You are on page 1of 17

ARTICLES

Reconstruction of
Identity After Brain Injury
Mark Ylvisaker
College of Saint Rose, Albany, USA

Timothy Feeney
Wildwood Institute, Schenectady, USA

ollowing severe traumatic brain injury, difficulty with behavioural adjustment


F and community reintegration is common. A potential contributor to this diffi-
culty is a sense of personal identity that is inconsistent with the restrictions on
activity and need for effortful compensation imposed by persistent impairment.
We summarise an information processing framework within which the impact of
schematic mental models of self is explained and present intervention proce-
dures designed to help individuals with traumatic brain injury reconstruct an
organised and positive sense of personal identity. We conclude the paper with
three instructive case illustrations.

“That’s who I am; that’s what am; right tive, and personally appealing identities as life
or wrong, I can’t change that!” unfolds after a serious brain injury.
At a pivotal moment in the mobster movie Carlito’s
Way, the hero utters these lines to the woman he
loves. They both know that he has recently chosen
The Population
a course of action that will result in personal disas- Many young people with TBI enjoy a surprisingly
ter. And they both know that the person Carlito successful return to social, educational, and voca-
plans to help deserves nothing. But because Carlito tional pursuits after their injury, with or sometimes
believes that he has a debt to pay and because he without intensive rehabilitation. The subpopula-
has deeply implanted in his sense of self the con- tion selected for special attention in this paper
viction that important debts must be repaid, he includes adolescents and young adults with TBI
who experience substantial difficulty with commu-
chooses self-destruction over life and love.
nity reintegration after their injury and early reha-
Our goal in this paper is to describe an infor-
bilitation. In many cases, their difficulty with
mation processing theory within which apparently self-regulation after the injury is a complex conse-
irrational and self-destructive choices of this sort quence of preinjury academic, vocational, social,
can be readily understood, and to apply this theory or behavioural problems, combined with direct
to the rehabilitation of young adults with traumatic cognitive and self-regulatory effects of the injury
brain injury (TBI) who have behavioural difficul- and with postinjury adjustment problems. When
ties associated with their community reintegration. the injury includes damage to the frontal lobes,
In contrast to the fatalism implicit in Carlito’s dec- which are especially vulnerable in TBI (Adams et
laration, “I can’t change that,” our view is that al., 1980; Levin et al., 1991; Mendelsohn et al.,
action-guiding schematic mental models of self 1992), the resulting impairment of executive func-
can change, and that rehabilitation professionals tions may exaggerate problems with behavioural
have the opportunity to collaborate with the indi- and cognitive self-regulation that predated the
viduals they serve in constructing organised, posi- injury. Self-regulatory disturbance after TBI, par-

Address for correspondence: Mark Ylvisaker, Ph.D., 1171 Van Antwerp Rd. Schenectady, NY 12309, USA. Email:
ylvisakm@mail.strose.edu

BRAIN IMPAIRMENT
12 VOLUME 1 NUMBER 1 MAY 2000 pp. 12–28
RECONSTRUCTION OF IDENTITY AFTER BRAIN INJURY

ticularly in the presence of ventral prefrontal • theories of frontal lobe function from cogni-
damage (Levine, 1999; Levine et al., 1999), has tive, social, and general executive system per-
been shown to be more common than in carefully spectives (Grafman, 1995; Grafman at al.,
matched controls from the same socioeconomic 1993; Shallice, 1982, 1988; Shallice &
cohort (Dikman et al., 1995). The resulting mix of Burgess, 1991; Stuss & Benson, 1986)
challenges for this group threatens to spiral out of • neuropsychological theories of the self and
control in the absence of effective intervention and consciousness (Damasio, 1999; Stuss,
ongoing support. 1991a,b; Stuss & Alexander, 1999, in press;
For several years we have served young adults Stuss et al., in press)
with TBI through a community support project • Damasio’s work on somatic markers and the
funded by the New York State Department of inefficiency of learning from consequences fol-
Health. Individuals referred to the project must lowing ventral frontal lobe injury (Bechera et
present evidence of ineffective community reinte- al., 1994; Bechera et al., 1996; Damasio, 1994)
gration associated in part with behavioural con-
• Barnard and Teasdale’s Interacting Cognitive
cerns. In addition, a majority of the people we
serve have a pre- and/or postinjury history of alco- Subsystems approach to information process-
hol or drug abuse. Over the first four years of this ing and psychotherapy (Teasdale & Barnard,
project, we served 325 young adults either directly 1993)
or indirectly by working collaboratively with their • Lakoff and Johnson’s analysis of abstract lan-
service providers, employers, family members, guage as metaphorical, including the lan-
or others. The total estimated cost to the state for guage used to construct a sense of self
these 325 individuals during the year before their (Lakoff & Johnson, 1980, 1999)
referral to the program exceeded $30,000,000US. • Anderson’s findings regarding the severe lim-
This figure includes medical, rehabilitation, and itations on transfer of cognitive skill in normal
correctional costs, but does not include lost pro- cognition (Singley & Anderson, 1989)
ductivity, lost taxes, lost productivity of family • associated theories of transfer in behavioural
caregivers, and enormous stress for family and psychology (Baer, 1981)
service providers. Multiplied by the roughly four • Carr’s work on antecedent-focused behav-
decades of additional expected life for the mem- ioural intervention (Carr et al., 1998)
bers of this group, the projected financial burden • the extensive experimental work of Deci and
is staggering if they are not effectively rehabili- Ryan on self-motivation and the dangers of
tated and supported. over-reliance on extrinsic motivation (Deci,
This population has been neglected in most 1995)
published reports of TBI rehabilitation research,
because exclusionary criteria used in these stud- • Vygotsky’s theories of cognitive development
ies typically include history of substance abuse, and the associated apprenticeship approach to
psychiatric diagnosis, or significant behaviour teaching (Vygotsky, 1962, 1978)
problems. This relative neglect of the group, com- • Mann’s exhaustive exploration of the 2500
bined with their economic and social importance, year history of cognitive process training for
justifies increased attention in the rehabilitation people with a variety of profiles of ability and
literature. However, the intervention themes disability (Mann, 1979).
described in this paper also apply to less complex In this paper, we restrict our attention to only two
and behaviourally troubling situations, a point to of these theoretical pillars, the Interacting
which we return later. Cognitive Subsystems theory of information pro-
cessing and clinical intervention, and the self-as-
metaphor hypothesis.
A Framework for Person- As we have incorporated these theoretical
Centred, Context-Sensitive foundations and associated empirical evidence
Intervention and Support into our clinical practice over a combined 40
Ylvisaker and Feeney (1998) described and illus- years of serving this population, the following
trated a comprehensive approach to rehabilitation critical intervention themes have emerged:
for individuals with combined cognitive, behav- • Context: Intervention delivered largely outside
ioural, communication, and executive system of the context of personally meaningful
impairment associated with TBI, or more specifi- themes, activities, settings, and interactions is
cally with frontolimbic injury. The approach has unlikely to have a lasting effect for people with
a number of theoretical supports: chronic cognitive and behavioural impairment.

