You are on page 1of 5

Personality and Individual Differences 90 (2016) 169–173

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences

journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

Groups of perfectionists, test anxiety, and pre-exam coping in


Argentine students
Fernán G. Arana a,⁎, Luis Furlan b
a
Universidad de Buenos Aires, Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas, Argentina
b
Universidad Nacional de Córdoba, Argentina

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: We addressed Gaudreau's claim (2012) relating to scrutiny the 2 × 2 model with narrow measures of perfection-
Received 19 August 2015 ism. Accordingly, we tested the model not only with another specific measure of perfectionism (the Almost Per-
Received in revised form 27 October 2015 fect Scale-Revised; Slaney, Rice, Mobley, Trippi, & Ashby, 2001) but also with a group-based approach, and with
Accepted 2 November 2015
specific outcome measures in an Argentine sample of university students. 277 participants completed measures
Available online xxxx
of perfectionism, test anxiety, and pre-exam coping. We concluded that our evidence does not support the purely
Keywords:
self-critical subtype as more pathological than the other subtypes. Regarding correlational results, test anxiety
Groups of perfectionists was strongly linked to the negative dimension of perfectionism (discrepancy), while problem-orientation coping
Test anxiety seemed to be associated with the positive dimension of perfectionism (high standards). From a group-based ap-
Pre-exam coping proach, maladaptive perfectionists tended to suffer more test anxiety than adaptive perfectionists, whereas this
Self critical group latter group did not do better than non-perfectionists. With respect to pre-exam coping, perfectionists were
Latin Americans prone to select more problem-oriented strategies regardless of their levels of discrepancy. Finally, although
this study was not focused on cross-cultural differences, findings on Latin Americans students could serve as a
starting point to promote new research in this field.
© 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction Otto, 2006). On the other hand, the person-oriented or group-based ap-
proach seeks to ascertain individual differences on dimensions of per-
Despite the fact that several measures of perfectionism have been fectionism (Lundh, Saboonchi, & Wangby, 2008). Group-based studies
developed from distinct theoretical bases (Flett & Hewitt, 2002; Frost, have consistently supported the notion that two subtypes of perfectionism
Marten, Lahart, & Rosenblate, 1990; Rice & Slaney, 2002), researchers can be empirically identified. In brief, maladaptive perfectionists (MP) refer
agree that this construct has two core aspects. The positive dimension to individuals who hold high standards but feel like failures if their goals are
of perfectionism comprises the possession of high standards while the not perfectly met. Conversely, those individuals who hold high standards
negative dimension of perfectionism involves an excessive concern but have a flexible assessment of their achievements tend to be considered
about falling short of these standards (Rice & Slaney, 2002). Thus, per- adaptive perfectionists (AP) (Rice & Ashby, 2007). Finally, non-
fectionism can be understood as a personality trait characterized by an perfectionists (NP) remain as a residual subtype for people who report
interaction between these positive and negative features. low standards. As a logical complement, research on subtypes of perfection-
ism has mirrored most findings of correlational approaches on measures of
both psychological distress and well-being (Stoeber & Otto, 2006).
1.1. Correlational and group-based approaches

1.2. The 2 × 2 model and the purely self-critical subtype


From a methodological perspective, there are two major approaches
in studying perfectionism. The correlational approach seeks to find
As Smith, Saklofske, Yan, and Sherry (2015) highlighted, the number
whether perfectionism is related to psychological constructs across peo-
and characterization of subtypes of perfectionism are the subjects of
ple. Consistent with this, results of early studies have provided support
vigorous debate. Added to the classical distinction among subtypes
for the link between evaluative concerns and negative psychological
(Rice & Slaney, 2002), Gaudreau and Thompson (2010) suggested a
outcomes (Shafran & Mansell, 2001). The positive dimension of perfec-
new model of perfectionism (the 2 × 2 model) resulting from the inter-
tionism, in turn, has received mixed results from research (Stoeber &
actions between both dimensions of this construct. These authors pro-
⁎ Corresponding author at: Facultad de Psicología, Universidad de Buenos Aires,
posed a new subtype of perfectionists arising from the combination of
Hipólito Yrigoyen 3242, C1207ABR Buenos Aires, Argentina. low standards but high evaluative concerns. They hypothesized that
E-mail address: fernanarana@psi.uba.ar (F.G. Arana). purely self-critical perfectionists (SC) would get more negative results

