You are on page 1of 13

| |

Received: 29 May 2020    Revised: 16 July 2020    Accepted: 17 July 2020

DOI: 10.1111/ijcs.12606

ORIGINAL ARTICLE
bs_bs_banner

Materialism and life satisfaction: The moderating roles of


alexithymia and product retention tendency

Anna J. Vredeveld

Campbell School of Business, Berry College,


Mount Berry, GA, USA Abstract
This research examines how alexithymia and product retention tendency affect the
Correspondence
Anna J. Vredeveld, Campbell School of relationship between materialism and life satisfaction. Extant research has estab-
Business, Berry College, 2277 Martha Berry lished that materialism has a negative effect on personal well-being and that con-
Hwy NW, Mount Berry, GA 30149-5024,
USA. sumer culture and marketing facilitates this effect by encouraging consumers to
Email: avredeveld@berry.edu focus on material pursuits to satisfy extrinsic goals. However, previous research has
not explored how emotional personality traits and lifestyle values influence this “dark
side” of materialism. Alexithymia is an emotional personality trait that inhibits an indi-
vidual's ability to identify, describe and regulate emotions. Results from survey data
show that alexithymia moderates the effect of materialism on life satisfaction, such
that the effect is negative for individuals without alexithymia, but positive for individ-
uals with alexithymia. Moreover, product retention tendency attenuates the negative
effect of materialism for individuals without alexithymia, but strengthens the positive
effect of materialism for individuals with alexithymia. The theoretical implications of
these findings are discussed.

KEYWORDS

alexithymia, emotion, life satisfaction, materialism, product retention, well-being

1  |  I NTRO D U C TI O N income has been found to attenuate the negative effect of material-
ism on well-being (La Barbera & Gürhan, 1997; Nickerson, Schwarz,
Materialism, defined as “the importance a consumer attaches to Diener, & Kahneman, 2003). At a higher (societal) level, cultural
worldly possessions” (Belk, 1985, p. 265), has been extensively values may also influence the effect of materialism on well-being
studied in marketing, psychology and sociology. Most of this extant because some cultures place more emphasis on the importance of
work has focused on the “dark side” of materialism with a particular money and status signalling, thereby legitimizing materialistic pur-
emphasis on the negative effects of materialistic values on personal suits (Sidhu & Foo, 2015; Xiao & Tessema, 2019).
well-being (e.g., Ahuvia & Wong, 2002; Richins & Dawson, 1992; In contrast to previous work, the present research examines how
Ryan & Dziurawiec, 2001). Yet, although the negative effect of ma- alexithymia influences the relationship between materialism and
terialism on well-being is well documented in previous work, the well-being. Alexithymia is an emotional personality trait that inhib-
strength of the correlation between materialism and well-being its an individual's ability to identify, describe and regulate emotions
outcomes (e.g., happiness, life satisfaction) varies across studies (Sifneos, 1972; Taylor, Bagby, & Parker, 1997). Individuals with alex-
(Dittmar, Bond, Hurst, & Kasser, 2014; Dittmar & Kapur, 2011; La ithymia also have an externally oriented thinking style that results in a
Barbera & Gürhan, 1997), thus, encouraging research on possible focus on external mundane aspects of life with a subsequent height-
moderators of this relationship. A recent meta-analysis revealed that ened reward sensitivity (Lyvers et al., 2014; Nemiah, Freyberger, &
age and gender moderates the relationship between materialism and Sifnos, 1976; Verissimo, Taylor, & Bagby, 2000). Alexithymia is rel-
well-being, such that the effect was more negative for women and atively common; it is estimated that 13%–19% of the general (non-
older individuals (Dittmar et al., 2014). In contrast, higher personal clinical) population has alexithymia (Mattila, Poutanen, Koivisto,

Int J Consum Stud. 2020;00:1–13. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/ijcs |


© 2020 John Wiley & Sons Ltd     1
|
2      
bs_bs_banner
VREDEVELD

Salokangas, & Joukamaa, 2007; Timoney & Holder, 2013). Because variables (Dittmar et al., 2014; La Barbera & Gürhan, 1997) and cul-
of their poor emotion regulation abilities and externally oriented ture (Sidhu & Foo, 2015; Xiao & Tessema, 2019), this research ex-
thinking style, the effect of materialism on well-being is likely to be amines how emotional aspects of an individual's personality may
different for individuals with alexithymia, as compared to individuals influence both degree of materialism and the consequences of mate-
without alexithymia. Kasser and Ryan (1996) argue that the detri- rialism. Moreover, this research examines the relationship between
mental effect of materialism on well-being stems from its extrinsic materialism and product retention tendency and finds that product
focus. Materialism is extrinsically motivated as it reflects the pur- retention attenuates the negative effect of materialism on well-be-
suit of extrinsic goals related to material success, fame and image, ing, thus, highlighting the importance of considering not just how
which detract from intrinsic goals related to important psychological possessions are viewed as means to success and happiness (i.e., de-
needs, including affiliation and self-acceptance (Kasser & Ahuvia, sire for possessions), but also how possessions are retained to reflect
2002; Kasser & Ryan, 1996). Therefore, materialistic individuals are success in such goal pursuits.
less likely to engage in activities that are intrinsically motivated, that
is, activities undertaken for the joy or challenge they provide, rather
than their tangible rewards (Kasser, 2002). However, for individu- 2  | LITE R AT U R E R E V I E W
als with alexithymia, intrinsically motivated activities are likely to
be unrewarding because these individuals lack the ability to engage 2.1 | Materialism, product retention tendency and
fully in subjective emotional experiences (Verissimo et al., 2000). well-being
Materialistic pursuits, in contrast, would allow individuals with alex-
ithymia to align specific identifiable extrinsic goals (i.e., fame, image, Materialism reflects how important possessions are to consumers
status) with extrinsic means (i.e., acquisition of possessions), thus, (Belk, 1985) and materialistic consumers are more likely to view
appealing to their externally oriented thinking style and preference products and goods not only as means to important life goals and
for behaviour over cognition and emotional experiences (Morie & success in life, but also to judge self and others based on what they
Ridout, 2018). The overarching purpose of this study is to test the own (Richins, 2013). Specifically, Richins and Dawson (1992) con-
hypothesis that alexithymia moderates the relationship between ceptualize materialism as a value system comprised of three dimen-
materialism and life satisfaction, such that the effect is positive for sions: success (i.e., possessions as means to success), centrality (i.e.,
individuals with alexithymia, but negative for individuals without importance of possessions) and happiness (i.e., possessions as means
alexithymia. to happiness). In this value system, materialism is inherently linked
In addition to exploring the moderating role of alexithymia, with extrinsic motivation, such as the attainment of image and social
this research also examines how product retention tendency in- recognition through money and possessions (Kasser & Ryan, 1993).
fluences the relationship between materialism and life-satisfaction Previous work has found that materialism is negatively related to age
and, in particular, how the interaction between product retention (Belk, 1985; Richins & Dawson, 1992) and research on materialism in
tendency and alexithymia moderates this effect. Product retention children and adolescence suggests that materialism increase in early
tendency reflects an individual's propensity to retain possessions adolescence (i.e., 12–13 years old) and decrease in late adolescence
(Haws, Naylor, Coulter, & Bearden, 2012). The effect of product re- (i.e., 16–18 years old) and that this effect is mediated by increasing
tention tendency on well-being has not been extensively explored self-esteem (Chaplin & John, 2007). Research has also found that
in previous work, but research shows that product retention, while materialism is related to religiosity (Arli, Gil, & van Esch, 2020) and
being positively related to materialism, is also positively related that, within a culture, materialistic values may differ across genera-
to concerns for the environment and frugality (Haws et al., 2012; tions (Yang & Stening, 2016) and vary with socio-economic status
Haws, Winterich, & Naylor, 2014; Lastovicka, Bettencourt, Hughner, (Ozgen & Esiyok, 2020). Although antecedents and outcomes of ma-
& Kuntze, 1999). This suggests that product retention tendency is terialism vary across cultures (Schaefer, Hermans, & Parker, 2004;
connected to a broader value system and that product retention Sidhu & Foo, 2015; Xiao & Tessema, 2019), materialism is prevalent
may not necessarily contribute to lower well-being. Importantly, in both Western and Eastern cultures and the degree of material-
this present research explores the possibility that product retention ism does not appear to be directly related to general affluence or
tendency attenuates the negative effect of materialism on well- degree of development within a culture (Cleveland, Laroche, &
being. Specifically, for individuals with alexithymia, product reten- Papadopoulos, 2009.
tion tendency should enhance the positive effect of materialism on Research supports that materialism is problematic for well-
life satisfaction because possessions provide evidence of attainment being at least in part because of its extrinsic focus. First, materialists,
of extrinsic goals. in their pursuit of external rewards, often forego opportunities to
This research makes important contributions to our understand- engage in intrinsically motivated activities that are more challenging
ing of materialism and, in particular, provides an understanding of the and enjoyable and satisfies important psychological needs, such as
possible boundary conditions of the negative effect of materialism affiliation, sense of community and self-acceptance (Kasser, 2002;
on well-being documented in previous research. In contrast to pre- Kasser & Ryan, 1996). Second, the extrinsic focus of material pur-
vious work that has examined the moderating roles of demographic suits also makes materialists vulnerable to the detrimental effects
VREDEVELD
bs_bs_banner
|
      3

