You are on page 1of 12

Current Zoology 58 (1): 146−157, 2012

As clear as mud: Turbidity induces behavioral changes in


the African cichlid Pseudocrenilabrus multicolor
Suzanne M. GRAY1*, Laura H. McDONNELL1, Fabio G. CINQUEMANI1**,
Lauren J. CHAPMAN1,2
1
Department of Biology, McGill University, 1205 Dr. Penfield Ave, Montreal, QC, Canada, H3A 1B1
2
Wildlife Conservation Society, 2300 Southern Boulevard, Bronx, NY 10460, USA

Abstract Aquatic biodiversity is being lost at an unprecedented rate. One factor driving this loss is increased turbidity, an en-
vironmental stressor that can impose behavioral, morphological, and/or physiological costs on fishes. Here we describe the be-
havioral response of a widespread African cichlid, Pseudocrenilabrus multicolor victoriae, to turbidity. We used a split-brood
rearing design to test if F1 offspring reared in turbid water, originating from river (turbid) and swamp (clear) populations, behave
differently than full-sibs reared in clear water. We examined two facets of behavior: (1) behaviors of fish in full sib groups, in-
cluding activity level and social dynamics collected during the rearing period; and (2) male aggressive behavior directed at poten-
tial male competitors after fish had reached maturity; this was done in an experimental set-up independent of the rearing aquaria.
Regardless of population of origin, fish reared in turbid water were marginally less active and performed fewer social behaviors
than those reared in clear water. On the other hand, when tested against a competitor in turbid water, males performed more ag-
gressive behaviors, regardless of population of origin or rearing environment. Our results suggest a plastic behavioral response to
turbidity that may allow P. multicolor to persist over a range of turbidity levels in nature by decreasing activity and general social
behaviors and intensifying reproductive behaviors to ensure reproductive success [Current Zoology 58 (1): 146–157, 2012].
Keywords Cichlidae, Behavioral plasticity, Environmental stressor, Male-male competition

Increased turbidity is an environmental stressor con- light (Lythgoe, 1979; Utne-Palm, 2002; Seehausen et al.,
tributing to the loss of diversity in aquatic ecosystems 2003). This can disrupt communication signals (re-
globally (Ricciardi and Rassmusen, 1999; Richter et al., viewed in van der Sluijs et al. (2011), which can alter
1997). Turbidity, or suspended particulate matter, comes predator-prey interactions (Abrahams and Kattenfeld,
in two forms: eutrophication or organic turbidity from 1997), breakdown species recognition signals
algal growth (Smith, 2003), and sedimentary turbidity (Seehausen et al., 1997), and alter mate choice and re-
resulting from inorganic inputs such as eroded soil and productive behavior (e.g., Luyten and Liley, 1985; See-
re-suspended silt (reviewed in Donohue and Garcia hausen et al., 1997; Järvenpää and Lindström, 2004;
Molinos (2009)). The effects of increased turbidity on Candolin et al., 2007; Wong et al., 2007; Maan et al.,
fish can be both direct and indirect. Direct effects in- 2010; Sundin et al., 2010). In the endemic hap-
clude mortality resulting from deoxygenation of water lochromine cichlid fishes of Lake Victoria, East Africa,
associated with massive algal blooms (Bruton, 1985), decreased light transmission and narrowing of the light
loss of condition from decreased foraging (Herbert and spectrum due to eutrophication is thought to constrain
Merkens, 1961; Sigler et al., 1984; Barrett et al., 1992), the functional diversity of color signals, leading to a loss
and clogged or damaged gills leading to infection of fish diversity (Seehausen et al., 1997). Furthermore,
(Herbert and Merkens, 1961; Bruton, 1985; Goldes et changes to the visual environment have lead to a reduc-
al., 1988; Sutherland and Meyer, 2007). Indirect effects tion in the strength of sexual selection in several fish
of increased turbidity are largely due to changes in the species, such as threespine sticklebacks Gasterosteus
visual environment, through a reduction in the amount aculeatus (Candolin et al., 2007; Wong et al., 2007), sand
of light available and shifts in the color of underwater gobies Pomatoschistus minutus (Järvenpää and Lind-

Received Aug. 1, 2011; accepted Nov. 1, 2011.


∗ Corresponding author. E-mail: suzanne.gray@mail.mcgill.ca, gray.suzanne@gmail.com
** Current address: Department of Biology, University of Toronto at Mississauga, 3359 Mississauga Road North, Mississauga, ON, Canada,
L5L 1C6
© 2012 Current Zoology
GRAY SM et al.: Turbidity induces behavioral changes in the African cichlid 147