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.11.001
0191-8869/© 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
170 F.G. Arana, L. Furlan / Personality and Individual Differences 90 (2016) 169–173

compared to other subtypes, given that lower levels of standards do not a group-based approach as its essence lies, as we understand it, in the dis-
ameliorate the relationship between evaluative concerns and negative out- tinction among individuals into different subtypes of perfectionism. Third,
comes (Gaudreau, 2012). The debate about the relevance of this fourth sub- we proposed two specific dependent variables. We were interested in
type has barely begun, although there is previous work that points to the testing how academic outcomes (test anxiety and pre-exam coping) dif-
need for studies that can confirm this (Alden, Ryder, & Mellings, 2002). fered among subtypes of perfectionists in students. Fourth, we addressed
Rice, Ashby, and Gilman (2011), for instance, indicated that perhaps this the call for cross-cultural research in the perfectionism field (Mobley,
group should not be considered perfectionist since subjects do not describe Slaney, & Rice, 2005). Although we have not made any statement about
themselves as having standards of excellence. However, several studies how Argentine students might differ from their American counterparts,
have reported that this subtype should be taken into account (Boone, this study can serve as an exploratory work for the study of perfectionism
Soenens, Braet, & Goossens, 2010; Wang, Slaney, & Rice, 2007). Following in Latin Americans.
this line, if there are differences between the SC group and the other sub- Hence, our aim is to see whether there are differences among sub-
types (MP, AP, and NP), therapeutic work with these clients should be types of perfectionism aimed at a greater dysfunction for SC in dimen-
adapted. Actually, SC possibly gain less benefit from current treatments of sions of test anxiety and pre-test coping. Specifically, we hypothesized
perfectionism (Lloyd, Schmidt, Khondoker, & Tchanturia, 2015). that SC have the highest levels of test anxiety among subtypes, and AP
have less test anxiety than MP. Furthermore, since no conclusive studies
1.3. Perfectionism, test anxiety, and pre-exam coping among college stu- have previously been undertaken before, we do not propose any hy-
dents: general and local findings pothesis about how groups of perfectionists differ among dimensions
of pre-exam coping. Nevertheless, we expected to find greater dysfunc-
While perfectionism has been investigated in various contexts, the uni- tional coping for SC following 2 × 2 model's theoretical claims.
versity environment is one of the most relevant (e.g., Suddarth & Slaney,
2001). Perfectionists excessively value their personal achievements, so it 2. Method
would be assumed that testing situations might result in significant distress.
Following this, test anxiety can be conceptualized as a disposition to react 2.1. Participants
with increased anxiety in the face of situations that are related
performance-related contexts (Hodapp, Glanzmann, & Laux, 1995). Accord- The sample consisted of 277 students from two public middle-
ingly, several studies showed that test anxiety was related to perfectionism region universities of Argentina: Capital Federal (n = 100) and Córdoba
in students (e.g., Bieling, Israeli, Smith, & Antony, 2003). For instance, find- (n = 175) (2 missing), 81% female, with a mean age of 23.91 (SD =
ings from a correlational approach yielded that negative perfectionism was 4.76). There were no differences across samples on the main variables.
strongly associated with test anxiety (Stoeber, Feast, & Hayward, 2009). The ethnicity of participants was Latin American. In addition, this
Other authors have found that this relationship occurred more sharply in fe- study was approved by an ethical committee for each university.
male students (Besharat, 2003; Eum & Rice, 2011). Surprisingly, we did not
find any study linking perfectionism and test anxiety from a group-based 2.2. Instruments
approach, nor from the SC group. In fact, we found only one study (Eum
& Rice, 2011) that uses the Almost Perfect Scale-Revised (APS-R, Slaney, Almost Perfect Scale-Revised (APS-R; Slaney et al., 2001; Argentine
