You are on page 1of 7

Article

Influence of Gender on Attention Deficit Hyperactivity


Disorder in Children Referred to a Psychiatric Clinic

Joseph Biederman, M.D. Objective: The substantial discrepancy school, intellectual, interpersonal, and
in the male-to-female ratio between family functioning.
Eric Mick, Sc.D. clinic-referred (10 to 1) and community (3 Results: Girls with ADHD were more likely
to 1) samples of children with attention than boys to have the predominantly inat-
Stephen V. Faraone, Ph.D. deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) sug- tentive type of ADHD, less likely to have a
gests that gender differences may be op- learning disability, and less likely to mani-
Ellen Braaten, Ph.D. erant in the phenotypic expression of fest problems in school or in their spare
ADHD. In this study the authors systemat- time. In addition, girls with ADHD were at
less risk for comorbid major depression,
Alysa Doyle, Ph.D. ically examined the impact of gender on
the clinical features of ADHD in a group of conduct disorder, and oppositional defiant
disorder than boys with ADHD. A statisti-
Thomas Spencer, M.D. children referred to a clinic.
cally significant gender-by-ADHD interac-
Method: The study included 140 boys and tion was identified for comorbid substance
Timothy E. Wilens, M.D. 140 girls with ADHD and 120 boys and 122 use disorders as well.
girls without ADHD as comparison sub- Conclusions: The lower likelihood for
Elizabeth Frazier, B.A. jects. All subjects were systematically as- girls to manifest psychiatric, cognitive,
sessed with structured diagnostic inter- and functional impairment than boys
Mary Ann Johnson, R.N., M.S., views and neuropsychological batteries for could result in gender-based referral bias
C.S. subtypes of ADHD as well as emotional, unfavorable to girls with ADHD.

(Am J Psychiatry 2002; 159:36–42)

D espite the fact that a large number of girls might be


suffering from attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
assessment, and the absence of gender-matched compar-
ison subjects (4). These limitations impede an adequate
(ADHD), the scientific literature on ADHD is almost exclu- understanding of gender effects and the ability to separate
sively based on boys. To address the paucity of informa- gender effects on ADHD from the main effect of gender
tion on ADHD in girls, in an earlier study (1) we compared within ADHD subjects.
the clinical correlates of ADHD in a large group of girls In view of the importance of early and appropriate treat-
with and without ADHD ascertained from pediatric and ments for ADHD, a comprehensive understanding of gen-
psychiatric facilities. This study showed that ADHD in girls der differences and similarities in the clinical manifesta-
was characterized by prototypical symptoms of the disor- tions of ADHD has major public health and scientific
der, comorbid psychopathology, social dysfunction, cog- importance for the advancement of women’s health. To
nitive impairments, school failure, and adversity in the this end, we systematically compared boys and girls with
family environment. and without ADHD in multiple domains of functioning.
We hypothesized that ADHD in girls would be character-
Despite this level of dysfunction, a substantial discrep-
ized by lower rates of comorbid disruptive behavior disor-
ancy exists in the male-to-female ratio between clinic-re-
der, a preponderance of the inattentive subtype of ADHD,
ferred (10 to 1) and community (3 to 1) samples of children
and less cognitive dysfunction than ADHD in boys.
with ADHD (2, 3). Although the reasons for the apparent
underidentification of girls with ADHD remain unclear,
Gaub and Carlson (3) suggested in their meta-analysis that
Method
gender differences in the phenotypic expression of the Subjects
disorder may be driving referral of more boys than girls.
Data from two identically designed family studies of ADHD (1,
However, Gaub and Carlson also pointed out that the 5) were combined. Our first study (5) ascertained boys with or
dearth of research comparing boys and girls with ADHD without ADHD who were 6–17 years old at the time of ascertain-
limits the conclusions that can be drawn from the extant ment; this study included 140 boys with ADHD and 120 boys
without ADHD. Our second study (1) ascertained girls with and
literature. In addition to the scarcity of such studies, the
without ADHD who were also 6–17 years old at the time of ascer-
previous studies of gender and ADHD that have been pub- tainment; this study included 140 girls with ADHD and 122 girls
lished are limited by small sample sizes, a limited scope of without ADHD.

36 Am J Psychiatry 159:1, January 2002


BIEDERMAN, MICK, FARAONE, ET AL.