ström, 2004), and Lake Victoria cichlids Pundamilia of reproductive behaviors to ensure reproductive success.
nyererei (Maan et al., 2010). This occurs when changes Pseudocrenilabrus multicolor victoriae (Seegers) is a
in the signaling environment induce behavioral changes wide-ranging cichlid found throughout the Lake Victo-
associated with reproduction, which can ultimately af- ria basin and Nile River drainage in Eastern Africa. It is
fect population viability. found across a broad range of habitats from the clear,
Behavior is often a plastic phenotypic trait and can be dark waters of hypoxic (low dissolved oxygen) swamps
the first mechanism used to cope with environmental to the highly oxygenated, turbid waters of deforested
change and/or stress (West-Eberhard, 2003; Ghalambor rivers (Chapman et al., 1996; Crispo and Chapman,
et al., 2010; Sih et al., 2011), i.e. upon contact with an 2008; Crispo and Chapman, 2010; Crispo and Chapman,
environmental stressor or novel environment, animals 2011). Extensive work in this system, focusing on dis-
can adjust their behaviors immediately to minimize solved oxygen as an environmental stressor, has shown
negative effects of an altered environment. Behavioral that P. multicolor responds both plastically and geneti-
changes in activity level, foraging, and antipredator be- cally to low dissolved oxygen (DO) across its range.
haviors might mitigate impacts of direct physiological There are strong patterns of morphological and physio-
effects of turbidity. On the other hand, increased energy logical variation across populations associated with
expenditure on activities related to reproduction may be dissolved oxygen gradients, such as larger gills and
necessary to offset difficulties in communication caused smaller brains, and lower standard metabolic rates in
by the indirect or direct effects of increased turbidity. females in fish from low oxygen habitats, compared to
For example, threespine stickleback males in the Baltic those in high oxygen environments (Chapman et al.,
Sea experiencing increased eutrophication turbidity 2000; Chapman et al., 2008; Crispo and Chapman, 2010;
spend more time courting than do males in clear water, Reardon and Chapman 2010; Crispo and Chapman,
with no net gain in reproductive success (Candolin et al., 2011). In addition to low dissolved oxygen environ-
2007). It is thought that the decreased ability to see ments, P. multicolor also experiences spatiotemporal
prompts increased courtship behaviors. While experi- gradients of turbidity. In particular, river populations are
mental studies like these have investigated the response subject to variably turbid waters across wet and dry
of adult fish to increased turbidity, common garden seasons, especially where surrounding land has been
rearing studies have yet to be used to explore develop- deforested. For example, in a heavily deforested section
mental response to divergent turbidity environments. of the Mpanga River of western Uganda, turbidity
Social behaviors associated with maintaining hierar- ranged from 3.8 to 25.9 NTU over a one year period
chical social systems and aggressive reproductive be- (Chapman and Gray, unpublished data). Turbidity, like
haviors associated with intrasexual competition (usually dissolved oxygen, is expected to act as a strong selective
between males) in fish can be energetically expensive agent on P. multicolor phenotype.
(Grantner and Taborsky, 1998; Ros et al., 2006; Cum- Like many cichlids, P. multicolor is a highly social
mings and Gelineau-Kattner, 2009). Agonistic interac- species: high aggressiveness increases social rank
tions associated with competition for females are ex- within the local population (Baerends and Baerends-van
pected to be especially costly because of the risk of in- Roon, 1950) and intense male-male aggression is used
jury and use of energy stores (Ros et al., 2006; Briffa to defend territories and attract mates (Fernö, 1986). In
and Sneddon, 2007). In stressful environments, such as low dissolved oxygen environments, P. multicolor
high turbidity, we might expect a decrease in overall shows decreased activity level compared to populations
activity level. For example, in salmonids, indirect ef- from highly oxygenated rivers, and shows behavioral
fects of turbidity include deterioration of social domi- flexibility to compensate for the physiological costs of
nance hierarchies, reduced territoriality, and reduced low oxygen availability (Chapman and McKenzie,
gill-flaring (Berg and Northcote, 1985). However, we 2009). For example, male P. multicolor acclimated to
might also expect to see a trade-off between social low and high DO for five months showed a decreased
group behaviors and reproductive behaviors in stressful number of reproductive displays under low DO, which
environments (e.g., polluted water) (Jones and Reynolds, may reflect the high energy cost of these displays in an
1997): a decrease in activity and general social beha- environment where oxygen acquisition is challenging
viors to mediate the physiological effects of the stressor (Gotanda et al., 2011).
(leading to a weakened hierarchical structure within the We used a common garden rearing design to detect
community; Ang and Manica, 2010) and an intensification effects of population of origin and developmental envi-
148 Current Zoology Vol. 58 No. 1

ronment on the behavioral responses of P. multicolor to removed from the aquarium and isolated so that we
turbidity. We reared F1 offspring of parental stock could ensure he did not fertilize other broods. The
originating from clear (swamp) vs. variably turbid (river) brooding female was also isolated in a separate aqua-
habitats under clear vs. turbid conditions. We looked at rium. This was repeated to produce five sets of known
behavioral response to turbidity in two different con- parents for each population (10 families in total).
texts: (a) in a social group of full sibs in the rearing en- Once the eggs hatched and the fry had absorbed most
vironment, and, (b) in a male-male competition experi- of their yolk sacks (i.e. just prior to exogenous feeding)
ment under both the rearing environment and the recip- they were removed from the mother’s mouth. The brood
rocal environment. was counted and split equally between a turbid-water
aquarium and a clear-water aquarium. This gave us a
1 Materials and Methods total of 20 aquaria (2 populations × 5 families ×2 treat-
1.1 Split-brood rearing experiment ments). All 20 aquaria (15-L each) were located on the
Wild-caught adult fish from natural field populations same shelving unit in our Animal Facility, with broad
were captured at two sites (swamp and river) within the spectrum lighting (CoralLife Aqualight 96 W with 50/50
Mpanga River drainage, western Uganda in June 2008 Power Compact Lamp) over every 5 aquaria. For each
and live-transferred to McGill University. The swamp family, the two aquaria (turbid, clear) were randomly
population, which represents a clear water population, assigned from the aquaria assembly.
was sampled at Kanyantale, while the river population, One week post-separation from the mother, turbidity
which represents a turbid water population, was sam- was incrementally increased daily in the turbid aquaria.
pled from Kamwenge [see Fig. 1 in Crispo and Chapman Turbidity was created using a concentrated Bentonite
(2010) for map]. These two populations are geographi- clay-water solution (5 g clay in 250 ml water) that was
cally isolated (Crispo and Chapman, 2008) and divergent suspended using water pumps and air stones. Each day
with respect to gill size, brain mass, and body shape in for 4 days we added 10 ml of the turbidity solution, for
association with different oxygen regimes (Crispo and a total turbidity solution concentration of 2.7 ml/L. Tur-
Chapman, 2010; Crispo and Chapman, 2011). bidity was measured in all aquaria weekly using a
To produce broods with known parentage, a single LaMotte 2020e turbidimeter and adjusted as necessary
male was placed in a 30-L aquarium with three to five to maintain clear aquaria at < 1.0 NTU (mean ± SE =
females. This ratio of males to females buffered male 0.81 NTU ± 0.01 over 18 months) and turbid aquaria
aggression toward females. As soon as one female was between 6.5 and 9.8 NTU (mean ± SE = 8.7 NTU ± 0.10
found to be holding eggs in her mouth, the male was over 18 months). To prevent the clay from being trapped