Rice, Mobley, Trippi, & Ashby, 2001), which is specifically designed to mea- version: Arana et al., 2009). This scale consists of 23 items taping three
sure perfectionism on college samples (Enns & Cox, 2002). subscales: High Standards (α = .75, 7 items related to the predilection
Added to these scarce findings, the contribution to this field from to impose high standards), Order (α = .74, with 4 items related to inter-
Latin American studies was also limited. At this point, we can only men- est and order and neatness) and Discrepancy (α = .91, with 12 items re-
tion the psychometric adaptation of multidimensional perfectionism lated to the degree to which respondents perceive the distance between
(Arana, Keegan, & Rutsztein, 2009) and multidimensional test anxiety desired and obtained goals). We exclude the Order subscale since it has
(Heredia, Piemontesi, Furlan & Hodapp, 2008) scales in Argentina. no predictive contribution to perfectionism construct (Rice & Ashby,
Finally, another relevant issue for further study in the context of perfec- 2007). Reliability and factor structure of the Argentine version yielded
tionists students is how they cope before exams. Many studies have offered similar results to the original scale (Arana et al., 2009). As we used only
support for the notion that MP engage more frequently in dysfunctional this instrument to measure perfectionism, and for clarity of description,
ways of coping while AP often employ a more functional strategy (Burns we consider from now on that discrepancy taps the negative dimension
& Fedewa, 2005; Flett, Russo, & Hewitt, 1994). Flett, Hewitt, Blankstein, of perfectionism, whereas high standards tap the positive dimension of
Solnik, and Van Brunschot (1996), for example, found that MP use negative perfectionism.
problem-solving strategies compared to AP. Apparently, the latter tend to German Test Anxiety Inventory (GTAI-A; Hodapp, 1991; Argentine
use more flexible coping resources (Karmakar & Ray, 2014; Larijani & version: Heredia,Piemontesi, Furlan, & Hodapp, 2008). This is a self-
Besharat, 2010). Furthermore, Noble, Ashby, and Gnilka (2014) encoun- report measure of 28 items (α = .90) which assess dimensions of anxiety
tered coping as a mediator between perfectionism and depression. Howev- about exams: Worry (α = .87, with 9 items related to thoughts about
er, to the best of our knowledge, there is no previous work on how negative consequences of poor performance), Interference (α = .74,
perfectionists deal with specific coping strategies before exams. with 5 items related to cognitions that interfere in testing situation), Emo-
tionality (α = .88, with 8 items related to perceptions of physiological
1.4. This study arousal), and Lack of confidence (α = .86, with 6 items about trust on
one's performance and self-control during the exam situation).
Gaudreau (2012) encouraged researchers to scrutinize its 2 × 2 Coping with Pre-Exam Anxiety and Uncertainty (COPEAU-A, Stöber,
model of perfectionism using not only broad (e.g., composite measures) 2004; Argentine version: Heredia, Piemontesi, Furlan, & Pérez, 2008). It
but also narrow measures of perfectionism (e.g., first-order facets). There- comprised Task-orientation and preparation (α = .87, refers to strate-
fore, the purpose of this study was to test the 2 × 2 model against four dif- gies that aim to address the situation of stress by anticipating, studying,
ferent conditions. First, we established that the APS-R (Slaney et al., 2001) and planning), Seeking social support (α = .85, refers to strategies that
had not already been used to test the model, despite this scale is designed aim to seek emotional and instrumental support from others to solve
to assess maladaptive and adaptive perfectionism. Moreover, this scale is situations of stress), and Avoidance (α = .70, refers to activities that re-
conceptually unconnected with psychopathological biases (Enns & Cox, duce discomfort by distraction, mental and behavioral distancing).
2002). Second, although the 2 × 2 model explored interactions under All alphas provided belong to Argentine versions of these scales and
regression-based models, it becomes clear that this must be mirrored by were in the range of reliabilities from original studies. Moreover, all
F.G. Arana, L. Furlan / Personality and Individual Differences 90 (2016) 169–173 171

