You are on page 1of 5

154

Examining the Structure of the Trait Meta-Mood Scale


Benjamin Palmer, Gilles Gignac, Timothy Bates and Con Stough
Swinburne University, Australia

Despite the development of numerous measures of emotional intelligence, there is a lack of independent
research substantiating their psychometric properties. In the present paper, exploratory factor analysis
and structural equation modelling were used to assess the factorial and construct validity of the Trait Meta-
Mood Scale (TMMS; Salovey Mayer, Goldman, Turvey, & Palfai, 1995). In a general population sample
(n = 310), the three factors (Attention, Clarity and Repair) were replicated, although support for a fourth
factor was also found. Support was also found for the construct validity of the emotional management
competency assessed by the TMMS. Overall, the findings support the Attention, Clarity and Repair decom-
position of the TMMS. Directions for further validation of the TMMS and the construct of emotional intelli-
gence are discussed.

ince Salovey and Mayer's (1990) conceptualisation of Preliminary psychometric analysis of the TMMS by Salovey
emotional intelligence (EI), the construct has generated et al. (1995) suggests that this scale may provide a reliable and
wide spread interest and attention. Several different models of valid self-report index of the ability to monitor and manage
El now exist (e.g., Bar-On, 1997; Cooper & Sawaf, 1997; emotions. Salovey et al. (1995) report that each of the three
Goleman, 1995; Mayer & Salovey, 1997) and a number subscales of the TMMS measure something coherent and inter-
of assessment measures have been developed (Mayer, Salovey nally consistent (coefficient a = .86, .88, .82 for Attention to
& Caruso, 2000). To-date, several self-report measures and an Feelings, Clarity of Feelings and Mood Repair respectively),
objective measure of EI, for which there are right and wrong and demonstrate evidence of both convergent and discriminant
answers to emotion-related questions based on consensual validity. Attention to Feelings was found to correlate positively
responses, have been developed (e.g., the Bar-On Emotional with the tendency to attend to aspects of ongoing conscious-
Quotient Inventory [Bar-On EQ-i] Bar-On, 1997; the Trait ness as measured by the Private Self-Consciousness and Public
Meta-Mood Scale [TMMS], Salovey, Mayer, Goldman, Self-Consciousness subscales of the Self-Consciousness Scale
Turvey, & Palfai, 1995; Schutte, et al., 1998; the Mayer, (SCS; Scheier & Carver, 1985), r = .42, r = .36 p < .01 respec-
Salovey, Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test, [MSCEIT], tively). Clarity of Feelings was found to correlate negatively
Mayer, Salovey, & Caurso, 1999). Despite these develop- with ambivalence over emotional expression and with depres-
ments, little is known about the measurement of EI (Salovey, sion, r = -. 25, r = .-. 27, p < .05 respectively. Finally,
Bedwell, Detweiler, & Mayer, 2000). Psychometric evalua- Mood Repair was found to negatively correlate with depres-