Ascertainment do not address the degree of agreement that would be attained


between two separate interviews of the same patient.
Potential subjects were excluded from both studies if they had
been adopted or if their nuclear family was not available for study. In addition to psychiatric data, we assessed social functioning
We excluded subjects if they had major sensorimotor handicaps with the Social Adjustment Inventory for Children and Adoles-
(paralysis, deafness, blindness), psychosis, autism, inadequate cents (9), socioeconomic status with the Hollingshead scale (10),
command of the English language, or a full-scale IQ (6) less than divorce or separation of parents in family of origin, the family en-
80. Parents provided written informed consent for their children, vironment with the Family Environment Scale (11), indicators of
and children and adolescents provided written assent to their social adversity from Rutter’s Isle of Wight study (12), perinatal
participation. complications with the Diagnostic Interview for Children and Ad-
olescents—Parent Version (13, 14), full-scale IQ based on the vo-
Two independent sources provided the children with ADHD.
cabulary and block design subtests of the Wechsler intelligence
We selected psychiatrically referred children with ADHD from
tests (15), reading and arithmetic achievement with subtests of
consecutive referrals to a pediatric psychopharmacology clinic at
the Wide Range Achievement Test, Revised (16), adaptive func-
the Massachusetts General Hospital (selection criteria are de-
tioning with the Global Assessment of Functioning Scale (DSM-
scribed later in this article). Parents, pediatricians, and schools
III-R), and school difficulties (special class, repeated grades, tu-
had referred these children for psychiatric evaluations. The other
toring) with structured diagnostic interview of the mother. The
source of children with ADHD were pediatric patients from a
definition of learning disabilities under Public Law 94-142 re-
health maintenance organization (HMO). Within each setting, we
quires a substantial discrepancy between a child’s potential and
selected comparison subjects without ADHD from outpatients at
achievement (17). We operationalized this with the procedure
pediatric medical clinics.
recommended by Reynolds (18) that we have used elsewhere (19).
A three-stage ascertainment procedure was used to select the
subjects. For children with ADHD, the first stage was the patient’s Statistical Analysis
referral to a psychiatric or pediatric clinic resulting in a clinical di-
agnosis of ADHD by a child psychiatrist or pediatrician, which Our main hypothesis was that there is no difference between
was recorded in the clinic record. A second stage confirmed the boys and girls with respect to the impact that ADHD exerts on
diagnosis of ADHD after all children with a positive diagnosis at functioning in multiple domains. Data from comparison subjects
the first stage were screened by administering a telephone ques- without ADHD are presented so that interactions could be tested
tionnaire asking the child’s mother about the 14 DSM-III-R symp- to determine if ADHD is a similar risk factor in boys and girls with
toms of ADHD. The third stage confirmed the diagnosis made on respect to sex-matched children without ADHD. To determine if
the basis of the telephone questionnaire with face-to-face struc- girls with ADHD have different clinic presentations from boys
tured interviews with the mother. Only patients who received a with the disorder, we also present within-ADHD between-gender
diagnosis of ADHD at all three stages were included in the final pairwise comparisons. Binary data were analyzed with logistic re-
analysis. Comparison subjects without ADHD were ascertained gression models, and continuous data were analyzed with ordi-
from referrals to medical clinics for routine physical examina- nary least-squares regression. Wald’s chi-square test was used to
tions at both the Massachusetts General Hospital and the HMO determine statistical significance. The effects of gender within the
sites. The mothers of these children were also interviewed by tele- ADHD groups were tested with pairwise comparisons by using
phone and face-to-face; children included in the study did not Pearson’s chi-square or analysis of variance for binary and contin-
meet ADHD criteria at any of the three stages. uous data, respectively. Statistical significance was determined at
p<0.05.
Measures
Psychiatric assessments of the children were made by using the Results
Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-
Age Children—Epidemiologic Version (K-SADS-E) (7). Diagnoses Demographic characteristics of the subjects are pre-
were based on independent interviews with the mothers and di- sented in Table 1. Although subjects from the study of girls
rect interviews with the children older than 12 years. Diagnostic with and without ADHD were slightly older than the sub-
assessments of parents were based on the Structured Clinical In-
jects from the study of boys, the interaction between gen-
terview for DSM-III-R (8) conducted with each parent. To assess
childhood diagnoses in the parents, modules from the K-SADS-E der and ADHD status was not statistically significant. This
covering childhood diagnoses were administered. All assess- indicated that the distance in age between the girls with
ments were made by raters who were blind to the child’s diagnosis and without ADHD was not different from the distance be-
(ADHD or no ADHD) and ascertainment site. Different interview- tween the boys with and without ADHD. Likewise, ADHD
ers met with mothers and children to maintain blindness to
was not associated with lower socioeconomic status and
ADHD status and to prevent information from one informant in-
fluencing the assessment of the other. divorce or separation differently in boys and girls. We also
A committee of three psychiatrists (J.B., T.S., and T.E.W.), each failed to identify any statistically significant differences in
board certified in both child and adult psychiatry, resolved all di- demographic variables in pairwise comparisons of boys
agnostic uncertainties. The committee members were blind to and girls with ADHD (Table 1).
the subjects’ ascertainment group, ascertainment site, all data
collected from family members, and all nondiagnostic data (e.g.,
Comparisons of children with and without ADHD for
neuropsychological tests). Kappa coefficients of agreement were both studies were ascertained by using DSM-III-R criteria
computed between raters and three board-certified psychiatrists for ADHD, which did not distinguish the subtypes of the
who listened to audiotaped interviews made by the raters. Based disorder introduced in DSM-IV. However, additional
on 173 interviews, the median kappa was 0.86; for ADHD, kappa= symptoms required to make DSM-IV diagnoses were col-
0.99; for conduct disorder, kappa=0.93; for multiple anxiety disor-
ders, kappa=0.80; for major depression, kappa=0.83; and for bi-
lected in the study of girls, and proxies for the subtypes
polar disorder, kappa=0.94. These statistics for multiple raters rat- were available for the study of boys (20). More girls who
ing the same interview show excellent agreement of ratings but met DSM-III-R criteria for ADHD failed to meet DSM-IV