factorial solutions arrived on Argentine samples support the same struc- (F (12) = 11.33), p b .000] with a power of 1. Groups of perfectionists
tures from original studies. explained 40% of the variance of dimensions of test anxiety. Tukey B's
post hoc comparisons identified significantly higher values for MP and
2.3. Procedure SC in all dimensions, except for worry, compared with NP and AP
groups. In relation to the dimension of worry, variations were more sub-
Students completed instruments at class-time. We required partici- tle: The highest scores were for SC and MP, and significantly lower for
pants to sign an informed consent. The statistical strategy involved cor- NP. However, AP differed from MP but not for SC (see Table 2 top). In
relations between variables and, secondly, two separated multivariate other words, the NP group was the only unaffected by test anxiety di-
analysis of variance (MANOVA) to test group differences for each di- mension of worry. Moreover, SC did not differentiate from MP on any
mension of test anxiety and pre-exam coping respectively. Subtypes of dimensions of test anxiety.
perfectionists were construed with the aid of clinical cut-off points de- With respect to pre-exam coping, the MANOVA (using dimensions
rived from a previous cluster analysis on Argentine students (Arana, of pre-exam coping as dependent variables and subtypes of perfection-
2012). ism as a factor) also revealed a significant multivariate effect [Wilks'
λ = .87, (F (9) = 3.51), p b .000)], with a power of .96. Subtypes of per-
3. Results fectionism explained 12% of pre-exam coping variance. Further, there
were no differences among subtypes, except for problem-orientation,
3.1. Descriptive statistics and correlations between variables which was significantly higher for perfectionists groups compared to
NP and SC (see Table 2 below).
Descriptive data are presented in Table 1. Reliability ranged from .57
(GTAI's Interference) to .91 (GTAI's Lack of trust), mirroring Argentine 4. Discussion
versions of the instruments but only within a slightly less degree.
After Bonferroni's correction (.05/100) we found that correlations The purpose of this study was to elucidate the relationships between
were in the line of previous research. In this sense, the dimensions of perfectionism, test anxiety, and pre-exam coping to provide additional
test anxiety were all associated with each other (in a range of .35 and support for the 2 × 2 model of perfectionism. The link between test anx-
.67) and also related to discrepancy. Standards, in turn, presented a iety and perfectionism yielded results in line with previous studies
less strong positive correlation with emotionality and worry. (Arana, 2012; Eum & Rice, 2011). Discrepancy was associated with all
For pre-exam coping, we found no association with discrepancy, dimensions of test anxiety, as demonstrated in other works from differ-
whereas standards were linked with problem-orientation (seek support ent (Mills & Blankstein, 2000; Stoeber et al., 2009) or similar (Eum &
became insignificant after correction). Rice, 2011) conceptualizations and measures of perfectionism.
On the other hand, standards were related to dimensions of worry
3.2. Subtypes of perfectionists and comparisons with test anxiety and and emotionality from test anxiety. Stoeber et al. (2009) found a similar
pre-exam coping result with worry and highlighted the ambivalent role of having high
standards. However, it is reasonable to expect that holding of this trait
Following cut-off points, participants who scored 36 or above in contributes to the activation of worry because assessment situations
standards were considered perfectionists. To discriminate between AP may trigger the desire to achieve excellence and, therefore, anticipatory
and MP, a score of 41 or above in discrepancy qualified for the latter thoughts of possible setbacks may play an adaptive function. Processing
group. Scores below this value ranked for the first one. SC was formed efficiency theory (Eysenck & Gutierrez-Calvo, 1992), in fact, explains
with those participants who scored below 36 on standards, but above why some anxious individuals may do well by appealing to auxiliary
40 on discrepancy. The remaining subtype, NP, was obtained from cognitive resources that reduce the efficiency of the learning process
lower scores from both subscales. According to this classification, the but maintain its effectiveness. We can argue that to a certain extent ac-
frequencies for each group were 31% (MP), 24% (NP), 23% (AP), and tivation of worry is mandatory when perfectionists face exams. More-
22% (SC), respectively. over, as Eum and Rice (2011, p. 168) stated, “the association between
Regarding group differences, the first MANOVA (using dimensions perfectionism, test anxiety, and performance appears to rest on the de-
of test anxiety as dependent variables and subtypes of perfectionism gree to which perfectionistic striving could be considered as adaptive or
as a factor) revealed a significant multivariate effect [Wilks' λ = .59, maladaptive.” On the contrary, it became evident that discrepancy,