Fig. 1 Experimental apparatus for male-male aggression trials (see text for details)
Note that fish photo size is not to scale with experimental set-up.
GRAY SM et al.: Turbidity induces behavioral changes in the African cichlid 149

in filters, these were removed from all pumps (clear and preceding observational trials. A curtain was used to
turbid treatments), which meant that small water separate experimental aquaria from any disturbances in
changes had to be made twice weekly to prevent a the room. After setting up an aquarium for observation
build-up of waste. Basic water quality parameters (dis- (i.e. placing the white board in the aquarium), the ob-
solved oxygen, conductivity, temperature, pH, Ammo- server would leave the curtained area and return after a
nia and Nitrite) were measured weekly to ensure good minimum of 15-min. The observer would then take po-
water quality. sition directly in front of the aquarium and wait mo-
In addition to turbidity, the water was colored to tionless for another 5 min to allow fish to become ac-
mimic the tea-stained nature of the water in Uganda climated to the observer’s presence. Preliminary tests
where the parental stocks were caught. We used the ave- showed that these acclimation times were sufficient for
rage maximum wavelength from measurements in the fish to resume regular behaviors (e.g., social interactions
field with a peak in the medium-long wavelength area resumed).
of the visible spectrum (van der Sluijs and Chapman, A scan technique was used to record activity (either
unpublished) as a guide in developing our tea-stain so- active or not active) of all fish in the aquarium. Activity
lution (12 drops green, 12 drops red, 36 drops yellow was recorded at 10-s intervals for 5 min and averaged.
Club House Food Colour in 500 ml water). We added Due to differences in fish density (across time due to
115 ml of this solution to each aquarium (7.7 ml culling and among aquaria due to the age of the fish),
tea-stain solution/L), and light measurements were we calculated the mean proportion of active fish (i.e.
made periodically throughout the experiment using a mean number of fish active over 5 min, divided by the
S2000 spectrometer (Ocean Optics) to ensure stability total number of fish, averaged over three observations
in water color. per aquarium). Immediately following the 5-min scan,
Brood size ranged from 18 to 48 fish (mean ± SD, 28 the observer performed a 3−5 min focal observation
± 8.5), such that each aquarium initially held between 9 (minimum = 3 min, maximum = 5 min observation time)
and 24 fish (mean ± SD, 14 ± 4.2). For the first six during which individual behaviors (see Table 1) of one
weeks fish were fed fry food (Hikari Tropical First bites) haphazardly selected fish (including both sexes and ju-
twice daily; at eight weeks they were fed a mixture of veniles) were recorded. After the observation period, a
crushed flake food (TetraMinPro Tropical Crisps) and water sample was taken to measure turbidity (see Gray
fry food twice daily; and, at 24 weeks they were fed et al. (2011) for measurement protocol). All observa-
only flake food once per day. Due to the small aquarium tions were conducted between 08:00 and 16:00 h.
size and rapid growth of the fish, broods were reduced The scan and focal observations were repeated three
to 8 fish (at six weeks post-release) and then to 5 fish (at times for each aquarium over a three month period. Us-
14 weeks post-release) per aquarium. Fish removed ing the focal data, we assessed two categories of behav-
from aquaria were euthanized with clove oil (2.0 ml ior of full sib groups reared in divergent turbidity envi-
1:10 clove oil:ethanol solution in 500 ml water). Some ronments: non-aggressive social (tail wagging, displays
fish started to show signs of reproductive maturity after (directed toward any other fish), yawning), and aggres-
nine weeks post-release (males: red anal fin spot; fe- sive (charges, bites) behaviors, and then calculated the
males: eggs in mouth). proportion of each type of behavior relative to all be-
1.2 Activity and social behaviors in full sib groups haviors observed (per min per fish). The individual be-
Behavioral observations that focused on general so- haviors we observed and recorded were performed by
cial activities in full sib groups (activity level, the focal fish, in the context of the family group, mean-
non-aggressive social interactions, aggressive behaviors) ing they were not performed in a reproductive context
began at least nine weeks after the experimental condi- (compared with male aggression behaviors, described
tions started (between October and December 2009) below). The data for each focal observation were aver-
using scan and focal observations on all aquaria. The aged for each aquarium (n = 19). Note that one aquar-
sequence of aquaria to be observed was randomized. ium could not be observed from a straight angle due to
Due to the limited visibility within the turbid aquaria, a its placement on the shelves and so was not included in
white, perforated plastic board was used to restrict fish the analyses.
to the front third of the aquarium during the observation For scan and focal observations, all data were square
period in both clear and turbid aquaria. Fish were ha- root transformed. For the three variables of interest (ac-
bituated to placement of the board daily for two weeks tivity, social, and aggressive behaviors) we used a
150 Current Zoology Vol. 58 No. 1

