You are on page 1of 3

Current Biology

Dispatches

or whether these observations were These studies promise to provide insights 5. Lieber, T., Jeedigunta, S.P., Palozzi, J.M.,
Lehmann, R., and Hurd, T.R. (2019).
merely correlated. The new work now into both evolution and mitochondrial Mitochondrial fragmentation drives selective
sheds light on this question by diseases. removal of deleterious mtDNA in the germline.
demonstrating that reduced availability of Nature 570, 380–384.
POLG itself is sufficient to inhibit selfish
mtDNA proliferation (Figure 1). REFERENCES 6. Chiang, A.C.-Y., McCartney, E., O’Farrell, P.H.,
and Ma, H. (2019). A genome-wide screen
These findings showcase the exciting reveals that reducing mitochondrial DNA
discovery that the availability of a vital 1. Ma, H., and O’Farrell, P.H. (2016). Selfish drive polymerase can promote elimination of
can trump function when animal mitochondrial deleterious mitochondrial mutations. Curr. Biol.
resource needed for mtDNA replication
genomes compete. Nat. Genet. 48, 798–802. 29, 4330–4336.
not only determines overall mtDNA copy
number within a cell, but also the relative 2. Gorman, G.S., Schaefer, A.M., Ng, Y., Gomez,
N., Blakely, E.L., Alston, C.L., Feeney, C., 7. Zhang, Y., Chen, Y., Gucek, M., and Xu, H.
fitness of different mtDNA variants. (2016). The mitochondrial outer membrane
Horvath, R., Yu-Wai-Man, P., Chinnery, P.F.,
Moreover, given that the expression of the et al. (2015). Prevalence of nuclear and protein MDI promotes local protein synthesis
mitochondrial DNA mutations related to adult and mtDNA replication. EMBO J. 35, 1045–
mtDNA polymerase gene was previously
mitochondrial disease. Ann. Neurol. 77, 1057.
shown to be important for the proliferation 753–759.
of a selfish mtDNA in nematodes [9], these
8. Zhang, Y., Wang, Z.H., Liu, Y., Chen, Y., Sun, N.,
findings raise the interesting possibility 3. Fan, W., Waymire, K.G., Narula, N., Li, P., Gucek, M., Zhang, F., and Xu, H. (2019). PINK1
Rocher, C., Coskun, P.E., Vannan, M.A., Narula, inhibits local protein synthesis to limit
that the underlying mechanism is widely J., Macgregor, G.R., and Wallace, D.C. (2008). A transmission of deleterious mitochondrial DNA
conserved between distantly related mouse model of mitochondrial disease reveals mutations. Mol. Cell 73, 1127–1137.
species. As the field of mitochondrial germline selection against severe mtDNA
mutations. Science 319, 958–962.
genetics moves forward, it will likely prove 9. Lin, Y.F., Schulz, A.M., Pellegrino, M.W., Lu, Y.,
illuminating to more fully explore the ways 4. Stewart, J.B., Freyer, C., Elson, J.L., Shaham, S., and Haynes, C.M. (2016).
in which the availability of vital resources Wredenberg, A., Cansu, Z., Trifunovic, A., and Maintenance and propagation of a deleterious
Larsson, N.G. (2008). Strong purifying selection mitochondrial genome by the mitochondrial
can be modified to alter the in transmission of mammalian mitochondrial unfolded protein response. Nature 533,
competitiveness of selfish genomes. DNA. PLoS Biol. 6, e10. 416–419.

Auditory Perception: A Rhythm Reflecting the Past


Benedikt Zoefel
MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
Correspondence: benedikt.zoefel@mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2019.10.061

Previous research has demonstrated that auditory perception fluctuates rhythmically after a cue. New
research shows that these ‘behavioural oscillations’ critically depend on expectations from preceding
stimulation.

