You are on page 1of 13

The Actual vs.

Predicted Effects of Intonation Accuracy on Vocal Performance Quality


Author(s): Richard A. Warren and Meagan E. Curtis
Source: Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal , Vol. 33, No. 2 (December 2015), pp.
135-146
Published by: University of California Press
Stable URL: https://www.jstor.org/stable/10.1525/mp.2015.33.2.135

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide
range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and
facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
https://about.jstor.org/terms

University of California Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access
to Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal

This content downloaded from


200.144.238.1 on Thu, 17 Jun 2021 12:26:33 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Intonation Accuracy and Performance Quality 135

T H E A C T UA L VS . P R E D I C T E D E F F E C T S O F I N T O NAT I O N A C C U R AC Y ON
V O C A L P E R F O R M A N C E Q UA L I T Y

R I C HA R D A. WA R R E N & M E AG A N E. C U RT I S beyond the scope of scientific investigation. This intu-


Purchase College, State University of New York ition is corroborated by research in which trained lis-
teners seem unable to discriminate between the quality
THE BELIEF THAT INTONATION ACCURACY IS A KEY of various performance components (Thompson &
determinant of musical performance quality is ubiqui- Williamson, 2003) as well as between timbral quality
tous in music pedagogy; nonetheless, empirical valida- and intonation accuracy (Madsen & Geringer, 1981).
tion of this belief is lacking. We investigated the effects Such ambiguity might be expected, as the perception of
of intonation accuracy on perceived performance qual- performance quality is likely influenced by a host of
ity and assessed whether music professionals’ beliefs factors exogenous to the performance itself, such as
about the importance of intonation are consistent with listener characteristics, cultural context, and musical
these effects. In Experiments 1 and 2, participants lis- genre. Nonetheless, understanding the determinants
tened to vocal performances that were in tune, moder- of performance quality is crucial to the extent that
ately out of tune, or severely out of tune. Only severe musicians aim to maximize the quality of their music.
mistunings caused decreases in performance quality rat- Although many professionals have strong convictions
ings for intonation insensitive listeners (those who per- regarding the importance of various performance char-
formed poorly on a mistuning detection prescreening). acteristics, these convictions are rarely held to empiri-
However, both moderate and severe mistunings were cal scrutiny.
detrimental to the ratings of intonation sensitive listen- Although many performance attributes may be diffi-
ers. These results indicate that moderate mistunings cult to study experimentally, a small number of measure-
exert a negative influence on the perceived quality of able and manipulable attributes may have predictable
a performance only if the listener can explicitly detect and meaningful influences on quality. Intonation is a
the mistunings. If a listener cannot explicitly detect the likely candidate for such an attribute. The heavy empha-
mistunings, those mistunings do not implicitly exert sis on intonation in musical performance and pedagogy
a negative influence on the perception of the perfor- suggests that it may be a crucial determinant of quality.
mance. In Experiment 3, music professionals heard Furthermore, the emergence of pitch editing software
samples of performances from Experiments 1 and 2 in allows for the quantification and manipulation of into-
each intonation condition and were asked to estimate nation with relative ease. Despite the promise of this line
the impact of the mistunings on listeners’ ratings of of investigation, the relationship between intonation
performance quality. Their predictions were compared accuracy and performance quality has received little
to the actual performance quality ratings obtained in empirical attention.
Experiment 2. Music professionals overestimated the The accuracy of intonation is determined by culturally
impact of moderate mistunings for both intonation sen- specific tuning systems. In Western music, fixed-pitch
sitive and insensitive listeners, suggesting that music pro- instruments are generally tuned to the equal tempered
fessionals may hold inaccurate beliefs about the tuning system. Under this system, the octave (two tones
importance of intonation accuracy in vocal performance. separated in fundamental frequency by a ratio of 2:1) is
Received: February 4, 2014, accepted September 28, 2014. divided into 12 logarithmically equal pitch classes. The
distance between any two adjacent pitch classes is per-
Key words: intonation, music perception, performance ceptually equal and measured in a unit called a semitone.
quality, singing, vibrato This tuning system is usually set relative to the pitch
class A tuned to 440 Hz. Thus, each pitch class under

R
ESEARCH ON THE DETERMINANTS OF MUSICAL this tuning system has a ‘‘correct’’ fundamental fre-
performance quality has been scarce, a phenom- quency tuning value relative to this standard. Devia-
enon that may reflect a tacit belief that per- tions in intonation from this standard are referred to as
formance quality is largely influenced by elusive factors flat or sharp—below or above the target fundamental

Music Perception, VOLUM E 33, ISSU E 2, PP. 135–146, IS S N 0730-7829, EL ECTR ONI C ISSN 1533-8312. © 2015 B Y THE R E GE N TS OF THE UN IV E RS I T Y O F CA LI FOR NIA A LL
R IG HTS RES ERV ED . PLEASE DIR ECT ALL REQ UEST S F OR PER MISSION T O PHOT O COPY OR R EPRO DUC E A RTI CLE CONT ENT T HRO UGH T HE UNI VE R S IT Y OF CALI FO RNIA P R E SS ’ S
R EPRIN TS AND P ERMISSI ONS WEB PAG E , HT T P :// W W W. UC PRESS . EDU / JO URNA LS . PHP ? P¼R E P RI NTS . DOI: 10.1525/ M P.2015.33.2.135

This content downloaded from


200.144.238.1 on Thu, 17 Jun 2021 12:26:33 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
136 Richard A. Warren & Meagan E. Curtis