Am J Psychiatry 159:1, January 2002 37


GENDER AND ADHD

TABLE 1. Demographic Characteristics of Girls and Boys in Two Studies of Children With and Without ADHD
Study of Girls Study of Boys Interaction of Gender
Characteristic ADHD (N=140) No ADHD (N=122) ADHD (N=140) No ADHD (N=120) and ADHD Status
Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Beta 95% CI

Age at ascertainment (years) 11.2 3.4 12.2 3.0 10.5 3.0 11.6 3.7 –0.02 –1.1 to 1.1
Socioeconomic status (Hollingshead
Four-Factor Index) 1.9 0.9 1.7 0.8 1.9 1.0 1.5 0.7 0.2 –0.1 to 0.5

N % N % N % N % Odds Ratio 95% CI

Parents divorced or separated 39 28 22 18 40 29 21 18 0.9 0.4 to 2.2

FIGURE 1. DSM-IV Subtypes of ADHD in Boys and Girls in in girls. The odds ratio of 0.05 indicates that ADHD in-
Two Studies of Children With DSM-III-R ADHD creased the risk for substance use disorders one-20th as
much in boys as it increased the risk for substance use dis-
100 Combined
orders in girls. For other disorders the gender-by-diagno-
90 Hyperactive/
Impulsive sis interaction was not significant, indicating that other
80 gender differences between boys and girls with ADHD
Inattentive
70 were the same as the gender difference observed for boys
60 and girls without ADHD.
Percent

50 For example, girls with ADHD were at significantly lower


risk for any other behavior disorder than boys with ADHD.
40
This is a result of a naturally lower base rate of behavior
30
disorders in girls regardless of ADHD status. Similar re-
20
sults were observed for conduct disorder, oppositional de-
10 fiant disorder, major depression, and (in the opposite di-
0 rection) panic disorder.
Girls Boys The children’s scores on the subscales of the WISC-R (6)
With ADHD With ADHD
(N=140) (N=140) are presented in Table 3. Again, gender did not modify the
impact of ADHD on measures of intelligence (no gender-
by-ADHD diagnosis interactions were statistically signifi-
criteria for ADHD (N=11 [8%]) than boys who met DSM-
cant). However, girls with ADHD had statistically signifi-
III-R criteria (N=1 [0.7%]). Although the combined type
cantly, but not clinically meaningful, lower scores than
was the most prevalent type for both boys and girls with
boys with ADHD for estimated full-scale IQ and the block
ADHD, girls with ADHD were 2.2 times (95% confidence
design, and oral arithmetic subscales. Boys with ADHD
interval=1.2–4.0) more likely to be diagnosed as primarily
had significantly lower scores on reading achievement
inattentive than were boys with ADHD (Figure 1).
and significantly greater rates of learning disability than
Girls with ADHD were statistically but not clinically
girls with ADHD, however (Table 3).
meaningfully older at onset of ADHD than boys with
Although there were no gender-by-ADHD interactions
ADHD (mean=3.5 years, SD=2.5, versus mean=2.7, SD=
for any of the psychosocial functioning items studied (Ta-
2.0) (t=–3.0, df=277, p=0.003). Although all of the children
ble 4), girls with ADHD had fewer school problems and
with ADHD were from clinical settings, there were differ-
participated in more spare-time activities than boys with
ences in the rates of medication and psychotherapy tar-
ADHD. Despite the differences in the rate of psychiatric
geted for ADHD between boys and girls. The majority of
comorbidity and slightly lower intellectual functioning,
both boys and girls received treatment, but girls were less
there were no gender-by-ADHD interactions for indexes of
likely to receive pharmacotherapy (70% [N=98] versus 82%
the family environment, nor were there any pairwise dif-
[N=115]) (χ2=5.7, df=1, p=0.02) and psychotherapy (51%
ferences between boys and girls with ADHD.
[N=71] versus 64% [N=89]) (χ2=5.7, df=1, p=0.02). How-
For each statistically significant within-ADHD gender
ever, the mean number of years from the onset of ADHD to
difference we evaluated the impact of the higher rate of
the first encounter with treatment for the disorder was not
the inattentive subtype of ADHD in girls (data not pre-
different in girls and boys with ADHD (mean=4.5, SD=3.5,
sented). In no case was a gender difference found to be ac-
versus mean=3.1, SD=2.0) (t=0.1, df=203, p=0.9).
counted for by subtype differences between boys and girls.
The profile of psychiatric comorbidity of ADHD was
quite similar in boys and girls (Table 2). For substance use
Discussion
disorders there was a significant gender-by-diagnosis in-
teraction, indicating that ADHD was a significantly weaker ADHD in girls was more likely to be predominantly the
risk factor for substance use disorders in boys than it was inattentive subtype, less likely to be associated with a