mixed model ANOVA with treatment and population as white dividers were placed on either side of the central
fixed factors and family as a random factor nested in transparent divider between the experimental compart-
population. In all cases family was non-significant (Pac- ments the night before a trial, and sized-matched males
tivity = 0.305, Psocial = 0.519, Paggressive = 0.138) and so were removed from their aquaria and placed in the
was removed from the models. Because one aquarium compartments (side was randomized). An opaque plastic
could not be observed, the aquarium holding the sib- cover (i.e. bubble wrap) was placed loosely on top of the
lings from that family in the opposite treatment was also compartments to reduce disturbance during the overnight
excluded from the initial analyses that included family, acclimation phase. Males remained in the experimental
but was used for subsequent two-way ANOVAs. All set-up for 24 hours prior the start of the trial. One hour
data were analyzed using SPSS 17.0. before the start of the trial a curtain surrounding the
1.3 Male-male aggression set-up was closed (preventing disturbance), and the ex-
Male aggression trials were performed to explore the perimental area was vacated for 30 minutes. After this
response of individual males to competitors under both period, the observer entered the experimental area quie-
clear and turbid conditions, controlling for rearing con- tly and sat in front of the experimental aquarium.
dition. Each pair was from the same rearing condition Prior to the start of the trial, the section in which each
(clear or turbid) but different population (river or swamp). male was located within its compartment (i.e. distance
Males were size matched to eliminate a confounding away from the central partition) and the presence of any
effect of body size in aggressive interactions. We tested territorial pits in the sand were noted. The plastic covers
each pair under both clear and turbid conditions to assess and dividers were gently removed simultaneously from
if rearing environment influenced the fish’s ability to each side of the transparent wall, marking the start of the
perform in clear or turbid waters (e.g., does a fish reared trial. The behavior of both males was observed for 20
in turbid water respond to a competitor more when the min. Specifically, the occurrence, timing, and location of
water is clear than when it is turbid?). Males were iso- four behaviors (lateral display, charging, quivering, and
lated from other males for at least one week prior to the tail beating; Table 1) were noted when directed at the
trials, although they still had contact with females. These male competitor. The observer also took note of the time
trials were conducted at least one year post-hatching elapsed before the males noticed each other as well as the
(September to December 2010) to ensure that all males section each male was in when this occurred. If the males
were sexually mature and of a similar size. did not notice one another, or if neither male had moved
The experimental aquarium (72 cm × 18 cm × 18 cm) by the 10-min mark, the trial was aborted and attempted
was divided into four compartments, separated by per- at a later date. At the end of the trial, the final behavior
forated and transparent walls, allowing both visual and and location of each male were recorded.
olfactory communication (Fig.1). The two central com- The male in the left compartment was immediately
partments were used to hold experimental males. Each of transferred to a separate container filled with clear water.
these compartments was visually divided into four equal This process was repeated with the male from the right
lateral sections using marks on the front of the tank so compartment. A water sample was obtained from the
that the observer could record the position (i.e. distance) central compartment of the experimental aquarium to
of each fish from the central barrier. Each of the end provide a measure of turbidity for the trial (for both clear
compartments contained a water pump and air stone that and turbid experiments). The experimental aquarium was
ensured suspension of the turbidity solution during turbid drained, filled, and rinsed three times in preparation for
trials. A separate stimulus aquarium, holding a mixture of the second treatment condition (clear vs. turbid). The
females and juveniles, was placed adjacent to the ex- opaque dividers and bubble wrap were replaced, and the
perimental male compartments (see Fig. 1). The re- males were put back into the experimental aquarium in
maining walls of the stimulus and the experimental opposite compartments. They were left in the experi-
aquaria were covered with black plastic to prevent any mental set-up for 24 hours prior to beginning the second
other visual stimuli. Prior to the beginning of all trials, trial for which the same methods but the alternate con-
we randomly determined which treatment would be used dition were used.
for the first trial for each male pair. If the upcoming trial We used a repeated measures ANOVA to test if males
was turbid, 110 ml of turbidity solution was added to the reared under different conditions and from parents
experimental aquarium; if it was clear, it was left as-is; originating from two different populations (river vs.
water was changed between all trials (see below). Solid swamp) behaved differently in clear and turbid water
GRAY SM et al.: Turbidity induces behavioral changes in the African cichlid 151

Table 1 Description of behaviors* observed in tests of response to divergent water turbidity levels in Pseudocrenilabrus multicolor

Behavior Description Experiment1


A quick, forward movement directed towards another fish with mouth opened or closed and without contact
Charge2 1, 2
(specifically toward the competing male in the aggression trials).
A quick forward movement directed towards another fish (the other males in aggression trials) with contact
Bite 1
being made between the attacker’s mouth and the receiver (only observed in social group experiment).
Body horizontal, fins close to body. Tail is quickly beaten back and forth, while fish remains in a steady posi-
Tail Beating 2
tion (directed toward competitors).
Tail Wagging3 A form of tail beating, performed in a non-aggressive interaction. 1
Fish opens mouth wide (as if it were yawning) while the dorsal fin becomes erect (only observed in social
Yawn 1
group experiment).
May be performed at varying degrees of intensity. At highest degree, the dorsal and anal fins are fully erect
Lateral Display while the caudal and pelvic fins are fully spread. Performed while in a perpendicular position to another fish 1, 2
(towards the other male in aggression trials).
Fish approach each other face-to face and open their mouths. Both adversaries try to grip the jaws of the
Frontal Display other. Once a firm grip has been established, they push and pull, beating heavily with their tails and fins (only 1
observed in social group experiment).
Quiver Rapid shaking of the entire body (i.e. not just the tail), generally perpendicular to a competitor or female. 2
* Modified from Baerends and Baerends-van Roon (1950).
1
Similar behaviors were observed in two experiments, but in different contexts noted within the definition. Experiment 1 = social behavior in full sib
groups; Experiment 2 = aggressive behavior in male-male competition.
2
Baerends and Baerends von-Roon (1950) do not use the term charge, but it is implied in their terminology “butting” and “mouth fighting”.
3
Baerends and Baerends von-Roon (1950) use tail wagging for both reproductive and non-reproductive behaviors; here we use it only in the social
context.

when faced with a potential competitor (and in the difference of 0.24 cm (± 0.059 SE) and 0.44 mg (± 0.09
presence of females). Each of the four behaviors ob- SE) between males within pairs. The difference in stan-
served and the total behaviors combined were analyzed dard length and in mass between paired males did not
separately, with trial (clear vs. turbid) as the repeated differ between rearing treatments (standard length: F1, 10
measure, and rearing environment and population of = 0.953, P = 0.352; mass: F1, 10 = 3.756, P = 0.081). All
origin as between-subjects fixed factors. All behavioral males used in the experiment displayed nuptial colora-
data were square root transformed. tion, including a red spot on the anal fin, blue lips, and