Table 1
Descriptive statistics and correlations among test-anxiety, pre-exam coping, and perfectionism.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 M (SD) α

Pre-exam coping
1. Problem-oriented .26⁎⁎⁎ −.05 −.04 .27⁎⁎⁎ −.26⁎⁎⁎ .17⁎ .11 .43⁎⁎⁎ −.05 30.12 (6.48) .85
2. Seek support .20⁎⁎ .14⁎ .31⁎⁎⁎ .12⁎ .33⁎⁎⁎ .31⁎⁎⁎ .20⁎⁎ .06 22.88 (7.42) .85
3. Avoidance −.08 .03 .15⁎ −.07 −.01 .03 .09 19.94 (5.43) .76

Test anxiety
4. Lack of confidence .45⁎⁎⁎ .40⁎⁎⁎ .40⁎⁎⁎ .67⁎⁎⁎ −.09 .63⁎⁎⁎ 15.11 (4.23) .91
5. Worry .35⁎⁎⁎ .67⁎⁎⁎ .86⁎⁎⁎ .30⁎⁎⁎ .44⁎⁎⁎ 31.21 (7.31) .88
6. Interference .38⁎⁎⁎ .62⁎⁎⁎ −.09 .38⁎⁎⁎ 10.77 (4.11) .57
7. Emotionality .86⁎⁎⁎ .22⁎⁎⁎ .39⁎⁎⁎ 23.30 (7.35) .86
8. Total GTAI .16⁎ .57⁎⁎⁎ 79.92 (18.29) .90

Perfectionism
9. High standards .13⁎ 36.00 (5.78) .67
10. Discrepancy 43.07 (13.9) .90
⁎⁎⁎ p b .0005 (After Bonferroni's correction).
⁎⁎ p b .001.
⁎ p b .01.
172 F.G. Arana, L. Furlan / Personality and Individual Differences 90 (2016) 169–173

Table 2
Comparison of subtypes of perfectionism, test anxiety, and pre-exam coping.

NP (n = 60) AP (n = 56) MP (n = 76) SC (n = 53)


Test anxiety F df Partial η2
M SD M SD M SD M SD

Lack of confidence 13.18a (3.07) 12.30a (2.61) 16.93b (4.04) 17.91b (3.83) 36.62 3, 241 .31
Worry 26.97a (7.05) 30.11b (6.44) 34.63c (6.93) 32.26bc (5.98) 15.77 3, 241 .16
Interference 8.87a (3.20) 9.54a (3.40) 11.62b (4.09) 12.58b (4.76) 11.59 3, 241 .13
Emotionality 19.70a (7.08) 21.30a (6.35) 26.24b (6.52) 25.26b (6.49) 14.15 3, 241 .15

NP (n = 56) AP (n = 57) MP (n = 79) SC (n = 50)


Pre-exam coping F df Partial η2
M SD M SD M SD M SD

Problem-oriented 28.50a (5.78) 31.51b (6.25) 31.90b (5.79) 27.54a (6.78) 7.42 3, 234 .09
Seek support 21.46 (7.52) 23.32 (6.59) 24.78 (7.14) 22.16 (7.51) 2.67 3, 234 .03
Avoidant 18.25 (4.74) 20.51 (5.35) 19.62 (6.01) 20.50 (5.30) 2.08 3, 234 .03

Note. All p-values derived from F-tests were significant at p b .0001 level, excepting Seek support (p = .047) and Avoidant (p = .026).