The human brain is an active constructor which resources might be used for other processing come from
of its own world [1]. It structures input into functions (such as exploration or electrophysiological or
meaningful units or objects, stores and interpretation) [2,3]. A new study by Ho neurophysiological correlates of visual
recalls relevant information, explores its et al. [4], reported in this issue of Current perception. Here, a behavioural variable
environment, and interprets incoming Biology, provides important evidence that of interest is typically correlated with a
stimuli based on previous experience. these oscillations indeed represent an rhythmic neural process (brain
This leads to a complex pattern of neural active, brain-initiated sampling oscillations). It has, for instance, been
activation, which needs to be coordinated mechanism, driven by expectations about shown that the probability of detecting a
to avoid interference and increase upcoming input. These results not only visual target co-varies with a 7-Hz rhythm
efficiency of the brain’s operation. It has indicate that oscillations structure measured over frontal
been proposed that brain oscillations are information flow in the brain, but that they electroencephalography (EEG) sensors in
such a coordinator of brain activity, can be adjusted to when and where they humans [5]. But arguably the strongest
rhythmically alternating between a are predicted to be useful. evidence for the functional relevance of
prioritization of sensory processing, and Most of our insights into oscillation- these oscillations comes from measuring
phases of reduced stimulus uptake during based mechanisms of stimulus them in the output of a system that is

R1318 Current Biology 29, R1301–R1325, December 16, 2019 ª 2019 Elsevier Ltd.
Current Biology

Dispatches

A Target B C

Correctly identified targets


Congruent (right) Congruent (left) Congruent (left)
More
More
left

Response
Preceded by targets in same ear (congruent) Incongruent (left)

Preceded by targets in other ear (incongruent)


Congruent (right)
Incongruent More
Fewer
right
Incongruent (right)

Cue-target interval Cue-target interval


Current Biology

Figure 1. Behavioural oscillations depend on preceding input.


Schematic illustration of the paradigm used by Ho et al. [4], and interpretation of their results. (A) In each trial, a binaural noise cue is followed by a target which is
presented monaurally at various delays. This target produces rhythmic fluctuations in neural sensitivity in subsequent trials, but only in circuits responsible for the
location of that target. This oscillatory process is therefore only visible in congruent (blue), but not incongruent trials (orange). (B) Participants’ accuracy — the
probability of correctly identifying target location — reflects this sampling rhythm, out-of-phase between the two ears. (C) Participants’ tendency to respond (‘left’
versus ‘right’) is biased by stimulation history, with an additional rhythmic component in trials in which their bias is confirmed. In panels B and C, ‘left’ and ‘right’
refer to the target location in preceding trials.