frequency value, respectively. The degree to which a (2012) found that this ‘‘vocal generosity effect’’ extends
tone deviates from the correct tuning can be measured to melodic contexts.
in cents, a unit on the semitone scale (100 cents ¼ The vocal generosity effect suggests that mistunings
1 semitone). are harder to recognize in the human voice. However, it
Intonation accuracy in musical performance is remains unclear, both for vocals and other instruments,
achieved when the fundamental frequency (F0) of how the ability to recognize tuning errors relates to the
a tone is identical to or closely approximates the F0 impact of those errors on perceived quality. One salient
prescribed by the melody. Pitch is the perceptual expe- possibility is a linear relationship between the recogniz-
rience of the F0. Pitch perception has been shown to be ability of mistunings and their detrimental effects on
influenced by a host of factors, such as timbre (Melara performance quality: the more one hears mistunings,
& Marks, 1990; Vurma, 2010; Vurma & Ross, 2006, the less one likes the performance. This would be con-
2007), spectral content (Moore, Glasberg, & Peters, sistent with the anecdotal observation that musicians
1985; Warrier & Zatorre, 2002), and the musical expe- tend to be more negatively affected by flaws in musical
rience of the listener (Loosen, 1995). Nonetheless, as performances than nonmusicians. However, it is possi-
the pitch tends to approximate the F0, musical perfor- ble that individuals who are unable to explicitly identify
mances in which the fundamental frequencies are con- mistunings may not be immune to their deleterious
gruous with the intended melody will be perceived as effects. Tuning errors unidentifiable to some listeners
maximally in-tune. may nonetheless exert an implicit influence on perfor-
But how might performing in-tune improve percep- mance quality. Research in affective science has revealed
tions of performance quality? The most salient possibil- that individuals can have negative responses to stimuli
ity is that intonation accuracy improves quality by without being able to explicitly identify why those
reducing harmonic dissonance. Helmholtz and Ellis responses have occurred (e.g., Bechara, Damasio, Tra-
(1885) proposed that discrepancies between the harmo- nel, & Damasio, 1997). In all artistic domains, it is typ-
nics of two or more complex tones result in a displeasing ical for impressions of aesthetic quality to be affected by
‘‘beating’’ sound. Preference for and liking of musical factors unidentifiable to the perceiver (‘‘I may not know
performances may decrease as a result of such displeas- art, but I know what I like’’).
ing harmonic content. Indeed, even four-month-old The relationship between intonation accuracy and per-
infants demonstrate preference for consonant over dis- formance quality may be mediated by music training,
sonant melodies (Zentner & Kagan, 1998), suggesting which has been shown to influence pitch perception.
that humans may be biologically prepared to prefer Trained musicians have increased pitch discrimination
auditory consonance over dissonance. (Geringer, MacLeod, & Allen, 2010; Madsen, Edmonson,
Intonation accuracy in vocal performance is highly & Madsen, 1969), interval identification (Siegel & Siegel,
relevant in today’s musical environment. Vocals remain 1977), and mistuning categorization (Vurma, 2010).
the most common instrument across musical genres. Therefore, musicians may be more capable of identifying
Unlike most other instruments, the voice is not a fixed- mistunings in musical contexts, possibly rendering them
pitch instrument. It is necessary for singers to manually more susceptible to their putative detrimental effects.
adjust each vocalized pitch in attempt to produce the Furthermore, the detrimental effects of mistunings
correct pitch. This continuous manual adjustment leaves may be masked to some extent by vibrato, which has
ample room for intonation errors. Consequently, the been shown to increase the range of acceptable mistun-
voice is the instrument most frequently subjected to dig- ings in musical intervals (Van Besouw, Brereton, &
ital pitch correction in recorded music. Howard, 2008). Although the pitch of notes with vibrato
While the relationship between vocal intonation accu- tends to approximate the average modulated fundamen-
racy and performance quality remains unexplored, two tal frequency (Geringer et al., 2010; Iwamiya, Kosugi, &
recent papers have advanced our understanding of the Kitamura, 1983; Sundberg, 1972), standard deviations
perception of mistunings in the human voice. Hutchins of pitch judgments increase with vibrato width (Iwa-
and Peretz (2012) found that nonmusicians could not miya et al., 1983), suggesting that vibrato may result
identify differences in the frequency of two vocal notes in some degree of fundamental frequency ambiguity.
unless they reached 50 cents. Musicians, on the other In melodic contexts, this ambiguity may be resolved
hand, were able to identify the mistuning at 30 cents. by perceiving the note as being congruent with the
Interestingly, participants were less likely to identify melodic and chordal context.
mistunings of the human voice compared to those of The following experiments were designed to investi-
synthesized voice timbres. Hutchins, Roquet, and Peretz gate the effects of intonation accuracy and to determine

This content downloaded from


200.144.238.1 on Thu, 17 Jun 2021 12:26:33 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Intonation Accuracy and Performance Quality 137

whether these effects are consistent with the beliefs of Home from Beauty and the Beast, Eidelweiss from The
musical professionals about the importance of intona- Sound of Music, and Somewhere Out There from Feivel
tion. The following questions were addressed: Goes West. Two of the vocalists were professional musi-
cians with vocal training, two were amateur singer-
1. Are tuning errors detrimental to judgments of per-
songwriters, one was a music therapist with little singing
formance quality, and if so, to what extent?
experience, and another was a nonmusician. Male per-
2. Are the detrimental effects of moderate tuning
formances were one octave lower than those of the
errors reserved to those who can explicitly detect
females. Equal tempered tuning was used in the vocal
the tuning errors?
performances as well as their accompaniment, with
3. Are the effects of tuning errors mitigated by the
a standard pitch of 440 Hz.
use of vibrato?
Each singer recorded each song approximately five
4. Do music professionals hold accurate beliefs about
times. To minimize the amount of digital manipulation
the importance of intonation accuracy?
necessary to create the in tune performances, composite
To address these questions, recordings of six vocalists performances were created in which the most in tune
performing three songs each were digitally manipulated notes from the various takes were put together to yield
to create three versions of each performance: one in a maximally in tune performance. The intonation accu-
tune, one moderately out of tune, and one severely out racy of these composite performances varied with the
of tune. Two vocalists who used vibrato also recorded skill level of the singer, but tuning errors in the original,
versions of each song with suppressed vibrato. In Exper- unedited recordings rarely exceeded 30 cents and never
iment 1, subjects rated the performance quality of each exceeded 50 cents.
vocalist in each intonation and vibrato condition. They The two trained singers (one male and one female)
then rated the intonation accuracy of the same perfor- used vibrato when prompted to record the song. For
mances. In Experiment 2, intonation sensitive and these singers, performances were created with (Vþ) and
insensitive subjects rated the performance quality and with suppressed (V) vibrato. The male used vibrato
intonation accuracy of the same performances. In with an average peak-to-peak width (i.e., difference in
Experiment 3, music professionals estimated the impact cents from crest to trough of wave) of about 175 cents
of mistunings on performance ratings in Experiment 2 and the female used vibrato with an average peak-to-
after hearing samples of performances in each intona- peak width of about 275 cents. The vibrato frequency of
tion condition. both singers ranged from 5 to 6 Hz on different notes.
Both singers used vibrato on about 27% of the notes.
After recording each song, these singers were asked to
Experiment 1
record them a second time with suppressed vibrato. Mul-
tiple suppressed vibrato performances were recorded,
To assess the effects of intonation accuracy on judg-
and performances of each note were selected that were
ments of performance quality for a general population
maximally similar to the original notes in all character-
of listeners, college students rated the quality and into-
istics other than the presence of vibrato. To further max-
nation accuracy of six vocalists performing three songs
imize the similarity between Vþ and V versions of each
each with varying degrees of intonation accuracy. Each
performance, composite performances were then created
performance was accompanied by karaoke tracks with
consisting of all notes from the Vþ performances that
instrumentation described below. Two singers who used
did not have vibrato combined with all the other selected
vibrato also recorded performances with suppressed
notes performed with suppressed vibrato. This resulted
vibrato, allowing us to assess whether vibrato mitigates
in Vþ and V performances that were 73% identical.
the effects of tuning errors.
We used ballads because we expected the effects of
intonation to be more evident in the context of slow
METHOD
to mid-tempo songs with sustained notes. All perfor-
Subjects. Eighteen students (6 male, 12 female) from
mances were accompanied by karaoke tracks consisting
a small liberal arts college in the northeastern United
of piano, strings, oboe, and flute (Home); classical guitar
States participated for course credit. Ages ranged from
and strings (Edelweiss); and piano, strings, electric bass,
19 to 23 (M ¼ 19.8).
and a drum kit (Feivel Goes West). A variety of multi-
Stimuli. Three male and three female vocalists of varying instrument accompaniments were used both to maxi-
skill level recorded short ( 20 s) samples of three musi- mize the points of pitch reference against which the
cal theater ballads with instrumental accompaniment: vocal performances could be judged.