of what has been labelled the ‘hedonic treadmill’ because attain- not related to the lifestyle trait aspects of product retention ten-
ment of new possessions only temporarily increases well-being and dency in the nonclinical population (Haws et al., 2012). Determining
therefore, encourages materialists to seek out more possessions the well-being outcomes of product retention tendency is further
to appease their ever increasing “appetite for positive stimulation complicated by the many possible reasons behind such retention, in-
through acquisition” (Dittmar et al., 2014, p. 856; Dunn, Gilbert, & cluding storage for practical just in case use (Coulter & Ligas, 2003)
Wilson, 2011). Materialists may also have unrealistic expectations and storage of possessions for emotional connections, such as
for what value and joy material goods will provide them with and when possessions are retained to preserve memories (Winterich,
therefore, continue to acquire possessions to reduce the negative Reczek, & Irwin, 2007). Moreover, product retention is also related
emotional experience that stems from dissatisfaction (Richins, 2013; to frugality (Lastovicka et al., 1999) and concern for the environ-
Solberg, Diener, & Robinson, 2004). Third, a focus on the extrinsic ment (Haws et al., 2014), both which may have positive effects on
rewards attained by material goods is also likely to lead to neg- well-being for consumers who have internalized such values (Welsch
ative self-appraisals, especially when materialists compare their & Kühling, 2018). Thus, there appears to be a need for research that
possessions to the possessions (and wealth) of others or when specifically examines well-being for consumers who have both ma-
watching media/advertising messages that encourage the acqui- terialistic values and product retention tendency.
sition of expensive goods that they are unlikely to afford (Ashikali
& Dittmar, 2012; Dittmar, 2008). Such negative self-appraisals are
likely to lead to lower well-being (Dittmar et al., 2014). Fourth and 2.2 | Alexithymia and well-being
finally, the extrinsic nature of materialism is also thought to decrease
well-being because materialists pursue possessions at the expense Sifneos (1972) coined the term alexithymia to describe individuals
of personal relationships, which are intrinsically motivated and carry that lack ability to describe emotions and the Greek meaning of
fewer concrete or tangible (identifiable) rewards (Kasser, 2002). alexithymia can be roughly translated to “no words for emotions”
Materialism reflects the belief that possessions are means to life (Timoney & Holder, 2013, p. 1). Almost five decades of research has
goals, such as status attainment (Dittmar et al., 2014), but does not established that alexithymia is a stable personality construct with
necessarily capture the importance an individual places on retaining several dimensions. First, individuals with alexithymia have diffi-
these possession once acquired. In contrast, product retention ten- culties identifying their subjective feelings. This is reflected in dif-
dency reflects an individual's need to keep possessions. Specifically, ficulty identifying causes of emotions in self and others (Grynberg,
Haws et al. (2012, p. 225), define product retention tendency as “a Luminet, Corneille, Grèzes, & Berthoz, 2010; Nemiah et al., 1976),
consumer lifestyle trait that reflects an individual's general propen- difficulties identifying facial expressions of sadness and fear in oth-
sity to retain consumption-related possessions”. Individuals who have ers (Prkachin, Casey, & Prkachin, 2009) and difficulty distinguishing
a stronger product retention tendency care about having posses- between emotions and the physical sensations that stem from those
sions and often engage in creative reuse and storage of possessions emotions (Taylor et al., 1997). Debate still exists on whether or not
for just in case purposes (Coulter & Ligas, 2003). Herein, product individuals with alexithymia actually differ in their ability to emo-
retention tendency differs from clinical compulsive hoarding where tionalize (i.e., experience emotional arousal) or just lack emotional
individuals acquire and retain seemingly useless possessions (e.g., awareness (Bagby et al., 2009; Timoney & Holder, 2013). Second,
used Band-Aids), resulting in overly cluttered and unsanitary living individuals with alexithymia also have difficulty describing their feel-
and challenges to everyday life (Frost & Gross, 1993; Frost, Steketee, ings to others and an overall limited expression and verbalization
& Williams, 2002). It is also different from compulsive buying which of emotions in common emotional situations (Nemiah et al., 1976;
is defined as “a consumer's tendency to be preoccupied with buying Timoney & Holder, 2013). Third, individuals with alexithymia also
that is revealed through repetitive buying and a lack of impulse con- have limited imagination, creativity and lack fantasies, most likely
trol over buying” (Ridgway, Kukar-Kinney, & Monroe, 2008, p. 622). because of their difficulty identifying and engaging with their emo-
Individuals who have a strong product retention tendency want to tions (Bagby et al., 2009; Czernecka & Szymura, 2008). Fourth, in ad-
keep their possessions, but are not necessarily enamored with the dition to difficulties identifying and describing feelings, individuals
act of acquiring those possessions. with alexithymia have an externally oriented thinking style with ex-
Not surprisingly, materialism and product retention tendency are ternal locus of control (Verissimo et al., 2000) and therefore, tend to
positively correlated (r = .21; Haws et al., 2012), but few studies have focus on external mundane or practical aspects of their lives, rather
specially examined the link between product retention tendency than their inner feelings and fantasies (Nemiah et al., 1976).
and personal well-being. In the case of clinical compulsive hoarding, Alexithymia is negatively correlated with life satisfaction (Mattila
the detrimental effects of possession retention are well known and et al., 2007) and positively correlated with negative well-being out-
include mental health issues, such as social exclusion/marginaliza- comes, including depression (Honkalampi, Hintikka, Tanskanen,
tion (Wilbram, Kellett, & Beail, 2008) and physical health issues due Lehtonen, & Viinamäki, 2000; Parker, Bagby, & Taylor, 1991). This is
to unsanitary living conditions (Thomas, 1997). However, compul- not surprising given the importance of emotion regulation to emo-
sive hoarding is closely related to various mental disorders, including tional well-being. Individuals with alexithymia also often lack social
obsessive-compulsive disorder (Frost et al., 2002), which are likely support because they are less likely to develop and maintain strong
|
4      
bs_bs_banner
VREDEVELD

social and romantic relationships (Humphreys, Wood, & Parker, 2009; are also more likely to attempt to regulate their emotions through be-
Lumley, Ovies, Stettner, Wehmer, & Lakey, 1996). This is most likely haviour rather than cognition (Morie & Ridout, 2018). Studies show
due to their inability not only to understand their own emotional re- that such poor emotion regulation often results in maladaptive con-
actions, but also the inability to understand the emotional reactions sumption behaviours, such as substance abuse, gambling and eating
of others (Humphreys et al., 2009). However, despite the clear evi- disorders, because such consumption provide immediate satisfaction
dence of the negative effect of alexithymia on emotional well-being, that can appease an elevated sensitivity to rewards (Lyvers et al., 2014;
there is mixed evidence related to its impact on behavioural aspects Taylor et al., 1997; Timoney & Holder, 2013). More generally, this focus
of well-being. On the one hand, alexithymia is linked to unhealthy be- on external means and heightened reward sensitivity would also sug-
haviours, such as eating disorders (Speranza et al., 2005), substance gest that individuals with alexithymia should be more likely to acquire
abuse (Haviland, Hendryx, Shaw, & Henry, 1994; Lyvers et al., 2014) and keep material goods not only as a way to convey desirable impres-
and pathological gambling (Lumley & Roby, 1995). This is likely due to sions to self and others, but also to compensate for emotional deficits.
poor emotion regulation in combination with heightened rewards sen- Possessing material goods satisfies extrinsic motivation by providing
sitivity, which results in maladaptive consumption behaviours (Lyvers external rewards (e.g., status) and, consequently, disposing of goods
et al., 2014; Morie & Ridout, 2018). On the other hand, the emotional would be counterproductive as these goods serve as physical and ob-
deficits presented in individuals with alexithymia are not always linked jective evidence of goal attainment. Therefore, individuals with alex-
to negative well-being and health outcomes. For example, in a study of ithymia should be more likely to have materialistic values and product
women undergoing in vitro fertilization (IVF), alexithymia was found to retention tendency, as compared to individuals without alexithymia.
be positively correlated with a positive outcome (i.e., birth of a healthy Thus, the following hypothesis is proposed:
infant) (Kakatsaki et al., 2009). This is thought to be a function of the
systematic process of the IVF procedure that may be particularly well Hypothesis 1 Individuals with alexithymia have (H1a) more materi-
suited to individuals with alexithymia who, because of their externally alistic values and (H1b) stronger product retention tendency, as
oriented thinking style, prefer concrete procedures and tasks and lack compared to individuals without alexithymia.
the emotional awareness and introspection that can cause anxiety
and stress during the IVF process (Kakatsaki et al., 2009). Hence, it
appears that alexithymia may not contribute to lower well-being in 3.2 | Materialism and life satisfaction: The
circumstances where nonemotional coping strategies are preferred. moderating roles of alexithymia and product
retention tendency