assumed to rely on such oscillations (cf. or criterion) fluctuated at a frequency of excitability) changes rhythmically, but not
[6]). And indeed, several recent studies 9 Hz, but only in congruent trials. In in those tuned to the unexpected target
have revealed rhythmicity in behavior addition, accuracy (defined as correctly location (Figure 1A). This pattern is
itself. An approach common to these reporting target location) did not peak for mirrored in both the number of correctly
studies is the presentation of a cue, both ears at the same time, but at near- identified target locations (Figure 1B) and
assumed to reset oscillations, and to test opposite phases of the 9-Hz rhythm, in line the tendency to respond (Figure 1C), with
perception at various delays after this with a previous study [10]. an oscillation in behaviour only for
cue, thereby sampling the hypothesized This finding is remarkable in several congruent trials. Stimulation history did
behavioural oscillation. Landau and Fries respects. First, it demonstrates that the not improve participants’ overall ability to
[7] demonstrated that visual perception observed oscillation is not only a simple discriminate target locations, suggesting
oscillates after such a cue, with an overall resonance phenomenon, reacting that these location-specific, rhythmic
sampling rhythm of 8 Hz, and other passively to a brief impulse, and without boosts in neural sensitivity result in
studies have confirmed this finding in obvious functional role. Instead, the cue changes in subjective perception, not
related paradigms [8,9]. In 2017, Ho and seems to trigger a process that necessarily in improvements on a
colleagues [10] reported similar effects in rhythmically samples a location that is decisional level (cf. [11]).
the auditory system, a much less predicted based on recent experience. What do these results mean for our
investigated domain in terms of oscillatory Second, as illustrated in Figure 1, this understanding of brain oscillations in the
processes. They showed that the behavioural study gives us important auditory system? In contrast to the visual
perception of a monaural target, insights into underlying neural system, audition does not seem to
presented after the onset of binaural mechanisms, to a degree that might have operate rhythmically all the time [12]. This
noise, fluctuates rhythmically at a been challenging to achieve in brain has been explained by the fast-fluctuating
frequency of 6–8 Hz. imaging data. The results revealed that nature of auditory information, making
With an elegant design, the new study participants rhythmically alternate in their audition particularly vulnerable to
[4] builds on, and critically advances, the tendency to report the location of the information loss during the phase of
described previous work. Human target (‘left’ versus ‘right’), but only in trials reduced sensory processing [13]. This
participants were again cued with binaural in which the target was presented at the loss of information might be reduced if the
noise (Figure 1A). They were then asked to predicted location. Importantly, oscillatory ‘window of opportunity’ for
identify the ear of origin of a subsequent predictions were identical in congruent sensory uptake is aligned with predicted
monaural target, presented at various and incongruent trials, with the only important events [12,14]. Brain
delays after noise onset (which remained difference that they were confirmed in the oscillations need to be resettable for this
present throughout the trial). Importantly, former and violated in the latter. If this alignment to be achieved [15]. Ho et al. [4]
targets could occur in either the same ear oscillation exclusively reflected the might have tapped into such a
as in the preceding trials (congruent trial) participants’ prediction, it should mechanism, with oscillations reset by the
or in the other ear (incongruent trial). The therefore also be visible in incongruent cue, ‘waiting’ for subsequent rhythmic
authors then tested whether the trials — which was not the case. events to occur at the expected location.
hypothesized oscillation in performance is Therefore, the effect must originate from Such events are ubiquitous in the auditory
modulated by the congruency of the circuits that specifically process the environment [16], and the auditory system
trial — by participants’ prediction of target expected location of the target (or might be specialized to adjust to them [12].
location. Interestingly, the tendency to ‘acoustic space’, as the authors suggest). The importance of a phase-reset for
respond ‘left’ or ‘right’ (the response bias, In these circuits, neural sensitivity (or oscillations to operate in the auditory