This content downloaded from


200.144.238.1 on Thu, 17 Jun 2021 12:26:33 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
138 Richard A. Warren & Meagan E. Curtis

Three different versions of each performance were judgments. Their ratings were to reflect the quality of
digitally created: one in which every note was in tune the vocal performances only.
(T-0), one in which half the notes were 25 cents flat Subjects then listened to and rated the quality of the
(T-25), and one in which the same notes were 50 cents six singers performing each of the three songs. The
flat (T-50). The detuned notes were randomly selected suppressed-vibrato versions of two vocalists were also
but were consistent across both conditions. The intona- rated, creating a total of 24 performances evaluated by
tion of the accompaniment was not manipulated. Melo- each subject. Each vocalist, as well as the suppressed-
dyne, a standard tool used in the recording industry, vibrato versions of the two vocalists, was represented in
allows for the independent manipulation of ‘‘pitch all three tuning conditions for every subject, but parti-
drift,’’ the low frequency pitch oscillations within a note, cipants only heard a given singer’s performance of a spe-
and ‘‘pitch modulation,’’ the higher frequency oscilla- cific song in one of the three tuning conditions. Song X
tions associated with vibrato. Pitch drift was eliminated intonation accuracy condition combinations were
from all notes. However, stylistic transient ‘‘scoops’’ at counterbalanced across subjects, and the order of pre-
the beginning of some notes were retained to maintain sentation was randomized. The vibrato and no-vibrato
a realistic sound. Subtle pitch variations of +5 cents versions of the same song were never in the same tuning
within a note were retained for all notes. Past research condition for a given subject.
has shown that such minimal variations do not affect After rating the quality of the 24 performances, sub-
perceptions of intonation accuracy (Sundberg, Prame, jects listened to the same performances again, this time
& Iwarsson, 1995). Pitch variations greater than 5 cents rating how ‘‘in tune’’ the performances sounded on
were only retained for notes with noticeable vibrato, as a scale from 1 to 7. The order of presentation was again
described previously. For notes with vibrato, pitch was randomized. The whole procedure took about 25
defined as mean fundamental frequency over time. minutes.
Formant correction was applied to minimize the audi-
bility of the digital manipulation. Formants are the RESULTS
spikes in the amplitude at certain frequencies that deter- To assess the relationship between intonation accuracy
mine vowel sounds in the human voice. Formant cor- and performance quality, a 6 x 3 repeated measures
rection maintains a natural sound by preventing the ANOVA was conducted with two within-subjects fac-
transposition of these formants when pitch is altered tors: Singer (1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6) and Intonation Accuracy
(Bernsee, 2000). Due to the limited range of pitch (T-0, T-25, T-50). The dependent measure was Quality
adjustments, degradation of the recordings resulting Rating, which had a potential range of 1 to 7. The per-
from the pitch manipulation was negligible. No audible formances with suppressed vibrato were not included in
artifacts were present. this analysis. Mean quality and tuning ratings for each
condition are depicted in Figure 1. There were significant
Apparatus. Performances were recorded at a sampling
main effects of both Singer, F(5, 85) ¼ 24.99, p < .001,
frequency of 44.1 kHz with a Neumann TLM 49 micro-
and Intonation Accuracy F(2, 34) ¼ 26.09, p < .001.
phone. Avid’s Pro Tools software (version 9.0) was used
There was no difference between T-0 and T-25 quality
to record and edit the performances (http://www.avid.
ratings, t(17) ¼ 0.85, p ¼ .41, but there was a significant
com/us/products/family/pro-tools/). Celemony’s Melo-
decrease of 0.81 points between T-25 and T-50, t(17) ¼
dyne Editor software Version 2 (http://www.celemony.
4.50, p < .001. There was a Singer X Intonation Accuracy
com/en/start) was used to manipulate and analyze
interaction, F(10, 170) ¼ 2.15, p < .05, that appeared to be
pitch. Pro Tools and Melodyne are standard tools in the
driven by one singer who did not have a significant
recording industry. Subjects listened to the recordings
decrease in ratings between T-0 and T-50, t(17) ¼ 0.48,
through Sony MDR-7506 headphones. Stimuli were
p ¼ .64 (comparisons of T-0 and T-50 were significant at
administered and responses were collected using
p < .05 for all other singers).
E-Prime and a Dell Optiplex 980 computer.
The above ANOVA was repeated with Tuning Rating
Procedure. Subjects were tested individually in a lab. as the dependent variable. There was a main effect of
Instructions on a computer screen informed them that Intonation Accuracy, F(2, 34) ¼ 14.53, p < .001, with T-0
they would hear a series of vocal performances. Their performances being rated more in tune (M ¼ 4.95,
task was to rate the quality of each performance on SD ¼ 0.97) than T-50 performances (M ¼ 3.87, SD ¼
a scale from 1 to 7 by clicking on the computer screen. 1.05). Also, despite each singer being represented
They were instructed to not let their feelings about the equally in each tuning condition, there was a main effect
musical genre or songs influence their performance of Singer, F(5, 85) ¼ 25.59, p < .001. An average

This content downloaded from


200.144.238.1 on Thu, 17 Jun 2021 12:26:33 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Intonation Accuracy and Performance Quality 139

and Intonation Accuracy (T-0, T-25, T-50) as within-


subjects variables and Quality Rating as the dependent
variable (Figure 2). There was a main effect of Vibrato,
F(1, 17) ¼ 14.25, p < .01, with the quality of Vþ per-
formances (M ¼ 5.38, SD ¼ 0.57) rated higher than V
performances (M ¼ 4.76, SD ¼ 0.69). There was no
Vibrato X Tuning interaction, F(2, 34) ¼ 1.56, p ¼
.22, 1  ¼ .308. This ANOVA was repeated with
Tuning Rating as the dependent variable and the same
pattern of effects and interactions emerged (Figure 3).