tions are scarce and relatively few independent studies have sion, and to positively correlate with optimism and beliefs
examined or confirmed the measurement properties reported about the changeability of negative moods as measured by the
by the test authors. In the present paper, we report a replicative CES-D r = .37, p < .01 (Radloff, 1997), the Life Orientation
effort of the factorial and construct validity of the TMMS Test r = .57, p < .01 (LOT; Scheier & Carver, 1985), and
(Salovey et al., 1995). the Expectancies for Negative Mood Regulation r = .53,
The TMMS has been developed from research on reflective p < .01 (NMR; Catanzaro & Mearns, 1990). Collectively,
processes that accompany mood states (e.g., Mayer & these psychometric properties suggest that the TMMS
Gaschke, 1988; Mayer, Salovey, Gomberg-Kaufman, & has potential utility as a measure of the capacity to monitor
Blainey, 1991; Mayer & Stevens, 1994). This research has and manage emotions.
identified an " ... ongoing process associated with moods In order to confirm this utility, further psychometric scrutiny
whereby individuals continually reflect upon their feelings, of the TMMS is needed. The dimensional structure of psycho-
monitoring, evaluating, and regulating them..." termed the logical tests is one of the most rudimentary psychometric
meta-mood experience (Salovey et al., 1995 p. 127). Drawing properties (Kline, 2000). There have been several independent
from this work, the TMMS is comprised of 30 items that psychometric evaluations of the TMMS confirming its re-
define three subscales based on factor analysis by Salovey lationship with ambivalence over emotional expression and
et al. (1995). These are labelled Attention to Feelings (e.g., depression (Lee, & Lee, 1998; Martinez-Pons, 1997). Further-
"I pay a lot of attention to my feelings"); Clarity of Feelings more, independent studies have also found scores on the test
(e.g., "I am usually very clear about my feelings"); and Mood to be related positively to measures of life satisfaction
Repair (e.g., "Although I am sometimes sad, I have a mostly (Martinez-Pons, 1997; Palmer, Donaldson, & Stough, 2002).
optimistic outlook"). As a measure of El. the TMMS assesses However, to-date, Salovey et al. (1995) are the only re-
one of the higher order, more complex, competencies of searchers to have examined the dimensional structure of the
EI proposed by Mayer and Salovey (1997), specifically, the TMMS. Martinez-Pons (1997) proposed a functional sequence
ability to effectively regulate and manage one's emotions. of the emotional management construct. Specifically, that
However, the TMMS measures individuals' perceived ability clarity of feelings (Clarity) would not be possible without
to effectively regulate and manage emotions as opposed attention to feelings (Attention); and that the capacity to repair
to their actual capacity or level of emotional intelligence negative moods and emotions (Repair) would not be possible
(Salovey et al., 2000). unless emotions were experienced clearly (Clarity). Using path