which is explained as a sort of severe criticism of own performance group really should be considered as a separate group. While previous
(Slaney et al., 2001), promotes test anxiety in its various dimensions, studies supported the relevance of this group as being more pathologi-
being particularly marked on dimensions of lack of confidence and in- cal than others (Gaudreau, 2012; Gaudreau & Thompson, 2010; Wang
terference. These dimensions, unlike worry, do not encourage behaviors et al., 2007), this differentiation has not been found in our study. In
that help to achieve adequate performance during test situations. this regard, as Rice et al. (2011) suggested, it would make no sense to
From a group-based approach, we observed that students with mal- separate this group from non-perfectionists. The question of whether
adaptive perfectionism or purely self-critical (in which levels of discrep- individuals should be considered purely self-critical perfectionist re-
ancy are high), obtained higher levels of test anxiety compared to AP or mains open for now. One possible future line of research could be to
NP (where discrepancy levels are lower). This result would lead to the link this group with students whose score is low on test anxiety, but
conclusion that discrepancy is more determinant in explaining the var- who also have low self-efficacy (McKeachie, Lin, & Middleton, 2004;
iability in test anxiety than holding standards of excellence itself. In Meijer, 2001). As Meijer (2001, p. 142) argued, “truly low anxious stu-
turn, the lack of differentiation between NP and AP, adds a new contri- dents are likely to be high in self-efficacy; while high-anxious students
bution to the debate about whether it is functional to have an adaptive generally report low self-efficacy.” The SC group resembles Meijer's de-
perfectionism (Stoeber & Otto, 2006). scription in terms of sharing a common high discrepancy process, so this
With respect to pre-exam coping, the influence of perfectionism was could provide a promising starting point for identifying these people
much lower. None of the pre-exam coping dimensions were related to and further developing specific interventions. In brief, our results sug-
negative perfectionism. However, it was found that standards were gest that from a group-based approach and using a narrow measure of
linked to problem-orientation. In this sense, our results suggest that perfectionism (APS-R), the SC subtype failed to distinguish from the
the possession of high standards promotes a task-oriented attitude, MP subtype in Argentine students.
and this phenomenon occurs relatively independently of the degree of With respect to cross-cultural conclusions, since we made no claims
discrepancy. From a group-based perspective, perfectionists appear to about differences, the only hypothesis we can suggest is that it appears
invest greater efforts in achieving their academic goals, as could be that distinctions in perfectionism across samples of students might not
expected from previous studies (Blankstein, Dunkley, & Wilson, represent a broader picture of real between-cultures differences.
2008; Miquelon, Vallerand, Grouzet, & Cardinal, 2005) compared to Finally, besides common inherent limitations in this kind of studies
NP. When feelings of uncertainty emerge in pre-exam situations, (e.g., intentional sampling, cross-sectional design), this work has two
perfectionists may opt for actions related to improving their prepa- limitations itself that should be mentioned. First, the classification
ration, compared to groups whose standards are not high (NP and given to represent the SC group was conducted focusing on clinical
SC). Nevertheless, this study was unable to identify differences cut-off points derived from a previous cluster analysis (Arana, 2012),
among subtypes of perfectionism in coping strategies at pre-exam whereas there are other classifications such as latent classes profiles
situations, as other works had done on assessing coping in general (Rice, Lopez, & Richardson, 2013) or discrimination functions (Rice &
(Karmakar & Ray, 2014; Larijani & Besharat, 2010; Noble et al., Ashby, 2007). However, as our classification was not based on
2014). Accordingly, it looks like either adaptive or maladaptive, per- median-split procedures, the dichotomization of continuous variables
fectionists do not engage in dysfunctional strategies to cope with should not be of concern (DeCoster, Iselin, & Gallucci, 2009). However,
pre-exam uncertainty. Again, this must be fueled by the fact that it would be desirable to see if this four-group structure could be empir-
pre-exam situation refers in perfectionist students to a high valued ically replicated with other samples of students as well as another type
opportunity to achieve inner academic standards and, therefore, of classification. Second, besides theoretical expectations, the use of a
the coping strategy chosen would naturally be problem-oriented college sample may hide the real functioning of the SC subtype, which
rather than an avoidant or excessively emotional one. is expected to be overrepresented in clinical samples (Alden et al.,
Finally, regarding Gaudreau's (2012) claim, the purely self-critical 2002; Lundh et al., 2008).
group did not differ from MP as to greater test anxiety neither in general
nor for its dimensions. The lack of differentiation may be because dis-
crepancy explains most of the variability in test anxiety, regardless of Acknowledgments
standards.
As for the pre-exam coping strategies, the SC group also failed to dif- The present work was partially funded with a post-doctoral fellow-
ferentiate themselves from their non-perfectionist peers concerning ship of the National Council of Scientific and Technical Research on be-
problem-orientation. This is an expected result given the link between half the first author.
this dimension and possession of high standards. Still, lack of variation The authors want to thank Alba Rosa Ubalton and Judy Staton for
in other coping dimensions leads to the question of whether this their help on revising English translation.
F.G. Arana, L. Furlan / Personality and Individual Differences 90 (2016) 169–173 173