2 Results Table 2 The effect of rearing treatment (clear vs. turbid) and
population of origin (river vs. swamp) on the behavior of P. multi-
2.1 Activity and social behaviors in full sib groups
color in full sib groups
Scan data indicated that fish reared in turbid water
F df P-value
and tested in a social environment of full sibs showed
marginally lower activity levels (P = 0.055) than fish a) Activity

reared in clear water (Table 2, Fig. 2A). Population of treatment 4.349 1, 15 0.055
origin (river vs. swamp) did not have an effect on activ- population 1.500 1, 15 0.239
ity levels, and there was no interaction between rearing treatment*population 0.191 1, 15 0.668
treatment and population of origin. Focal data showed b) Social behaviors
that fish reared in turbid water performed significantly
treatment 8.493 1, 15 0.011
fewer non-aggressive social behaviors (tail-wagging,
population 0.001 1, 15 0.981
yawning, and displays) than fish reared in clear water,
regardless of population of origin (Table 2, Fig. 2B). treatment*population 0.538 1, 15 0.474

There was no effect of treatment, population, or their c) Aggressive behaviors


interaction on aggressive behavior (Table 2, Fig. 2C). treatment 1.099 1, 15 0.311
2.2 Male-male aggression population 0.935 1, 15 0.639
Males ranged in size from 4.1 to 6.1 cm standard treatment*population 0.638 1, 15 0.437
length (mean ± SE, 5.15 cm ± 0.10) and 2.55 to 6.99 mg
Data were derived from scan (activity) and focal (social and aggres-
wet mass (mean ± SE, 4.28 mg ± 0.22). Males were sive behaviors) observations, averaged over three independent obser-
size-matched using both measures, with a mean size vations per aquarium.
152 Current Zoology Vol. 58 No. 1

Fig. 2 Proportion (mean ± SE) of active fish (A), social behaviors (B), and aggressive behaviors (C) from river and swamp
origin-populations reared for 9 to 24 weeks in clear or turbid treatment in small full sib groups
Means were calculated per tank from scanning observations. Data were square root transformed.

yellow ventral coloration, indicating sexual maturity against the same male in clear water, regardless of
(Fernö, 1986). Territorial pits in the sand were common population of origin or rearing environment. Since be-
(>75% of trials). The average (± SE) turbidity recorded havior can be a quickly activated reaction to an envi-
for clear trials was 0.47 NTU (0.14) and 5.65 NTU (0.77) ronmental stressor (Timmerman and Chapman, 2004), it
for turbid trials. is not surprising that behavioral response to turbidity is
We assessed the number of each individual type of flexible. Nonetheless, these findings have important
behavior and the total number of aggressive behaviors implications for the ability of fishes to respond to in-
performed by a male when tested against a male in clear creasing levels of human-induced turbidity. The behav-
water and in turbid water. Repeated measures ANOVA ioral plasticity exhibited by P. multicolor may facilitate
indicated a significant effect of experimental condition its persistence over a range of turbidity levels in nature
(clear vs. turbid) on three of the individual behaviors by decreasing both overall activity and general social
(lateral display, tail beats, charge, Fig. 3) and for all behaviors, and by intensifying reproductive behaviors to
behaviors combined (Table 3). The number of quivers ensure reproductive success. It is possible that in a
did not differ between clear and turbid trials and was only group, more aggressive behaviors are used in a social
observed 25 times (out of 471 total behaviors observed, context for establishing dominance hierarchies, while
or 0.005%). In no case did rearing condition or popula- non-aggressive social behaviors are used for maintain-
tion of origin have a significant effect on the number of ing social hierarchies. Since the full sib groups were
behaviors performed. Fish tested in turbid water per- reared together, hierarchies were likely already estab-
formed more aggressive behaviors than when they were lished, and behaviors used only in their maintenance.
tested in clear water, regardless of rearing condition or Thus, turbidity did not influence aggression in that con-
population of origin (Fig. 4). There was no difference text, whereas in the male-male competition trials the
between treatments in the section where males first no- motivation to interact aggressively with a novel com-
ticed their competitor (F1, 20 = 0.711, P = 0.409). petitor would have been increased.
Our behavioral plasticity results support two,
3 Discussion non-mutually exclusive ideas: (1) Turbidity alters the
Results of this study indicate plastic behavioral re- visual environment, and males subsequently increase
sponse to turbidity in P. multicolor, and no significant aggressive behaviors directed at male competitors. This
population effects. Fish reared in turbid water in full sib is consistent with studies on other fish species that have
groups were marginally less active and performed fewer shown increased courtship behaviors in turbid condi-
social behaviors than those reared in clear water, re- tions at a similar level of turbidity as that used in our
gardless of population of origin. On the other hand, study (Candolin et al., 2007; Engström-Öst and Can-
when tested against a competitor in turbid water, males dolin, 2007; Wong et al., 2007). (2) In addition, there
performed more aggressive behaviors than when paired appears to be a trade-off with respect to energy spent on
GRAY SM et al.: Turbidity induces behavioral changes in the African cichlid 153

basic activity and social behaviors and energy spent on groups, including males, females, and juvenile fish.
aggressive male-male interactions. This idea is consis- These types of behavior can be expensive (Grantner and
tent with basic cost-benefit theory for sexually selected Taborsky, 1998). Reducing the amount of social interac-
traits (Candolin and Heuschele, 2008). We explore both tions might weaken hierarchies but may be an adaptive
of these ideas below. mechanism to cope with environmental stress. Activity
The social behaviors that we observed to decrease level was only marginally lower in turbid- vs.
under turbid conditions in small full sib groups of P. clear-reared fish; again, this may reflect energy costs
multicolor were those generally associated with estab- associated with maintenance activity, but the weak re-
lishment and maintenance of social hierarchies in sponse may not be surprising given the low level of

Fig. 3 Number (mean ±SE) of individual aggressive behaviors: tail beats(A), lateral displays (B), charges (C), and quivers
(D) performed by males when paired with a size-matched male under clear (black circles) and turbid (grey circles) condi-
tions
Paired males were from the same rearing condition (clear or turbid) but different population of origin (river or swamp). Population of origin was not
a significant factor in any case and so populations were pooled within rearing treatment in the figures for clarity. Behaviors are defined in Table 1.
Data were square root transformed.
154 Current Zoology Vol. 58 No. 1