38 Am J Psychiatry 159:1, January 2002


BIEDERMAN, MICK, FARAONE, ET AL.

TABLE 2. Psychiatric Comorbidity Among Girls and Boys in Two Studies of Children With and Without ADHD
Study of Girls Study of Boys Interaction of Gender
ADHD (N=140) No ADHD (N=122) ADHD (N=140) No ADHD (N=120) and ADHD Status
Diagnosis N % N % N % N % Odds Ratio 95% CI p
Any behavior disordera 50 36 5 4 93 66 13 11 1.3 0.4 to 4.0
Conduct disorderb 11 8 0 0 30 21 4 3 0.6 —c 0.71
Oppositional defiant disordera 49 35 5 4 92 66 13 11 1.3 0.4 to 4.0
Any mood disorder 38 27 2 2 50 36 3 3 1.0 0.1 to 6.3
Major depression (severe)d 21 15 1 1 40 29 2 2 0.9 0.1 to 11.3
Bipolar disorder 15 11 0 0 15 11 0 0 —e
Dysthymia 13 9 1 1 6 4 1 1 0.4 0.1 to 8.3
Multiple anxiety disorders 46 33 6 5 39 28 5 4 0.9 0.3 to 3.5
Simple phobia 40 29 13 11 26 19 6 5 1.3 0.4 to 4.1
Social phobia 20 14 4 3 18 13 4 3 0.9 0.2 to 4.2
Agoraphobia 22 16 3 2 13 9 2 2 0.8 0.1 to 5.7
Panic disorderf 7 5 1 1 1 1 0 0 —e
Separation anxiety 36 26 12 10 40 29 6 5 2.4 0.8 to 7.5
Overanxious disorder 40 29 2 2 42 30 8 7 0.3 0.05 to 1.3
Substance use disorders 9 6 0 0 3 2 7 6 0.05 —c 0.004
Psychosis 1 1 0 0 0 0 1 1 1.0 —c 0.99
OCD 7 5 1 1 5 4 2 2 0.3 0.02 to 5.1
Tic disorder 21 15 5 4 23 16 5 4 1.1 0.3 to 4.5
Enuresis 35 25 7 6 45 32 17 14 0.5 0.2 to 1.5
Encopresis 6 4 8 7 3 2 9 8 0.5 —c 0.48
Anorexia 2 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 —e
Bulimia 2 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 —e
a Significant difference between boys and girls with ADHD (χ2=26.4, df=1, p<0.001).
b Significant difference between boys and girls with ADHD (χ2=10.3, df=1, p=0.001).
c Exact logistic regression used; meaningful confidence intervals were not estimated.
d Significant difference between boys and girls with ADHD (χ2=5.2, df=1, p=0.002).
e Not defined.
f Significant difference between boys and girls with ADHD (χ2=7.7, df=1, p=0.006).