13
MARK YLVISAKER AND TIMOTHY FEENEY

• Everyday Routines: When appropriately ongoing disability, an attempt is finally made to


structured, the everyday routines of life are reduce the associated handicap (i.e., increase
the ideal context within which to deliver reha- participation in chosen life activities) by creat-
bilitation services and offer ongoing support. ing environmental or task modifications and
• Everyday People: When everyday people in modifying the supportive behaviours of others
the life of the person with TBI (possibly in the environment. The approach that we have
including family members, work supervisors, used for individuals with chronic
job coaches, direct care staff, and others) play cognitive, behavioural, and communication
their support roles effectively, rehabilitation impairment reverses this hierarchy. In some
is ongoing and appropriately contextualised. cases, though not in all, long-term practice of
• Apprenticeship Teaching: Using the participation-enhancing and disability-reduc-
Vygotskyan apprenticeship approach to ing strategies results in automatisation and
teaching and cognitive development, relevant internalisation of the strategies and ultimate
information and skills are acquired as the reduction of the underlying impairment.
individual is engaged in meaningful tasks
designed so that the target information or skill Interacting Cognitive
is processed during completion of the task
and in order to achieve the goal of the task.
Subsystems (ICS) Approach
The facilitator provides needed supports and ICS was originally developed by Barnard as a
mediation at the outset, systematically comprehensive information-processing theoretical
removing the supports as it becomes possible account of varied cognitive phenomena, such as
to do so. Success can be guaranteed if errors verbal learning, text comprehension, and the like
or failure pose serious threats to learning. (e.g., Barnard, 1985). The theoretical model
Large numbers of learning trials may be includes standard cognitive components (e.g.,
required, but not delivered through massed, nine cognitive subsystems) and their interrelation-
discrete, decontextualised training trials as ships. Three of the codes encode sensory informa-
conceived in traditional training models. tion (Acoustic, Visual, and Body-State codes),
• Collaboration: Associated with apprentice- two encode intermediate structural level descrip-
ship teaching is the principle that profession- tions (Morphonolexical and Object codes), two
als interact with the people they serve in a encode higher levels of meaning (Propositional
collaborative manner as well as with profes- and Implicational codes), and two are action or
sional colleagues. effector codes (Articulatory and Limb codes).
Information processing is based on storage within
• Meaningful Engagement in Chosen Life code-specific storage systems and on the transfor-
Activities: Our experience with several hun- mation of information from one code into another,
dred young adults with chronic impairment transformations that are learned on the basis of
after TBI has led us to conclude that interven- regularities in experience. Teasdale and Barnard
tions are likely to fail in the absence of mean- (1993) offer detailed descriptions of this cognitive
ingful engagement in chosen life activities. architecture and illustrate its explanatory power in
• Reconstruction of a Satisfying Sense of Self: relation to both normal cognition and clinically
The changes in ability profiles and activity significant phenomena.
possibilities after TBI often necessitate From our perspective, the critical features of
restructuring one’s schematic mental model the theory are its distinction between proposi-
of self, a process that can be facilitated by tional meaning and implicational meaning, its
using powerful and personally compelling integration of emotion into theoretical accounts
self-metaphors and by ensuring a large of information processing, and the central posi-
number of successful experiences associated tion accorded implicational meaning in the pro-
with the new model. cessing of information and regulation of emotions
• Impairment, Disability (Activity), and and behaviour (Teasdale & Barnard, 1993). The
Handicap (Participation): Traditional rehabili- Propositional code includes meanings that are
tation attempts first to eliminate or reduce the represented as discrete concepts (e.g., dogs as
underlying neurologically-based impairment. four-legged animals) and the relationships among
In the event of ongoing impairment, an attempt them (e.g., dogs chase cats) Propositional mean-
is then made to reduce the associated func- ings are relatively specific, can be expressed by
tional disability through compensatory strategy literal uses of language, bear truth value, and are
training or task-specific training. In the event of supported or rejected on the basis of standard fac-

14
RECONSTRUCTION OF IDENTITY AFTER BRAIN INJURY

tual evidence. The knowledge structures encoded language, as a cognitive phenomenon, is under-
in the Propositional code may include schemas stood within ICS as (1) registration of sensory
and scripts of some generality (e.g., a-going-out- information in the Acoustic code, (2) transforma-
to-eat script), but tied to verifiable facts and rela- tion to the Morphonolexical code for intermedi-
tionships, not to affect. Propositional-level ate-level linguistic analysis, (3) comprehension of
knowledge structures contribute to the organisa- the message’s literal meaning, evaluation of its
tion of experience and efficiency of memory and truth value, and storage as semantic meaning in
verbal expression, without engaging motivation the Propositional code, and (4) higher level inter-
or directly influencing decision making. Much of pretation of the message and its potentially
the stored knowledge typically referred to as the hidden meaning (e.g., threat or flirtation) in the
semantic knowledge base or declarative knowl- Implicational code. Implicational-level interpre-
edge is included in the Propositional code. tation may influence propositional-level interpre-
In contrast, meanings stored in the tation. However, separate storage systems are
Implicational code are generic and holistic, cap- hypothesised for the separate codes, explaining
turing deep regularities, themes, and interrela- the frequently observed disparity between propo-
tionships in human experience. These meanings sitional-level understanding of an event or unit of
do not map directly onto literal language, but discourse and its implicational-level interpreta-
rather rely on indirect language for their expres- tion and associated affective response.
sion, including metaphor, narrative, image, Schematic mental models of experience and
symbol, poetry, parables and the like. Beliefs at of the self are constructed in the Implicational
the implicational level (e.g., I am strong and code, representing relations among generic fea-
heroic; I am a worthless victim) are not evaluated tures of experience, relations between recurring
as true or false, but rather as motivating or dis- experiences and their associated emotional states,
heartening, productive or stifling, inspiring or and procedural knowledge or action strategies
boring, calming or troubling, organised or dif- associated with the experiences. Propositional-
fuse, personally compelling or personally repug- level knowledge about the self-as-object may
nant (Teasdale, 1997). include a large number of objective biological
Meanings stored in the Implicational code are and biographical facts. However, the sense of
bidirectionally tied to emotion and affect. Thus, self-as-subject, complete with aspirations, stan-
visceral (somatic) states and emotions can trigger dards, self-evaluations, emotional associations,
and influence the development of beliefs at this and related action strategies, is stored in the
level (e.g., the negative emotions associated with Implicational code, with a multitude of connec-
punishment can elicit and strengthen the implica- tions to all other codes, including the sensory
tional sense of self-as-victim); conversely, attend- code. Thus, for example, a specific tone of voice
ing to implicational-level representations can in the speech of an authority figure may automat-
induce emotions (e.g., thinking of oneself as a ically trigger a “self-as-victim” mental model and
person in control of one’s destiny can elicit posi- the procedural strategies (e.g., flee or fight) asso-
tive emotional states). In this sense, the ciated with that model.
Implicational code is directly tied to motivation,
decision making, and action. ICS in Psychotherapy as
Furthermore, complex implicational-level a Model for Rehabilitation
representations, such as an elaborated sense of Applied to the treatment of depression, ICS offers
personal identity, influence the processing of an explanation for the success of cognitive and
apparently factual information. For example, other therapies (e.g., physical exercise) when they
some apparently obvious “facts” (e.g., “I cannot are successful (Teasdale & Barnard, 1993). ICS
walk because of my brain injury”) may be auto- also suggests an explanation for the frequent fail-
matically rejected because of their incompatibil- ure of cognitive therapy, understood as the
ity with important implicational-level beliefs. attempt to marshal factual evidence in opposition
Other beliefs (e.g., “the rehabilitation staff are to beliefs about the self that are judged to be the
trying to make me an invalid”) may be fabricated basis of the depressed emotional state (Beck et
in an attempt to maintain important implicational- al., 1979). Because factual evidence, discussed
level representations of self in the face of experi- dispassionately, is represented only in the
ences that threaten those representations. Propositional code, there is no reason to expect
Explanations of everyday information pro- that it will automatically affect implicational-
cessing are also enhanced within the ICS frame- level schematic mental models of the self, and
work. For example, auditory comprehension of therefore no reason to expect that it will automat-