Table 3 The effect of trial condition (clear vs. turbid) on indi-


vidual male-male aggressive behaviors in Pseudocrenilabrus mul-
ticolor originating from two populations (swamp vs. river) and
reared under two treatments (clear vs. turbid)
Within-Subjects F df P-value
a) lateral displays
trial 13.14 1, 20 0.002
trial*population 0.316 1, 20 0.580
trial*rearing treatment 0.552 1, 20 0.466
trial*population*rearing treatment 1.949 1, 20 0.178
Between-Subjects
population 1.923 1, 20 0.181
rearing treatment 0.179 1, 20 0.677
population*rearing treatment 1.178 1, 20 0.291
b) tail beats
Within-Subjects
trial 11.14 1, 20 0.003
trial*population 0.227 1, 20 0.639
trial*rearing treatment 1.841 1, 20 0.190
Trial*population*rearing treatment 0.119 1, 20 0.734
Between-Subjects
population 0.747 1, 20 0.398
Fig. 4 Total number (mean ± SE) of aggressive behaviors
rearing treatment 0.878 1, 20 0.360
performed by large adult males under clear (black circles)
population*rearing treatment 0.081 1, 20 0.779
and turbid (grey circles) conditions when paired against a
c) charges
size-matched male from the same rearing condition (clear
Within Subjects
or turbid) but different population of origin (river or
trial 11.23 1, 20 0.003
swamp)
trial*population 0.001 1, 20 0.977 Paired males were allowed to interact through a clear, perforated bar-
trial*rearing treatment 0.827 1, 20 0.374 rier (see Fig. 1). Data were square root transformed.
trial*population*rearing treatment 0.276 1, 20 0.605
Between-Subjects
population 1.101 1, 20 0.306
turbidity used in this study (~ 9 NTU). This level of
rearing treatment 0.191 1, 20 0.667 turbidity is comparable to levels used in other studies
population*rearing treatment 0.120 1, 20 0.733 (e.g., Wong et al., 2009, ~ 3.5 NTU; Engström-Öst and
d) quivers Candolin, 2007, ~ 10-15 NTU; Heuschele and Candolin,
Within-Subjects 2007, ~ 6 NTU); however, it is much lower than values
trial 3.208 1, 20 0.088 reached at field sites where we have captured P. multi-
trial*population 0.019 1, 20 0.891 color (> 26 NTU, Gray and Chapman, unpublished
trial*rearing treatment 0.124 1, 20 0.729 data). In our experimental set-up, such high levels of
trial*population*rearing treatment 6.976 1, 20 0.016 turbidity would have limited our ability to accurately
Between-Subjects
observe mating behaviors. Therefore, our estimates of
population 0.250 1, 20 0.622
behavioral plasticity with respect to activity and social
rearing treatment 2.133 1, 20 0.160
behaviors in small full sib groups may be conservative,
population*rearing treatment 1.483 1, 20 0.237
but clearly support a reduction in these behaviors under
e) total behaviors
Within-Subjects
increased turbidity.
trial 12.02 1, 20 0.002 In contrast to activity and social behaviors in full sib
trial*population 0.030 1, 20 0.865 groups, we found that the level of aggressiveness in
trial*rearing treatment 1.101 1, 20 0.307 males when encountering another male significantly and
trial*population*rearing treatment 0.741 1, 20 0.400 quite dramatically increased under turbid conditions.
Between-Subjects For example, when faced with the same competitor,
population 1.231 1, 20 0.280 males increased their level of aggressive behaviors by
rearing treatment 0.096 1, 20 0.760 77% in turbid water (Fig. 3, 4), independent of popula-
population*rearing treatment 0.178 1, 20 0.678 tion of origin and rearing treatment. Other studies have
GRAY SM et al.: Turbidity induces behavioral changes in the African cichlid 155

shown similar trends of increased courtship displays bidity (i.e. the developmental environment was not as
with high turbidity. For example, Candolin and col- important as current environment with respect to in-
leagues (2007) found that male threespine sticklebacks trasexual competition). However, we did not test activ-
increased their courtship activity in turbid (highly eu- ity and social interactions in the alternate condition to
trophied) water. However, the observed increase in the rearing environment and so do not know if those
courtship activity did not result in the same level of re- behaviors are as flexible as aggressive male-male inter-
sponse from females as when male sticklebacks courted actions. A similar observation to our aggression results
in clear water (Candolin et al., 2007). This suggests that was reported in male guppies Poecilia reticulata that
although male sticklebacks are compensating for re- alter mating tactics under low light conditions. Male
duced visibility in turbid conditions, the behavioral guppies switched from using sigmoidal courtship dis-
change may be maladaptive or inadequate for the level plays to using non-consensual pelvic thrusts in low light,
of the environmental stressor. In this case we might ex- regardless of whether they were reared in low or high
pect alternate sensory cues to be used instead of vis- light environments, suggesting that flexible behavioral
ual-based cues. In fact, this appears to be one way that plasticity is more important in responding to changes in
threespine sticklebacks in the Baltic Sea are dealing the visual environment (Chapman et al., 2009). Such
with increased turbidity: in turbid water females rely flexibility in reproductive behaviors may allow these
more on male olfactory than visual cues to choose a species to persist, even under increasingly hu-
mate (Heuschele et al., 2009). man-induced environmental change (Lane et al., 2011;
Male nuptial coloration is likely an important signal Sih et al., 2011).
for both male-male competition and female mate choice The apparent trade-off we observed with respect to
in P. multicolor. Male P. multicolor generally have in- energy spent on social behaviors in small full sib groups
tense yellow coloration ventrally, blue lips, and a vs. intrasexual reproductive behaviors points to a
prominent red spot on the anal fin, compared with physiological cost of turbidity driven by development
brown-grey coloration in females (see fish photos in Fig. under turbid conditions. Though we have not yet di-
1). Since visual signals are likely impaired by even low rectly explored physiological response to development
levels of turbidity (Utne-Palm, 2002), males may need under turbidity in P. multicolor, we do know that low
to highlight the extent of their color patterns to other DO is a significant driver in shaping population varia-
males (and to females) via increased behavioral signa- tion in phenotype, and that many morpho-physiological
ling. If color signals are masked by turbidity, increased traits show a high level of developmental plasticity (e.g.,
displays may ensure honest signaling of the males’ abili- gill size; Crispo and Chapman (2010)). Across the
ty to deal with the environmental stressor (Wong et al., range of aquatic systems inhabited by P. multicolor, low
2007). DO and low sedimentary turbidity seem to be coupled:
The energetic cost of aggressive/agonistic behaviors hypoxic swamps tend to have clear, tannin-stained water,
in the context of male-male competition can be very whereas well-oxygenated rivers tend to be more turbid.
high (Grantner and Taborsky, 1998; Ros et al., 2006; The combined influence of both stressors likely shapes
Briffa and Sneddon, 2007). In the cichlid Oreochromis population-level phenotypic differences. Future research
mossambicus, aggressive behaviors displayed by adult will examine combined effects of two stressors on the
males to its mirror image increased energy expenditure behavior, morphology, and physiology of this species.
up to five times greater than resting levels (Ros et al.,
2006). Thus, a further increase in such behaviors in the Acknowledgements We thank the Kibale Fish and Monkey
face of an environmental stressor suggests the impor- Project field assistants and the logistical assistance of P. Omeja
tance of the behaviors and that a trade-off in energy and D. Twinomugisha for collection of adult fish in Uganda. I.
expenditure with other behaviors is likely. van der Sluijs, E. Reardon, J. Hunter, L. Grigoryeva, M. Bieber,
and E. Crispo were instrumental in setting-up and maintaining
Population of origin was not a significant driver of
the rearing experiment. Collection and export permits were
altered behavior at any level, suggesting that the diffe-
provided by the Uganda National Council on Science and
rences in behavior we observed are plastic rather than Uganda Wildlife Authority. All procedures were approved by
genetic. In the male-male aggression trials, the results the McGill University Animal Care Committee (protocol
also indicated that the observed behavioral plasticity #5069). Funding was provided by National Science and Engi-
was not a developmental phenotype, but rather a neering Research Council of Canada (NSERC) Postdoctoral
short-term, quickly activated response to increased tur- Fellowship and the American Cichlid Association (SMG),
156 Current Zoology Vol. 58 No. 1