learning disability in reading or mathematics, and less dicating that ADHD-associated impairments are correlates
likely to be associated with problems in school or fewer of ADHD in both boys and girls. However, between-gender
spare-time activities than ADHD in boys. Since these gen- differences were identified among the children with ADHD,
der differences were apparent in the absence of gender- such as the higher rate among girls of symptoms of inatten-
by-ADHD interactions, our results suggest that the risk for tion and lower rates of comorbidity with disruptive behav-
ADHD-associated impairments may be similarly elevated ior disorders, major depression, and learning disabilities.
in both boys and girls, but that gender-specific variation in Since these differences were attributable to the main effects
baseline risks may result in different rates of psychiatric of gender rather than modification of the ADHD effect by
morbidity and dysfunction that may adversely affect the gender, these findings indicate that girls were at the same
identification of the disorder in girls. relative risk for these adverse outcomes as boys but that
The single statistically significantly gender-by-ADHD girls had different clinical presentations.
interaction identified was the association between ADHD For example, although girls with ADHD were at signifi-
and substance use disorders (alcohol or drug abuse or de- cantly greater risk for disruptive behavior disorders (con-
pendence). That ADHD in girls was a more serious risk duct and oppositional defiant disorder) than girls without
factor for substance use disorders than it was in boys was ADHD (1), disruptive behavior disorders were clearly less
an unanticipated and surprising finding. In the light of on- prevalent in girls, regardless of ADHD status. As suggested
going concerns regarding ADHD as a putative risk factor by Gaub and Carlson (3), the lower risk of disruptive be-
for substance use disorders (21), this finding may indicate havior disorders in girls could have led to the underidenti-
that girls are particularly at risk in early adolescence. fication and underreferral of girls with ADHD because
Given that the ages at onset of ADHD and substance use clinical referrals are often based on overt problem behav-
disorders are separated by at least a decade (22, 23), this ior and aggression. Consistent with this suggestion, girls
finding would support the targeting of substance abuse with ADHD had fewer school problems and participated
prevention programs to girls with ADHD. in more spare-time activities than boys with ADHD.
With the single exception of substance use disorders, Furthermore, our results show that, although the com-
however, no statistically significant gender-by-ADHD inter- bined type of ADHD was the predominant type in both
actions were identified in the multiple outcomes evaluated. girls and boys, girls with ADHD were twice as likely as boys
These results suggest that with the exception of substance with ADHD to manifest the predominantly inattentive
use disorders, ADHD expresses itself similarly in boys and type of the disorder. Since symptoms of inattention are
girls relative to comparison subjects of the same gender, in- more covert than those of hyperactivity and impulsivity,

Am J Psychiatry 159:1, January 2002 39


GENDER AND ADHD

TABLE 3. Cognitive Functioning Among Girls and Boys in Two Studies of Children With and Without ADHD
Study of Girls Study of Boys Interaction of Gender
Measure ADHD (N=140) No ADHD (N=122) ADHD (N=140) No ADHD (N=120) and ADHD Status
Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Beta 95% CI

Block designa 11.2 3.1 12.4 3.2 13.3 3.8 15.2 3.3 –0.9 –2.0 to 0.3
Vocabulary 10.4 3.1 11.7 2.5 11.1 3.2 13.0 2.7 –0.7 –1.7 to 0.3
Digit span 8.8 3.1 10.5 2.9 8.9 2.8 10.7 2.8 –0.1 –1.1 to 0.9
Digit symbol codingb 10.7 3.1 12.2 3.1 9.9 3.5 11.8 2.6 –0.4 –1.5 to 0.6
Oral arithmeticc 9.9 2.7 12.0 2.9 10.6 3.1 12.4 2.9 0.3 –0.7 to 1.3
Freedom from distractibility IQ 98.4 13.7 108.9 16.7 98.8 15.8 111.1 13.3 –1.8 –7.0 to 3.4
Estimated full-scale IQd 104.0 12.5 109.9 11.2 109.5 13.5 118.4 10.2 –3.0 –7.2 to 1.1
Mathematical achievement 95.5 13.3 106.2 15.4 96.7 17.2 111.5 16.4 –3.9 –9.4 to 1.5
Reading achievemente 100.1 15.2 109.6 10.9 93.1 16.8 100.6 10.1 1.9 –2.9 to 6.7

N % N % N % N % Odds Ratio 95% CI

Learning disabilityf 16 12 7 6 40 30 12 10 1.7 0.5 to 5.5


Arithmeticg 9 7 5 4 28 21 9 8 1.9 0.5 to 7.7
Readingh 7 5 2 2 24 18 4 3 1.9 0.3 to 13.2
a Significant difference between boys and girls with ADHD (F=22.0, df=1, 275, p<0.001).
b Significant difference between boys and girls with ADHD (F=4.4, df=1, 275, p=0.04).
c Significant difference between boys and girls with ADHD (F=4.8, df=1, 275, p=0.03).
d Significant difference between boys and girls with ADHD (F=12.5, df=1, 275, p<0.001).
e Significant difference between boys and girls with ADHD (F=1.3, df=1, 268, p<0.001).
f Data were available for 509 subjects: 138 girls with ADHD, 122 girls without ADHD, 133 boys with ADHD, and 116 boys without ADHD. Signif-
icant difference between boys and girls with ADHD (χ2=13.9, df=1, p<0.001).
g Significant difference between boys and girls with ADHD (χ2=12.1, df=1, p<0.001).
h Significant difference between boys and girls with ADHD (χ2=11.3, df=1, p=0.001).