15
MARK YLVISAKER AND TIMOTHY FEENEY

ically influence emotion or action. Teasdale and


Barnard (1993) describe a “depressive interlock”
or vicious cycle of depression that occurs when
depressogenic mental models (e.g., sense of self
as unworthy failure and of life as hopeless) are
stored in the Implicational code, fed by selected
body states that are associated with and intensify
the core mental model as well as by proposi-
tional-level negative facts (e.g., I did yet another
task poorly) that are selected for attention by the
core depressogenic mental model and recipro-
cally intensify the model.
These reciprocal transformations of meaning
among various information codes are at the core of
all controlled information processing, according to
the ICS model (Teasdale & Barnard, 1993). It is
also at the core of the vicious cycle of depression,
wherein core depressogenic beliefs generate their
own sensory and factual evidence base, which in
turn strengthens the core beliefs. In our experience,
a similar self-perpetuating vicious cycle is associ-
ated with escalating behavioural disorders in many
young adults with TBI. For example, a person upset
with restrictions imposed by the disability and by
caregivers may react negatively to the restrictions
and the sense of identity implied by the restrictions,
leading caregivers to increase their control, result-
ing in intensified negative reactions from the FIGURE 1
person with disability. Just as a depressed mental
A negative schematic model of self in the Implicational
state feeds itself with evidence generated by the code (e.g., Jason’s victim model) generates problem
mental model, so also negative behaviour and the behaviours when provocation (defined by that model) is
associated oppositional sense of identity can feed perceived. The problem behaviours produce negative
on themselves by creating a world in which the feedback in the environment, yielding negative somatic
mental model fits the facts, which in turn strength- effects and associated negative somatic markers, which
ens the mental model. Furthermore, once con- in turn intensify the negative schematic model. In addi-
structed, a negative mental model has the power to tion, negative somatic effects can be generated directly
from the model with no need for environmental feed-
trigger negative somatic states, feeding a self-sus-
back. At the same time, the model selects propositional-
taining loop that does not require negative feedback level meanings that fit and reciprocally strengthen the
in the environment. Figure 1, adapted from model. Based on Teasdale (1999).
Teasdale (1999), illustrates this analog to “depres-
sive interlock” in many individuals with TBI.
Psychotherapy, within an ICS framework, has Application of ICS to TBI Rehabilitation
as its primary task “to create, in situations that Severe TBI almost always results in some type and
might otherwise elicit maladaptive emotions, degree of persistent impairment, associated restric-
alternative schematic models (generic, implicit tions on activities and reductions in experiences of
meanings) that do not produce dysfunctional success (as judged by the person’s preinjury stan-
emotional reactions.” (Teasdale, 1997, p. 148). In dards). For example, a student accustomed to
this way, the vicious cycle or depressive interlock receiving As and Bs may struggle to earn Cs after
is broken. In the remainder of this paper, we the injury; an athlete may not be able to return to
argue that a critical component of rehabilitation favoured sports; a young person who enjoyed great
for many people with TBI is the process of help- popularity may see friends go their separate ways.
ing them construct alternative schematic models In situations such as these, the schematic mental
of the self that can be triggered automatically by model of personal identity undergoes inevitable
otherwise problematic stimuli and that are associ- modification, ranging from relatively minor and
ated with positive emotional states and socially helpful (e.g., “I used to be a person who succeeded
successful action strategies. with little effort; now I must try harder, use special

16
RECONSTRUCTION OF IDENTITY AFTER BRAIN INJURY

strategies, and sometimes seek the help of others”) sciousness (i.e., consciousness of the self as a
to wholesale and destructive (e.g., “I used to be continuous entity across time, Tulving, 1985) to
king of the hill; now I’m nothing more than a mis- self-regulatory disturbances following TBI
erable helpless victim”). (Levine, 1999; Levine et al., 1999).
In some cases, like those presented later, the ICS offers one effective resolution to Hans
person with TBI may fight heroically to maintain Teuber’s famous “riddle” of the frontal lobes,
a preinjury sense of self, intensely resisting the namely the curious dissociation between knowing
identity implicitly communicated by rehabilita- and doing (Teuber, 1964). When knowing is
tion professionals whose focus is predictably on understood as a Propositional code phenomenon
the person’s disability and the changes in life and doing is understood largely as a product of
apparently mandated by the disability. This iden- Implicational code forces, the dissociation loses
tity battle, often subconscious, runs the risk of some of its curiosity. If certain types of frontal
creating a dangerous new identity, namely that of lobe injury weaken connections between these
a person for whom a sense of strength and self- two codes or if the schematic model of self stored
control and other positive emotions are associated in the Implicational code is fundamentally at odds
only with a mental model of a person committed with facts stored in the Propositional code, then
to fighting against the control and restrictions the dissociation described by Teuber is pre-
imposed by others. In such cases, positive out- dictable rather than curious.
comes of rehabilitation may require the collabo-
rative construction of a mental model of self that
includes the required sense of strength and con- Self as Metaphor
trol, but at the same time accounts for the changes With the psychological and neuropsychological
in behaviour and life circumstances possibly dic- themes of the previous section as background, it
tated by ongoing impairment. has become increasingly clear that effective brain
These dynamics may, of course, be associated injury rehabilitation may require a planned and
with any acquired disability. With severe TBI, organised effort at collaboratively constructing
typically associated with some degree of frontal one or more positive schematic mental models of
lobe injury, there is the added complication that self. Facilitating this process with exploration of
pre-existing mental models of self may be frag- concrete and personally compelling metaphors
mented and new effective models may be hard to has its basis in three related hypotheses: that all
construct because of damage to the frontal lobes. thinking about the self is essentially metaphori-
It has been hypothesised for many years that pre- cal; that social construction of identity through
frontal regions are instrumental in storing and metaphor is a natural developmental process; and
applying complex schemas, scripts, mental that people whose thinking has been rendered
models, or other Managerial Knowledge Units concrete by frontal lobe injury may especially
that guide organised information processing, effi- benefit from identity construction using person-
cient memory and retrieval, organised discourse, ally meaningful and compelling metaphors, rep-
and planful, goal-directed behaviour in complex resented within concrete graphic organisers.
situations (Grafman, 1989). Lakoff and Johnson (1999) have argued per-
With respect to the neuropsychology of suasively that all thinking about the self is
models of self and personal identity, studies of metaphorical. For example, self-control is repre-
patients with frontal lobe lesions have increas- sented as object control (e.g., “I dragged myself
ingly suggested that prefrontal structures are crit- out of bed”, “You’re pushing yourself too hard”,
ical for constructing and effectively using a “He is not holding himself together”) or object
coherent model of the self, organised over time possession (“I lost myself in the activity”, “He was
and diverse experiences, to remember, integrate, in the grip of fear”) or location (e.g., “I was beside
and interpret ongoing experience (Stuss et al., in myself”, “You’re really out of it today”, “He’s out
press). Specifically, the right hemisphere frontal to lunch”). Personal integration and its opposite
lobe appears to play a critical role in the repre- are represented as physical intactness (e.g., “I’m
sentation of self (Craik et al., 1999). “The right feeling very scattered today”, “You’re all over the
frontal lobe does serve as a specific convergence place”, “She’s perfectly centred”). The social self
site for all of the neural processes essential to is similarly represented with social metaphors
affectively personalise higher order experience of (e.g., “I’m at war with myself about this”, “You
self and to represent awareness of that experi- need to nurture yourself; wean yourself; pamper
ence” (Stuss & Alexander, 1999, p. 223). Levine yourself”, “He needs to be true to himself, not let
has connected these theories of autonoetic con- himself down”). Lakoff and Johnson presented

