You are on page 1of 30

CHAPTER TWENTY-SIX

intelligence and Personality


MOSHE ZEIDNER AND GERALD MATTHEWS

OVERVIEW We begin by comparing two approaches to under-


standing the personality-intelligence interface. The
For more than half a century now psychologists
first is the traditional psychometric approach based
have been exploring the avenues linking human in-
on intercorrelating dimensions of individual differ-
telligence to a wide array of personality traits in
ences as identified largely by factor analysis. We
the hope of unravelling the mutual impact of in-
then survey empirical research on the direction
telligence and personality in the course of devel-
and magnitude of the relationship between intel-
opment and day-to-day behavior. Although these
ligence and personality constructs. The second ap-
constructs are frequently kept apart, the artificial-
proach is the cognitive science one that emphasizes
ity of the distinction has been recognized by many
process rather than structure. We review the bio-
of the key figures of differential psychology, in-
logical, cognitive, and adaptive processes that may
cluding Binet, Terman, Thorndike, and Thurstone.
contribute to both personality and intelligence. We
Most notably, David Wechsler (1950) viewed intel-
conclude by pointing out some potential directions
ligence as a manifestation of personality as a whole
for future research on the intelligence-personality
and argued that certain affective and motivational
interface.
factors (interest, volition, etc.) are integral compo-
nents of the construct of intelligence. According The material included in this chapter is, perforce,
to Wechsler, because conative and affective factors exemplary rather than exhaustive; a full coverage of
function in concert with intelligence in determin- all relevant topics and studies would have required a
ing intellectual performance, what is needed are chapter of monograph length. For further informa-
not tests from which personality factors have been tion, the reader is referred to two recently published
eliminated but tests in which these nonintellective edited books examining the interface between the
factors are clearly present and objectively apprais- constructs of personality and intelligence: Personal-
able. Today, it is commonly agreed that some of the ity and intelligence (Sternberg & Ruzgis, 1994) and the
International Handbook of Personality and Intelligence
most interesting work in individual differences is be-
ing done at the interface of personality and intel- (Saklofske & Zeidner, 1995).
ligence (Snow, 1995). Because intelligence is often
construed as the cognitive part of the construct of
personality, the two constructs are in effect mutu- APPROACHES TO INTEGRATING PERSONALITY
ally intertwined (Brody, 1972). AND INTELLIGENCE
This chapter sets out to survey some of the past Following Eysenck (1994), we need to distin-
and present attempts at shedding light on the nexus guish between psychometric and experimental ap-
of relationships between intelligence and person- proaches in examining the personality-intelligence
ality, two grand constructs of modern psychology. interface. The psychometric approach is concerned

581
Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. Boston University Theology Library, on 21 May 2017 at 22:14:51, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511807947.027
582 MOSHE ZEIDNER AND GERALD MATTHEWS