Address for correspondence: Professor Con Stough, Centre for Neuropsychology, School of Biophysical Sciences and Electrical Engineering, Swinburne
University of Technology, PO Box 218 Hawthorn VIC 3122, Australia. Email: cstoughswin.edu.au

I Australian Journal of Psychology Vol. 55, No. 3, 2003 pp. 154- 158 - -
Examining the Structure of the Trait Meta-Mood Scale 155
analytic methodology, Martinez-Pons (1997) found evidence ExploratoryAnalysis
for this proposed functional sequence of the construct assessed To determine the correct number of factors to extract from the
by the TMMS x2 (108) = 1.73, p = ns. However, the three data set, a parallel analysis (Principal Components Analysis)
factors Attention, Clarity and Repair and the functional was performed using the procedure provided by O'Connor
sequence proposed by Martinez-Pons (1997) have not been (2000). There is an emerging consensus in the factor analytic
further examined. Further replica studies are necessary to literature that two decision strategies, Parallel Analysis
establish the factorial validity of EI instruments such as the and the Minimum Average Partial method (i.e., Velicer, 1976),
TMMS. are substantially more accurate than competing algorithms
We performed an exploratory principal components factor (i.e., the Kaiser Criterion and maximum-likelihood based
analysis of the TMMS with a sample taken from the general fit statistics) for determining the correct number of factors
population. We also used structural equation modelling to to extract from a data set (Velicer, Eaton, & Fava, 2000;
assess the functional sequence of the construct the TMMS Zwick & Velicer, 1986). The parallel analysis suggested that
assesses as proposed by Martinez-Pons (1997). It was expected four factors should be extracted as shown in Figure 1.
that the three-factor model and functional sequence would be These four factors accounted for 47.87% of the variance
confirmed by these analyses. The resolution of the three-factor in the data set with factors 1 through 4 accounting for 23.54%,
structure and functional sequence would provide additional 11.71%, 6.64%, and 5.98% of the variance respectively.
To further assess the dimensional structure of the TMMS these
support for factorial validity of the TMMS and the construct
four factors were rotated according to an oblique rotation
validity of the emotional management competency of EI it has
(Direct Oblimin with Kaiser Normalisation). The resulting
been purported to assess.
factor (pattern matrix) loadings are shown in Table 1.
The first three factors of the present data closely replicated
METHOD
the Clarity, Attention and Repair factors of the TMMS
Participants as reported by Salovey et al. (1995). The first factor corre-
The sample comprised 310 participants, (221 females, sponded to the Clarity of Feelings subscale, the highest
86 males, 3 unreported) ranging in age from 15 to 79 years loading item being "I almost always know exactly how I am
with a mean age of 39.42 years (SD = 13.81). The ethnic feeling". The second factor replicated the Attention to Feelings
composition of the sample was relatively representative subscale the highest loading item being "It is usually a waste
of the Australian population with the majority being White/ of time to think about your emotions." The third factor corre-
Caucasian Australians (72.5%), White/Caucasian immigrants sponded to the Mood Repair subscale with the highest loading
(17%), and Asian or of Pacific Islander decent (9.5%). item being "No matter how badly I feel, I try to think about
In comparison to the Australian population, the sample pleasant things". The results of the present study also
was slightly above average in education. Two point one per suggested the existence of a possible fourth factor within the
cent had completed primary school education only, 22.9% had TMMS. Six items loaded significantly on this fourth factor;
completed secondary school education only, 20.5% had however, only two of these items (14, and 9) loaded signifi-
completed a tertiary certificate/diploma, 30.8% had completed cantly on the fourth factor alone. The internal consistency
an undergraduate degree, and 21.2% had completed estimates (Cronbach's alpha) for these four factors were
a postgraduate degree. Participants were recruited for the largely respectable: Clarity (a = .87), Attention ( = .84) and
study via advertisements in local newspapers and received a Mood Repair (a = .71), with the exception of the fourth factor
(ac = .62) not achieving the criterion of > .70 (Kline, 2000).
small stipend for participating.

Structural Equation Modelling


Materials
Given the discrepancy between the present findings and previ-
Trait-Meta Mood Scale (TMMS; Salovey et al., 1995). We
ous dimensional analyses of the TMMS (i.e., four-verses-three
used the 30-item version of the TMMS as recommended by
primary factors), we decided to assess the goodness of fit of a
Salovey et al. (1995). Participants responded on a 5-point scale
where 1 = strongly disagree and 5 = strongly agree. Total and
subscale scores were derived by summing the item responses.

Procedure
Participants self-administered the questionnaire in their own
time and were debriefed afterwards where possible.
E
RESULTS
a
The means (and standard deviations in parentheses) for the a,

Attention, Clarity and Repairs sub scales were 51 (7.9), 42.5 D


a
(7.9), 23.2 (4.3) respectively. The means and standard devia- .c
w2
0
tions of the present sample were very similar to a sample
of United States (US) Air Force recruits, but significantly
higher than a sample of first year psychology students on the PCA

Clarity, M = 35.21 SD(8.65) t(l, 408) = 7.87, p < .05, d = .90, Parallelanalysis
and Repair M = 20.53 SD(5.0) t(l, 408) = 5.18, p < .05 d = .60
sub scales (Davies, Stankov & Roberts 1998). The means Eingenvalue number
and standard deviations of the present sample provide some
of the first Australian general population norms for the
Figure I
TMMS and indicate that there may be differences between
Scree plot of eigenvalues derived from the parallel
sub-populations. and principal component analysis.
Australian Journal of Psychology - December 2003
156 Benjamin Palmer, Gilles Gignac, Timothy Bates and Con Stough