3  | H Y P OTH E S E S As discussed, the link between materialism and lower well-being has
been established in previous research (Dittmar, 2008; Kasser, 2002).
3.1 | Alexithymia, materialism and product retention However, this extant research does not account for differences in emo-
tendency tion regulation and thinking style. Because individuals with alexithymia
lack emotion regulation abilities and are focused on extrinsic means
As noted, individuals with alexithymia have difficulty identifying and through externally oriented thinking, they are unlikely to derive sat-
describing their emotions, lack fantasies and imagination and have isfaction from inherently intrinsic experiences (Verissimo et al., 2000).
an externally oriented thinking style (Taylor et al., 1997; Timoney & This is particularly evident in the lack of enjoyment and satisfaction in-
Holder, 2013). In particular, because of their externally oriented think- dividuals with alexithymia derive from emotional relationships, such as
ing style and emotion regulation deficits, individuals with alexithymia romantic relationships. In fact, research supports that lack of lasting
are more extrinsically motivated and are more likely to exhibit external emotional connections with others contribute to lower life satisfac-
locus of control (Morie & Ridout, 2018; Zimmerman, Rossier, Meyer tion for individuals with alexithymia (Humphreys et al., 2009; Lumley
de Stadelhofen, & Gaillard, 2005). Intrinsic goals, such as engaging in et al., 1996). This is intuitive when considering how individuals with
activities for the sake of enjoyment and building deep connections alexithymia favour extrinsic means to realize extrinsic goals; extrinsic
with others (Kasser & Ryan, 1996), are less appealing to individu- social comparison cues would suggest that you should have social/ro-
als with alexithymia because they lack the ability to connect and re- mantic relationships, but individuals with alexithymia lack the ability to
flect on their emotional (subjective) experiences (Taylor et al., 1997; maintain such relationships because they require intrinsic means and
Timoney & Holder, 2013). Instead, they are more likely to focus on the establishment of deep emotional connections. In contrast, acquisi-
mundane and external details of everyday life because these appeal tion of material goods does not require intrinsic means or the establish-
to rational thought rather than subjective feelings (Nemiah et al., ment of emotional connections. Therefore, in contrast to individuals
1976). Therefore, material goods should provide value to individu- without alexithymia, materialistic pursuits are likely to improve life sat-
als with alexithymia because they are physical (tangible) objects that isfaction for individuals with alexithymia as it aligns externally oriented
carry external rewards (e.g., status, fame, and image) that can satisfy thinking with extrinsic goals (having possessions), extrinsic means (ac-
extrinsic goals. Moreover, because of their externally oriented thinking quiring and possessing goods) and extrinsic rewards (e.g., status, image).
style and difficulty identifying emotions, individuals with alexithymia Accordingly, the following hypothesis is proposed:
VREDEVELD
bs_bs_banner
|
      5

Hypothesis 2 Materialism has a negative effect on life satisfaction for is desired (Richins, 2004). Product retention tendency was measured
individuals without alexithymia, but a positive effect on life satis- using the four-item scale developed by Haws et al. (2012) and life
faction for individuals with alexithymia. satisfaction was measured using the five-item Satisfaction with Life
Scale (SWLS) developed by Diener, Emmons, Larsen, and Griffin
Externally oriented thinking style is also related to impres- (1985). All items for these scales were measured using a seven-point
sion management and social detachment (Davydov, Luminet, & scale (1 = “strongly disagree”, 7 = “strongly agree”).
Zech, 2013). Individuals with alexithymia may care about how others Exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was conducted to examine
see them, but lack the ability to connect with others at a deeper level construct validity of the materialism, product retention tendency
(Humphreys et al., 2009). While materialism reflects an individual's and life satisfaction measures. The EFA utilized principal compo-
desire to attain possessions to satisfy extrinsic goals (i.e., rewards), nents analysis with oblique rotation as it can be assumed that the
product retention tendency reflects and individual's need to retain constructs are correlated (Netemeyer, Bearden, & Sharma, 2003).
possessions. Importantly, the externally oriented thinking style, in This analysis revealed three factors with eigenvalue greater than
combination with a lack of ability to identify emotions, should allow one (total variance = 70.3%). All items loaded primarily on their main
individuals with alexithymia to derive more satisfaction from their factor (component) and all item loadings exceeded .40 (see Table 1).
material purchases (as compared to those without alexithymia) be- A confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) utilizing AMOS 25 had accept-
cause these purchases validate desired social status and satisfies able fit according to the criteria suggested by Bagozzi and Yi (1988)
extrinsic goals. Thus, the positive effect of material values on life (χ2 (105) = 449.5; RMSEA = .055; GFI = .96; CFI = .98; IFI = .98). The
satisfaction for individuals with alexithymia should be stronger for average variance extracted (AVE) were equal to or exceeded .50 for
those who, in addition to alexithymia, also have a stronger product all three constructs and the composite reliabilities (CR) were equal
retention tendency. Hence, the effect of materialism on life satisfac- to or exceeded .90, thus, supporting convergent validity. The AVE
tion should be moderated by both alexithymia and product retention for each construct was also higher than the squared correlations be-
tendency. Formally, the following hypothesis is proposed: tween the construct and other constructs, supporting discriminant
validity of the constructs (Fornell & Larcker, 1981). All Cronbach's
Hypothesis 3 The positive effect of materialism on life satisfaction is alphas exceeded .90, thereby exceeding the cut-off value of .70 sug-
stronger for individuals with alexithymia who also have a stronger gested by Nunnally (1978). See Table 1 for an overview of factor
product retention tendency. loadings, AVE and reliabilities for each construct.
As this study utilizes self-reported survey data, precautions were
taken when designing the survey to help mitigate the risk of common
4 | M E TH O D O LO G Y methods bias. Respondents were guaranteed anonymity and this was
further emphasized by the anonymous nature of the survey portal
4.1 | Data collection and measures used (MTurk). The dependent variable (i.e., life satisfaction) was po-
sitioned after the independent variables (i.e., materialism and product
Data were collected on an online survey portal (MTurk) and 1,278 re- retention tendency) to help reduce the effects of consistency artifacts
spondents completed the survey that included measures of materialism, and objective demographic measures, such as gender and age, were
product retention tendency, alexithymia and life satisfaction, along with included and controlled for in the analysis (Mudambi, Pedersen, &
demographic variables and three embedded attention check questions. Andersson, 2014). A Harman's single factor test showed no evidence of
Alexithymia was measured using the 20-item version of the Toronto common methods bias as a one-factor CFA model had a very poor fit,
Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20) developed by Bagby, Parker, and Taylor (χ2 (103) = 2,845.5; RMSEA = .157; GFI = .83; CFI = .81; IFI = .81) and
(1994). All items were measured on a five-point scale (1 = “strongly an un-rotated factor analysis (EFA) had a first-factor variance (30.9%)
disagree”, 5 = “strongly agree”) and the Cronbach's Alpha for the overall that accounted for less than half of the overall variance (Podsakoff,
scale was .89. TAS-20 is the most widely used measure of alexithymia MacKenzie, Lee, & Podsakoff, 2003; Podsakoff & Organ, 1986).
(Timoney & Holder, 2013) and particularly well-suited for the purpose
of this study as it can be used to calculate an overall alexithymia score
for each individual, thus, allowing for direct comparisons of individu- 4.2 | Sample characteristics
als with alexithymia to individuals without alexithymia. Hence, for the
purpose if this study, this measure was only used to identify individuals Although 1,278 respondents completed the survey, 15.4% did not pass
with alexithymia; respondents with a score of 61 or higher on the TAS- all three embedded attention-check questions and these respondents
20 are considered to have alexithymia (Taylor et al., 1997). were, therefore, excluded from further analysis. Of the remaining
Materialism was measured using the nine-item version of the 1,081 respondents, 18.8% (n = 203) scored 61 or higher on the TAS-20
Material Values Scale (MVS) developed by Richins (2004). This measure and were, therefore, considered to have alexithymia (Taylor
shorter version of the original 15-item MVS (Richins & Dawson, 1992) et al., 1997). This is on the higher end, but still within range, of the rates
is considered appropriate when an overall measure of materialism is of alexithymia reported in previous studies, which typically range from
needed (rather than measures for each subscale) and survey brevity 13% to 19% of the general (nonclinical) population (Mattila et al., 2007;
|
6      
bs_bs_banner
VREDEVELD