17
MARK YLVISAKER AND TIMOTHY FEENEY

many more examples from English and Japanese similarly relied on metaphor and parables to
to support their hypothesis that the self is a explain their teachings and make them appealing.
metaphorical construct and that the metaphors are In our work with young people with TBI, we
rather consistent across cultures. have similarly found metaphor effective in explor-
ing abstract or challenging concepts. Although
Identity Construction some texts in the psychology of language group
Through Appealing Metaphors metaphor with other abstract uses of language,
requiring literal processing and, in addition, pro-
From very early in development, parents help their cessing of the indirect meaning, familiar
children construct a positive sense of self through metaphors can be more concrete and require less
metaphor. For example, praising a toddler who complex processing than a literal formulation of
uses a potty chair by saying, “You’re such a big the same content. The language of the streets, pris-
boy!! You’re just like Daddy!! Good job!” has the ons, and similar nonacademic linguistic venues is
effect of contributing to an evolving mental model richly metaphorical. For example, most familiar
of self and at the same time attaching positive curses are metaphors that do not require two
emotions to that model. Similarly, on the negative stages of processing for their interpretation. (See
side, ill-conceived reprimands can contribute to Nippold, 1998, for a useful review of the develop-
the construction of a negative model of self asso- mental literature on metaphor comprehension.)
ciated with negative emotions (e.g., “There’s the In the absence of planned metaphorical recon-
klutz tripping over his own feet again!” “You’re struction of identity, unplanned reconstruction
such a loser!” “Why do you always act like a cry often occurs and may not be positive or effective.
baby?”). When identity metaphors are constructed As a result, other apparently well conceived inter-
out of carefully selected attributes of personal ventions may fail because the behaviours to be
heroes, the process is well designed for adoles- trained do not fit with the person’s identity-defin-
cents or others at a generally adolescent level of ing, implicational-level mental model of self. For
development, given the power of heroes and role example, practicing social skills in a therapy
models in shaping adolescent identity and moti- group will predictably fail at the level of transfer
vating their behaviour (Conger, 1984; to everyday interactions if those social behaviours
Czikszentmilhalyi & Larson, 1984). are not consistent with the person’s underlying
sense of self. This important theme is illustrated
Metaphor and Comprehension by the young adult who successfully practices
Across many cultures and many domains of polite requests for clarification in therapy, but
teaching, those teachers acknowledged to be most never uses such a request in real life because his
effective are famous for their creative exploitation model of identity dictates that all requests for help
of concrete metaphors, parables, narratives, are signs of weakness and incompetence.
proverbs, and other analogical representations of
reality in explaining complex or abstract material. Metaphor and Processing
For example, Socrates explained executive func- Efficiency: Metaphoric Compaction
tions to sports-minded students with his famous Because people with TBI often have reduced
charioteer metaphor: human reason is like a char- working memory capacity, there is great value in
ioteer struggling to control two unruly steeds, the combining several procedures into one thought
appetites and the passions. (Interestingly, the con- unit. Metaphoric compaction is common in many
cept of “executive functions” in modern psychol- activities, including sports. For example, in base-
ogy is itself a metaphor, using an analogical base ball the “squeeze play” includes the following
more fitting for late 20th century students, procedures, all of which are represented automat-
although both metaphors misleadingly suggest ically when the play is sufficiently well
that the executive dimensions of processing are rehearsed: (1) The pitcher begins his wind up; (2)
like a little man in the frontal lobes directing traf- The batter turns and squares to bunt; (3) The
fic.) Similarly, Jesus explained hard-to-grasp the- pitcher releases the ball; (4) The runner on 3rd
ological and moral concepts with simple stories takes off for home; (5) The batter bunts the ball;
and metaphors (e.g., stories about fishing and (6) The runner crosses the plate.
farming) that were familiar and compelling to his Similarly, for Jason, described below, the
uneducated audience. Moses, Solomon, Buddha, Clint Eastwood metaphor included in one thought
Confucius, Lao Tsu and many other extraordinary unit the following series of events and actions: (1)
teachers who have influenced the lives of count- recognise a socially volatile situation, (2) sense
less millions of people throughout history have specific provocation from another person, (3) do

18
RECONSTRUCTION OF IDENTITY AFTER BRAIN INJURY

not act, (4) appear calm and in control, (5) use a of strength with restraint, is unlikely to be elicited
simple script (e.g., “We can talk about this later”), by a threatening situation in the world without
(6) leave the situation. Whereas six or even two or many experiences of threat followed by an act of
three procedures may be too many for a person restraint (associated with the self-as-master
with working memory impairment possibly com- model) followed by positive consequences and
bined with catastrophic reactions to emotional positive emotional states. Ensuring that the
stimuli, one well-selected metaphoric thought person has a large number of such experiences
unit, automatically associated with the six proce- brings in the entire apparatus of antecedent-
dures, may be sufficient. focused behaviour management, which we have
discussed elsewhere (Feeney & Ylvisaker, 1995,
Concrete Organisation of Identity 1997; Ylvisaker & Feeney, 1998, 2000).
Models Using Graphic Organisers
Graphic organisers, similar to the identity maps Case Illustrations
presented later in this paper, have become a stan- In the three case illustrations that follow, collabo-
dard instructional support in both regular and spe- rative identity reconstruction was a component of
cial education, and have been applied successfully a larger package of collaborative intervention and
to cognitive rehabilitation after brain injury support, designed to help the individuals succeed
(Ylvisaker et al., 1998). Language arts workbooks in their chosen contexts of life. In each case, the
for elementary school students are filled with flow identity metaphor was collaboratively negotiated
charts and other diagrams that graphically represent and organised in conversation with the individual,
the organisation of many forms of complex think- the components of the schematic mental model of
ing and discourse, including narratives, similari- self were organised as concretely as possible, and
ties-differences reasoning, character analysis, self-presentation on video tape was used to
scientific explanations, persuasive discourse, and rehearse and internalise the metaphoric sense of
many more. The underlying premise is that people self and also to communicate the identity and
unfamiliar with complex conceptual territory bene- associated strategies to other relevant people.
fit from a “map” that illustrates the way in which
that territory is organised. Because schematic Jason
models of identity include many components that
would be difficult to grasp and remember without Elsewhere we have used Jason’s instructive history
clear organisation, graphic advance organisers may to illustrate specific aspects of brain injury rehabil-
facilitate the process of constructing the model and itation (Ylvisaker & Feeney, 1996, 1998, 2000). In
remembering it. For this reason we developed this paper we focus only on identity reconstruction,
“identity mapping” (see below) as a component of a critical component of his successful rehabilita-
the intervention for those individuals who have tion. Prior to his injury at age 24, Jason had been a
some degree of organisational impairment and who reasonably successful student and hard worker,
are free of serious visual perceptual impairment often working two jobs to support the activities that
that might cause such maps to be confusing. he enjoyed. He also had a long history of opposi-
tional behaviour, including conflicts with family
members and peers, serious behavioural difficulties
Automatising Positive Identity Models at two universities, including an expulsion for com-
Positive implicational-level models of self are not bative behaviour, abuse of alcohol and drugs, and
automatically elicited by relevant situations in the discharge from the marines because of his com-
world (associated with sensory and visceral states manding officer’s concern about potential noncom-
of the individual) without extensive practice and pliance under battle conditions.
a substantial history of positive experiences asso- Jason was hit by a car while riding his motor-
ciated with the new model (Teasdale & Barnard, cycle, incurring widespread brain injury, includ-
1993). For example, a person may respond habit- ing bilateral frontal lobe damage. He remained
ually to perceived acts of disrespect by verbally largely unresponsive for about six weeks, and con-
or physically attacking the perpetrator of the dis- tinued to receive acute inpatient rehabilitation ser-
respect. This automatically triggered action strat- vices for an additional six weeks. At that time, he
egy may be firmly associated with a schematic discharged himself against medical advice and
mental model of self-as-victim connected with a attempted to resume his preinjury life. Although
conviction that strength lies in counter-attack. A his injuries were severe, including prolonged
newly constructed mental model of self-as- coma and several orthopedic injuries that required
master, connected with a procedural association a series of surgical interventions, Jason’s cognitive