the higher rate of these symptoms in girls with ADHD than Modest but statistically significant differences were ob-
in boys with ADHD may also partially explain the mark- served in the treatment variables examined: boys with
edly higher male-to-female ratios in groups of children ADHD tended to receive more pharmacotherapy and psy-
who are clinically referred for ADHD than in children with chotherapy than girls with the disorder. Although this re-
the disorder who are not clinically referred. sult suggests that girls are less likely to receive appropriate
Although girls with ADHD had statistically significantly treatment for ADHD, the differences observed between
lower IQs than boys, these differences were small and of boys and girls were quite small and are not consistent with
limited clinical significance. These results are largely con- the notion of inadequate treatment of girls with ADHD.
sistent with those of other studies that have failed to iden- Rather, these findings could be interpreted as suggesting
tify meaningful effects of gender on cognitive perfor- that, once identified, ADHD may be treated similarly in
mance (24, 25), but we found that girls with ADHD had boys and girls.
significantly lower rates of psychometrically defined Our results must be viewed in the light of some method-
learning disabilities, reflecting the absence of a significant ological limitations. Because only Caucasian subjects
discrepancy between IQ and achievement scores in read- were included, our results do not generalize to children of
ing and mathematics. Since academic underachievement other racial or ethnic backgrounds. Since these results are
represents another leading reason for parents and teach- cross-sectional, we cannot test the longitudinal impact of
ers to seek help for children with ADHD, the markedly gender on ADHD or the relative effect of treatment in girls
lower rate of learning disability in girls with this disorder and boys. Our assessments relied on indirect parental re-
may also decrease identification of ADHD in girls. ports and direct interviews with those children older than
Girls with ADHD were at significantly greater risk for co- 12 years (half of the children studied) but did not include
morbid major depression than girls without ADHD (1) but information collected from teachers or younger children.
had a significantly lower rate of comorbid major depres- This limitation is not likely to affect the findings presented
sion than boys with ADHD. This finding was not antici- here because 1) both boys and girls were subjected to the
pated, since depression is commonly viewed as a predom- same methods, 2) we have found parent reports to be very
inantly female disorder (26). Although the reasons for this reliable and stable over time in our studies (29), and
result are not entirely clear, it is consistent with findings 3) others have raised questions regarding the validity of re-
documenting that prepubertal-onset major depression is ports taken from very young children (30).
largely male predominant (27). More consistent with the In this study, children who met DSM-III-R criteria for
adult literature is the finding of a higher rate of panic dis- ADHD but not DSM-IV criteria were included in the analy-
order in girls with ADHD than in boys with ADHD. The ex- sis. We did not use DSM-IV criteria to define ADHD in the
tant literature documents a higher rate of panic disorder study groups in this report because both samples were as-
in females over males, regardless of age (28). certained on the basis of DSM-III-R. On the basis of previ-

40 Am J Psychiatry 159:1, January 2002


BIEDERMAN, MICK, FARAONE, ET AL.

TABLE 4. Psychosocial Environment of Girls and Boys in Two Studies of Children With and Without ADHD
Study of Girls Study of Boys Interaction of Gender
ADHD (N=140) No ADHD (N=122) ADHD (N=140) No ADHD (N=120) and ADHD Status
Psychosocial Variable Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Beta 95% CI
Interpersonal relationships
School behavior problemsa 2.6 0.8 1.5 0.6 2.8 0.8 1.7 0.7 –0.04 –0.3 to 0.2
Spare time activitiesb 2.1 0.7 1.9 0.5 1.9 0.6 1.7 0.5 0.06 –0.1 to 0.3
Spare time problems 2.1 0.8 1.4 0.5 2.2 0.9 1.5 0.6 –0.03 –0.3 to 0.2
Activity with peers 2.1 0.7 1.7 0.6 2.1 0.8 1.7 0.6 –0.01 –0.2 to 0.2
Problems with peers 2.1 0.8 1.3 0.5 2.2 0.9 1.5 0.5 –0.03 –0.3 to 0.2
Relationships with opposite sexc 2.5 0.8 2.3 0.7 2.3 0.9 2.3 0.8 –0.20 –0.6 to 0.3
Problems with opposite sexc 1.6 0.8 1.2 0.5 1.4 0.7 1.4 0.5 –0.40 –0.7 to 0.01
Activity with siblings 1.8 0.7 1.7 0.7 1.8 0.9 1.5 0.7 0.10 –0.1 to 0.4
Problems with siblings 1.9 0.7 1.5 0.6 1.9 1.1 1.5 0.8 –0.06 –0.4 to 0.2
Relationship with mother 1.5 0.6 1.5 0.6 1.6 0.7 1.4 0.5 0.09 –0.1 to 0.3
Relationship with father 1.8 0.8 1.7 0.8 1.8 1.0 1.6 0.8 0.05 –0.3 to 0.3
Problems with parents 2.0 0.8 1.3 0.5 2.1 0.8 1.3 0.5 0.10 –0.1 to 0.4
Family environment
Conflict 58.4 11.4 51.2 12.2 57.6 13.2 50.7 13.1 –0.30 –4.7 to 4.0
Cohesion 42.2 19.8 53.9 16.0 43.6 21.7 53.1 18.2 2.30 –4.4 to 8.9
Expressiveness 46.5 12.9 47.5 14.9 47.4 14.8 51.9 14.6 –3.50 –8.5 to 1.5
a Significant difference between girls and boys with ADHD (F=4.5, df=1, 265, p=0.03).
b Significant difference between girls and boys with ADHD (F=6.6, df=1, 265, p=0.01).
c Based on a subgroup of children (N=224) for whom the questions were answered by a parent or by self-report.