TA B L E 1   Factor loadings, AVE and reliabilities

Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3 AVE CR α

Materialism (MVS−9) .50 .90 .91


1. I admire people who own expensive homes, cars and clothes .88 −.05 .13
2. The things I own say a lot about how well I am doing in life .78 .01 .07
3. I like to own things that impress people .83 .01 .19
4. I try to keep my life simple, as far as possessions are concerned .46 .02 −.03
5. Buying things gives me a lot of pleasure .81 .02 .03
6. I like a lot of luxury in my life .87 −.07 .17
7. My life would be better if I owned certain things I don't have .73 .02 −.19
8. I'd be happier if I could afford to buy more things .76 .03 −.22
9. It sometimes bothers me quite a bit that I can't afford to buy all the things I'd like .66 .06 −.31
Product retention tendency .75 .92 .92
1. Getting rid of stuff is difficult for me .01 .90 .01
2. I tend to hold on to my possessions .03 .90 .01
3. I do not like to dispose of possessions −.03 .92 −.02
4. Unless I have a really good reason to throw something away, I keep it −.01 .89 .05
Life satisfaction (SWLS) .72 .93 .92
1. In most ways my life is close to my idea .01 .01 .93
2. The conditions of my life are excellent .04 −.01 .90
3. I am satisfied with my life .03 .03 .92
4. So far I have gotten the important things I want in life −.03 .03 .88
5. If I could live my life over, I would change almost nothing .03 −.01 .77

Note: Highest item loading shown in bold.

Timoney & Holder, 2013). In the full sample, 49.3 percent were male previous studies (Mattila et al., 2007), individuals with alexithymia had
with an average age of 36.89 (SD = 12.18). Most were in a relation- significantly lower life satisfaction (M = 4.22), as compared to those
ship (68.1%), but only about half had children (49.1%). A majority of re- without alexithymia (M = 4.57, t(1,079) = 2.95, p < .01). In support of
spondents were employed (80.8%) with an average household income Hypotheses 1a and 1b, individuals with alexithymia were also more
of at least $50,000 (52.5%). As can be seen in Table 2, there were no materialistic (M = 4.34), as compared to those without alexithymia
statistical differences between individuals with alexithymia and indi- (M = 3.61, t(1,079) = 7.04, p < .001) and had a stronger product reten-
2
viduals without alexithymia in terms of relationship status (χ (1) = .017, tion tendency (M = 4.85), as compared to individuals without alexithy-
p = .895), likelihood of having children (χ2 (1) = 2.79, p = .095), edu- mia (M = 3.97, t(1,079) = 7.16, p < .001). When comparing respondents
cation level (χ2 (2) = 3.92, p = .141), employment status (χ2 (3) = 4.53, in terms of high and low materialism (defined as one standard devia-
p = .209) or income level (χ2 (4) = 3.75, p = .441). However, individuals tion above or below the mean), 27.6% of individuals with alexithymia
with alexithymia were significantly younger (t(1,079) = 6.41, p < .001) also scored high on materialism, as compared to only 14.8% of individ-
and more likely to be male (χ2 (1) = 13.87, p < .001). These findings uals without alexithymia (χ2 (1) = 33.69, p < .001). In fact, although the
are consistent with previous work (Kench & Irwin, 2000; Parker, Bagby, overall sample only included 18.8% alexithymic individuals, 30.1% of
Taylor, Endler, & Schmitz, 1993) and subsequent analyses, therefore, those with high materialism also had alexithymia. Interestingly, mean
include both age and gender as control variables. life satisfaction for individuals with alexithymia who also had high ma-
terialism (M = 5.09) was not significantly different from individuals
without alexithymia who had low materialism (M = 5.19, t(361) = .396,
5  | R E S U LT S p = .693). Thus, it appears that materialism can attenuate the negative
effect of alexithymia on life satisfaction.
5.1 | Mean comparisons

A series of planned simple contrasts were conducted to examine the 5.2 | Correlation analyses
mean differences in life satisfaction, materialism and product reten-
tion for those with alexithymia (n = 203) as compared to those with- As shown in Table 3, a partial correlation analysis of the full sam-
out alexithymia (n = 878). These analyses revealed that, in line with ple (controlling for age and gender) revealed that materialism was
VREDEVELD
bs_bs_banner
      7 |
TA B L E 2   Sample characteristics
No
All alexithymia Alexithymia Difference testa 

n (%) 1,081 (100%) 878 (81.2%) 203 (18.8%)


Mean age (SD) 36.89 (12.18) 38.01 (12.48) 32.04 (9.33) t = 6.41 (p < .001)
Male 533 (49.3%) 409 (46.6%) 124 (61.1%) χ2 = 13.87 (p < .001)
In a relationship 736 (68.1%) 597 (68.0%) 139 (68.5%) χ 2 = .017 (p = .895)
(Yes)
Children (Yes) 531 (49.1%) 442 (50.3%) 89 (43.8%) χ 2 = 2.79 (p = .095)
Education (highest χ 2 = 3.92 (p = .141)
earned)
High-school/ 490 (45.3%) 410 (46.7%) 80 (39.4%)
Technical
school
Bachelor's 440 (40.7%) 351 (40.0%) 89 (43.8%)
degree
Master's degree 151 (14.0%) 117 (13.3%) 34 (16.7%)
or higher
Employment status χ 2 = 4.53 (p = .209)
Unemployed 115 (10.6%) 91 (10.4%) 24 (11.8%)
Employed 873 (80.8%) 706 (80.4%) 167 (82.3%)
Student 49 (4.5%) 40 (4.6%) 9 (4.4%)
Retired 44 (4.1%) 41 (4.7%) 3 (1.5%)
Household Income χ 2 = 3.75 (p = .441)
Less than 216 (20.0%) 168 (19.1%) 48 (23.6%)
$30,000
$30,000- 298 (27.6%) 239 (27.2%) 59 (29.1%)
$49,999
$50,000-$69,999 251 (23.2%) 205 (23.3%) 46 (22.7%)
$70,000-$89,999 136 (12.6%) 115 (13.1%) 21 (10.3%)
$90,000 or more 180 (16.7%) 151 (17.2%) 29 (14.3%)
a
Difference test comparing individuals without alexithymia to individuals with alexithymia.