19
MARK YLVISAKER AND TIMOTHY FEENEY

recovery from a psychometric perspective was


good, although probably depressed relative to
preinjury levels. Three years postinjury he attained
a Full Scale IQ of 109, above average performance
on the Wechsler Memory Scale, and satisfactory
performance on measures said to be sensitive to
executive system impairment. Persistent disability
in real-world situations included mildly slowed
processing, mild memory problems in demanding
real-world tasks, and significant executive system
weakness, including difficulty with planning and
organising, mood regulation problems, severe
impulsiveness, and inefficiency in learning from
consequences. This disparity between psychomet-
ric and real-world performance is reminiscent of
classic frontal lobe patients, such as EVR
(Damasio, 1994; Eslinger & Damasio, 1985). In
addition, Jason had received a number of psychi-
atric diagnoses associated with his multiple admis-
sions to psychiatric hospitals.
Jason has characterised the next four years of FIGURE 2
his life as a downward spiral of failure, frustration, The schematic model of identity (identity map) as Jason
anger, anxiety, depression, and defiance. During perceived it to be implied by the controlling and conde-
those four years he married and quickly divorced; scending behaviour of medical professionals and
attempted work, but could not maintain employ- others. He resisted this model.
ment; returned to school, but could not succeed
academically. Several acts of violence (including body who chose to exercise control over him or
two that involved guns) and attempts at suicide led place unreasonable restrictions on his choices and
to multiple incarcerations and admissions to psy- behaviour. In retrospect, Jason recognised that the
chiatric hospitals. Jason spent most of his fourth extreme actions and values associated with this
year postinjury, the year before we began to work identity were in part a reaction to the identity that
with him, in jail and psychiatric hospitals. he believed others wished to impose on him. The
Through the New York State TBI Behavioral acts of violence that resulted from the procedural
Support Project, we worked with Jason to support knowledge stored with this mental model were
his community reintegration and independent acknowledged by Jason to be ultimately self-
living after these difficult four years. A long destructive, but, like Carlito’s fateful decision,
series of conversations with Jason, all of which were dictated by a sense of self securely stored in
took place in natural settings (e.g., in his home or his Implicational code.
over breakfast in a diner), revealed that two As Jason was leaving his last psychiatric hos-
schematic models of identity had evolved over pitalisation and preparing for another attempt at
the previous four years. Figure 2, Jason-the- community living, we helped him make a self-
Victim, represents the sense of self that Jason advocacy video tape (protocol in Ylvisaker et al.,
believed many members of the medical commu- 1998), designed in part to let potential community
nity and others wished to impose on him. Much support staff know who he was, what he valued,
of his resistance to ongoing rehabilitation and to and how they would need to act if they wished to
the compensations and modifications recom- help him. As he produced and subsequently
mended by rehabilitation professionals was dic- watched this video, he began to fashion for himself
tated by his negative reaction to this (for him) a new model of self, consistent with his cherished
implied identity. themes of power, success, independence, and con-
Figure 3 represents the sense of identity that trol, but also consistent with his need to work
Jason had come to accept as his true self. He rep- extremely hard at directing himself to achieve suc-
resented this sense of self with a marine metaphor, cess as he defined it. He represented this identity
but included only selected aspects of marine iden- metaphorically as Jason-as-Clint-Eastwood, based
tity for this model. The major themes of the model on the critical insight that, like Clint Eastwood,
were success, power, control, extreme actions and Jason would now have to take seriously his roles as
beliefs, and uncompromising resistance to any- both actor and director and, also like Clint

20
RECONSTRUCTION OF IDENTITY AFTER BRAIN INJURY

FIGURE 4
The positive schematic model of identity (identity map)
FIGURE 3
that Jason collaboratively constructed in an attempt to
Jason’s accepted schematic model of identity (identity be successful in community living despite residual
map) as it evolved over four years postinjury in reaction impairment.
to his circumstances and the behaviour of others.

year, the self-direction and positive interaction


Eastwood, he would succeed by attaching positive scripts that he initially implemented with great
emotions, like strength and power over others, effort and external support, have become increas-
with restraint, careful use of scripts, and effective
ingly automatic. An intervention that began largely
use of support personnel (see Figure 4).
as a set of participation-oriented external supports
Having created for himself an emotionally
compelling, metaphorically represented sense of and environmental modifications gradually became
personal identity consistent with his goals, but disability-reducing strategies that Jason used with
also with his executive system impairment, Jason little external support, and finally, with a great deal
willingly worked at developing scripts, strategies, of practice, were internalised to effect reduction of
systems of support, and explicit rules of self- his underlying executive system impairment.
direction that he then practiced in his everyday Furthermore, Jason now creates for himself
activities and interactions (Lewin, 1998; Ylvisaker personal metaphors to help him succeed in situa-
& Feeney, 1996). It is now five years later and he tions in which he anticipates difficulty. For exam-
reports that the Clint Eastwood metaphor was a ple, during the anxious weeks preceding his
critical contributor to stopping his four-year wedding, he imagined himself in a dust storm,
downward cycle of frustration, failure, anger, and with the only intelligent strategy being to wait for
defiance. The compelling vision of self that it the dust to settle before making any important
offered not only made him receptive to the hard decisions. Jason was recently honoured by the
work needed to habituate positive alternative Department of Health for his successful use of
action strategies, it also automatically yielded
department resources. He negotiated the ceremony
important procedures at those times of stress when
he otherwise would not have been able to retrieve and associated interactions, which were extremely
from memory a sequence of positive actions. stressful for him, by picturing himself as an officer
Jason’s life has become increasingly successful in the marines, complete with the formal attire,
over the past five years. He maintains a home posture, and script to match this rank. In his role as
with his wife of one year and works with us as a peer counselor, Jason now works with us to help
peer counselor and trainer for staff needing to other individuals with TBI reconstruct positive
understand behaviour management after TBI. schematic models of self with which they might
Furthermore, Jason reports that with each passing rebuild their lives after the injury.

21
MARK YLVISAKER AND TIMOTHY FEENEY

An additional measure of success for Jason, would prefer social interaction at work to be free
especially relevant in an era of cost containment, of conflict. However, he rejected out of hand sug-
is the dramatic reduction in cost for his services gestions that he simply ignore or walk away from
and supports. Whereas in the fourth year follow- perceived peer provocation. He said that his sense
ing his injury (the year before this intervention of self and honour — and associated reactions to
began) his cost to the state exceeded $125,000US, provocation — were shaped on the streets in the
the cost had shrunk to about $2000US for the context of drug dealing and use. At best, walking
fourth year after the intervention began. away from provocation caused him to feel con-
fused about who he was; at worst, it was an
Bill abhorrent admission of weakness.
Bill’s brain injury resulted from a fall from a fifth A brief exploration of heroes revealed that
floor window (a possible suicide attempt). Prior to Bill had profound admiration for the rap singer
his injury, Bill had lived in homeless shelters or on LL Cool J, both as a performer and as a person.
the streets, where he sold and used drugs. He had Over the course of two or three conversations in
been arrested several times on drug-related charges. his home, we collaboratively organised an iden-
Reports indicated that Bill was an inpatient in an tity map that included values, goals, and action
acute care hospital for six weeks (including three strategies important to Bill, metaphorically
weeks in coma) and in acute rehabilitation for exploiting two importantly different and very real
approximately one year. Brain injury included aspects of the LL Cool J persona. Bill chose the
severe bilateral frontal lobe damage with additional term Lyrical Lord to refer to the rapper and the
primary impact damage to the temporal and pari- cool behaviours associated with that life style.
etal regions. A subsequent MRI scan revealed dif- The other side of LL Cool J, the successful hus-
fuse bilateral degeneration of the dorsal and medial band, father, and business man, came to be
frontal regions and dorsal occipital regions, with labeled LL the Business Man.
significant ventricular enlargement. We briefly worked on associating feelings
Approximately five years postinjury, Bill’s and action strategies with both aspects of this
motor functions, including speech, were grossly identity metaphor. In both cases, acting like LL
normal and psychometrically assessed cognitive Cool J meant feeling cool, powerful, strong, con-
functions were only mildly depressed (Full Scale trolled, and confident. However, LL the Business
IQ: 80). Functionally, he had ongoing visual-per- Man had to act in a planful manner, create scripts
ceptual problems, spatial and temporal orienta- in advance, direct himself to use the scripts, and
tion problems, and generalised difficulty with monitor his behaviour in relation to goals. In con-
planning and organising. Furthermore, he contin- trast, the Lyrical Lord, reserved for social situa-
ued to evidence self-regulatory problems, includ- tions, could dress and act cool, rap and speak in
ing impulsive and verbally aggressive behaviour, rhymes, and tease peers. These components of the
outside the context of familiar routines.
identity metaphor are illustrated in Figure 5.
At this time, Bill had successfully completed
To communicate his plan to the work supervi-
a TBI-substance abuse program, was living in his
sor, Bill created (with our assistance) a self-advo-
own apartment (with the support of an indepen-
dent living support counsellor as needed), had a cacy videotape in which he explained the LL Cool
job in a supported employment setting, and regu- J metaphor and the mildly condescending script
larly attended Narcotics Anonymous and other that he had agreed to use to terminate unwelcome
support groups offered by the program he had peer interaction (“Talk to the hand; the ears aren’t
recently completed. He valued his job and was listening”) followed by turning away. Bill watched
described by his supervisor as a hard worker, but this video several times in an attempt to internalise
had ongoing difficulty accepting and interacting the sense of self and associated scripts. In addi-
effectively with co-workers with disability. When tion, his supervisor wrote “LL Cool J” on a small
we began our “LL Cool J” intervention with Bill, cue card, which she inconspicuously showed to
conflicts at work were escalating, with reports Bill when she anticipated a possible negative
from both Bill and his independent living coun- interaction. Bill also carried a small card with the
selor indicating that at least 50% of his days at message “I need a place to go chill out,” which he
work were marred by peer conflicts that resulted inconspicuously showed the supervisor when he
in punitive action from the supervisor, including anticipated difficulty regulating his behaviour.
suspension from work. Serious altercations with peers were reduced to
Bill readily acknowledged that he was a zero at work, and for the next several months,
major contributor to these conflicts and that he there were no reports of punishment.