ous work showing that DSM-III-R-diagnosed ADHD is Supported in part by National Institute of Child Health and Human
Development grant HD-36317 and NIMH grant MH-50657 (Dr. Bied-
highly convergent with DSM-IV-diagnosed ADHD in both
erman).
boys and girls (positive predictive value of 93%) (1, 31), we
believe that these results are informative today. The groups
of boys and girls were also ascertained 5 years apart from References
each other, so that cohort effects could be misconstrued as
1. Biederman J, Faraone S, Mick E, Williamson S, Wilens T, Spen-
gender effects. Although the baseline risk of psychopathol- cer T, Weber W, Jetton J, Kraus I, Pert J, Zallen B: Clinical corre-
ogy and other functional characteristics examined here lates of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in females: find-
could be expected to change with time, the odds ratio in ings from a large group of pediatrically and psychiatrically
contemporaneous comparison subjects should stay con- referred girls. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1999; 38:
966–975
stant. Thus, if we assume that there are no other confound-
2. Arnold L: Sex differences in ADHD: conference summary. J Ab-
ing variables, a statistically significant interaction could
norm Child Psychol 1996; 24:555–569
not be due to a cohort effect and may reasonably be attrib- 3. Gaub M, Carlson CL: Gender differences in ADHD: a meta-anal-
uted to gender. In addition, we could not specifically test ysis and critical review. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry
the hypothesis that girls are underidentified because all of 1997; 36:1036–1045
the children with ADHD were referred for treatment. 4. Sharp W, Walter J, Marsh W, Ritchie G, Hamburger S, Castellanos
Despite these considerations, our results suggest that X: ADHD in girls: clinical comparability of a research sample. J
Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1999; 38:40–47
although gender is a limited effect modifier of ADHD as a
5. Biederman J, Faraone SV, Keenan K, Benjamin J, Krifcher B,
risk factor for ADHD-associated dysfunction in referred Moore C, Sprich-Buckminster S, Ugaglia K, Jellinek MS, Stein-
children and adolescents, gender did have an effect on the gard R, Spencer T, Norman D, Kolodny R, Kraus I, Perrin J,
clinical presentation of the disorder. This was largely due Keller MB, Tsuang MT: Further evidence for family-genetic risk
to the finding that girls with ADHD were less likely than factors in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: patterns of
comorbidity in probands and relatives in psychiatrically and
boys with ADHD to have comorbid disruptive behavior
pediatrically referred samples. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1992; 49:
problems, learning disabilities, and social dysfunction 728–738
and that the rate of symptoms of inattention was higher in 6. Wechsler D: Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Chil-
girls with ADHD. Since these features could result in a gen- dren—Revised. New York, Psychological Corp, 1974
der-based referral bias unfavorable to girls, more work is 7. Orvaschel H, Puig-Antich J: Schedule for Affective Disorders
needed in both referred and nonreferred samples of chil- and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children—Epidemiologic
Version (K-SADS-E), 4th revision. Fort Lauderdale, Fla, Nova
dren with ADHD to assess this issue more fully.
University, Center for Psychological Studies, 1987
8. Spitzer RL, Williams JBW, Gibbon M, First MB: Structured Clini-
Received Nov. 27, 2000; revision received April 10, 2001; accepted cal Interview for DSM-III-R—Non-Patient Edition (SCID-NP, Ver-
June 26, 2001. From the Pediatric Psychopharmacology Unit, Child
sion 1.0). Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press, 1990
Psychiatry Service, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical
School. Address reprint requests to Dr. Biederman, Pediatric Psycho- 9. John K, Gammon GD, Prusoff BA, Warner V: The Social Adjust-
pharmacology Unit (WACC 725), Massachusetts General Hospital, 15 ment Inventory for Children and Adolescents (SAICA): testing of
Parkman St., Boston, MA 02114; biederman@helix.mgh.harvard.edu a new semistructured interview. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psy-
(e-mail). chiatry 1987; 26:898–911