References Karmakar, R., & Ray, A. (2014). Adaptive and maladaptive perfectionists: Do they really
differ on hardiness and using coping strategy? International Journal of Innovative
Alden, L.E., Ryder, A.G., & Mellings, T.M.B. (2002). Perfectionism in the context of social Research & Development, 3, 380–386.
fears: Toward a two-component model. In G.L. Flett, & P.L. Hewitt (Eds.), Perfectionism: Larijani, R., & Besharat, M.A. (2010). Perfectionism and coping styles with stress. Procedia,
Theory, research, and treatment. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. 5, 623–627.
Arana, F. G. (2012). Perfeccionismo en estudiantes universitarios Argentinos. Unpublished Lloyd, S., Schmidt, U., Khondoker, M., & Tchanturia, K. (2015). Can psychological interven-
doctoral thesis. Universidad de Buenos Aires, Buenos Aires, Argentina. tions reduce perfectionism? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Behavioural and
Arana, F., Keegan, E., & Rutsztein, G. (2009). Adaptación de una medida multidimensional Cognitive Psychotherapy, 43(6), 705–731.
de perfeccionismo: La Almost Perfect Scale-Revised (APS-R). Un estudio preliminar Lundh, L.G., Saboonchi, F., & Wangby, M. (2008). The role of personal standards in clinical-
sobre sus propiedades psicométricas en una muestra de estudiantes argentinos. ly significant perfectionism. A person-oriented approach to the study of patterns of
Evaluar, 9, 35–53. perfectionism. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32, 333–350.
Besharat, M.A. (2003). Parental perfectionism and children's test anxiety. Psychological McKeachie, W., Lin, Y., & Middleton, M.J. (2004). Two types of low test-anxious (low
Report, 93(3 Pt 2), 1049–1055. worry) students. Counseling and Clinical Psychology Journal, 1, 141–152.
Bieling, P.J., Israeli, A.L., Smith, J., & Antony, M.M. (2003). Making the grade: The behavior- Meijer, J. (2001). Learning potential and anxious tendency: Test anxiety as a bias factor in
al consequences of perfectionism in the classroom. Personality and Individual educational testing. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 14, 339–362.
Differences, 35, 163–178. Mills, J.S., & Blankstein, K.R. (2000). Perfectionism, intrinsic versus extrinsic motivation,
Blankstein, K.R., Dunkley, D.M., & Wilson, J. (2008). Evaluative concerns and personal and motivated strategies for learning: A multidimensional analysis of university stu-
standards perfectionism : Self-esteem as a mediator and moderator of relations dents. Personality and Individual Differences, 29, 1191–1204.
with personal and academic needs and estimated GPA. Current Psychology, 27, 29–61. Miquelon, P., Vallerand, R.J., Grouzet, F.M.E., & Cardinal, G. (2005). Perfectionism, academ-
Boone, L., Soenens, B., Braet, C., & Goossens, L. (2010). An empirical typology of perfec- ic motivation, and psychological adjustment: An integrative model. Personality and
tionism in early-to-mid adolescents and its relation with eating disorder symptoms. Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(7), 913–924.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48, 686–691. Mobley, M., Slaney, R.B., & Rice, K.G. (2005). Cultural validity of the Almost Perfect Scale-
Burns, L., & Fedewa, B. (2005). Cognitive styles: Links with perfectionistic thinking. Revised for African American college students. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 52,
Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 103–113. 629–639.
DeCoster, J., Iselin, A.R., & Gallucci, M. (2009). A conceptual and empirical examination of Noble, C.L., Ashby, J.S., & Gnilka, P.B. (2014). Multidimensional perfectionism, coping, and
justifications for dichotomization. Psychological Methods, 14, 349–366. depression: Differential prediction of depression symptoms by perfectionism type.
Enns, M.W., & Cox, B.J. (2002). The nature and assessment of perfectionism: A critical Journal of College Counseling, 17, 80–94.
analysis. In G.L. Flett, & P.L. Hewitt (Eds.), Perfectionism: Theory, research, and treat- Rice, K.G., & Ashby, J.S. (2007). An efficient method for classifying perfectionists. Journal of
ment (pp. 33–62). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Counseling Psychology, 54, 72–85.
Eum, K., & Rice, K.G. (2011). Test anxiety, perfectionism, goal orientation, and academic Rice, K.G., & Slaney, R.B. (2002). Clusters of perfectionists: Two studies of emotional ad-