DISCUSSION
The detrimental effects of tuning errors evinced in the
results correspond to the commonly held belief that
intonation accuracy is a determinant of performance
quality. However, given the severity of the tuning
FIGURE 1. Quality and tuning ratings of performances in different manipulation, the authors were surprised by the mod-
intonation accuracy conditions (Experiment 1). T-0 is perfectly in tune, esty of this effect. There was no difference in the quality
T-25 is moderately out-in tune, and T-50 is severely out of tune. Error ratings of the in tune and moderately out of tune per-
bars represent standard error.
formances. Furthermore, the severely out of tune con-
dition, in which half of the notes were 50 cents flat, only
correlation of .55 between each singer’s Quality Rating saw a 20.5% decrease from the in tune condition. This is
and Tuning Rating (both pooled across Intonation perhaps most surprising given the rich harmonic con-
Accuracy conditions) revealed that singers with greater text available for pitch matching. Despite the ample
average quality ratings tended to be rated as more in harmonic cues, severe mistunings were only modestly
tune. The Singer X Intonation Accuracy interaction that detrimental to performance quality.
emerged with Quality Rating as the dependent variable Interestingly, the overall quality of the singers was asso-
did not emerge with Tuning Rating as the dependent ciated with the perception of intonation accuracy. Singers
variable, F(10, 170) ¼ 1.43, p ¼ .17. with higher quality scores overall were rated as being
To test the hypothesis that vibrato mitigates the effects more in tune, despite all singers being equally repre-
of tuning errors, a 2 x 2 x 3 repeated measures ANOVA sented in each tuning condition. While it is possible that
was conducted with Singer (1, 2), Vibrato (Vþ, V), the tuning errors of better singers are less perceptible, it

FIGURES 2 AND 3. Quality and tuning ratings of performances with vibrato and with suppressed vibrato in different intonation accuracy conditions
(Experiment 1). T-0 is perfectly in tune, T-25 is moderately out-in tune, and T-50 is severely out of tune. Error bars represent standard error.

This content downloaded from


200.144.238.1 on Thu, 17 Jun 2021 12:26:33 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
140 Richard A. Warren & Meagan E. Curtis

seems more likely that subjects in this experiment did not classes and 51 students in music classes at a small liberal
have the skill to assess the intonation accuracy of the arts college. All students were subsequently invited to
performances. Subjects may therefore have conflated participate in the study and were grouped according to
intonation accuracy with performance quality, a hypoth- their scores on the intonation discrimination test. Thirty-
esis supported by the correlation (r ¼ .55) between tun- one music students (60.8%) and fifteen psychology stu-
ing and quality ratings of the same performances. dents (16.0%) correctly answered at least five out of seven
Subjects may simply be substituting a difficult question of the items on the test. All of the psychology students in
(‘‘How in tune are these performances?’’) with an easier this group reported playing an instrument, but three
one (‘‘Of what quality are these performances?’’). This students who were ultimately included in the study had
possibility was addressed in Experiment 2. no formal training. Twenty-four of these IS students
The influence of vibrato on quality was also assessed. enrolled in the study and twenty II students scoring less
Performances with vibrato were given higher quality than five also enrolled. All participants were compen-
and tuning ratings than performances with suppressed sated with $10.
vibrato, suggesting that vibrato may generally be per- Music training data were collected and compared for
ceived as characteristic of high quality singing (at least IS and II subjects. IS subjects played their instruments
in this musical context). The hypothesis that vibrato for more years (M ¼ 9.88, SD ¼ 4.49) than II subjects
mitigates the effects of tuning errors was not supported (M ¼ 4.45, SD ¼ 4.35), t(42) ¼ 4.05, p < .001. They also
by the data, as a vibrato by intonation accuracy interac- had more years of formal training, t(42) ¼ 3.61, p < .01.
tion failed to emerge. However, the low observed power There was no difference in the age at which the two
of this test (1  ¼ .308) renders this observation groups began playing, t(42) ¼ 1.27, p ¼ .21.
inconclusive.
Mistuning Identification Test. A male vocalist was
recorded singing the last line of the first verse of the
Experiment 2
song London Bridge is Falling Down at a slow tempo.
The voice was accompanied by a piano playing both the
We failed to find a difference between the quality ratings
chords and melody of the song. The vocal was digitally
of in tune and moderately out of tune performances in
manipulated to create six different versions of the per-
Experiment 1. However, it is possible that individuals
formance: one in which every note of the eleven note
with greater pitch discrimination ability are more neg-
phrase (‘‘London Bridge is falling down, my fair lady’’)
atively affected by mistunings than those less capable of
was in tune, and five in which one pitch—occurring on
discriminating pitch. While recent studies have shed
‘‘fair,’’ ‘‘fall-,’’ ‘‘Bridge,’’ ‘‘down,’’ or ‘‘my’’—was tuned flat
light on the identifiably of various degrees of mistunings
25 cents (all other notes remained in tune).
in vocal performance (Hutchins & Peretz, 2012; Hutch-
Subjects listened to the samples and attempted to
ins et al., 2012), the relationship between the ability to
identify the out of tune notes. They were tested either
identify mistunings and the effects of those mistunings
alone or in a classroom setting. Those tested alone lis-
on quality perception remains unexplored. In Experi-
tened to the recordings through headphones, and those
ment 2, we identified intonation sensitive and intona-
tested in a classroom listened over speakers. To ensure
tion insensitive listeners using a pitch discrimination
that students tested in classrooms were unaware of their
prescreening task in which subjects attempted to identify
classmates’ responses, they were spaced adequately
mistuned notes in a series of short vocal performance
apart and instructed to respond to each sample only
samples. Subjects then rated the performance quality and
after the sample had played completely. Each sample
intonation accuracy of the same performances from
was played once and the in tune sample was played twice
Experiment 1. The inclusion of the prescreening task
in pseudorandom order. Subjects were told that each
enabled us to address whether the ability to explicitly
sample would have either one or no notes out of tune.
detect mistunings makes listeners more susceptible to the
After each sample subjects identified which note, if any,
detrimental influence of mistunings on performance
sounded out of tune by circling either the corresponding
quality.
syllable or the word ‘‘none’’ on a response sheet.
METHOD Stimuli, Apparatus, and Procedure. Prior to the main
Subjects. Twenty-four Intonation Sensitive (IS) and experiment, college students were administered the
twenty Intonation Insensitive (II) subjects were identified prescreening mistuning identification test in groups
using a mistuning identification test (described below). or individually. Those tested in groups were invited
The test was administered to 94 students in psychology to participate via email, and those tested individually