Am J Psychiatry 159:1, January 2002 41


GENDER AND ADHD

10. Hollingshead AB: Four-Factor Index of Social Status. New Ha- 22. Biederman J, Wilens TE, Mick E, Faraone SV, Weber W, Curtis S,
ven, Conn, Yale University, Department of Sociology, 1975 Thornell A, Pfister K, Garcia Jetton J, Soriano J: Is ADHD a risk
11. Moos R: Family Environment Scale, in The Ninth Mental Health factor for psychoactive substance use disorders? findings from
Measurements Yearbook, vol 1. Edited by Mitchell JV. Lincoln, a four-year prospective follow-up study. J Am Acad Child Ado-
University of Nebraska, Buros Institute of Mental Measure- lesc Psychiatry 1997; 36:21–29
ments, 1985, pp 573–575 23. Mannuzza S, Klein RG, Bessler A, Malloy P, LaPadula M: Adult
12. Rutter M: Studies of Psychosocial Risk: The Power of Longitudi- outcome of hyperactive boys: educational achievement, occu-
nal Data. Cambridge, UK, Cambridge University Press, 1988 pational rank and psychiatric status. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1993;
13. Herjanic B, Reich W: Development of a structured psychiatric 50:565–576
interview for children: agreement between child and parent 24. Houghton S, Douglas G, West J, Whiting K, Wall M, Langsford S,
on individual symptoms. J Abnorm Child Psychol 1982; 10: Powell L, Carroll A: Differential patterns of executive function
307–324 in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder accord-
14. Orvaschel H: Psychiatric interviews suitable for use in research ing to gender and subtype. J Child Neurol 1999; 14:801–805
with children and adolescents. Psychopharmacol Bull 1985; 25. Arcia E, Conners CK: Gender differences in ADHD? J Dev Behav
21:737–745 Pediatr 1998; 19:77–83
15. Sattler J: Psychological Assessment. New York, McGraw-Hill,
26. Kessler RC, McGonagle KA, Zhao S, Nelson CB, Hughes M, Eshle-
1988
man S, Wittchen H-U, Kendler KS: Lifetime and 12-month prev-
16. Jastak S, Wilkinson GS: The Wide Range Achievement Test, Re-
alence of DSM-III-R psychiatric disorders in the United States:
vised. Wilmington, Del, Jastak Associates, 1985
results from the National Comorbidity Survey. Arch Gen Psychi-
17. Assistance to States for Education for Handicapped Children:
atry 1994; 51:8–19
Procedures for Evaluating Specific Learning Disabilities: Fed-
27. Angold A, Costello J, Erkanli A: Comorbidity. J Child Psychol Psy-
eral Register. Bethesda, Md, US Department of Health, Educa-
chiatry 1999; 40:57–87
tion, and Welfare, 1977
18. Reynolds CR: Critical measurement issues in learning disabili- 28. Wittchen H, Reed V, Kessler R: The relationship of agoraphobia
ties. J Spec Educ 1984; 18:451–476 and panic in a community sample of adolescents and young
19. Faraone SV, Biederman J, Krifcher Lehman B, Keenan K, Nor- adults. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1998; 55:1017–1024
man D, Seidman LJ, Kolodny R, Kraus I, Perrin J, Chen WJ: Evi- 29. Faraone SV, Biederman J, Milberger S: How reliable are mater-
dence for the independent familial transmission of attention nal reports of their children’s psychopathology? one year recall
deficit hyperactivity disorder and learning disabilities: results of psychiatric diagnoses of ADHD children. J Am Acad Child Ad-
from a family genetic study. Am J Psychiatry 1993; 150:891– olesc Psychiatry 1995; 34:1001–1008
895 30. Achenbach TM, McConaughy SH, Howell CT: Child/adolescent
20. Faraone S, Biederman J, Friedman D: Validity of DSM-IV sub- behavioral and emotional problems: implications of cross-in-
types of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a family study formant correlations for situational specificity. Psychol Bull
perspective. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2000; 39:300– 1987; 101:213–232
307 31. Biederman J, Faraone SV, Weber W, Russell RL, Rater M, Park K:
21. Biederman J, Wilens T, Mick E, Spencer T, Faraone SV: Pharma- Correspondence between DSM-III-R and DSM-IV attention defi-
cotherapy of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder reduces cit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psy-
risk for substance use disorder. Pediatrics 1999; 104:e20 chiatry 1997; 36:1682–1687

42 Am J Psychiatry 159:1, January 2002

You might also like