TA B L E 3   Means, standard deviations


Partial correlationsa 
and partial correlations
Group Construct Mean SD (1) (2)

Full sample (1) Materialism (MVS−9) 3.75 1.34


(N = 1,081) (2) Product retention 4.14 1.61 .36**
(3) Life satisfaction (SWLS) 4.50 1.52 −.14* −.14*
No (1) Materialism (MVS−9) 3.61 1.35
alexithymia (2) Product retention 3.97 1.62 .33**
(n = 878)
(3) Life satisfaction (SWLS) 4.56 1.51 −.21** −.17**
Alexithymia (1) Materialism (MVS−9) 4.34 1.21
(n = 203) (2) Product retention 4.85 1.35 .35**
(3) Life satisfaction (SWLS) 4.22 1.56 .29** .10
a
Partial correlations after controlling for age and gender.
*p < .01; **p < .001.

significantly negatively correlated with life satisfaction (r = −.14, negatively correlated with product retention tendency (r = −.14,
p < .001), but significantly positively correlated with product re- p < .001) for the full sample. A correlation analysis comparing partial
tention (r = .36, p < .001). Life satisfaction was also significantly correlations (controlling for age and gender) between alexithymic
|
8      
bs_bs_banner
VREDEVELD

and non-alexithymic individuals (Table 3) using Fisher's z-test re- effect of materialism on life satisfaction for individuals with alex-
vealed that materialism was significantly positively correlated with ithymia (b = .396, t(1,075) = 4.275, p < .001), after controlling for
life satisfaction for individuals with alexithymia (r = .29, p < .001), age and gender. However and importantly, as shown in Table 3
but significantly negatively correlated for individuals without alex- (Model 3), the three-way interaction between materialism, alex-
ithymia (r = −.21, p < .001); this difference was significant (z = 6.53, ithymia and product retention tendency was significant (b = −.162,
p < .001). Moreover, product retention tendency was significantly t(1,071) = 2.821, p = .005). A floodlight analysis using PROCESS
negatively correlated with life satisfaction for individuals without (Model 3) and the Johnson-Neyman method (Hayes, 2018) revealed
alexithymia (r = −.17, p < .001), but there was no significant corre- that the conditional interaction of alexithymia and materialism was
lation for those with alexithymia (r = .10, p = .170); the difference significant at a product retention tendency of 3.1 or higher (on a
between these correlations was also significant (z = 3.47, p < .001). 7-point scale) (b = −0.314 t(1,071) = 2.312, p = .021, CI = [−.580,
There was no significant difference between individuals with alex- −.048]. Figure 1 illustrates this conditional effect of materialism on
ithymia (r = .35) and individuals without alexithymia (r = .33) in terms life satisfaction as a function of alexithymia across three levels of
of the correlation between materialism and product retention ten- product retention tendency (after controlling for age and gender). As
dency (z = .29, p = .386). suggested by Hayes (2018), one standard deviation below the mean,
the mean and one standard deviation above the mean (i.e., 16th,
50th, 84th percentile) were utilized to illustrate the effects across
5.3 | Multiple linear regression and low, medium and high product retention tendency. As hypothesized,
moderation analysis the effect (slope) of materialism on life satisfaction was negative,
regardless of product retention tendency, for individuals without
Multiple linear regression analyses were conducted to examine the alexithymia. However, this negative effect decreased (i.e., become
moderating roles of alexithymia and product retention tendency on more positive) with stronger product retention tendency and was
the effect of materialism on life satisfaction. As shown in Table 4 only marginally significant for high product retention (bLow = −.274,
(Model 1), as expected, materialism had a negative effect on life sat- t(1,071) = 5.291, p < .001; bMedium = −.187, t(1,071) = 4.684, p < .001;
isfaction, after controlling for age and gender, when no moderators bHigh = −.111, t(1,071) = 1.947, p = .052). For individuals with alex-
were included in the model (b = −.169, t(1,077) = 4.779, p < .001). ithymia, the effect of materialism was positive and significant at
When including alexithymia in the model (Model 2), the interaction medium (bMedium = .313, t(1,071) = 3.109, p < .01) and high levels
between materialism and alexithymia was significant, with a nega- of product retention (bHigh = .673, t(1,071) = 5.507, p < .001), but
tive effect of materialism on life satisfaction for individuals without negative and not significant at low product retention (bLow = −.098,
alexithymia (b = −.246, t(1,075) = 6.514, p < .001) and a positive t(1,071) = .604, p = .546). Hence, stronger product retention

TA B L E 4   Regression coefficients, standard errors and model summary

Model 3: Materialism,
Model 2: Materialism and alexithymia and product
Model 1: Materialism only alexithymia retention

Antecedent β SE p β SE p β SE p

Constant 5.529 .225 p < .001 2.895 .448 p < .001 6.29 1.040 p < .001
Gender −.274 .092 p = .003 −.228 .091 p = .013 −.189 .091 p = .038
Age −.007 .004 p = .069 −.008 .004 p = .038 −.007 .004 p = .075
Materialism −.169 .035 p < .001 .396 .093 p < .001 −.561 .269 p = .038
Alexithymia (2 groups)*,a 2.969 .439 p < .001 .359 1.075 p = .739
Interaction 1: Materialism × Alexithymia −.642 .100 p < .001 .189 .283 p = .506
Product retention −.773 .220 p < .001
Interaction 2: Materialism × Product .206 .054 p < .001
retention
Interaction 3: Alexithymia × Product .508 .234 p = .029
retention
Interaction 4: −.162 .057 p = .005
Materialism × Alexithymia × Product
retention
Model summary F(3,1,077) = 11.59, p < .001 F(5,1,075) = 16.41, p < .001 F(9,1,071) = 12.76, p < .001
a
Individuals with alexithymia coded as zero; individuals with no alexithymia coded as one.
VREDEVELD
bs_bs_banner
|
      9

F I G U R E 1   Conditional effects of
materialism on life satisfaction as a
function of alexithymia and product
retention tendency

tendency attenuates the negative effect of materialism on life sat- between different types of materialistic consumers based on atti-
isfaction for those without alexithymia and enhances the positive tudes towards spending money (i.e., degree of frugality) and degree
effect of materialism on life satisfaction for those with alexithymia. of material values (Tatzel, 2014). This present research suggests that
accounting for emotional personality traits and, in particular, think-
ing style, can help explain the different types of motives and traits
6 | D I S CU S S I O N A N D I M PLI C ATI O N S that create such different materialistic pursuits. Individuals with
alexithymia have an externally oriented thinking style that makes ex-
The results support the notion that it is important to consider alex- trinsic motivation more satisfying. Thus, spending money on goods
ithymia when assessing both degree of materialism and outcomes to achieve extrinsic goals is more appealing to individuals with alex-
of materialism. First, in support of Hypotheses 1a and 1b, individu- ithymia. Importantly, although the results show that individuals with
als with alexithymia exhibited significantly more materialistic values alexithymia have more materialistic values and a stronger product
and a stronger product retention tendency, as compared to individ- retention tendency, as will be discussed in the next section, this does
uals without alexithymia. Second, in support of Hypothesis 2, the not necessarily mean that alexithymic individuals are more likely to
effect of materialism on well-being is different for individuals with suffer from the detrimental effects of materialism observed in previ-
alexithymia. Specifically, materialistic values had a positive effect ous research. In fact, materialism and product retention tendency
on life satisfaction for individuals with alexithymia, but a negative have a positive effect of well-being for individuals with alexithymia.
effect on life satisfaction for individuals without alexithymia. Finally, Thus, the present findings support that materialistic values vary
in support of Hypothesis 3, product retention tendency moderates in both degree and outcomes across different types of consumers
this effect such that the relationship between materialistic values and that researchers need to consider individual characteristics, be-
and life satisfaction is more positive for individuals with alexithymia yond demographics and income, to get a complete picture of how
who also have a strong product retention tendency. The implications individuals derive value from materialistic pursuits and retention of
of each of these findings are discussed in the subsequent sections. possessions.
This research also supports that materialism and product reten-
tion tendency are different but related constructs (Haws et al., 2012).
6.1 | Materialism and product retention tendency Some materialistic consumers place value on retaining their posses-
sions and deriving continued use and value from these possessions.
In contrast to previous research that examine demographic and As will be discussed, it is possible that these individuals may derive
cultural moderators of materialism (Sidhu & Foo, 2015; Xiao & emotional benefits from such retention to help mitigate the threat
Tessema, 2019), this study shows that it is important to also con- of the “hedonic treadmill” where materialistic individuals engage in
sider how degree of materialism is influenced by emotional traits (and contemplate) unnecessary buying in order to derive continued
and personality. Previous research has attempted to distinguish pleasure from their materialistic pursuits (Dittmar et al., 2014, p. 856;
|
10      
bs_bs_banner
VREDEVELD