This content downloaded from


200.144.238.1 on Thu, 17 Jun 2021 12:26:33 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Intonation Accuracy and Performance Quality 141

FIGURES 4 AND 5. Quality and tuning ratings of intonation sensitive and intonation insensitive listeners in different intonation accuracy conditions
(Experiment 2). T-0 is perfectly in tune, T-25 is moderately out-in tune, and T-50 is severely out of tune. Error bars represent standard error.

participated in the experiment immediately after they ratings decreased between T-0 and T-25, t(23) ¼ 5.44,
finished the prescreening. Those included in the main p < .001, as well as between T-25 and T-50, t(23) ¼ 8.59,
experiment filled out a brief music training question- p < .001, but II subjects’ quality ratings only decreased
naire and then completed the main experiment, which between the T-25 and T-50 conditions, t(19) ¼ 8.03, p <
was identical to that of Experiment 1. .001. In contrast to Experiment 1, there was no Singer X
Intonation Accuracy interaction, F(10, 420) ¼ 0.75, p ¼
RESULTS .68. The same pattern of results was obtained for the
The standard deviations of quality and tuning ratings tuning ratings.
were compared between IS and II subjects to determine Per subject correlations were used to assess the rela-
whether both groups utilized the rating scales similarly. tionship between quality ratings, tuning ratings, and the
The standard deviations of the quality ratings of II and intonation accuracy of each performance. For each
IS subjects did not differ, t(42) ¼ 0.02, p ¼ .58, nor did subject, correlations were computed for 1) Quality Rat-
the standard deviations for the tuning ratings, t(42) ¼ ing and Intonation Accuracy, 2) Tuning Rating and
0.47, p ¼ .71. Intonation Accuracy, and 3) Quality Rating and Tun-
To test the hypothesis that intonation errors are more ing Rating. All of these correlations were stronger for
detrimental to the performance ratings of IS subjects, IS than II subjects, Quality Rating and Intonation
a 6 x 3 x 2 ANOVA was conducted with three variables: Accuracy: t(42) ¼ 5.59, p < .001; Tuning Rating and
Singer (1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6), Intonation Accuracy (T-0, T-25, Intonation Accuracy: t(42) ¼ 7.12, p < .001; Quality
T-50), and Intonation Sensitivity (IS, II). Both Singer Rating and Tuning Rating: t(42) ¼ 4.14, p < .05. The
and Intonation Accuracy were repeated measures, and correlation between Quality Rating and Tuning Rating
Intonation Sensitivity was a subject variable. The depen- was .77 for IS subjects, .68 for intonation insensitive
dent measure was Quality Rating (see Figures 4 and 5 subjects, and .73 overall.
for Quality and Tuning Ratings). The performances To assess whether intonation sensitivity mediates the
with suppressed vibrato were not included in this relationship between vibrato and perceived perfor-
analysis. mance quality, a 2 x 2 x 2 ANOVA was conducted with
As in Experiment 1, the quality ratings exhibited Singer (1, 2), Intonation Sensitivity (IS, II), and Vibrato
significant main effects of Singer, F(5, 210) ¼ 117.86, (Vþ, V), as independent variables and Quality Rating
p < .001, and Intonation Accuracy, F(2, 84) ¼ 129.83, as the dependent variable. Both Singer and Vibrato were
p < .001, with T-50 performances scoring an average of repeated measures and Intonation Sensitivity was a sub-
1.77 points lower than T-0 performances. There was also ject variable (Figure 6).
an Intonation Accuracy X Intonation Sensitivity inter- As in Experiment 1, there was a main effect of Vibrato,
action, F(2, 42) ¼ 18.99, p < .001. IS subjects’ quality F(1, 42) ¼ 89.73, p < .001, with Vþ performances scoring

This content downloaded from


200.144.238.1 on Thu, 17 Jun 2021 12:26:33 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
142 Richard A. Warren & Meagan E. Curtis

FIGURES 6 AND 7. Quality and tuning ratings of performances with vibrato and with suppressed vibrato in different intonation accuracy conditions
(Experiment 2). Means were pooled for intonation sensitive and insensitive subjects because intonation sensitivity did not mediate the interaction
between vibrato and intonation accuracy. T-0 is perfectly in tune, T-25 is moderately out-in tune, and T-50 is severely out of tune. Error bars represent
standard error.

