
10.2
5.6

3.1

1.6

PDD-NOS subgroups differed from IQ matched


autistic children. Although the H-PDD-NOS subgroup
approached signi®cance (p = 0.054) in having fewer
(communication and

endorsements on the restricted and repetitive inter-


(narrow behavioral

ests scale than the HAD subgroup, the L-PDD-NOS


WADIC Total

children did not differ in this area from the LAD.


(sociability)

repertoire)
WADIC A

WADIC B

WADIC C

Neither PDD-NOS subgroup differed from the corre-


Scores

play)

sponding AD subgroup on the Social Abnormalities


Scales.
74 European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, Vol. 10, No. 1 (2001)
Ó Steinkopff Verlag 2001

Fig. 1 Comparisons between scores (means and standard deviations) on mean scores of the PDD-NOS sample were interme-
verbal measures among PDD-NOS, autistic disorder, and non-autistic children diate between those of the AD and non-autistic
divided into cognitively ``high'' and ``low'' groups on the basis of a nonverbal IQ
at or above 80 (three bars on the left of each figure) or below 80 (three bars groups which, as expected, differed signi®cantly
on the right). H-PDD-NOS high pervasive disorder-not otherwise specified; HAD from one another, with the one exception that the
high autistic disorder; DLD developmental language disorder; L-PDD-NOS low- three cognitively ``high'' subgroups had equivalent
PDD-NOS; LAD low autistic disorder; NALIQ non-autistic low IQ language skills in everyday life, as judged by the
Vineland parent interview. Third, when it came to
maladaptive behaviors, although PDD-NOS scores
Discussion were still intermediate between those of AD and
non-autistic controls, the PDD-NOS children were
j The autistic spectrum vs. categorical subtypes similar to the AD children, with both groups
exhibiting many more maladaptive behaviors than
Three ®ndings in this study illustrate why it is often the non±autistic children. In this respect, the only
dif®cult to partition preschool children on the PDD difference between the PDD-NOS and AD groups
spectrum into AD and PDD-NOS groups. First, the was that the ``high'' PDD-NOS subgroup had some-
18 DSM III-R PDD-NOS children obtained scores for what fewer restricted and maladaptive behaviors
measures of language and adaptive functioning that than its ``high'' AD counterpart. In short, the
failed to differ signi®cantly from those of 176 aberrant behaviors shared by the AD and PDD-
children who met full criteria for AD nor from NOS children set them dramatically apart from non-
those of 311 non±autistic controls matched for autistic children.
nonverbal IQ. Second, on virtually all of the A limitation of this study is that it sought to enroll
measures of language and adaptive function, the children with DSM III-R AD, rather than attempting
to capture the entire autistic spectrum. The small
Fig. 2 Comparisons between scores (means and standard deviations) on number of inadvertently captured children with PDD-
adaptive function as measured by the Vineland among PDD-NOS, autistic NOS limits the power of its conclusions. A second
disorder, and non-autistic children divided into cognitively ``high'' and ``low'' limitation is that no child was given a diagnosis of
groups on the basis of a nonverbal IQ at or above 80 (three bars on the left of Asperger syndrome, a subtype that had not been
each figure) or below 80 (three bars on the right). H-PDD-NOS high pervasive
disorder-not otherwise specified; HAD high autistic disorder; DLD developmen-
de®ned in DSM III-R when the study was carried out.
tal language disorder; L-PDD-NOS low-PDD-NOS; LAD low autistic disorder; Therefore the study cannot address the question of
NALIQ non-autistic low IQ whether Asperger syndrome and PDD-NOS are
D. A. Allen et al. 75
Autistic disorder versus other pervasive