Table I 1998). Also, the RMSEA value of .103 is larger than the
Factor (pattern matrix) Loadings of Principal Components allowable value of .05 (Byrne, 2001). In contrast, all four of
Analysis with Oblimin Rotation for the TMMS the last models assessed demonstrated good fit based on the
Kline adjustment values. Further, the CFI values for these four
Item Factor I Factor 2 Factor 3 Factor 4 models were all comfortably above the recognised critical
value of .95 (Byrne, 2001). Finally, the RMSEA values for all
30 .848
four models approach the critical value of .05 (Byrne, 2001).
25 .820 -.111 Thus, taken as a whole, the general factor model can be
16 .752 .197 rejected outright, in favour of three and/or four factor models.
5 .736 Further, the models based on oblique (correlated) factors
28 .699 .119 represented the data more accurately (as suggested by their
respective fit indices as shown in Table 2) than orthogonal
6 .682 -.122 (uncorrelated) factors.
15 .576 .172 -.117 All of the correlations between the factors were positive and
22 .574 .219 statistically significant as shown in Figure 2. The correlation
20 .434 .335 .227 between Attention and Clarity (r = .39) and Clarity and Repair
(r = .50) are both distinctively larger than the correlation
11 .338 .246 .284 between Attention and Repair (r = .12), suggesting a sequen-
29 .720 .136 tial model of emotional intelligence. To further test this
17 .705 hypothesis, a structural equation model was tested to deter-
23 .184 .655 -.123 -.106 mine if Clarity mediated the relationship between Attention
and Repair as proposed by Martinez-Pons (1997). As shown in
24 .645 -.425
Figure 2, the hypothesis of mediation was supported, with the
21 .174 .629 -.327 correlation of r =.12, p < .05 being reduced to a non-signifi-
18 .581 .116 cant value of r = -.05 ns, replicating the results of Martinez-
2 .560 -.183 .206 Pons (1997). With the path from Attention to Repair omitted,
the fit indices differed from the three oblique factors model
7 .556 .254 -.250
minimally by only decimal points ( = 953.18; Kline Adjust-
27 .553 .273 ment 2.37; CFI = .98; RMSEA = .067).
4 .551
DISCUSSION
10 .544 .209 -.150
The findings of the current study support the Attention, Clarity
3 .457 .242
and Repair subscale structure of the TMMS as reported by
12 .296 .183 -.229 Salovey et al. (1995) in an Australian general population
26 -.135 .798 sample. Evidence for a fourth factor was found; however, only
I .741 two items loaded independently on this fourth factor. Confir-
matory analyses suggested that both three- and four-factor
13 -.125 .707
models provided a good fit with the present data. The discrep-
8 .174 .534 .369 ancy between the three-factor dimensional structure of the
14 .212 .666 TMMS reported by Salovey et al. (1995) and the exploratory
9 .170 .188 .580 four-factor model of the present study may be attributable to
differences in the samples assessed. Subject sample size,
19 .197 .378 .516
variable sample size, and variations in the gender, social
Note: Factor loadings have been sorted ascending and those > .3 1 classes, levels of education and other such sampling parame-
are in bold text. ters can all affect factor pattern reproduction (Velicer & Fava,
1998). Indeed, the dimensional analysis of the TMMS reported
by Salovey et al. (1995) was based on a sample size of 200
range of structural models for the data using confirmatory American individuals as apposed to the 310 Australian partici-
factor analysis (AMOS; Arbuckle, 1999). Five models were pants in the present study. Furthermore, the means of the
tested (a) a General Factor model; (b) an orthogonal three- present sample were found to differ significantly from a
factor model; (c) an oblique three-factor model; (d) an orthog- sample of first year psychology students on the Clarity and
onal four-factor model; and (e) an oblique four-factor model. Repair subscales. This finding highlights potential differences
As can be seen in Table 2, the General Factor model did not in the way sub-populations respond to the TMMS and the need
satisfy the Kline adjustment critical value of less than 3 (Kline, to establish sub-population and cross cultural norms for the

Table 2
Goodness of Fit Indices for the Five Different Models of the TMMS Assessed