NSERC and Canada Research Chair funds (LJC). in cichlid body shape. Evolutionary Ecology 25: 949–964.
Cummings ME, Gelineau-Kattner R, 2009. The energetic costs of
References
alternative male reproductive strategies in Xiphophorus ni-
Abrahams M, Kattenfeld M, 1997. The role of turbidity as a con- grensis. Journal of Comparative Physiology A - Neuroethology
straint on predator-prey interactions in aquatic environments. Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology 195: 935–946.
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 40: 169–174. Donohue I, Garcia Molinos J, 2009. Impacts of increased sedi-
Ang TZ, Manica A, 2010. Aggression, segregation and stability in ment loads on the ecology of lakes. Biological Reviews 84:
a dominance hierarchy. Proceedings of the Royal Society of 517–531.
London B-Biological Sciences 277: 1337–1343. Engström-Öst J, Candolin U, 2007. Human-induced water turbidity
Baerends GP, Baerends-van Roon JM, 1950. An Introduction to alters selection on sexual displays in sticklebacks. Behavioral
the Study of the Ethology of Cichlid Fishes. Leiden: E.J. Brill. Ecology 18: 393–398.
Barrett JC, Grossman GD, Rosenfeld J, 1992. Turbidity-induced Fernö A, 1986. The aggressive behavior between territorial pseu-
changes in reactive distance of rainbow trout. Transactions of docrenilabrus multicolor (Pisces, Cichlidae) with unlimited or
the American Fisheries Society 121: 437–443. only visual contact. Behavioural Processes 13: 353–366.
Berg L, Northcote TG, 1985. Changes in territorial, gill-flaring, Ghalambor CK, Angeloni LM, Carroll SP, 2010. Behavior as
and feeding behaviour in juvenile coho salmon Oncorhynchus phenotypic plasticity. In: Westneat DF, Fox CW ed. Evolution-
kisutch following short-term pulses of suspended sediment. ary Behavioral Ecology. Oxford: Oxford University Press,
Canadian Journal of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences 42: 90–107.
1410–1417. Goldes SA, Ferguson HW, Moccia RD, Daoust PY, 1988. Histo-
Briffa M, Sneddon LU, 2007. Physiological constraints on contest logical effects of the inert suspended clay kaolin on the gills of
behaviour. Functional Ecology 21: 627–637. juvenile rainbow trout Salmo gairneri Richardson. Journal of
Bruton MN, 1985. The effects of suspensoids on fish. Hydrobi- Fish Diseases 11: 23–33.
ologia 125: 221–241. Gotanda KM, Reardon EE, Chapman LJ, 2011. Hypoxia and male
Candolin U, Heuschele J, 2008. Is sexual selection beneficial behaviour in an African cichlid Pseudocrenilabrus multicolor
during adaptation to environmental change? Trends in Ecology victoriae. Journal of Fish Biology 78: 2085–2092.
and Evolution 23: 446–452. Grantner A, Taborsky M, 1998. The metabolic rates associated
Candolin U, Salesto T, Evers M, 2007. Changed environmental with resting, and with the performance of agonistic, submis-
conditions weaken sexual selection in sticklebacks. Evolution- sive and digging behaviours in the cichlid fish Neolam-
ary Biology 20: 233–239. prologus pulcher (Pisces: Cichlidae). Journal of Comparative
Chapman BB, Morrell LJ, Krause J, 2009. Plasticity in male Physiology B-Biochemical Systemic and Environmental
courtship behaviour as a function of light intensity in guppies. Physiology 168: 427–433.
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 63: 1757–1763. Gray SM, Sabbah S, Hawryshyn CW, 2011. Experimentally in-
Chapman LJ, Albert J, Galis F, 2008. Developmental plasticity, creased turbidity causes behavioural shifts in Lake Malawi
genetic differentiation, and hypoxia-induced trade-offs in an cichlids. Ecology of Freshwater Fish OnLineFirst
African Cichlid Fish. The Open Evolution Journal 2: 75–88. Herbert WM, Merkens JC, 1961. The effect of suspended mineral
Chapman LJ, Chapman CA, Chandler M, 1996. Wetland ecotones solids on the survival of trout. International Journal of Air and
as refugia for endangered fishes. Biological Conservation 78: Water Pollution 5: 56–74.
263–270. Heuschele J, Candolin U, 2007. An increase in pH boosts olfac-
Chapman LJ, Galis F, Shinn J, 2000. Phenotypic plasticity and the tory communication in sticklebacks. Biology Letters 3:
possible role of genetic assimilation: Hypoxia-induced 411–413.
trade-offs in the morphological traits of an African cichlid. Heuschele J, Mannerla M, Gienapp P, Candolin U, 2009. Envi-
Ecology Letters 3: 387–393. ronment-dependent use of mate choice cues in sticklebacks.
Chapman LJ, McKenzie DJ, 2009. Behavioural responses and Behavioral Ecology 20: 1223–1227.
ecological consequences. In: Richards JG, Farrel AP, Brauner Järvenpää M, Lindström K, 2004. Water turbidity by algal blooms
CJ ed. Hypoxia in Fishes. San Diego: Elsevier, 26–77. causes mating system breakdown in a shallow-water fish, the
Crispo E, Chapman LJ, 2008. Population genetic structure across sand goby Pomatoschistus minutus. Proceedings of the Royal
dissolved oxygen regimes in an African cichlid fish. Molecular Society of London B 271: 2361–2365.
Ecology 17: 2134–2148. Jones JC, Reynolds JD, 1997. Effects of pollution on reproductive
Crispo E, Chapman LJ, 2010. Geographic variation in phenotypic behaviour of fishes. Reviews in Fish Biology and Fisheries 7:
plasticity in response to dissolved oxygen in an African cichlid 463–491.
fish. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 23: 2091–2103. Lane JE, Forrest MNK, Willis CKR, 2011. Anthropogenic influ-
Crispo E, Chapman LJ, 2011. Hypoxia drives plastic divergence ences on natural animal mating systems. Animal Behaviour 81:
GRAY SM et al.: Turbidity induces behavioral changes in the African cichlid 157