Current Biology 29, R1301–R1325, December 16, 2019 R1319


Current Biology

Dispatches

system, how long they linger if no rhythmic 4. Ho, H.T., Burr, D.C., Alais, D., and Morrone, frequencies separately for the two ears. Curr.
M.C. (2019). Auditory perceptual history is Biol. 27, 3643–3649.
input follows, and other experimental propagated through alpha oscillations. Curr.
parameters that might have been critical Biol. 29, 4208–4217. 11. Iemi, L., and Busch, N.A. (2018). Moment-to-
for the observed results (such as binaural moment fluctuations in neuronal excitability
5. Busch, N.A., Dubois, J., and VanRullen, R. bias subjective perception rather than
noise) remain unknown, opening up (2009). The phase of ongoing EEG oscillations strategic decision-making. eNeuro 5, https://
exciting questions for future studies. predicts visual perception. J. Neurosci. 29, doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0430-17.2018.
7869–7876.
These studies also need to clarify how 12. Zoefel, B., and VanRullen, R. (2017).
much the observed alternation between 6. Krakauer, J.W., Ghazanfar, A.A., Gomez- Oscillatory mechanisms of stimulus
ears (Figure 1B) [10] depends on the Marin, A., MacIver, M.A., and Poeppel, D. processing and selection in the visual and
(2017). Neuroscience needs behavior: auditory systems: state-of-the-art,
specific experimental protocol. correcting a reductionist bias. Neuron 93, speculations and suggestions. Front.
Participants knew that targets would 480–490. Neurosci. 11, https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.
2017.00296.
either appear in one ear or the other (but 7. Landau, A.N., and Fries, P. (2012). Attention
not in both). This is unlikely to happen in a samples stimuli rhythmically. Curr. Biol. 22, 13. VanRullen, R., Zoefel, B., and Ilhan, B. (2014).
more natural scenario, in which sounds in 1000–1004. On the cyclic nature of perception in vision
versus audition. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B
the two ears often differ in their intensity 8. Song, K., Meng, M., Chen, L., Zhou, K., and 369, 20130214.
(inter-aural level difference), but only Luo, H. (2014). Behavioral oscillations
in attention: rhythmic a pulses 14. Schroeder, C.E., and Lakatos, P. (2009). Low-
marginally in their timing (inter-aural time mediated through q band. J. Neurosci. 34, frequency neuronal oscillations as instruments
difference). It needs to be tested whether a 4837–4844. of sensory selection. Trends Neurosci. 32,
similar sampling mechanism, alternating 9–18.
9. Fiebelkorn, I.C., Saalmann, Y.B., and Kastner,
between the two ears, exists during S. (2013). Rhythmic sampling within and 15. Lakatos, P., Gross, J., and Thut, G. (2019). A
binaural input. Modelling studies might be between objects despite sustained new unifying account of the roles of neuronal
attention at a cued location. Curr. Biol. 23, entrainment. Curr. Biol. 29, R890–R905.
able to reveal whether such a mechanism 2553–2558.
is advantageous as compared to an in- 16. Ding, N., Patel, A.D., Chen, L., Butler, H., Luo,
10. Ho, H.T., Leung, J., Burr, D.C., Alais, D., and C., and Poeppel, D. (2017). Temporal
phase sampling of both ears. Morrone, M.C. (2017). Auditory sensitivity and modulations in speech and music. Neurosci.
What exactly happens during phases of decision criteria oscillate at different Biobehav. Rev. 81, 181–187.
reduced sensory processing, when brain
resources might be needed for other
purposes? Where in the brain are different
phases of the oscillatory cycle
coordinated, and adjusted to both internal
and external events? Although the answer
to these questions is largely unknown, it
has been suggested that phases of higher Microbial Ecology: How to Fight the
visual sensitivity alternate with phases of
active exploration (for example, via Establishment
saccades), controlled by fronto-parietal
networks [3]. In the auditory system, Katherine M. Moccia and Sarah L. Lebeis*
equivalent phases of exploration, and the Department of Microbiology, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN 48996, USA
conductor of the oscillatory symphony, *Correspondence: slebeis@utk.edu
remain to be identified. Fully https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2019.10.067
understanding the oscillatory cycle and
the underlying neural circuits might be the Creating microbial consortia capable of consistently producing desired
key to understand how the brain can qualities requires a detailed understanding of community interactions.
coordinate and optimize a complex A new paper demonstrates the role of historical contingency in
pattern of interacting neural processes
Arabidopsis thaliana leaf-microbiome formation using an adaptable
with such an apparent ease.
experimental approach, which could be applied to other host
organisms.
REFERENCES

1. Buzsaki, G. (2019). The Brain from Inside Out


(Oxford, New York: Oxford University Press).
Over the last decade, emerging Arabidopsis thaliana is uniquely suited as
sequencing technologies have been used a model in which to dissect the
2. Wutz, A., Muschter, E., van Koningsbruggen, with great success to reveal the microbial determinants of microbiome formation
M.G., Weisz, N., and Melcher, D. (2016).
Temporal integration windows in neural components of diverse hosts and due to the extensive availability of host
processing and perception aligned to environments. To predictably recreate genetic resources and the high
saccadic eye movements. Curr. Biol. 26,
1659–1668.
and harness microbial communities, culturability of its microbiota in both
however, it is critical not only to identify above- and below-ground tissues — the
3. Fiebelkorn, I.C., and Kastner, S. (2019). A the players involved, but also to define the phyllosphere and rhizosphere,
rhythmic theory of attention. Trends Cogn. Sci.
23, 87–101. rules of community assembly. The plant respectively [1,2]. Constructing synthetic

R1320 Current Biology 29, R1301–R1325, December 16, 2019 ª 2019 Elsevier Ltd.

You might also like