General Three Three Four Four


Factor Orthogonal Oblique Orthogonal Oblique
Factors Factors Factors Factors
X2 1727.37 1037.64 952.75 897.01 781.92
Kline adjust 4.27 2.56 2.37 2.25 1.99
CFI .951 .977 .980 .982 .986
RMSEA .103 .070 .067 .064 .057
- ----

Australian Journal of Psychology - December 2003


Examining the Structure of the Trait Meta-Mood Scale I 57

* =p<.05; ** = p<.001

Figure 2
Structural equation model depicting the mediating effect of Clarity on the relationship between Attention and Repair.

scale. Although the present data provides some of the first REFERENCES
general population norms for the TMMS more Australian Arbuckle, J.L (1999). Amos 4.01. Chicago: SmallWaters Corp.
normative data for this scale is needed. Bar-On, R. (1997). Emotional Quotient Inventory: Technical manual.
The findings of the current study also provided support for Toronto: Multi-Health Systems.
the three-phase functional sequence of the emotional manage- Byrne, B. (2001). Structural Equation Modelling with AMOS: Basic
concepts, applications, and programming. Manwah, NJ: Lawrence
ment competency of EI assessed by the TMMS (Martinez- Erlbaum Associates.
Pons, 1997). In the confirmatory analyses, oblique (correlated) Catanzaro, S.J., & Mearns, J. (1990). Measuring generalized expectancies
three and four-factor models provided better fit statistics than for negative mood regulation: Initial scale development and implica-
orthogonal (uncorrelated) factor models. Indeed, the correla- tions. Journalof PersonalityAssessment, 54, 546-563.
tions between the Attention, Clarity and Repair factors Cooper, R.K., & Sawaf, A. (1997). Executive EQ: Emotional intelligence
in leadership and organisations.Grosset/Putnum: New York
assessed in the oblique three-factor model were all positive
Costa, P.T., & McCrae, R.R. (1992). Revised NEO PersonalityInventory
and significant. Furthermore, the relationship between the (NEO-PIR) and NEO Five-FactorInventory professional manual.
Attention and Repair factors was found to be mediated Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
by Clarity. This finding provides support for the notion Davies, M., Stankov, L., & Roberts, R. (1998). Emotional intelligence:
proposed by Martinez-Pons (1997), that clarity of feelings is In search of an elusive construct. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 75, 989-1015.
not possible without attention to feelings, and that emotional
Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional intelligence. New York: Bantam Books.
repair is not possible unless one is clear about the moods and
Kline, R.B. (1998). Principles and practice of structural equations
emotions they are experiencing. modelling. New York: The Guildford Press.
The dimensions assessed by the TMMS, have a strong Kline, P. (2000). A psychometrics primer. London: Free Association
theoretical basis from research on the reflective processes that Books.
accompany mood states, and empirical evidence for Attention, Lee, S.J., & Lee, H.K. (1998). The research on the validation of the Trait
Clarity and Repair as components of emotional monitoring and Meta-Mood Scale: The domain exploration of emotional intelligence.
Korean Journalof Social &PersonalityPsychology, 11, 95-116.
management is mounting. However, future research needs Martinez-Pons, M. (1997). The relation of emotional intelligence with
to replicate the extent to which the TMMS is related to other selected areas of personal functioning. Imagination, Cognition and
measures of mood and emotional management, and to deter- Personality, 17, 3-14.
mine the extent to which it correlates with measures of person- Mayer, J.D., & Gaschke, Y.N. (1988). The experience and meta-experi-
ality such as the NEO PI-R (Costa & McCrae, 1992) and other ence of mood. Journal of personality and social psychology, 55,
102-111.
emotional dispositions (i.e., general affectivity and optimism).
Mayer, J., & Salovey, P. (1997). What is emotional intelligence? In P.
Comparative research examining the value of the TMMS Salovey & D. Sluyter (Eds.), Emotional development and emotional
and other measures of emotional monitoring and management intelligence: Implicationsfor educators (pp. 3-31). New York: Basic-
over and above normal personality and other constructs (stress books, Inc.
tolerance, adaptability, charisma) in predicting theoretically Mayer, J.D., Salovey, P., &Caruso, D.R. (1999). Working Manualfor the
MSCEIT Research Version 1.1. Toronto, Canada: Multi-Health
related life criteria (i.e., life satisfaction, interpersonal success)
Systems.
would also be of much value. A comparison of the TMMS and Mayer, J.D., Salovey, P., & Caruso (2000). Models of emotional
the performance-based or objective, Emotional Management intelligence. In R.J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of intelligence.
sub scales of the MSCEIT (Mayer, Salovey, & Caruso, 1999), (pp. 396-420) New York: Cambridge.
may help delineate whether this aspect of EI is best assessed Mayer, J.D., Salovey, P., Gomberg-Kaufman, S., & Blainey, K. (1991).
A broader conception of mood experience. Journalof Personalityand
with self-report or performance based measures.
Social Psychology, 60, 100-111.
Whether EI is best assessed with performance-based or self- Mayer, J.D., & Stevens, A.A. (1994). An emerging understanding of the
report scales is currently unknown. Indeed little is known reflective (meta-) experience of mood. Journalof Research in Person-
about the measurement of EI and much more psychometric ality, 28, 351-373.
research on existing scales is needed. In particular independent O'Connor, P. (2000). SPSS and SAS programs for determining the
research is required to evaluate claims made by test publishers number of components using parallel analysis and Velicier's MAP
test. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 32(3),
concerning the reliability and validity of EI scales. 396-402.