22
RECONSTRUCTION OF IDENTITY AFTER BRAIN INJURY

worked odd jobs and continued to abuse drugs. In


his mid-20s he was arrested for extortion and
served four years in prison. Shortly after his
release, he violated parole and served another
four years in prison. After his second release, he
was chased by a police officer and fell from a
roof, incurring a severe TBI. Nine months of
acute hospitalisation and early rehabilitation were
followed by six years in a post-acute brain injury
facility. Following transfer to home, he was in
and out of short-term substance abuse programs.
At age 41 he was referred to a last resort TBI-sub-
stance abuse program, where we became increas-
ingly involved in his rehabilitation.
Salient features of Stan’s behaviour at this
time (10 years postinjury) were, on the positive
side, a high level of energy, a desire to help
others, an interesting sense of humour, and a
stated desire to turn his life around. On the nega-
tive side, he evidenced a number of ventral frontal
lobe signs: extreme impulsiveness, sexually inap-
propriate behaviour (including uninvited sexually
suggestive verbal and physical contact with
FIGURE 5 women), perseveration, an apparent inability to
The flexible schematic model of identity (identity map) learn from the negative consequences of his
that Bill collaboratively constructed in an attempt to be actions, generally weak monitoring of his behav-
true to himself and also avoid peer conflict at work.
iour and its consequences, difficulty interpreting
the intent of others’ actions (typically reading
It is now approximately one year since this malicious intent into the actions of staff), spotty
intervention was initiated. A recent report from and selective episodic memory, and an apparent
work indicated increases in negative interaction inability to connect his long-term goals in life
with peers, related in part to new workers in the with his choices and actions. In addition, Stan had
work setting and increased incidental contact an unstable view of his own brain injury, some-
among peers. In addition, procedural drift was times denying injury and sometimes excusing
reported, with the supervisor no longer using the behaviour because of his injury. Physically, he
LL Cool J prompt as an antecedent control proce- had mild motor impairment (he walked with a
dure and Bill no longer using his chill-out request cane) and mild dysarthria, although his speech
or reviewing his self-advocacy videos at home. We was fully intelligible to familiar listeners.
have therefore met with both Bill and the supervi- Collaborative intervention designed to recon-
sor, and are helping Bill to create a new self-advo- struct Stan’s mental model of self began after sev-
cacy video in which he proposes ways to organise eral behaviour plans designed to reduce
social interaction at work to avoid conflict. We unacceptable behaviour and increase successful
highlight this resumption of the intervention social interaction had failed. Not only were these
because of the frequency with which maintenance plans unsuccessful, each had come to be a provo-
intervention is required for this clinical population, cation for anger even though they had been cre-
necessitating long-term monitoring systems. ated in negotiation with Stan. Furthermore, at the
time that this intervention began, Stan’s trial reg-
Stan imen of Prozac was being titrated with the goal of
Stan was raised in New York City where, accord- discontinuing its use. The medication was judged
ing to his report, he began to abuse alcohol and to increase rather than decrease problem behav-
subsequently drugs in junior high school. His iours. Subsequent experimental medication regi-
high school career was dominated by gambling, mens appeared to have little positive impact on
drinking, drugs (primarily cocaine), and irregular Stan’s behaviour.
school attendance. He and his friends reportedly Episodes of uncontrolled anger, sexual acting
robbed trucks and stripped cars in high school, out, cursing, threatening staff, and other negative
but he was never caught. After high school, he interactions were increasing. Because of these

23
MARK YLVISAKER AND TIMOTHY FEENEY

120
Total

100
ofofTotal

80
%%
Interaction:
Interaction:

60
Negative

40
Negative

20

0
1 3 5 7 9 11 13 15 17 19 21 23 25 27 29 31 33 35 37 39 41 43 45 47 49 51 53

3-days baseline; 7 weeks intervention

FIGURE 6
Stan’s percentage of total interactions with selected staff judged as negative. Three day’s baseline indicated over 90%
negative interactions. The first two weeks of intervention suggested substantial improvement. This was followed by an
apparent extinction burst, followed by inconsistent, but generally disappointing performance.

behaviours, Stan had been banned from several He used the metaphor of master mason to capture
nearby businesses and from a health care facility the critical aspects of this model of self. The mason
where he had received some services. As indi- metaphor was used because of its usefulness in
cated in Figure 6, more than 90% of Stan’s inter- relation to positive, controlled, successful action
actions with key individuals in the program strategies and because Stan had admired master
(including the director, nurse, and direct care staff masons during his earlier work in construction. He
with whom he had the most problematic interac- had no interest in sports, music, movies, literature,
tions) were judged to be negative at baseline (the or other potential sources of personal metaphors.
first three days on the graph). Negative interac- We then helped Stan make several self-
tions were defined as those that included angry instructional and self-advocacy videos, one of
accusations, perseverative complaining, sexually which included his explanation of these two
suggestive language or touch, and impulsive models of self and also several vignettes in which
demands for immediate attention. We chose to he demonstrated for himself interactions from the
track Stan’s interaction with individuals with perspective of both models. We identified the
whom he had a history of negative communica- onset of the intervention to be the day that Stan
tion, rather than the staff with whom he most began to watch these videos on a daily basis and
often interacted positively, because safe and suc- to receive nonthreatening reminders from staff
cessful community living demands self-control in based on the positive master mason identity that
difficult as well as easy social situations. Due to he had chosen. At that time he also was assigned
the severity of his behavioural disorder and one-on-one behavioural assistance, mandated by
apparently poor prognosis for improvement, the his negative behaviours but potentially counter-
director of the program and a consulting psychol- productive because of Stan’s association of mon-
ogist had recommended that Stan be transferred itoring with the activity of prison guards.
to a more restrictive treatment setting. As indicated in Figure 6, the data looked
The identity map in Figure 7 represents promising for the first two weeks of the interven-
Stan’s reflections about his internalised model of tion (negative interactions were reduced from over
self that had evolved over three decades of prob- 90% of the total to around 35%). Stan then visited
lematic behaviour, hardened in prison, and inten- home for several days during a national holiday.
sified by restrictions imposed by corrections and When he returned, negative interactions increased
rehabilitation professionals. Stan chose to label to close to the baseline rate, possibly representing
this identity with a vulgar name. In moments of an extinction burst. Over the next four weeks, we
calm reflection, he acknowledged that this was attempted to intensify the intervention by prepar-
not the identity he envisioned for himself and that ing a “momentum” video, including positive inter-
he wished he could change. However, he added action and praise from staff, designed to accelerate
that he did not know how he could change. internalisation of the positive model of self and the
The identity map in Figure 8 represents Stan’s associated positive emotions. At the same time,
reflections about the person he wanted to become. Jason, who had successfully negotiated a transfor-