higher (M ¼ 5.30, SD ¼ .85) than V performances psychological studies), the results may generalize well
(M ¼ 4.35, SD ¼ .84). In contrast to Experiment 1, there to individuals who have the same level of music train-
was a significant Vibrato X Tuning interaction, F(2, 84) ¼ ing as our II participants (i.e., individual who have
5.28, p < .01, 1  ¼ .823, with Vþ performances played a musical instrument for fewer than 5 years).
suffering less in the out of tune conditions compared to One may extrapolate that the population at large is
V- performances. The difference between T-0 and T-50 dominated by listeners who are insensitive to moderate
was 1.73 for Vþ performances and 2.54 for V perfor- mistunings.
mances, t(43) ¼ 3.29, p < .01. There was no Intonation It is interesting to note that the tuning ratings for
Sensitivity X Intonation Accuracy X Vibrato interaction, perfectly in tune performances in both Experiments 1
F(2, 42) ¼ 1.73, p ¼ .18. This ANOVA was repeated with and 2 were less than the perfect score of 7 (M ¼ 4.95,
Tuning Rating as the dependent variable and the same M ¼ 5.28, respectively). Correctly identifying degrees of
pattern of effects and interactions emerged (Figure 7). mistuning can be difficult, especially for II listeners.
Indeed, even trained musicians have difficulty discrim-
DISCUSSION inating between timbral quality and intonation accuracy
As predicted, tuning errors were more detrimental to (Madsen & Geringer, 1981). Subjects in our experiments
perceived quality for intonation sensitive listeners. may similarly be conflating other performance charac-
While quality ratings fell by only 28.0% between T-0 teristics with intonation accuracy. This is supported by
to T-50 conditions for II listeners, they fell by 47.1% the finding from Experiment 1 that singers with higher
for IS listeners. Additionally, quality ratings fell signif- overall quality ratings were considered more in tune
icantly between the T-0 and T-25 conditions for IS despite intonation accuracy being equal across singers.
listeners, but no difference was observed between these Furthermore, in Experiment 2 IS subjects gave higher
conditions for II listeners. The pattern of results tuning ratings for perfectly in tune performances than II
obtained for the II listeners is similar to the pattern subjects (M ¼ 5.70, M ¼ 4.78, respectively); they also
of results obtained in Experiment 1. This suggests that had higher correlations between intonation accuracy
our convenience sample of Experiment 1 participants, and tuning ratings. These findings suggest that subjects
recruited from an undergraduate psychology subject with higher pitch discrimination can more accurately
pool, was dominated by listeners who were apparently identify performances’ tuning accuracy. Conversely, the
insensitive to moderate mistunings. While we cannot lower tuning ratings of in tune performances by general
claim that results obtained from an undergraduate psy- listeners in Experiment 1 and II subjects in Experiment
chology subject pool generalize well to the population 2 are likely due to difficulties in rating tuning accuracy,
at large (a limitation that unfortunately applies to most rather than actual perceptions of mistunings.

This content downloaded from


200.144.238.1 on Thu, 17 Jun 2021 12:26:33 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Intonation Accuracy and Performance Quality 143

The per subject correlation between quality rating and suffer in all out of tune conditions. Therefore, we
intonation accuracy served as an indicator of the impor- decided to test whether professional musicians would
tance of intonation for each subject. Those whose per- make similar misjudgments regarding the impact of
ception of quality is heavily influenced by intonation mistunings on performance quality.
should exhibit a strong relationship between intonation
accuracy and quality ratings of the same performances.
Experiment 3
That this correlation was stronger for IS compared to II
subjects offers further support for the hypothesis that
In Experiment 3, we investigated whether music profes-
intonation accuracy is more important for IS listeners.
sionals hold accurate beliefs about the importance of
The IS participants had more music training than the
intonation accuracy. To address this question, music
II participants, as indicated by significant differences
professionals were asked to estimate the results of
in their years of formal training and total years spent
Experiment 2 after being described the design of the
playing an instrument. These differences, along with the
study and listening to performance samples in each
aforementioned differences in the quality ratings, sug-
intonation accuracy condition.
gest that individuals with more music training are more
likely to be influenced by intonation accuracy when
METHOD
judging performance quality. Although only associa-
Subjects. Eighteen music professionals (12 male, 6
tional conclusions can be drawn from this observation,
female) were invited to participate in research on into-
the potential causal role of music training in increasing
nation accuracy. Eight were college music professors,
the influence of intonation is supported both by
seven were college music production professors, and
research demonstrating that music training is associated
three taught music to children and adolescents
with better pitch discrimination (Geringer et al., 2010;
professionally.
Madsen et al., 1969) and superior neural encoding of
pitch (Wong, Skoe, Russo, Dees, & Kraus, 2007). It is Stimuli, Apparatus, and Procedure. Experimenters met
therefore possible that music training leads to increased subjects in their school offices and handed them a sheet
intonation sensitivity, which in turn leads to a greater of paper summarizing the design of Experiment 2. They
influence of intonation on perceived quality. were told that subjects in the experiment listened to
We also found that vibrato mitigates the detrimental performances that were ‘‘perfectly in tune, moderately
impact of tuning errors. The quality ratings of perfor- out of tune, or very out of tune’’ and that they rated
mances with vibrato suffered less in the out of tune performance quality on a scale of 1 to 7. They were told
conditions among both IS and II subjects. Even perfectly that ‘‘listeners with good ears for intonation’’ and ‘‘nor-
in tune performances with vibrato were rated as being mal listeners without good ears for intonation’’ both
more in tune than the same performances with sup- completed this task.
pressed vibrato. It appears that regardless of pitch dis- Subjects listened to a single performance in each into-
crimination ability, vibrato masks tuning errors that are nation condition before answering the questions, but
otherwise detrimental. This may seem counterintuitive, were given no specific information regarding the degree
as performances with vibrato spend very little time on of the tuning manipulation. Each subject listened to one
the correct note. Nonetheless, our findings are consis- of the six singers performing one of the three songs in
tent with previously cited research showing that the all intonation conditions on Senhheiser HD 280 head-
pitch of notes with vibrato tends to approximate the phones. Singer X song combinations were counterba-
average modulated fundamental frequency (Geringer lanced across subjects such that each stimulus from
et al., 2010; Iwamiya et al., 1983; Sundberg, 1972). Experiment 2 was heard exactly once by a subject in
Although the interaction between vibrato and intonation Experiment 3 (except for V performances, which were
accuracy on quality ratings failed to emerge in the first not used).
experiment, the second experiment had a larger sample First, subjects listened to the T-0 followed by the T-25
size and greater observed power (1  ¼ .82 compared performance and were told that these were ‘‘perfectly in
to 1  ¼ .31). The effect sizes of this interaction were tune’’ and ‘‘moderately out of tune,’’ respectively. They
comparable across Experiments 1 and 2 (sp2 ¼ .08, were told that II subjects gave an average quality rating
sp2 ¼ .11, respectively). of 4.5 to perfectly in tune performances and were asked
The finding that only severe mistunings are detrimen- to estimate the average rating of moderately out of tune
tal to perceived quality for II listeners was surprising performances for these listeners. They were then told
to the experimenters, as we expected quality ratings to that IS subjects gave an average rating of 4.8 to perfectly

This content downloaded from


200.144.238.1 on Thu, 17 Jun 2021 12:26:33 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
144 Richard A. Warren & Meagan E. Curtis

FIGURES 8 AND 9. Actual and predicted mean quality ratings for intonation insensitive (left) and intonation sensitive (right) listeners in each of three
intonation accuracy conditions. Actual ratings are taken from Experiment 2, and predicted ratings are those of the music professionals from Experiment
3. Subjects in Experiment 3 were given the average T-0 quality ratings for II and IS subjects as an anchor, so the actual and predicted T-0 values are
identical. T-0 is perfectly in tune, T-25 is moderately out-in tune, and T-50 is severely out of tune. The error bars represent standard error.