Australian Journal of Psychology - December 2003


158 Benjamin Palmer, Gilles Gignac, Timothy Bates and Con Stough

Palmer, B.R., Donaldson, K., &Stough, C. (2002). Emotional Intelligence Schutte, N., Malouff, J., Hall, L., Haggerty, D., Cooper, J., Golden, C..
and Life Satisfaction. Personality and Individual Differences, 33, & Dornheim, L. (1998). Development and validation of a measure
1091-1100. of emotional intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences,
Radloff, L.S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for 25, 167-177
research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measure-
Velicer, W.F. (1976). Determining the number of components from
ment, 1, 385-401.
the matrix of partial correlations. Multivariate Behavioural Research,
Salovey, P., Bedell, B.T., Detweiler, J.B., & Mayer, J.D. (2000). Current
12, 3-22.
directions in emotional intelligence research. In M. Lewis & J.M.
Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 504-511). Velicer, W.F., Eaton, C.A., & Fava, J.L. (2000). Construct explication
New York: Guilford Press. through factor or component analysis: A review and evaluation
Salovey, P., & Mayer, J.D. (1990). Emotional intelligence. Imagination, of alternative procedures for determining the number of factors
Cognitionand Personality, 9, 185-21 1. or components. In R.D. Goffen & Helmes, E. (Eds.), Problems
Salovey, P., Mayer, J., Goldman, S., Turvey, C., & Palfai, T. (1995). and solutions in human assessment: Honoring Douglas N. Jackson at
Emotional attention, clarity and repair: Exploring emotional intelli- seventy (pp. 41-71). Norwell, MA, US: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
gence using the Trait Meta-Mood Scale. In J.W. Pennebaker (Ed.),
Velicer, W.F., &Fava, J.L. (1998). Affects of variable and subject sampling
Emotion, disclosure, and health (pp. 125-154) Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association. on factor pattern recovery. PsychologicalMethods, 3. 231-251.
Scheier, M.F., & Carver, C.S. (1985). Optimism, coping, and health: Zwick, W.R., &Velicer, W.F. (1986). Comparison of five rules for deter-
Assessment and implications of generalized outcome expectancies. mining the number of components to retain. Psychological Bulletin,
Health Psychology, 4. 219-247. 99, 432-442

Australian Journal of Psychology - December 2003

You might also like