performance. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 24, 167–178. justment and academic achievement. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and
Eysenck, M.W., & Gutierrez-Calvo, M. (1992). Anxiety and performance: The processing Development, 35, 35–47.
efficiency theory. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 409–434. Rice, K.G., Ashby, J.S., & Gilman, R. (2011). Classifying adolescent perfectionists.
Flett, G.L., & Hewitt, P.L. (2002). Perfectionism and maladjustment: An overview of theo- Psychological Assessment, 23, 563–577.
retical, definitional, and treatment issues. In G.L. Flett, & P.L. Hewitt (Eds.), Perfection- Rice, K.G., Lopez, F.G., & Richardson, C.M.E. (2013). Perfectionism and performance among
ism: Theory, research, and treatment (pp. 5–13). Washington, DC: American STEM students. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 82, 124–134.
Psychological Association. Shafran, R., & Mansell, W. (2001). Perfectionism and psychopathology: A review of re-
Flett, G.L., Russo, F.A., & Hewitt, P.L. (1994). Dimensions of perfectionism and constructive search and treatment. Clinical Psychology Review, 21, 876–906.
thinking as a coping response. Journal of Rational-Emotive and Cognitive-Behavior Slaney, R.B., Rice, K.G., Mobley, M., Trippi, J., & Ashby, J.S. (2001). The revised Almost Per-
Therapy, 12, 163–179. fect Scale. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 34, 130–145.
Flett, G.L., Hewitt, P.L., Blankstein, K.R., Solnik, M., & Van Brunschot, M. (1996). Perfection- Smith, M.M., Saklofske, D.H., Yan, G., & Sherry, S.B. (2015). Perfectionistic concerns inter-
ism, social problem-solving ability, and psychological distress. Journal of Rational- act to predict negative emotionality: Support for the tripartite model of perfectionism
Emotive and Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 14, 245–275. in Canadian and Chinese university students. Personality and Individual Differences, 81,
Frost, R.O., Marten, P., Lahart, C., & Rosenblate, R. (1990). The dimensions of perfection- 141–147.
ism. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14, 449–468. Stöber, J. (2004). Dimensions of test anxiety: Relations to ways of coping with pre-exam
Gaudreau, P. (2012). A methodological note on the interactive and main effects of dualis- anxiety and uncertainty. Anxiety, Stress, & Coping, 17, 213–226.
tic personality dimensions: An example using the 2 × 2 model of perfectionism. Stoeber, J., & Otto, K. (2006). Positive conceptions of perfectionism: Approaches, evidence,
Personality and Individual Differences, 52, 26–31. challenges. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10(4), 295–319.
Gaudreau, P., & Thompson, A. (2010). Testing a 2 × 2 model of dispositional perfection- Stoeber, J., Feast, A.R., & Hayward, J.A. (2009). Self-oriented perfectionism and socially
ism. Personality and Individual Differences, 48, 532–537. prescribed perfectionism: Differential relationships with intrinsic and extrinsic moti-
Heredia, D., Piemontesi, S., Furlan, L., & Hodapp, V. (2008). Adaptación del inventario vation and test anxiety. Personality and Individual Differences, 47(5), 423–428.
Alemán de ansiedad frente a los exámenes (GTAI-A). Evaluar, 8, 46–60. Suddarth, B.H., & Slaney, R.B. (2001). An investigation of the dimensions of perfectionism
Heredia, D., Piemontesi, S., Furlan, L., & Pérez, E. (2008). Adaptación de una escala de in college students. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 34,
afrontamiento de la ansiedad e incertidumbre pre-examen para estudiantes 157–165.
universitarios argentinos. Avaliação psicológica, 7(1), 1–9. Wang, K., Slaney, R., & Rice, K. (2007). Perfectionism in Chinese university students from
Hodapp, V. (1991). Das Prüfungsängstlichkeitsinventar TAI-G: Eine erweiterte und Taiwan: A study of psychological well-being and achievement motivation. Personality
modifizierte Version mit vier Komponenten. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, and Individual Differences, 42(7), 1279–1290.
5, 121–130.
Hodapp, V., Glanzmann, P.G., & Laux, L. (1995). Theory and measurement of test anxiety
as a situation specific trait. In C.D. Spielberger, & P.R. Vagg (Eds.), Test anxiety. Theory,
assessment, and treatment (pp. 47–58). London: Taylor & Francis.

You might also like