in tune performances and were asked to estimate the DISCUSSION


average rating of moderately out of tune performances Although subjects in Experiment 3 were accurate in
for these listeners. Subjects then listened to the T-0 and their estimates of the impact of severe mistunings on
T-50 performances, were told that these performances performance quality, they overestimated the impact of
were ‘‘perfectly in tune’’ and ‘‘very out of tune,’’ and were moderate mistunings for both II and IS subjects. For II
asked the same two questions for the T-50 condition. subjects, musicians predicted decreases of 18.2% in
quality ratings for moderate mistunings when there was
RESULTS actually no significant decrease in perceived quality for
Average T-25 and T-50 quality ratings were computed these listeners. For IS subjects, professionals predicted
for each subject in Experiment 2 and compared to the a decrease of 35.6% for moderate mistunings when
predicted T-25 and T-50 quality ratings of subjects in there was an actual decrease of only 17.1%.
Experiment 3. The average T-0 ratings were not That music professionals overestimate the impact of
included, as these were given to subjects in Experiment mistunings for untrained listeners might be expected.
3 as an anchor. The means and standard deviations for Musicians are likely more sensitive to mistunings and
the actual and predicted scores for IS and II subjects can may mistakenly believe that untrained listeners are sim-
be found in Figures 8 and 9. ilarly sensitive (or at least underestimate the discrep-
We used a series of t-tests to compare the predicted and ancy between their own and others’ ability to detect
actual ratings for T-25 and T-50 conditions for both II mistunings). Surprisingly, this overestimation extended
and IS subjects. Predictions of T-25 ratings for II subjects to intonation sensitive listeners as well, with music pro-
were significantly lower than the actual ratings (M ¼ fessionals predicting twice the actual decrease in quality
3.68, SD ¼ 0.90 and M ¼ 4.44, SD ¼ 0.81, respectively), ratings for intonation sensitive listeners. This suggests
t(36) ¼ 2.76, p < .01. There was no difference between the that professional musicians may mistakenly believe that
predicted and actual T-50 ratings for II subjects (M ¼ moderate mistunings are more detrimental than they
3.25, SD ¼ 0.75 and M ¼ 3.23, SD ¼ 0.62, respectively). actually are.
Similarly, predictions of T-25 ratings for IS subjects A limitation of Experiment 3 is that explaining the
were significantly lower than the actual ratings (M ¼ experimental procedure to subjects required labeling
3.09, SD ¼ 1.38 and M ¼ 3.98, SD ¼ 1.07, respectively), the three sample audio clips heard by each subject (‘‘per-
t(40) ¼ 2.34, p < .05. There was no difference between the fectly in tune,’’ ‘‘moderately out of tune,’’ and ‘‘very out
predicted and actual T-50 ratings for IS subjects (M ¼ of tune’’). These labels may have affected both percep-
2.27, SD ¼ 1.12 and M ¼ 2.56, SD ¼ 0.81, respectively). tions of mistunings as well as expectations about the

This content downloaded from


200.144.238.1 on Thu, 17 Jun 2021 12:26:33 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Intonation Accuracy and Performance Quality 145

effects of those mistunings on quality ratings. For exam- to accurately predict perceptions of performance quality
ple, an unprompted music professional might consider based on such variables, researchers may be able to
the moderately out of tune performances very, moder- identify the handful of measurable and manipulable
ately, or only slightly out of tune. Therefore, framing the characteristics that have a relatively stable influence
performances as moderately out of tune may decrease, on quality. To determine whether a characteristic meets
not affect, or increase predictions about quality detri- the latter criterion will require testing its effects across
ments depending on each subject’s baseline sensitivity a range of musical modalities. The results of the current
to mistunings. The directionality of these putative study, while suggestive, cannot be generalized unless
effects would likely vary from subject so subject, thereby conceptual replications reveal similar patterns in differ-
mitigating their influence on the results. ent genres, instruments, listeners, tempos, and musical
contexts. Past research (Hutchins, Roquet, & Peretz,
General Discussion 2012) suggests that listeners may be less likely to iden-
tify notes as out of tune when melodies are performed
The results of Experiments 1 and 2 corroborate the by the voice rather than an instrument. This ‘‘vocal
common belief that mistunings are detrimental to vocal generosity effect’’ underlines the importance of testing
performance quality. They also demonstrate that these the effects of intonation in different contexts.
effects are exaggerated among intonation sensitive lis- It would be useful to conduct conceptual replications
teners and mitigated by the presence of vibrato. Surpris- of the current study in which intonation is manipulated
ingly, moderate mistunings were not detrimental to naturalistically. Manually manipulating intonation in
quality ratings of untrained listeners. Thus, mistunings the current study afforded a high degree of independent
do not seem to have an implicit influence on those who variable control at the expense of ecological validity
cannot explicitly detect them. This finding challenged (singers do not generally sing with exactly half of the
the expectations of the experimenters and the intuitions notes flat by the same amount). Future studies could
of music professionals, who predicted sharp decreases increase ecological validity by using actual perfor-
in the quality ratings of untrained listeners for moder- mances of varying degrees of intonation accuracy. They
ately out of tune performances. could also include performances with both sharp and
The discrepancy between the actual and expected flat notes, as prior research has shown differential
effects of intonation may be of pedagogical significance. effects of sharp and flat intonation (Madsen et al.,
Many music teachers are tasked with preparing musi- 1969; Madsen & Geringer, 1976; Ward & Martin,
cians to perform for audiences consisting primarily of 1961). More naturalistic dependent variables may also
untrained listeners. If the ultimate goal of training is to be used, such as time spent listening and other operant
maximize the audience’s enjoyment of a performance, indices of preference (Geringer & Madsen, 1981, 1998).
then understanding the determinants of performance Although musical professionals often have strong con-
quality for one’s audience is paramount. Misconceptions victions about the relative importance of various aspects of
regarding the importance of various performance com- musical performance, experimental research testing these
ponents may lead to ineffectual training (e.g., an overem- convictions is oftentimes lacking. The discrepancy
phasis on intonation at the expense of other performance between the actual effects of intonation and those pre-
characteristics). While it is unlikely that optimizing into- dicted by musical professionals suggests that some of these
nation accuracy could have a negative effect, efforts beliefs may be challenged by experimental evidence.
might be better directed towards improving performance
characteristics that are more important determinants Author Note
of quality. Still, as audiences may vary in different genres,
the importance of intonation may depend on the extent Correspondence concerning this article should be
to which an audience consists of skilled listeners. addressed to Richard Warren, Columbia University,
In addition to intonation accuracy, performance qual- Hammer Health Sciences Center, 701 West 168th
ity is likely determined by a large number of variables Street, Room 501, New York, NY 10032. E-mail:
with complex interactions. While it may be impossible raw2163@columbia.edu