Dunn et al., 2011). Based on the findings from this study, individu- Interestingly, although not directly hypothesized as part of this
als with alexithymia are likely to belong to this group of consumers. study, product retention tendency also appears to have an atten-
Other consumers have strong product retention tendency without uating effect on the negative relationship between materialism
materialistic values. It is likely that these consumers, instead of ma- and life satisfaction for individuals without alexithymia. According
terialistic values, have values related to frugality, thrift and envi- to Solberg et al. (2004), materialists set unrealistic expectations
ronmental concerns that allows them to derive value and emotional for what material goods will bring them and therefore, continue to
benefits from creative reuse and repairs of current possessions to acquire more possessions to reduce the negative emotional states
avoid having to purchase new possessions. Thus, future research on that stem from dissatisfaction (see also Richins, 2013). However, for
materialism and its outcomes should consider the relationship (or individuals without alexithymia who had strong product retention
lack thereof) between product retention tendency and materialism. tendency, the negative effect of materialism on life satisfaction was
only marginally significant. Thus, it appears that materialistic values,
when they align with broader lifestyle traits that place value on re-
6.2 | Materialism and well-being taining possessions, are less damaging than materialistic pursuits
without such emphasis on retention. That is, keeping possessions, at
This research contributes to our understanding of how material- least to some degree, help satisfy materialistic consumers' wanting
ism affects consumer well-being and, in particular, provides a more of possessions in terms of realizing these deeply held values of ma-
nuanced view of the ‘dark side’ of materialism so often proclaimed terial goods as means to success and happiness. Moreover, it is likely
in extant research. This research lends further support to the idea that product retention tendency allows materialistic individuals to
that materialism is detrimental to well-being because of its extrinsic place more value on the possessions they have acquired (rather than
focus that detracts from satisfying important intrinsic psychological possessions they wish to acquire), thereby deriving more utility from
needs (Kasser, 2002; Kasser & Ahuvia, 2002; Kasser & Ryan, 1996). these possessions and perhaps mitigating the negative effect of the
Indeed, for most consumers, intrinsically motivated experiences are “hedonic treadmill” often experienced by materialistic individuals.
likely to be more rewarding than an extrinsic, materialistic lifestyle. Product retention is often considered and encouraged in pub-
However, not all individuals have the ability to satisfy psychological lic policy endeavours undertaken for the purpose of encouraging
needs through the pursuit of intrinsically motivated experiences; in- environmentally friendly (green) consumption and reducing waste/
dividuals with alexithymia are externally oriented thinkers with poor increasing recycling (Fuentes, 2014; Nam, 2020). However, while
emotion regulation abilities (Lyvers et al., 2014; Nemiah et al., 1976; green consumer values are related to self-control in spending and
Taylor et al., 1997). For these individuals, materialistic pursuits are frugality (Haws et al., 2014), they are also negatively related to ma-
more appealing and more rewarding because they align extrinsi- terialism (Banerjee & McKeage, 1994). Thus, a focus on the environ-
cally identifiable rewards with attainable extrinsic means. As noted, mental benefits of retention is likely not effective in reaching and
individuals with alexithymia (as compared to those without) are appealing to materialistic consumers. Instead, the findings from this
more likely to suffer from poorer well-being, including depression present research would suggest that policies and marketing efforts
(Honkalampi et al., 2000; Parker et al., 1991) and lower life satisfac- encouraging product retention more directly, with a focus on valuing
tion (Mattila et al., 2007). However, this study finds that materialism and using what the products acquired (rather than a focus on envi-
can attenuate these negative effects of alexithymia on well-being. ronmental benefits), would attenuate the negative effects of materi-
In fact, individuals with alexithymia who were also materialistic had alism on subjective well-being for materialistic consumers, while also
similar life satisfaction to those individuals without alexithymia who encouraging the reduction of wasteful consumption.
were not materialistic. This is particularly true when alexithymia is
coupled with a strong product retention tendency, most likely be-
cause retention of possessions allow for validation of the realization 7  | LI M ITATI O N S A N D FU T U R E R E S E A RC H
of successful extrinsic goal pursuit.
As noted, individuals with alexithymia are more likely to engage This research has several limitations. First, although this study uti-
in maladaptive consumption, such as gambling, substance abuse and lized a large sample, it surveyed American consumers. More cross-
eating disorders (Haviland et al., 1994; Lumley & Roby, 1995; Lyvers cultural comparisons are needed to control for possible interaction
et al., 2014; Speranza et al., 2005). The results of the present study effects of cultural materialistic values and alexithymia as extrinsic
suggest that policymakers and clinicians should consider that buying values and (material) rewards may differ across cultures. Second,
and consuming is not inherently bad for individuals with alexithymia. this research measured well-being in terms of life satisfaction, using
In particular, prevention and treatment policies for maladaptive con- the SWLS developed by Diener et al. (1985). It is possible that other
sumption/disorders should consider that including ‘benign’ materi- measures of well-being, such as measures of happiness and positive
alistic consumption, especially consumption with focus on product affect, would yield different results as they tap into more emotional
retention and the importance of valuing what has been acquired, and hedonic aspects of well-being, which are likely experienced dif-
might actually be beneficial for individuals with alexithymia as it al- ferently by individuals with alexithymia. In addition, future studies
lows for focus on extrinsic goal pursuit and fulfilment. can explore negative well-being outcomes, such as depression and
VREDEVELD
bs_bs_banner
|
      11

negative subjective emotions experienced. Materialism may have a relationships exhibited by individuals with alexithymia (Humphreys
different impact on such negative well-being outcomes for individu- et al., 2009). Thus, future research should examine the implications
als with alexithymia, especially when considering the general nega- of alexithymia for possession attachment and the specific nature of
tive outcomes of alexithymia uncovered in previous work. the consumer-object relationship for individuals with alexithymia.
The results show that product retention tendency, on its own,
may be detrimental to well-being as product retention tendency is ORCID
negatively related to life satisfaction (for individuals without alex- Anna J. Vredeveld https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8970-7640
ithymia). Thus, the positive effect of product retention appears to
be directly tied to materialism. However, this study did not consider REFERENCES
other values that could interact with product retention tendency Ahuvia, A. C., & Wong, N. Y. (2002). Personality and values based materi-
for individuals without strong materialistic values. Future research alism: Their relationship and origins. Journal of Consumer Psychology,
12(4), 389–402. https://doi.org/10.1207/15327660260382414
should examine if other values, such as frugality and environmental
Arli, D., Gil, L. D. A., & van Esch, P. (2020). The effect of religiosity on
concerns, interacts with product retention tendency to attenuate its luxury goods: The case of Chilean youths. International Journal
negative effect on well-being. of Consumer Studies, 44(3), 181–190. https://doi.org/10.1111/
Future research should also explore the implications of alexithy- ijcs.12559
Ashikali, E. M., & Dittmar, H. (2012). The effect of priming materialism on
mia for other constructs related to materialism, such as frugality,
women's responses to thin-ideal media. British Journal of Social Psychology,
thrift and hyperopia. As noted, the role of alexithymia in maladap- 51(4), 514–533. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.2011.02020.x
tive consumption behaviour (e.g., gambling, substance abuse, eating Bagby, R. M., Parker, J. D., & Taylor, G. J. (1994). The twenty-item Toronto
disorders) is well documented in previous research. However, more Alexithymia Scale—I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor
structure. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 38(1), 23–32. https://
work is needed to understand how alexithymia affects other types
doi.org/10.1016/0022-3999(94)90005-1
of consumer behaviour. As noted by Kasser (2011), use of money
Bagby, R. M., Quilty, L. C., Taylor, G. J., Grabe, H. J., Luminet, O.,
and control over spending may afford opportunities to satisfy either Verissimo, R., … Vanheule, S. (2009). Are there subtypes of alexithy-
intrinsic or extrinsic goals, depending on how money is spend (or mia? Personality and Individual Differences, 47(5), 413–418. https://
saved) and for what purpose. Thus, future research can examine how doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.04.012
Bagozzi, R. P., & Yi, Y. (1988). On the evaluation of structural equation
individuals with alexithymia choose to spend their money and the
models. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 16(1), 74–94.
motivation behind such spending. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf02723327
Another research opportunity concerns the distinction between Banerjee, B., & McKeage, K. (1994). How green is my value: Exploring
material and experiential purchases and the differential satisfac- the relationship between environmentalism and materialism. In C.
T. Allen & D. Roedder John (Eds.), NA—Advances in consumer re-
tion consumers derive from each type of purchase. Van Boven and
search (Vol. 21, 147–152). Provo, UT: Association for Consumer
Gilovich (2003) find that individuals derive more enduring satisfac- Research.
tion from experiential purchases than material purchases and that Belk, R. W. (1985). Materialism: Trait aspects of living in the material
this effect, at least in part, stems from the enduring value of expe- world. Journal of Consumer Research, 12(3), 265–280. https://doi.
org/10.1086/208515
riences (in memory), the identity relevance of experiences and be-
Chaplin, L. N., & John, D. R. (2007). Growing up in a material world: Age
cause experiences can facilitate the establishment of interpersonal differences in materialism in children and adolescents. Journal of
relationships. It would be interesting to explore if this differential Consumer Research, 34(4), 480–493. https://doi.org/10.1086/518546
effect holds true for individuals with alexithymia. Experiences are Cleveland, M., Laroche, M., & Papadopoulos, N. (2009). Cosmopolitanism,
consumer ethnocentrism, and materialism: An eight-country study of
inherently more subjective and emotional and thus, it is possible that
antecedents and outcomes. Journal of International Marketing, 17(1),
individual's with alexithymia would derive more satisfaction from 116–146. https://doi.org/10.1509/jimk.17.1.116
material purchases than experiential purchases. Coulter, R. A., & Ligas, M. (2003). To retain or to relinquish: Exploring the
Finally, research can examine the implications of the lack of so- disposition practices of packrats and purgers. ACR North American
Advances, 30, 38–43.
cial connections and poor success in romantic relationships exhib-
Czernecka, K., & Szymura, B. (2008). Alexithymia–imagination–creativ-
ited by individuals with alexithymia (Humphreys et al., 2009; Lumley ity. Personality and Individual Differences, 45(6), 445–450. https://doi.
et al., 1996). Research suggests that loneliness may encourage org/10.1016/j.paid.2008.05.019
consumers to attach emotionally to their possessions (Lastovicka Davydov, D. M., Luminet, O., & Zech, E. (2013). An externally oriented
& Anderson, 2014; Lastovicka & Sirianni, 2011). However, it is un- style of thinking as a moderator of responses to affective films in
women. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 87(2), 152–164.
clear if such compensatory consumption would allow individuals
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.12.003
with alexithymia to alleviate the self-threat of loneliness. On the Diener, E. D., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satis-
one hand, individuals with alexithymia lack the ability to identify faction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49(1), 71–75.
and regulate their own emotions, likely making emotional attach- https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13
Dittmar, H. (2008). Consumer culture, identity and well-being: The search
ment and connections to possessions more difficult. On the other
for the “good life” and the “body perfect”. Hove, England: Psychology
hand, possessions are inanimate objects and thus, would not re- Press.
quire an individual to have the ability to identify emotions in others Dittmar, H., Bond, R., Hurst, M., & Kasser, T. (2014). The relationship be-
and act appropriately, a likely antecedent of the poor interpersonal tween materialism and personal well-being: A meta-analysis. Journal
|
12      
bs_bs_banner
VREDEVELD