24
RECONSTRUCTION OF IDENTITY AFTER BRAIN INJURY

FIGURE 7 FIGURE 8
The negative schematic model of identity (identity map) The positive schematic model of identity (identity map)
that Stan collaboratively constructed in an attempt to that Stan collaboratively constructed in an attempt to con-
understand his ongoing conflicts and lack of success in trol his behaviour and improve his interaction with others.
community living.
having the last word, and walking away uninjured
mation in his primary mental model of self similar constituted an extremely reinforcing sequence of
to that desired by Stan, began to spend time with events during his many years in prison; con-
Stan with the goal of helping him understand that versely, turning away from provocation was per-
his desired changes were not only possible with ceived as weakness and elicited in him negative
effort, but also very satisfying. emotional states. Understood simply in terms of
Unfortunately, after seven weeks of the inter- his history of reinforcement, these patterns of
vention, Stan’s progress has been inconsistent response are unlikely to be overtaken by more
and generally insubstantial (see Figure 6). His positive responses in the absence of extensive
reaction to perceived provocation continues to be reinforcing experience with an alternative model
anger, confrontation, and expletive-filled perse- of self. Unfortunately, providing those reinforc-
verative outbursts. In addition, sexually explicit ing experiences becomes increasingly difficult in
language and physical contact, although reduced a world in which everyday communication part-
in frequency, continue to be reported. ners have been the object of the individual’s
Although the final results of this intervention ongoing verbal abuse.
cannot be confidently predicted at this time, we
have included a potentially negative case illustra-
tion for several reasons. First, Stan’s case under- Summary and Cautions
scores the point that helping people to perceive In the previous section, we presented three case
the world and their own actions in the world illustrations of the intervention theories and strate-
through the lens of a revised sense of personal gies described earlier in this paper. In the first
identity is a major undertaking, potentially case, the intervention had an importantly positive,
requiring months of intensive and consistent sup- enduring, and generalised effect. In the second
port for success. This is particularly true if the case, the effect was specific and positive, but may
negative behaviour patterns and associated continue to require periodic intervention for main-
schematic mental model of self have been “burnt tenance. In the third case, the intervention may
in” over many years of negative training. For have no lasting effect, although the jury is still out.
example, in Stan’s case, facing down provocation, Table 1 summarises potential obstacles to the suc-

25
MARK YLVISAKER AND TIMOTHY FEENEY

TABLE 1
Social Reconstruction of Identity: Obstacles, and Strategies to Overcome the Obstacles

Obstacles Strategies To Overcome The Obstacles


Oppositionality 1. Self-Instruction
2. Self-Selected Metaphors
3. Peer Support
Impaired Working Memory 1. Metaphoric Compaction
2. Large Numbers Of Video Learning Trials For Habituation
Need For Repetition 1. Videotaped Negotiation For Repeated Reviews
2. Videotaped Self-Modeling
Concrete Thinking 1. Graphic Organisers
2. Motivating, Familiar, Concrete Metaphors
Need To Associate Positive Visceral 1. Positive, Supportive Scripts For Everyday Communication Partners
Feedback/ Somatic Markers With 2. Positive Role: Planned Achievement With New Identity
New, Positive Sense Of Self 3. Momentum Video, Illustrating Success With The New Identity
4. Peer Support
5. Compelling Positive Metaphor

cess of this type of intervention and strategies that metaphor and to help the person organise his most
may be used to overcome the obstacles. reflective thinking about firmly embraced long-
A number of cautions regarding this interven- term goals, the strategies that might facilitate
tion should be highlighted. First, rehabilitation achievement of those goals, and the contextualised
clinicians untrained in principles and practices of practice needed to habituate those strategies.
counselling psychology must team with an experi- Finally, members of this clinical group often
enced psychotherapist or counsellor in delivering benefit from other interventions, including phar-
the intervention. In our judgment, collaboration is macologic management, in conjunction with
the key, not assignment of the intervention process behavioural therapies, and should therefore be fol-
to a psychotherapist working in isolation. Second, lowed by a psychiatrist or related physician with
some individuals may not be candidates for explo- expertise in pharmacologic management of behav-
ration of identity models represented metaphori- ioural disturbances following TBI.
cally. Individuals who are extremely concrete in
their thinking and who therefore may not grasp Application to Less Severe Disability
the metaphoric nature of the alternative identity We have deliberately chosen to discuss a poten-
model may be excluded, as well as those with tially useful component of brain injury rehabilita-
multiple personality disorder. In our practice, we tion as it applies to individuals who are
have also terminated this intervention when the particularly difficult to serve because of their self-
person insisted on using an identity metaphor that regulatory disorder. Exploration of metaphoric
we considered psychologically dangerous. For representations of self and associated action
example, one young man who had been abused by strategies may also be useful for individuals with
his father said that he would like to organise a pos- less severe disability and disability that may not
itive identity map for himself using his father as directly include behavioural or self-regulator con-
the analogical base; another, wracked by religious cerns. For example, individuals with disability
guilt, insisted on using Jesus as his new identity associated with memory or language impairment
metaphor. In these cases, we proceeded with a may not integrate helpful compensatory manoeu-
comprehensive program of intervention minus the vers into their everyday life until they find a way
component discussed in this paper. of representing those strategies as consistent with
Caution must also be exercised so that the an acceptable sense of self. For example, the strat-
goals and values organised within an identity map egy of requesting clarification of another’s mes-
are truly those of the person with disability. In the sage may be acceptable if framed as the
case of our illustrations, all of the material in the professional conduct of a sophisticated journalist
collaboratively constructed identity maps came rather than a person with language processing
from the individuals with disability. The clinician’s impairment. Similarly, producing periodic organ-
role was to facilitate identification of a compelling ised summaries of a conversation may become

26
RECONSTRUCTION OF IDENTITY AFTER BRAIN INJURY

appealing if framed as the behaviour of a success- Bechera, A., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A.
ful business executive rather than a person with (1996). Failure to respond autonomically to antici-
brain injury implementing a memory strategy. pated future outcomes following damage to pre-
Because TBI disproportionately selects sports- frontal cortex. . Cerebral Cortex, 6, 215–225.
minded young men, we have made extensive use of Beck., A.T., Rush, A.J., Shaw, B.F., & Emery, G. (1979).
sports analogies to explain and render palatable Cognitive therapy of depression. New York:
Guilford Press.
compensatory strategies. More generally, the
metaphor of coach or self-coach has been helpful in Carr, E.G., Carlson, J.I., Langdon, N.A., Magito-
McLaughlin, D., & Yarbrough, S.C. (1998). Two
explaining executive or self-regulatory functions
perspectives on antecedent control. In J.K. Luiselli
and creating positive associations with the effort & M.J. Cameron (Eds.), Antecedent control:
required to improve self-regulation of behaviour. Innovative approaches to behavioral support (pp.
3–28). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.
Evidence of Effectiveness Conger, J.J. (1984). Adolescence and youth. New York:
To our knowledge, there exist no experimental Harper and Row.
studies of the effectiveness of this intervention Craik F.I.M., Moroz, T.M., Moscovitch, M., Stuss, D.T.,
strategy applied to TBI rehabilitation. Teasdale Winocur, G., Tulving, E., & Kapur, S. (1999). In
and Barnard (1993) summarised empirical evi- search of the self: A positron emission tomography
dence supporting their application of the ICS study. Psychological Science, 10, 27–35.
model to the treatment of depression. In addition, Czikszentmihalyi, M., & Larson, R. (1984). Being ado-
Ylvisaker and Feeney (1998) summarised a vari- lescent. New York: Basic Books.
ety of empirical supports for the general approach Damasio, A.R. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion,
to TBI rehabilitation described in the opening sec- reason, and the human brain. New York: Avon
Books.
tion of this paper. They also offered several posi-
tive single subject experiments and case studies. Damasio, A.R. (1999). The feeling of what happens:
Body and emotion in the making of consciousness.
The general contextualised, collaborative,
New York: Harcourt Brace and Co.
antecedent-focused approach to TBI rehabilitation
Deci, E.L. (1995). Why we do what we do:
for the population under examination can also be Understanding self-motivation. New York: Penguin
supported as cost effective. During the fiscal year Books.
1996-97 (the most recent year for which financial Dikmen, S.S., Ross, B.L., Machamer, J.E., & Temkin,
analyses are available), the program through N.R. (1995). One year psychosocial outcome in
which we deliver our services cost $144,000US. head injury. Journal of the International
Based on conservative New York State Health Neuropsychological Society, 1, 67–77.
Department calculations, the program saved the Eslinger, P.J., & Damasio, A.R. (1985). Severe distur-
state $1,486,000 during that year (a savings of bance of higher cognition following bilateral frontal
about $10 for every $1 spent). Cost effectiveness, lobe ablation: Patient EVR. Neurology, 35,
combined with solid theoretical support and sug- 1731–1741.
gestive case material, provides motivation for con- Feeney, T.J., & Ylvisaker, M. (1995). Choice and rou-
trolled experimental investigation of the tine: Antecedent behavioral interventions for ado-
effectiveness of the intervention. lescents with severe traumatic brain injury. Journal
of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 10, 67–86.
Feeney, T.J., & Ylvisaker, M. (1997). A positive, com-
References munication-based approach to challenging behavior
Adams, J.H., Graham, D.I., Scott, G., Parker, L.S., & after TBI. In A. Glang, G. Singer, & B. Todis (Eds.),
Doyle, D. (1980). Brain damage in fatal non-missile Children with acquired brain injury: The school’s
head injury. Journal of Clinical Pathology, 33, response (pp. 229–254). Baltimore, MD: Paul
1132–1145. Brookes.
Baer, D. (1981). How to plan for generalization. Grafman, J. (1989). Plans, actions, and mental sets:
Manhattan, KS: H&H Enterprises, Inc. Managerial knowledge units in the frontal lobes. In
Barnard, P. (1985). Interacting cognitive subsystems: A E. Perecman (Ed.), Integrating theory and practice
psycholinguistic approach to short-term memory. In in clinical neuropsychology (pp. 93–138). Hillsdale,
A. Ellis (Ed.), Progress in the psychology of lan- NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
guage, Vol 2 (pp. 197–258). Hove, UK: Lawrence Grafman, J. (1995). Similarities and distinctions among
Erlbaum Assoc. current models of prefrontal cortical functions. In J.
Bechera, A., Damasio, A., Damasio, H., & Anderson, S. Grafman, K.J. Holyoak, & F. Boller (Eds.),
(1994). Insensitivity to future consequences follow- Structure and function of the human prefrontal
ing damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, cortex (pp. 337–368). New York: The New York
50, 7–15. Academy of Sciences.