advantages, despite their fuzzy borders. One of the


ways to determine whether the boundaries separating
AD and other PDD subgroups are meaningful will be to
examine their prognostic value in longitudinal studies.
Other studies support the concept of a spectrum of
autistic behavioral symptomatology (1, 17). In an
exhaustive review of PDD-NOS, Towbin (32) found
that interrater reliability was low for diagnosing this
disorder within the continuum of PDD. Volkmar and
Cohen (35), who played a key role in the ®eld trials to
determine the sensitivity and speci®city of the behav-
ioral descriptors chosen as criteria for a DSM IV
diagnosis of PDD subtypes, found that most disagree-
ments among raters were for ®ne-grained distinctions
between AD and other PDD disorders. Although
Buitelaar et al. (7) provide an algorithm for making
a DSM IV diagnosis of PDD-NOS, they state that it is
too early to determine whether this diagnosis merely
represents the upper tail of the autistic spectrum.
Mahoney et al. (20) reported that clinicians had
dif®culty differentiating atypical autism (PDD-NOS)
from AD but were much better at differentiating
Asperger syndrome from AD, and PDD from ``non-
Fig. 3 Comparisons between numbers of aberrant behaviors (means and
standard deviations) from the Wing Autistic Disorder Interview Checklist autistic'' disorders. Kurita's study (19), however,
(WADIC) among PDD-NOS, autistic disorder, and non-autistic children divided questions the speci®city of Asperger syndrome as
into cognitively ``high'' and ``low'' groups on the basis of a nonverbal IQ at or distinct from the upper end of the autistic spectrum
above 80 (three bars on the left of each figure) or below 80 (three bars on the (individuals with an IQ > 90) and an ICD 10 diagnosis
right). H-PDD-NOS high pervasive disorder-not otherwise specified; HAD high
autistic disorder; DLD developmental language disorder; L-PDD-NOS low PDD- of atypical autism (which corresponds to DSM IV
NOS; LAD low autistic disorder; NALIQ non-autistic low IQ PDD-NOS).

categorically distinct, another contentious issue. Oth- j Importance of cognition


er investigators have found that there are more
overlaps than differences between AD and PDD-NOS Current DSM and ICD criteria for subtyping PDD rely
(e.g., 7, 23), as contrasted to differences between PDD on behavioral criteria (sociability, communication,
disorders and non-autistic developmental disorders. play, and stereotypic, rigid, perseverative characteris-
The intermediate position of PDD-NOS language tics). Asperger syndrome is currently the only diag-
and adaptive behavior scores between those of the AD nosis in DSM IV and ICD 10 for which cognitive and
and non-autistic groups with comparable NVIQs, also adaptive levels (usually interpreted as an IQ above 70,
found by Mayes et al. (21), makes it clear why the cut-off for mental retardation) are de®ning
clinicians often ®nd it dif®cult to make the diagnostic features. Cognitive level may provide a strong addi-
distinction between AD and PDD-NOS, especially tional criterion to the behavioral criteria in current
when the child has a nonverbal IQ above 80. The use for de®ning other PDD subtypes. There is
distinction between a diagnosis of PDD-NOS and AD discussion in the literature regarding the relationship
in DSM-IV (or DSM III-R) is the number and between cognitive ability and social/behavioral de®-
distribution of endorsed autistic behaviors elicited by cits within the overall PDD population (5, 12, 13, 25,
history and observation. Buitelaar et al. (7) examined 28, 33, 37). A number of investigators have suggested
the power of DSM-IV/ICD-10 criteria, and their that ``high'' and ``low'' IQ groups be considered
relation to clinical and DSM III-R diagnoses and subtypes of PDD (5, 9, 10, 13, 33). The NVIQ cut-point
found that PDD-NOS is''...basically a lesser variant of for most of these studies was 70 (10 points lower than
autism with impairment in social interaction as a key the lower limit for our ``high-functioning'' groups). In
characteristic.'' The implication is that PDD is a most studies there was a lack of comparison with
disorder with a spectrum of severity and its subtypes other developmentally disordered groups with similar
are quantitative rather than categorical. Dividing this IQ levels. In instances in which such groups were
behavioral spectrum into operationally de®ned sub- used, the IQ levels of the AD subjects tended to be
types has brought with it many scienti®c and practical very low and the control groups were matched for
76 European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, Vol. 10, No. 1 (2001)
Ó Steinkopff Verlag 2001