909–917. Transactions of the American Fisheries Society 113: 142–150.


Luyten PH, Liley NR, 1985. Geographic variation in the sexual Sih A, Ferrari MCO, Harris DJ, 2011. Evolution and behavioural
behavior of the guppy Poecilia reticulata (Peters). Behaviour responses to human-induced rapid environmental change.
95: 164–179. Evolutionary Applications 4: 367–387.
Lythgoe JN, 1979. The Ecology of Vision. Oxford: Clarendon Smith VH, 2003. Eutrophication of freshwater and coastal marine
Press. ecosystems: A global problem. Environmental Science and
Maan ME, Seehausen O, van Alphen JJM, 2010. Female mating Pollution Research 10: 126–139
preferences and male coloration covary with water transpa- Sundin J, Berglund A, Rosenqvist G, 2010. Turbidity hampers
rency in a Lake Victoria cichlid fish. Biological Journal of the mate choice in a pipefish. Ethology 116: 713–721.
Linnean Society 99: 398–406. Sutherland AB, Meyer JL, 2007. Effects of increased suspended
Reardon EE, Chapman LJ, 2010. Hypoxia and energetics of sediment on growth rate and gill condition of two southern
mouth brooding: Is parental care a costly affair? Comparative Appalachian minnows. Environmental Biology of Fishes 80:
Biochemistry and Physiology A - Molecular and Integrative 389–403.
Physiology 156: 400–406. Timmerman CM, Chapman LJ, 2004. Behavioral and physiologi-
Ricciardi A, Rassmusen JB, 1999. Extinction rates of North Ameri- cal compensation for chronic hypoxia in the sailfin molly
can freshwater fauna. Conservation Biology 13: 1220–1222. Poecilia latipinna. Physiological and Biochemical Zoology 77:
Richter BD, Braun DP, Mendelson MA, Master LL, 1997. Threats 601–610.
to imperiled freshwater fauna. Conservation Biology 11: Utne-Palm AC, 2002. Visual feeding of fish in a turbid environ-
1081–1093. ment: Physical and behavioural aspects. Marine and Freshwa-
Ros AFH, Becker K, Oliveira RF, 2006. Aggressive behaviour and ter Behaviour and Physiology 35: 111–128.
energy metabolism in a cichlid fish Oreochromis mossambicus. van der Sluijs I, Gray SM, Amorim MCP, Barber I, Candolin U et
Physiology & Behavior 89: 164–170. al., 2011. Communication in troubled waters: The evolutionary
Seehausen O, van Alphen JJM, Witte F, 1997. Cichlid fish diver- implications of changing environments on fish communication
sity threatened by eutrophication that curbs sexual selection. systems. Evolutionary Ecology 25: 623–640.
Science 277: 1808–1811. West-Eberhard MJ, 2003. Developmental Plasticity and Evolution.
Seehausen O, Van Alphen JJM, Witte F, 2003. Implications of Oxford: Oxford University Press.
eutrophication for fish vision, behavioural ecology, and species Wong BBM, Candolin U, Lindström K, 2007. Environmental
coexistence. In: Crisman TL, Chapman LJ, Chapman CA, deterioration compromises socially enforced signals of male
Kaufman L ed. Conservation, Ecology and Management of quality in three-spined sticklebacks. American Naturalist 170:
African Freshwaters. Gainesville: University Press of Florida, 184–189.
268–287. Wong BBM, Järvenpää M, Lindström K, 2009. Risk-sensitive
Sigler JW, Bjornn TC, Everest FH, 1984. Effects of chronic tur- mating decisions in a visually compromised environment. Bi-
bidity on density and growth of steelheads and coho salmon. ology Letters 5: 600–602.

You might also like