27
MARK YLVISAKER AND TIMOTHY FEENEY

Grafman, J., Sirigu, A., Spector, L., & Hendler, J. Stuss, D.T., & Alexander, M.P. (1999). Affectively burnt
(1993). Damage to the prefrontal cortex leads to in: A proposed role of the right frontal lobe. In E.
decomposition of structured event complexes. Tulving (Ed.). Memory, consciousness and the
Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 8, 73–87. brain: The Tallinn Conference (pp. 215–227).
Lakoff , G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live Philadelphia: Psychology Press.
by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Stuss, D.T., & Alexander, M.P. (In press). Executive
Lakoff , G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the functions and the frontal lobes: A conceptual view.
flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to west- Psychological Report.
ern thought. New York: Basic Books. Stuss, D.T., & Benson, D.F. (1986). The frontal lobes.
Levin, H.S., Goldstein, F.C., Williams, D.H., & New York: Raven Press.
Eisenberg, H.M. (1991). The contribution of frontal Stuss, D.T., Picton, T.W., & Alexander, M.P. (In press).
lobe lesions to the neurobehavioral outcome of Consciousness, self-awareness and the frontal
closed head injury. In H.S. Levin, H.M. Eisenberg, lobes. In S. Salloway, P. Malloy, & J. Duffy (Eds.),
& A.L. Benton (Eds.), Frontal lobe function and The frontal lobes and neuropsychiatric illness.
dysfunction (pp. 318–338). New York: Oxford Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press, Inc.
University Press. Teasdale. J.D. (1997). The transformation of meaning:
Levine, B. (1999). Self-regulation and autonoetic con- The interacting cognitive subsystems approach. In
sciousness. In E. Tulving (Ed.), Memory, conscious- M. Power & C.R. Brewin (Eds.), The transforma-
ness, and the brain: The Tallinn Conference (pp. tion of meaning in psychological therapies (pp.
20–214). Philadelphia: Psychology Press. 141–156). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Levine, B., Freedman, M., Dawson, D., Black, S., & Teasdale, J.D. (1999). Metacognition, mindfulness and
Stuss, D.T. (1999). Ventral frontal contribution to the modification of mood disorders. Clinical
self-regulation: Convergence of episodic memory Psychology and Psychotherapy, 6, 146–155.
and inhibition. Neurocase, 5, 263–275. Teasdale, J.D., & Barnard, P.J. (1993). Affect, cognition,
Lewin, J. (1998). Epilogue to Ylvisaker, M., & Feeney, and change: Re-modelling depressive thought.
T., Collaborative brain injury intervention: Positive Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
everyday routines. San Diego, CA: Singular Teuber, H.L. (1964). The riddle of frontal lobe function
Publishing Group. in man. In J.M. Warren & K. Akert (Eds.), The
Mann, L. (1979). On the trail of process: A historical frontal granular cortex and behavior. New York:
perspective on cognitive processes and their train- McGraw Hill.
ing. New York: Grune and Stratton. Tulving, E. (1985). Memory and consciousness.
Mendelsohn, D., Levin, H.S., Bruce, D., Lilly, M.A., Canadian Psychology, 26, 1–12.
Harward, H., Culhane, K., & Eisenberg, H.M. Vygotsky, L.S. (1962). Thought and language.
(1992). Late MRI after head injury in children: Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press.
Relationship to clinical features and outcome. Vygotsky, L.S. (1978). Mind in society: The develop-
Child’s Nervous System, 8, 445–452. ment of higher psychological processes (M. Cole, V.
Nippold, M. (1998). Later language development: The John-Steiner, S. Scribner, & E. Souberman, Eds. &
school-age and adolescent years (2nd edition). Trans.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. Ylvisaker, M., & Feeney, T. (1996). Executive functions
Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning. after traumatic brain injury: Supported cognition
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of and self-advocacy. Seminars in Speech and
London, 298, 199–209. Language, 17, 217–232.
Shallice, T. (1988). From neuropsychology to mental Ylvisaker, M., & Feeney, T. (1998). Collaborative brain
structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. injury intervention: Positive everyday routines. San
Shallice, T., & Burgess, P.W. (1991). Deficits in strategy Diego: Singular Publishing Group.
application following frontal lobe damage in man. Ylvisaker, M., & Feeney, T. (2000). Reflections on
Brain, 114, 727–741. dobermans, poodles, and social rehabilitation for
Singley, M., & Anderson, J.R. (1989). Transfer of cog- difficult-to-serve individuals with TBI.
nitive skill. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Aphasiology, 14, 407–432.
Press. Ylvisaker, M., Szekeres, S.F., & Haarbauer-Krupa. J.
Stuss, D.T. (1991a). Disturbance of self-awareness after (1998). Cognitive rehabilitation: Organization,
frontal system damage. In G.P. Prigatano & D.L. memory and language. In M. Ylvisaker (Ed.),
Schacter (Eds.), Awareness of deficit after brain Traumatic brain injury rehabilitation: Children and
injury (pp. 63–83). New York: Oxford University adolescents (Revised Edition)(pp. 181–220).
Press. Boston: Butterworth-Heinemann.
Stuss, D.T. (1991b). Self, awareness, and the frontal Ylvisaker, M., Szekeres, S.F., & Feeney, T. (1998).
lobes: A neuropsychological perspective. In J. Cognitive rehabilitation: Executive functions. In M.
Strauss & G.R. Goethals (Eds.), The self: Ylvisaker (Ed.), Traumatic brain injury rehabilita-
Interdisciplinary approaches. (pp. 225–278). New tion: Children and adolescents (Revised Edition)
York: Springer Verlag. (pp. 221–269). Boston: Butterworth-Heinemann.

28

You might also like