of Personality and Social Psychology, 107(5), 879–924. https://doi. of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(2), 410–422. https://doi.
org/10.1037/a0037409 org/10.1037/0022-3514.65.2.410
Dittmar, H., & Kapur, P. (2011). Consumerism and well-being in India and Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1996). Further examining the American
the UK: Identity projection and emotion regulation as underlying dream: Differential correlates of intrinsic and extrinsic goals.
psychological processes. Psychological Studies, 56(1), 71–85. https:// Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22(3), 280–287. https://doi.
doi.org/10.1007/s12646-011-0065-2 org/10.1177/0146167296223006
Dunn, E. W., Gilbert, D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2011). If money doesn't Kench, S., & Irwin, H. J. (2000). Alexithymia and childhood family en-
make you happy, then you probably aren't spending it right. Journal vironment. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 56(6), 737–745. https://
of Consumer Psychology, 21(2), 115–125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. doi.org/10.10 02/(sici)1097-4679(20 0 0 0 6)56:6<737:aid-jclp4
jcps.2011.02.002 >3.0.co;2-u
Fornell, C., & Larcker, D. F. (1981). Evaluating structural equation mod- La Barbera, P. A., & Gürhan, Z. (1997). The role of materialism, religiosity,
els with unobservable variables and measurement error. Journal of and demographics in subjective well-being. Psychology & Marketing,
Marketing Research, 18(1), 39–50. https://doi.org/10.2307/3151312 14(1), 71–97. https://doi.org/10.1002/(sici)1520-6793(199701)14:
Frost, R. O., & Gross, R. C. (1993). The hoarding of possessions. Behaviour 1<71:aid-mar5>3.0.co;2-l
Research and Therapy, 31(4), 367–381. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005- Lastovicka, J. L., & Anderson, L. (2014). Loneliness, material pos-
7967(93)90094-b session love, and consumers' physical well-being. In M. Tatzel
Frost, R. O., Steketee, G., & Williams, L. (2002). Compulsive buy- (Ed.), Consumption and well-being in the material world (pp.
ing, compulsive hoarding, and obsessive-compulsive disorder. 63–72). Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Springer. https://doi.
Behavior Therapy, 33(2), 201–214. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0005 org/10.1007/978-94-007-7368-4_4
-7894(02)80025-9 Lastovicka, J. L., Bettencourt, L. A., Hughner, R. S., & Kuntze, R. J.
Fuentes, C. (2014). Managing green complexities: Consumers' strat- (1999). Lifestyle of the tight and frugal: Theory and measure-
egies and techniques for greener shopping. International Journal ment. Journal of Consumer Research, 26(1), 85–98. https://doi.
of Consumer Studies, 38(5), 485–492. https://doi.org/10.1111/ org/10.1086/209552
ijcs.12124 Lastovicka, J. L., & Sirianni, N. J. (2011). Truly, madly, deeply: Consumers
Grynberg, D., Luminet, O., Corneille, O., Grèzes, J., & Berthoz, S. (2010). in the throes of material possession love. Journal of Consumer
Alexithymia in the interpersonal domain: A general deficit of empa- Research, 38(2), 323–342. https://doi.org/10.1086/658338
thy? Personality and Individual Differences, 49(8), 845–850. https:// Lumley, M. A., Ovies, T., Stettner, L., Wehmer, F., & Lakey, B. (1996).
doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.07.013 Alexithymia, social support and health problems. Journal of
Haviland, M. G., Hendryx, M. S., Shaw, D. G., & Henry, J. P. (1994). Psychosomatic Research, 41(6), 519–530. https://doi.org/10.1016/
Alexithymia in women and men hospitalized for psychoactive sub- s0022-3999(96)00227-9
stance dependence. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 35(2), 124–128. Lumley, M. A., & Roby, K. J. (1995). Alexithymia and pathological gam-
https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-440x(94)90056-n bling. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 63(3–4), 201–206. https://
Haws, K. L., Naylor, R. W., Coulter, R. A., & Bearden, W. O. (2012). doi.org/10.1159/000288960
Keeping it all without being buried alive: Understanding product Lyvers, M., Hinton, R., Gotsis, S., Roddy, M., Edwards, M. S., & Thorberg,
retention tendency. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 22(2), 224–236. F. A. (2014). Traits linked to executive and reward systems function-
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2011.05.003 ing in clients undergoing residential treatment for substance depen-
Haws, K. L., Winterich, K. P., & Naylor, R. W. (2014). Seeing the dence. Personality and Individual Differences, 70, 194–199. https://doi.
world through GREEN-tinted glasses: Green consumption values org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.07.004
and responses to environmentally friendly products. Journal of Mattila, A. K., Poutanen, O., Koivisto, A. M., Salokangas, R. K., &
Consumer Psychology, 24(3), 336–354. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Joukamaa, M. (2007). Alexithymia and life satisfaction in primary
jcps.2013.11.002 healthcare patients. Psychosomatics, 48(6), 523–529. https://doi.
Hayes, A. F. (2018). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional org/10.1176/appi.psy.48.6.523
process analysis. New York, NY: The Guilford Press. Morie, K. P., & Ridout, N. (2018). Alexithymia and maladaptive regula-
Honkalampi, K., Hintikka, J., Tanskanen, A., Lehtonen, J., & Viinamäki, tory behaviors in substance use disorders and eating disorders. In O.
H. (2000). Depression is strongly associated with alexithymia in the Luminet, R. M. Bagby, & G. J. Taylor (Eds.), Alexithymia: Advances in
general population. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 48(1), 99–104. research, theory, and clinical practice (pp. 158–173). Cambridge, UK:
https://doi.org/10.1016/s0022-3999(99)00083-5 Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108241
Humphreys, T. P., Wood, L. M., & Parker, J. D. (2009). Alexithymia and satis- 595.012
faction in intimate relationships. Personality and Individual Differences, Mudambi, R., Pedersen, T., & Andersson, U. (2014). How subsidiaries gain
46(1), 43–47. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2008.09.002 power in multinational corporations. Journal of World Business, 49(1),
Kakatsaki, D., Vaslamatzis, G., Chatziandreou, M., Anastasiadi, K., Dafni, 101–113. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jwb.2013.02.001
U., Tzavara, C., & Antsaklis, A. (2009). Alexithymia is positively as- Nam, S. J. (2020). Moderating effects of consumer empowerment on
sociated with the outcome of in vitro fertilization (IVF) treatment. the relationship between involvement in eco-friendly food and eco-
Psychological Reports, 105(2), 522–532. https://doi.org/10.2466/ friendly food behaviour. International Journal of Consumer Studies,
pr0.105.2.522-532 44(4), 297–305. https://doi.org/10.1111/ijcs.12568
Kasser, T. (2002). The high price of materialism. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Nemiah, J. C., Freyberger, H., & Sifnos, P. E. (1976). Alexithymia: A view
Kasser, T. (2011). Can thrift bring well-being? A review of the research of the psychosomatic process. In O. W. Hill (Ed.), Modern trends in
and a tentative theory. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, psychosomatic medicine (pp. 430–439). London, UK: Butterworths.
5(11), 865–877. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2011.00396.x https://doi.org/10.3109/09638289609166302
Kasser, T., & Ahuvia, A. (2002). Materialistic values and well-being in Netemeyer, R. G., Bearden, W. O., & Sharma, S. (2003). Scaling proce-
business students. European Journal of Social Psychology, 32(1), 137– dures: Issues and applications. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
146. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.85 Nickerson, C., Schwarz, N., Diener, E., & Kahneman, D. (2003). Zeroing
Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1993). A dark side of the American dream: in on the dark side of the American dream: A closer look at the neg-
Correlates of financial success as a central life aspiration. Journal ative consequences of the goal for financial success. Psychological
VREDEVELD
bs_bs_banner
|
      13