mental age, but therefore not chronologic age (8, 21, Many parents and professionals use the terms PDD
34). This study differs in that comparisons were made and PDD-NOS interchangeably to refer to children
between diagnostic subgroups with similar chrono- who have autistic behaviors but do not ®t their
logic ages and nonverbal IQs. conceptions of autism. They seem unaware that PDD
We had previously applied an empirical taxometric is a generic umbrella term that refers to the entire
classi®cation method to partition the 194 AD and autistic spectrum, whereas PDD-NOS is but one of the
PDD-NOS children in this study (12, 25). By this subtypes of PDD (26). Many children carry the PDD-
approach 95% of the children were divided into two NOS diagnosis which, by de®nition, states that they
subgroups, high and low, with a cut off at NVIQ 65, are ``not autistic'', i.e., do not have autistic disorder
which is ®ve points lower than in most other studies. (AD). Parents and educators are understandably
Of the 18 PDD-NOS children, 78% were classi®ed into confused by a clinical diagnosis that implies that a
one or the other of these two empirical AD subgroups, child who exhibits autistic behaviors but seems
which lends further support to the fact that the otherwise intelligent is ``de®nitely not autistic but
majority of these PDD-NOS children did not differ may have PDD or PDD-NOS''. PDD and PDD-NOS
substantially from the DSM III-R autistic group. (and Asperger syndrome) are often used synony-
mously to stand for a milder form of autism. This may
not be justi®ed, as in the present study there was a
j Prognosis wide range of aberrant behaviors and of NVIQ scores
in the PDD-NOS sample. Therefore this study pro-
Prognosis is a potent way to determine the power of the vides no support for the terms PDD and PDD-NOS to
current behavioral boundaries used to partition PDD- be synonymous with ``high functioning autism.''
NOS from AD groups are meaningful, or whether also Children with a diagnosis of PDD-NOS or Asperger
taking NVIQ into consideration might be better. syndrome are often classi®ed as ``emotionally dis-
Stevens et al. (30) followed at seven or nine years or turbed'' or ``language impaired'' in order to be eligible
both the 116 available PDD children in this study. for special education services. Intermediate language
Unfortunately this included only nine of the 18 PDD- and adaptive scores between AD and non-autistic
NOS group, making it impossible to determine whether subgroups, shared aberrant behaviors with AD chil-
these nine children had a better prognosis than their dren, and cognitive heterogeneity illustrate why PDD-
``high'' and ``low'' AD counterparts. What the longitu- NOS does not stand out as a distinct diagnostic entity.
dinal indicated is that preschool behavioral and social Whether outcome will discriminate PDD-NOS from
measures were less reliable predictors of outcome at AD with matched NVIQ level would require a much
schoolage than cognitive variables, although NVIQ larger longitudinal sample than this one, but taking
alone was also inadequate. This suggests that both cognition as well as behavior into consideration
NVIQ and behavioral social measures are required for seems to improve cognitive accuracy and may provide
a more reliable prognosis. a stronger basis for subtyping PDD.
There is a current trend among many clinicians to
replace the term PDD by non-categorical terms such as
j Implications for the provision of services autistic continuum (38) and autistic spectrum disor-
ders (1, 14, 16, 27). We would argue that the concept of
Classi®cations and/or diagnostic perspectives derive a behaviorally de®ned autistic spectrum speaks to the
from different disciplines and serve many different fact that all individuals share the core de®cits in
purposes: educational, medical, and research, for socialization, communication, and imagination, de-
example. Access to educational services should not spite their many different etiologies (15). The speci®c
be based on diagnostic labels inasmuch as a same manner in which these core de®cits are expressed
diagnosis does not imply identical educational/voca- singly and in combination results in a wide range of
tional needs (see 26). Nonetheless, in many states clinically observable behavioral pro®les that are
across the United States, autism is now recognized as a strongly modulated by an individual's level of cogni-
fundable developmental disability, whereas PDD-NOS tive function and by the adequacy and precocity of
and Asperger syndrome are not. This is not the case educational intervention.
for New York State where a recent evidence-based
review of the literature revealed that the distinction j Acknowledgement This study was supported in part by NINDS
between DSM IV/ICD 10 AD, Asperger disorder, and program project grant NS 20489. The authors acknowledge the
PDD-NOS is blurred, at least in children under three contributions of Drs. Dorothy Aram, Ronald David, Nancy Hall,
Robin Morris and Barbara C. Wilson, of the many research
years. According to this guideline the distinction does assistants named in Rapin (24) who took part in data collection,
not provide a basis for discrimination in delivery of and of the children and their parents who participated in the
services (22). research.
D. A. Allen et al. 77
Autistic disorder versus other pervasive