This content downloaded from


200.144.238.1 on Thu, 17 Jun 2021 12:26:33 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
146 Richard A. Warren & Meagan E. Curtis

References

B ECHARA , A., DAMASIO, H., T RANEL , D., & DAMASIO, A. R. M ELARA , R. D., & M ARKS , L. E. (1990). Interaction among
(1997). Deciding advantageously before knowing the advan- auditory dimensions: Timbre, pitch, and loudness. Perception
tageous strategy. Science, 275, 1293-1295. and Psychophysics, 48, 169-178. doi:10.3758/BF03207084
B ERNSEE , S. (2000, February 6). On the importance of formants in M OORE , B. C., G LASBERG , B. R., & P ETERS , R. V. (1985). Relative
pitch shifting. Retrieved from http://www.dspdimension.com/ dominance of individual partials in determining the pitch of
admin/formants-pitch-shifting/ complex tones. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 77,
G ERINGER , J. M., M AC L EOD, R. B., & A LLEN , M. L. (2010). 1853-1860.
Perceived pitch of violin and cello vibrato tones among music S IEGEL , J. A., & S IEGEL , W. (1977). Absolute identification of
majors. Journal of Research in Music Education, 57, 351-363. notes and intervals by musicians. Perception and
doi: 10.1177/0022429409350510 Psychophysics, 21, 143-152. doi:10.3758/BF03198717
G ERINGER , J. M., & M ADSEN , C. K. (1981). Verbal and operant S UNDBERG , J. (1972). Pitch of synthetic sung vowels. Speech
discrimination/preference for tone quality and intonation. Transmission Laboratory—Quarterly Progress and Status
Psychology of Music, 9, 26-30. Report, 1, 34-44. Stockholm: Royal Institute of Technology.
G ERINGER , J. M., & M ADSEN , C. K. (1998). Musicians’ ratings of S UNDBERG , J., P RAME , E., & I WARSSON , J. (1995). Replicability
good versus bad vocal and string performances. Journal of and accuracy of pitch patterns in professional singers.
Research in Music Education, 46, 522-534. Department for Speech, Music, and Hearing – Quarterly
H AILSTONE , J. C., O MAR , R., H ENLEY, S. D., F ROST, C., Progress and Status Report, 36(2-3), 51-62.
K ENWARD, M. G., & WARREN , J. D. (2009). It’s not what you T HOMPSON , S., & W ILLIAMON , A. (2003). Evaluating evaluation:
play, it’s how you play it: Timbre affects perception of emotion Musical performance assessment as a research tool. Music
in music. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(11), Perception, 21, 21-41. doi:10.1525/mp.2003.21.1.21
2141-2155. doi:10.1080/17470210902765957 VAN B ESOUW, R. M., B RERETON , J. S., & H OWARD, D. M. (2008).
H ELMHOLTZ , H. F., & E LLIS , A. J. (1885). The sensations of tone Range of tuning for tones with and without vibrato. Music
as a physiological basis for the theory of music (6th ed.). New Perception, 26, 145-156. doi:10.1525/mp.2008.26.2.145
York: Peter Smith. doi: 10.1037/12740-000 V URMA , A. (2010). Mistuning in two-part singing. Logopedics
H UTCHINS , S., & P ERETZ , I. (2012). A frog in your throat or in Phoniatrics Vocology, 1(1), 24-33.
your ear? Searching for the causes of poor singing. Journal of V URMA , A., & R OSS , J. (2006). Production and perception of
Experimental Psychology: General, 141, 76-97. doi:10.1037/ musical intervals. Music Perception, 23, 331-344. doi:10.1525/
a0025064 mp.2006.23.4.331
H UTCHINS , S., R OQUET, C., & P ERETZ , I. (2012). The vocal V URMA , A., & R OSS , J. (2007). Timbre-induced pitch deviations
generosity effect: How bad can your singing be? Music of musical sounds. Journal of Interdisciplinary Music Studies,
Perception, 30, 147-159. doi:10.1525/mp.2012.30.2.147 1(1), 33-50.
I WAMIYA , S., KOSUGI , K., & K ITAMURA , O. (1983). Perceived WARD, W. D., & M ARTIN , D. W. (1961). Psychophysical com-
principal pitch of vibrato tones. Journal of the Acoustical parison of just tuning and equal temperament in sequences of
Society of Japan (E), 4, 73-82. individual tones. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,
L OOSEN , F. (1995). The effect of musical experience on the con- 33, 586-588.
ception of accurate tuning. Music Perception, 12, 291-306. WARRIER , C. M., & Z ATORRE , R. J. (2002). Influence of tonal
M ADSEN , C. K., E DMONSON , F. A., & M ADSEN , C. H. (1969). context and timbral variation on perception of pitch.
Modulated frequency discrimination in relationship to age and Perception and Psychophysics, 64, 198-207. doi:10.3758/
musical training. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, BF03195786
46(6, Pt. 2), 1468-1472. doi:10.1121/1.1911888 WONG , P. C. M., S KOE , E., R USSO, N. M., D EES , T, & K RAUS , N.
M ADSEN , C. K., & G ERINGER , J. M. (1976). Preference for (2007). Musical experience shapes human brainstem encoding
trumpet tone quality vs. intonation. Council for Research in of linguistic pitch patterns. Nature Neuroscience, 10 (4), 420-
Music Education Bulletin, 46, 13-22. 422.
M ADSEN , C. K., & G ERINGER , J. M. (1981). Discrimination Z ENTNER , M. R., & K AGAN , J. (1998). Infants’ perception of
between tone quality and intonation in unaccompanied flute/ consonance and dissonance in music. Infant Behavior and
oboe duets. Journal of Research in Music Education, 29, Development, 21(3), 483-492. doi:10.1016/S0163-
305-313. doi: 10.2307/3345006 6383(98)90021-2

This content downloaded from


200.144.238.1 on Thu, 17 Jun 2021 12:26:33 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms

You might also like