Science, 14(6), 531–536. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003. Speranza, M., Corcos, M., Loas, G., Stéphan, P., Guilbaud, O., Perez-Diaz,
psci_1461.x F., … Jeammet, P. (2005). Depressive personality dimensions and
Nunnally, J. C. (1978). Psychometric theory. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. alexithymia in eating disorders. Psychiatry Research, 135(2), 153–163.
Ozgen, O., & Esiyok, E. (2020). Consumer ethics, materialism and material https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2005.04.001
satisfaction: A study on Turkish adolescent consumers. International Tatzel, M. (2014). Value seekers, big spenders, non-spenders, and
Journal of Consumer Studies, 44(1), 14–24. https://doi.org/10.1111/ experiencers: Consumption, personality, and well-being. In M.
ijcs.12541 Tatzel (Ed.), Consumption and well-being in the material world
Parker, J. D., Bagby, R. M., & Taylor, G. J. (1991). Alexithymia and depres- (pp. 75–108). Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Springer. https://doi.
sion: Distinct or overlapping constructs? Comprehensive Psychiatry, org/10.1007/978-94-007-7368-4_5
32(5), 387–394. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-440x(91)90015-5 Taylor, G. J., Bagby, R. M., & Parker, J. D. (1997). Disorders of affect reg-
Parker, J. D., Bagby, R. M., Taylor, G. J., Endler, N. S., & Schmitz, P. (1993). ulation: Alexithymia in medical and psychiatric illness. New York, NY:
Factorial validity of the 20-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale. European Cambridge University Press.
Journal of Personality, 7(4), 221–232. https://doi.org/10.1002/ Thomas, N. D. (1997). Hoarding: Eccentricity or pathology: When to in-
per.2410070403 tervene? Journal of Gerontological Social Work, 29, 45–55. https://doi.
Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J. Y., & Podsakoff, N. P. org/10.1300/J083v29n01_04
(2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: A crit- Timoney, L. R., & Holder, M. D. (2013). Emotional processing deficits
ical review of the literature and recommended remedies. and happiness. Assessing the measurement, correlates, and well-be-
Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(5), 879–903. https://doi. ing of people with alexithymia. New York, NY: Springer. https://doi.
org/10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879 org/10.1007/978-94-007-7177-2
Podsakoff, P. M., & Organ, D. W. (1986). Self-reports in organizational Van Boven, L., & Gilovich, T. (2003). To do or to have? That is the ques-
research: Problems and prospects. Journal of Management, 12(4), tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(6), 1193–1202.
531–544. https://doi.org/10.1177/014920638601200408 https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.6.1193
Prkachin, G. C., Casey, C., & Prkachin, K. M. (2009). Alexithymia Verissimo, R., Taylor, G. J., & Bagby, R. M. (2000). Relationship between
and perception of facial expressions of emotion. Personality and alexithymia and locus of control. New Trends in Experimental and
Individual Differences, 46(4), 412–417. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Clinical Psychiatry, 16(1/4), 11–16.
paid.2008.11.010 Welsch, H., & Kühling, J. (2018). How green self image is related to
Richins, M. L. (2004). The material values scale: Measurement properties subjective well-being: Pro-environmental values as a social norm.
and development of a short form. Journal of Consumer Research, 31(1), Ecological Economics, 149, 105–119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecole
209–219. https://doi.org/10.1086/383436 con.2018.03.002
Richins, M. L. (2013). When wanting is better than having: Materialism, Wilbram, M., Kellett, S., & Beail, N. (2008). Compulsive hoarding: A quali-
transformation expectations, and product-evoked emotions in the tative investigation of partner and career perspectives. British Journal
purchase process. Journal of Consumer Research, 40(1), 1–18. https:// of Clinical Psychology, 47(1), 59–73. https://doi.org/10.1348/01446
doi.org/10.1086/669256 6507X 240740
Richins, M. L., & Dawson, S. (1992). A consumer values orientation for Winterich, K. P., Reczek, R. W., & Irwin, J. R. (2017). Keeping the mem-
materialism and its measurement: Scale development and valida- ory but not the possession: Memory preservation mitigates identity
tion. Journal of Consumer Research, 19(3), 303–316. https://doi. loss from product disposition. Journal of Marketing, 81(5), 104–120.
org/10.1086/209304 https://doi.org/10.1509/jm.16.0311
Ridgway, N. M., Kukar-Kinney, M., & Monroe, K. B. (2008). An expanded Xiao, G., & Tessema, K. (2019). Values, materialism and life satisfaction:
conceptualization and a new measure of compulsive buying. Journal of A study of cultural influence and gender differences in China. Journal
Consumer Research, 35(4), 622–639. https://doi.org/10.1086/591108 of Business Diversity, 19(5), 101–107. https://doi.org/10.33423/jbd.
Ryan, L., & Dziurawiec, S. (2001). Materialism and its relationship to life v19i5.2648
satisfaction. Social Indicators Research, 55(2), 185–197. Yang, S., & Stening, B. W. (2016). Antecedents of materialism in China—
Schaefer, A. D., Hermans, C. M., & Parker, R. S. (2004). A cross-cultural An intergenerational analysis. International Journal of Consumer
exploration of materialism in adolescents. International Journal of Studies, 40(6), 701–711. https://doi.org/10.1111/ijcs.12284
Consumer Studies, 28(4), 399–411. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1470- Zimmermann, G., Rossier, J., Meyer de Stadelhofen, F., & Gaillard, F.
6431.2004.00395.x (2005). Alexithymia assessment and relations with dimensions of
Sidhu, J. K., & Foo, K. H. (2015). Materialism: The road to happiness and personality. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 21(1), 23–
life satisfaction among Singaporeans. The Journal of Happiness & 33. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759.21.1.23
Well-Being, 3(1), 77–92.
Sifneos, P. E. (1972). Is dynamic psychotherapy contraindicated for a large
number of patients with psychosomatic diseases? Psychotherapy and
How to cite this article: Vredeveld AJ. Materialism and life
Psychosomatics, 21(1–6), 133–136. https://doi.org/10.1159/000287665
Solberg, E. G., Diener, E., & Robinson, M. D. (2004). Why are material-
satisfaction: The moderating roles of alexithymia and product
ists less satisfied? In T. Kasser & A. D. Kanner (Eds.), Psychology and retention tendency. Int J Consum Stud. 2020;00:1–13. https://
consumer culture: The struggle for a good life in a materialistic world doi.org/10.1111/ijcs.12606
(pp. 29–48). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
https://doi.org/10.1037/10658-003

You might also like