References
1. Allen DA (1988) Autistic spectrum 15. Gillberg C, Coleman M (2000) The quate Communication: Developmental
disorders: clinical presentation in pre- Biology of the Autistic Syndromes. 3rd Language Disorder, Autism, Low IQ
school children. Journal of Child Neu- edn. Mac Keith Press, London, Clinics Mac Keith Press, London. Clinics in
rology 3 Suppl:S48±S56 in Developmental Medicine No. 153/4 Developmental Medicine No. 139, pp
2. APA (eds) (1980) Diagnostic and Sta- 16. Greenspan SI, Wieder S (1997) Devel- 190±213
tistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 3rd opmental patterns and outcomes in 26. Rutter M, Schopler E (1992) Classi®ca-
edn. Washington, D.C. infants and children with disorders in tion of pervasive developmental disor-
3. APA (eds) (1987) Diagnostic and Sta- relating and communicating: a chart ders: some concepts and practical
tistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 3rd review of 200 cases of children with considerations. Journal of Autism and
edn. Revised DSM-III-R, Washington, autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Developmental Disorders 22:459±482
D.C. Developmental and Learning Disorder 27. Siegel B (1996) The World of the
4. APA (eds) (1994) Diagnostic and Sta- 1:87±141 Autistic Child. Understanding and
tistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th 17. Happe F, Frith U (1991) Is autism a Treating Autistic Spectrum Disorders.
edn. Washington, D.C. pervasive developmental disorder? De- Oxford University Press, New York
5. Bartak L, Rutter M (1976) Differences bate and argument: how useful is the 28. Sigman M, Dissanayake C, Arbelle S,
between mentally retarded and normal- ``PDD'' label? Journal of Child Psychi- Ruskin E (1997) Cognition and emotion
ly intelligent autistic children. Journal atry and Psychology 32:1167±1168 in children and adolescents with au-
of Autism and Childhood Schizophre- 18. Karmiloff-Smith A (1998) Development tism. In: Cohen DJ, Volkmar FR (eds)
nia 6:109±120 itself is the key to understanding de- Handbook of Autism and Pervasive
6. Bayley N (1969) Bayley Scales of Infant velopmental disorders. Trends in Cog- Developmental Disorders. 2nd edn.
Development. The Psychological Cor- nitive Sciences 2:389±398 John Wiley & Sons, New York, pp
poration, Berkeley, CA 19. Kurita H (1997) A comparative study of 248±265
7. Buitelaar JK, Van der Gaag R, Klin A, Asperger syndrome with high-function- 29. Sparrow SS, Balla DA, Cichetti DV
Volkmar F (1999) Exploring the foun- ing atypical autism. Psychiatry and (1984) Vineland Adaptive Behavior
daries of pervasive developmental dis- Clinical Neurosciences 51:67±70 Scales. American Guidance Service,
order not otherwise speci®ed: analyses 20. Mahoney WJ, Szatmari P, MacLean JE, Circle Pines, MN
of data from DSM-IV autistic disorder Bryson SE, Bartolucci G, Walter SD, 30. Stevens MC, Fein DA, Dunn M, Allen D,
®eld trial. Journal of Autism and De- Jones MB, Zwaigenbaum L (1998) Re- Waterhouse LH, Feinstein C, Rapin I
velopmental Disorders 29:33±43 liability and accuracy of differentiating (2000) Subgroups of children with
8. Burack JA, Volkmar FR (1992) Devel- pervasive developmental disorder sub- autism by cluster analysis: a longitudi-
opment of low- and high-functioning types. Journal of the American Acade- nal examination. Journal of the Amer-
autistic children. Journal of Child Psy- my of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry ican Academy of Child and Adolescent
chology and Psychiatry 33:607±616 37:278±285 Psychiatry 39:346±352
9. Cohen DJ, Paul R, Volkmar FR (1987) 21. Mayes L, Volkmar FR, Hooks M, Cich- 31. Thorndike RL, Hagen EP, Sattler JM
Issues in the classi®cation of PDD and etti D (1993) Differentiating Pervasive (1986) Stanford-Binet Intelligence
associated conditions. In: Cohen DJ, Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Scale-Revised. Riverside Publishing
Donnellan AM, Paul R (eds) Handbook Speci®ed from autism and language Co, Chicago
of Autism and Pervasive Developmen- disorders. Journal of Autism and De- 32. Towbin KE (1997) Pervasive develop-
tal Disorders. 2nd edn. John Wiley & velopmental Disorders 23:79±90 mental disorder not otherwise speci-
Sons, New York, pp 20±40 22. New York State Department of Health ®ed. In: Cohen DJ, Volkmar FR (eds)
10. DeMyer MK, Barton S, DeMyer WE, (1999) Clinical Practice Guideline: Handbook of Autism and Pervasive
Norton JA, Allen J, Steele R (1973) Report of the Recommendations for Developmental Disorders. 2nd edn.
Prognosis in autism: a followup study. Autism/Pervasive Developmental Dis- John Wiley & Sons, New York, pp
Journal of Autism and Childhood orders. Assessment and Intervention 123±147
Schizophrenia 3:199±246 for Young Children (Age 0±3 Years). 33. Tsai L (1992) Diagnostic issues in high
11. Dunn LM, Dunn L (1981) Peabody New York State Department of Health functioning autism. In: Schopler E, Mes-
Picture Vocabulary Test ± Revised Early Intervention Program, Albany, ibov GB (eds) High Functioning Indi-
American Guidance Service, Circle NY viduals with Autism. Plenum, New York,
Pines, MN 23. Prior M, Eisenmajer R, Leekman S, pp 11±40
12. Fein D, Stevens M, Dunn M, Water- Wing L, Gould J, Ong B, Dowe D (1998) 34. Volkmar FR, Cohen, DJ, Hoshino Y,
house L, Allen D, Rapin I, Feinstein C Are there subgroups within the autistic Rende RD, Paul R (1988) Phenomenol-
(2000) Subtypes of pervasive develop- spectrum? A cluster analysis of a group ogy and classi®cation of the childhood
mental disorder: clinical characteristics. of children with autistic spectrum dis- psychoses. Psychological Medicine
Child Neuropsychology 5:1±23 orders. Journal of Child Psychology and 18:191±201
13. Freeman BJ, Ritvo ER, Schroth PC, Psychiatry 39:893±902 35. Volkmar FR, Klin A, Cohen DJ (1997)
Tonick I, Guthrie D, Wake L (1981) 24. Rapin I (ed) (1996a) Preschool Chil- Diagnosis and classi®cation of autism
Behavioral characteristics of high- and dren with Inadequate Communication: and related conditions: consensus and
low-IQ autistic children. American Developmental Language Disorder, Au- issues. In: Cohen DJ, Volkmar FR (eds)
Journal of Psychiatry 138:25±29 tism, Low IQ. Mac Keith Press, London, Handbook of Autism and Pervasive
14. Filipek PA (1999) Autistic spectrum Clinics in Developmental Medicine No. Developmental Disorders. 3rd edn.
disorders. In: Swaiman KF, Ashwal S 139 John Wiley & Sons, New York, pp 1±40
(eds) Pediatric Neurology: Principles 25. Rapin I, Allen DA, Aram DM, Dunn 36. Volkmar FR, Klin A, Schultz R, Cohen
and Practice. 3rd edn. Mosby, St Louis MA, Fein D, Morris R, Waterhouse L DJ (1998) ``Nonautistic'' pervasive de-
MO, pp 606±628 (1996b) Classi®cation issues. In: Rapin velopmental disorders. In: Coffey CE,
I (ed) Preschool Children with Inade- Brumback RA (eds) Textbook of Pedi-
78 European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, Vol. 10, No. 1 (2001)
Ó Steinkopff Verlag 2001

atric Neuropsychiatry. APA, Washing- 38. Wing L (1988) The continuum of to their Classi®cation in Accordance
ton DC, pp 429±447 autistic characteristics. In: Schopler E, with the Tenth Revision of the Interna-
37. Waterhouse L, Morris R, Allen D, Dunn Mesibov GB (ed) Diagnosis and Assess- tional Classi®cation of Diseases. WHO,
M, Fein D, Feinstein C, Rapin I, Wing L ment in Autism. Plenum, New York, pp Geneva, Switzerland
(1996) Diagnosis and classi®cation in 91±110
autism. Journal of Autism and Devel- 39. World Health Organization (1993)
opmental Disorders 26:59±86 Mental Disorders: Glossary and Guide

You might also like