with the alignments of ability and personality pressed at this level. Thirdly, explanations in terms
dimensions within a common measurement frame- of the person's goals, intentions, and efforts at adap-
work. For example, Cattell (1971) described the re- tation to the external environment are required
lationships between various cognitive (ability) and (the semantic or knowledge level). Much traditional
noncognitive (personality and motivation) dimen- personality theory, such as psychoanalysis, is ex-
sions. Cattell's model of individual differences ac- pressed at this level, but its neglect of other levels
commodates the conjoint effects of intelligence, of explanation, especially the cognitive architecture,
personality, and motivation on behavior. For exam- makes it difficult or impossible to test such theo-
ple, test situations may evoke arousal and negative ries empirically. Adaptive facets of intelligence have
emotions that impact on test performance, affect- been explored through work on ability and mo-
ing the inferences that may be drawn about intel- tivation (Kanfer & Ackerman, 1989), and through
ligent behavior (Zeidner, 1998). Cattell (1971) also Sternberg's (1985) concept of practical intelligence,
described developmental relationships between dif- the application of intelligence to real-life adaptive
ferent constructs: there may be causal and recip- challenges.
rocal relationships between personality and intelli- Matthews (1997a) discusses how Pylyshyn;s
gence. Hence, the main tasks for research are the (1984) levels of explanations apply to differential
operationalization and assessment of constructs and psychology (see Figure 26.1). The three-rung ladder
the modeling of correlational relationships between of explanation is reconceptualized as a loop to
constructs, including longitudinal data to test causal accommodate the partially genetic basis for ability
models. and personality traits. Natural selection hard-wires
Experimental approaches require a theoretical successful adaptive choices into the brain, and thus
framework. In recent years, the psychobiological knowledge and biological levels are linked as shown.
theories promoted by Eysenck (1994) have been Psychologists are perhaps more concerned with the
challenged by cognitive psychological analyses of other two bridging of levels. Studies of strategy use
intelligence (e.g., Sternberg, 1977). Analyses of cog- bridge the knowledge and architecture levels by
nitive processes may reveal interrelationships of specifying how the person's goals are translated into
intelligence and personality overlooked by purely sequences of computation. Also, connectionism is a
psychometric analyses; for example, working mem- first attempt to represent computation in terms of
ory is associated with both types of construct biologically plausible models. Individual difference
(Matthews & Dorn, 1995). However, cognition can- factors may require understanding at all three
not be understand solely through development levels. For example, depending on the research
of the information-processing models now com- context, it may be useful to conceptualize anxiety
monly used in individual differences research. Ac- in terms of brain systems for handling punishment
cording to the "classical theory" of cognitive science
(Pylyshyn, 1984), three complementary levels of FIGURE 26.1. Levels of explanation for the cognitive science
explanation are required. The first level is the of individual differences (Matthews, 1997a).
biological or "hardware" level, which describes Adaptation
the behavioral consequences of neural functions. (environmental fitness)
This approach is familiar in differential psychology
through psychophysiological analyses, and, indi- Strategies
(performance Knowledge
rectly, through behavior genetics also (e.g., Eysenck
and learning)
& Eysenck, 1985). Secondly, cognitive phenomena
may be described in terms of the formal proper-
Information Natural
ties of the cognitive architecture or "software" com- Architecture
processing selection
prising (1) the rules governing the computational
operations performed on symbols (algorithms) and
(2) the processing structures supporting computa-
Connectionism Biology
tion in real time, such as memory stores. Accounts
of intelligence in terms of individual differences in
processing limitations (e.g., Anderson, 1992) are ex- Neuroscience

Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. Boston University Theology Library, on 21 May 2017 at 22:14:51, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511807947.027
INTELLIGENCE AND PERSONALITY 583

and fear cues (Gray, 1982) of flow diagrams for quently, the examinee is expected to give as truthful
information processing (Williams et al., 1988), or a response as possible. The differentiation is not en-
in terms of the anxious individual's self-knowledge tirely straightforward: some researchers (Ackerman
and motivations (Matthews & Wells, in press). & Heggestad, 1997; Hofstee, 1997) have reflected on
whether it may sometimes make sense to talk about
THE PSYCHOMETRIC FRAMEWORK personality as maximal performance (e.g., under in-
tense stress) or 'TQ typicality" (e.g., typical intellec-
Operationalization and Assessment tual engagement).
Factor analytic research shows that when per-
sonality and intelligence measures are analyzed to- MOTIVATION AND CONTROL IN MODIFYING
gether the two constructs can generally be differen- BEHAVIOR. The individual has some degree of vol-
tiated (McCrae, 1987). The question arises: On the untary control in expressing personality. For exam-
basis of which criteria can we differentiate the two ple, the test-anxious person can cope with his or her
concepts? Table 26.1 provides a structure for this anxiety through stress management techniques. By
differentiation. contrast, intelligence is under less personal control
and is less amenable to instructions to change or
NATURE OF THE TRAIT. Intelligence is typically modify behavior.
considered to be unidirectional, whereas personal-
ity can be considered bidirectional. Accordingly, in- INSTRUCTIONS UNDER ASSESSMENT CONDI-
telligence is typically thought of as extending in TIONS. Intelligence is generally assessed using test
a single direction, from "little of" to "much of" conditions involving a set of standard tasks that ex-
(Thorndike, 1982), and it is clear how to set optimal aminees respond to at a designated time and place
parameters for intelligent behavior. By
contrast, personality is conceived of as
being bipolar. A personality trait need TABLE 26.1. Some Relationships Differentiating Personality
not take on an extreme value for it and intelligence
to be set optimally, and midpoint val-
ues are often considered as optimal. Dimension intelligence Personality
Hence, a veridical criterion is generally Trait Unidirectional Bidirectional
employed in evaluating a response in ("little of" to (polar extremes)
the intelligence domain (cf. Nunnally, "much of")
Goals and optimal Test situation Real-life situation
1978), but behaviors falling under the
assessment requiring
domain of personality often have no situation maximal
veridical criteria for judging responses. performance
Motivation in High motivation Tends to vary
taking the
GOALS AND OPTIMAL ASSESS-
instrument
MENT SITUATION. An intelligence Instructions To do one's best To provide a
test aims at eliciting maximal perfor- candid response
mance from examinees, and they are Criteria for evaluating Veridical criterion Direction and intensity
expected to give their best shot in responses (no correct response)
Stability of the Relatively stable Tends to fluctuate
solving the problem or engaging the
instrument
task at hand. Consequently, intelli- Reliability of the Generally high Varies from high
gence is generally measured in eval- instrument to low
uative and maximal performance test Interpreting Relatively More open and
conditions. By contrast, a personality results straightforward controversial
Practical utility Moderate Low to moderate
measure is designed to gauge a person's
typical performance in a real-life situ- 1 & M. Zeidner, 1995. Constructing
Source: Adapted from B.Vfost
ation (Ackerman & Heggestad, 1997) personality and intelligence test instruments: Methods and issues. In
D. Saklofske & M. Zeidner (Eds.). International handbook of personality and
to provide the best estimate of what intelligence (pp. 475-503). New York: Plenum.
a person is most likely to do. Conse-

Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. Boston University Theology Library, on 21 May 2017 at 22:14:51, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511807947.027
584 MOSHE ZEIDNER AND GERALD MATTHEWS

(cf. Zeidner & Most, 1992). Intelligence has not cision-making purposes in a wide array of sectors
been profitably assessed by observer ratings or self- and settings. Among the various practical uses of per-
reports. By contrast, personality researchers have sonality and intelligence measures are clinical diag-
worked most frequently with subjective media (e.g., nosis, vocational guidance and counseling, school
projective devices), behavioral ratings, and self- prediction and placement college selection, and per-
inquiry modes of gathering evidence (e.g., inven- sonnel selection and placement. The main differ-
tories). In fact, most measures of personality rely ence between the two are that intelligence tests
on someone's impression, be it the individual's per- appear more valid and have been more widely used
sonal impressions or those of someone observing in school prediction and college placement and
the individual. selection, whereas personality measures have been
more used in clinical diagnosis and counseling
STABILITY. Whereas theorists generally accept (Jensen, 1980).
the fact that cognitive traits display considerable Despite the distinctions among the two con-
stability across situation and over time (Willerman, structs, it is commonly agreed that intelligence and
1979), the cross-situational consistency of person- motivational-personality processes are never uti-
ality traits has been more controversial (Mischel, lized in isolation from each other; once activated
1968). Some recent studies of personality (cf. Costa there is basically no way to separate the processes
& McCrae, 1986) show promising findings of sta- or isolate the relative contribution of one from the
bility during the adulthood years, at least for basic other. Thus, in attempting to adapt to any practical
traits (see Maciel, Heckhausen, & Baltes, 1994, for a situation, an individual needs to apply both men-
recent review of the literature). tal and motivational-affective processes which, re-
spectively, draw upon factual knowledge and knowl-
RELIABILITY OF THE INSTRUMENT. It follows
edge related to values, beliefs, and standards. In
from the modifiability of personality expression most practical contexts, intelligence and person-
that the units of observation in personality as- ality contexts are bound to form a coalition of
sessment (moods, dispositions, emotional states) synergistic forces (Maciel, Heckhausen, & Baltes,
are quite troublesome for measurement purposes 1994).
because they are "probabilistic" rather than "all
or none" phenomena. Personality measures are
also generally more susceptible to a wide variety Empirical Links between Psychometric
of sources of measurement error (faking, response Measures of intelligence and Personality
sets, social desirability). A considerable body of
research attests to the lower reliability and validity In the sections that follow we survey the re-
of personality compared with intelligence measures sults of empirical research exploring the links be-
(cf. Anastasi, 1986). tween personality and psychometric measure of in-
telligence. There now seems to be a more solid
INTERPRETING RESULTS. Subjects' interpreta- and coherent theoretical basis for examining the
tions of personality tests may vary considerably personality-intelligence interface than ever before.
from subject to subject, causing considerable am- Although there are several competing theories of
biguity and further increasing measurement error. intellect, most theories converge on a hierarchical
Furthermore, in contrast to ability measures, we may model of intelligence, specifying both general and
not get a typical response on personality measures group factor(s) (e.g., Cattell, 1971; Gustafsson, 1997;
because the subject chooses not to give it or because Snow, Kyllonen, & Marshalek, 1984). Although
the subject does not have enough self-insight to there is considerably less consensus about structure
give it (Thorndike, 1982). Interpretation of ability of individual differences in personality than intel-
test scores is more straightforward, though it may be ligence, theoretical and empirical research over the
difficult to assess the roles of affective and conative past 30 years prompted several investigators to con-
factors. verge on a set of five factors (the Big Five: Costa &
McCrae, 1992) that appear across numerous per-
PRACTICAL UTILITY. Both personality and intel- sonality inventories (Extraversion, Agreeableness,
ligence measures have been used for practical de- Conscientousness, Neuroticism, Intellect).

Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. Boston University Theology Library, on 21 May 2017 at 22:14:51, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511807947.027
INTELLIGENCE AND PERSONALITY 585

Methodological Considerations light on the magnitude and pattern of relationships


First, it may be useful to make a number of distinc- between ability and the "Big Five" factors of per-
tions and methodological comments that may help sonality. The largest effect observed in this analy-
the reader appraise the evidence relating intelligence sis is the meaningful positive correlation between
to personality. To begin with, one should keep in intelligence (g) and the Openness-Intellect factor
mind the distinction between personality traits and (r = 33) - a factor associated with a broad class of
states. Traits are broad determining tendencies or intellectually oriented traits. In addition, a small
propensities to behave, whereas states are tempo- but significant positive correlation was reported be-
rary states of mind and mood determined by the in- tween g and Extraversion (r = .08), and a small but
teraction between a person's trait and present state significant negative correlation with ability was re-
or situation (Spielberger, Gonzales, Taylor, Algaze, & ported for Neuroticism (r = -.15). Correlations be-
Anton, 1978). States and traits are typically corre- tween g and both Conscientiousness (r = .02) and
lated because a trait is actually defined as a dispo- Agreeableness (r = .01) were negligible. The authors
sition that behavior is likely to be manifested in suggest that the personality tests that have ubiq-
particular situations. For example, "trait anxiety'' uitous positive correlations with ability fall under
refers to an individual's disposition to respond with the broad category of Positive Affect, whereas neg-
worry, tension, and physiological arousal across a ative correlations with ability tend to be from broad
variety of conditions (physical danger, evaluative categories of Negative Affect, such as Neuroticism
situations, social encounters, sports events), whereas (stress reaction) and Psychoticism. Kyllonen (1997)
"state anxiety''refers to a specific level of anxiety ex- recently reported correlations between the Big Five
perienced in a particular situation, such as an impor- factors and measures of General, Verbal, and Quan-
tant athletic competition. Two people may have the titative Ability in a U.S. sample of Air Force recruits.
same level of trait anxiety but differ in their anxiety Just as in the reported meta-analysis, Openness and
elevations in a particular situation or "state." Neuroticism were the factors most strongly related
Zeidner (1995) delineated several different mod- to ability measures. Thus, Openness was meaning-
els employed in studying the direction of the causal fully correlated with both g (r = .45), Verbal ability
relationship between intelligence and personality. (r = .58), and Quantitative ability (r = .29), whereas
In Model A intelligence is depicted as the indepen- Neuroticism was reported to be inversely correlated
dent variable and personality as the dependent vari- with both g(r = -.23) and Verbal ability (r = -.20).
able. In Model B the causal direction is reversed, The correlations between Conscientiousness, Agree-
and intelligence now serves as the dependent vari- ableness, and Extraversion were very low, ranging
able and personality as the independent variable. In from —.11 to .11. It is interesting to note, in pass-
Model C there is a bidirectional relationship and re- ing, that only cognitive factors (working memory,
ciprocal determinism between the two constructs. In knowledge, etc.), but not personality factors pre-
Model D the relationship is artifactual with a third dicted short-term performance on learning tasks
extraneous variable responsible for the relationship. (e.g., computer programming, flight engineering,
In Model E, personality is conceived as an interven- economics) and psychomotor performance (e.g., fly-
ing or "nuisance" variable intervening between the ing, control). However, personality did account for
intelligence construct (as input) and manifest level larger, long-term performance (e.g., productivity on
of intelligence (as output, evidenced in intelligence the job).
test scores). Each of these models has been espoused The inference to be drawn from these data is that
as a possible model for the intelligence-personality at the level of latent constructs, IQ and personality
relationship, although it is often difficult to discrim- are orthogonal constructs (see Eysenck's, 1994, re-
inate among them. cent review). Furthermore, neither subtest patterns
nor profiles of IQ have been systematically found to
be related to personality variables (Eysenck, 1994).
Meta-Analytic Results for the Although actual performance on IQ tests may be in-
Personality-intelligence interface fluenced by an array of noncognitive states (fatigue,
A recent meta-analysis (Ackerman & Heggestad, anxiety, arousal), personality and intelligence are es-
1997), based on 135 separate studies, helps shed sentially unrelated. However, such conclusions may

Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. Boston University Theology Library, on 21 May 2017 at 22:14:51, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511807947.027
586 MOSHE ZEIDNER AND GERALD MATTHEWS

understate the personality-intelligence associations plex the task, the weaker the effect. Sarason (1975)
to be found through more fine-grained examina- reported that the differences between anxious and
tion of the evidence. Three especially productive ar- nonanxious students in performance are realized
eas of research have focused on negative affectivity, mainly in the competitive atmosphere; under neu-
extraversion-introversion, and openness. Another tral conditions the differences between anxious and
approach is to assess the IQs of people showing var- nonanxious students are minimal.
ious abnormalities of behavior: the mentally ill, the Anxiety may have somewhat more general effects
deviant, and children with unusually high or low on intellectual performance in real-world contexts.
IQs. It has been estimated that there is a correlation
of about - . 2 between anxiety and both measures
intelligence and Negative Affectivity of performance and achievement at school and
We now examine the relationship between intelli- college (Hembree, 1988; Siepp, 1991) and sports
gence and various personality traits subsumed under performance (Zeidner, 1997). The relationship be-
the higher order category of negative affectivity, in- tween neurotic personality and occupational per-
cluding neuroticism, anxiety, depression and anger. formance has also been estimated at about —.2
Most studies have addressed negative affect through in studies in which there was an a priori ratio-
anxiety, which, as a state, is characterized by appre- nale for performance being sensitive to neuroticism
hension, worry, and emotional and somatic tension. (Tett, Rothstein, & Jackson, 1991). Metanalytic data
Anxiety has received particular attention because (Ackerman & Heggestad, 1997) show a mean correla-
it may impair intellectual functioning and perfor- tion of -.33 between g and self-reports of test anx-
mance in variety of contexts, ranging from intelli- iety. Test anxiety measures may be a stronger pre-
gence test scores and school achievement (Sarason, dictor of situational anxiety than general anxiety in
1980) to dating (Hope & Heimberg, 1990). In fact, such contexts. However, meta-analysis also shows
the literature suggests that the relationship between that there is little difference between state and trait
trait anxiety and psychometric tests of intelligence anxiety measures as predictors of educational perfor-
is negligible (Matarazzo, 1972). Thus, neither pat- mance criteria. Hence, detrimental effects of anxiety
tern nor scatter-analysis approaches to the Wechsler in the real world may represent more than just dis-
scales have shown any relation to a trait measure of traction from performance by the person's immedi-
anxiety. However, studies utilizing situationally in- ate worries about the test situation, which relates to
duced anxiety (i.e., state anxiety) do reveal decre- state rather than to trait anxiety.
ments in performance on the same measures of in- Depression is characterized by strong negative
tellectual functioning. Only when we separate the emotions, impairment of everyday social function-
currently anxious (state anxious) from the chroni- ing, and negative beliefs such as hopelessness and
cally anxious (trait anxious) can we show a decre- lack of self-worth. There is little evidence to suggest
ment in intellectual performance due to anxiety that subclinical levels of depression reliably relate to
(Siegman, 1956). Discriminating different compo- impaired intelligence test performance in laboratory
nents of anxiety shows that worry, but not emo- settings (e.g., Matthews, 1986). Deficits are found
tionality, is related to impaired intelligence test in clinical populations, however, especially on the
performance under time pressure (Matthews, 1986; performance subtests of the Wechsler Adult Intelli-
Morris & Liebert, 1969). gence Scale (Endler & Summerfeldt, 1995). Impair-
Both test and trait anxiety may interact with ment of performance appears to generalize across a
situational variables (e.g., task complexity, test at- variety of high-level cognitive tasks such as prob-
mosphere, etc.) and personal variables (e.g., opti- lem solving and reading comprehension (Hartlage,
mism, personal resources, social support) in impact- Alloy, Vazquez, & Dykman, 1993). The effects of
ing upon cognitive test performance (see Zeidner, depression on intellectual functioning in the real
1998, for a review). For example, there is consider- world are presently unclear. There has been little
able evidence that the performance of highly test work on the manic symptoms central to some mood
anxious individuals on complex tasks is detrimen- disorders, although low levels of symptoms (hypo-
tally affected by evaluational stressors: the less com- mania) may be associated with enhanced creativity

Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. Boston University Theology Library, on 21 May 2017 at 22:14:51, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511807947.027
INTELLIGENCE AND PERSONALITY 587

(Endler & Summerfeldt, 1995). Anger is another whereas introverts are socially reserved and cau-
potentially important influence on intelligence tious. Extraversion-lntroversion relates to a variety
test performance that remains to be investigated of aspects of human performance (Matthews, 1992).
(cf. Zeidner, 1995). However, as we have seen, it shows only small pos-
The causal status of anxiety and other negative af- itive associations with intelligence in meta-analytic
fects is somewhat controversial. It is often supposed data (Ackerman & Heggestad, 1997). As in the case of
that the worry component of state anxiety uses up anxiety, though, there may be some more subtle re-
working memory or attentional resources, and thus lationships. Relationships between extraversion and
performance is impaired if the task is sufficiently intelligence may be moderated by the nature of
demanding (e.g., Eysenck, 1992; Sarason, 1975). the test. Robinson (1985) claimed that E is inti-
Subjects low in intelligence, for whom the task is mately associated with different intellectual styles
objectively more difficult, may thus be especially and intelligence profiles but not to absolute levels of
vulnerable to more general anxiety-related impair- performance on intelligence tests. Accordingly, in-
ments - an hypothesis confirmed by meta-analysis troverts and extraverts were found not to differ in
(Hembree, 1988). We will return to cognitive mech- overall IQ, only on profile: introverts were found to
anisms for anxiety impairment in a later section. do relatively better on verbal tests and extraverts on
Causal effects of clinical depression on cognitive performance tests. However, these effects may not
performance are generally accepted, especially for be replicable (Saklofske & Kostura, 1990). Matthews
tasks requiring concentration or memory (Endler (1992) observed that introverts tend to do better
& Summerfeldt, 1995). However, there are alterna- on problem-solving tasks requiring insight and re-
tive explanations for negative correlations between flection. Conversely, extraverts may benefit from
anxiety and intelligence. First, anxiety in the cog- speeded tasks (Rawlings & Carnie, 1989). Eysenck
nitive test situation may be generated by more or (1994) reviewed studies showing that E is related to
less veridical appraisals that failure is likely owing speed of working and that extraverted subjects
to low ability or lack of preparation and study. In spend lower total time doing the test. Thus, Jensen
other words, anxiety may be a marker for poor in- (1966) found that E correlated .44 with speed of so-
tellectual skills rather than a factor that directly af- lution of Raven's matrices, although extraverts made
fects performance (Mueller, 1992). Second, to the significantly more errors. Eysenck (1994) surveyed
extent that anxiety leads to beliefs of lack of self- evidence suggesting that extraverts tend to give up
efficacy in achievement-oriented situations, individ- towards the end of tests and take longer to obtain
uals high in anxiety may avoid activities that en- correct solutions towards the end of the test, a pro-
hance intellectual growth, and thus apprehension cess he attributes to greater reactive inhibition or
about academic contexts and lack of intellectual lower cortical arousal.
skills become mutually reinforcing (Cattell, 1971). Extraversion effects are often moderated by fac-
Third, the observed relationship of anxiety to in- tors related to stress, arousal, and emotion. Some
telligence may be due to the artifactual influences of this work shows interactions between extraver-
of extraneous variables (e.g., social class, childrear- sion and arousal (e.g., Revelle et al., 1980) and
ing patterns, test situation) affecting both variables has been inspired by the arousal theory hypothe-
(Weiner, 1973). Similar explanations are possible for sis that the tonic or resting level of arousal for in-
depression findings. It is conceivable that depres- troverts is higher than that of extraverts (Eysenck &
sion will result if low intelligence leads to a succes- Eysenck, 1985). This hypothesis is further discussed
sion of performance failures, for example. in the section on neural processes. Extraversion has
also been linked to positive emotionality (Watson
Extraversion-lntroversion & Clark, 1992), but associations between E and
Extraversion-introversion (E) is a basic dimension positive affect in performance contexts are small
of individual differences in human behavior and is in magnitude (Matthews, 1997b). However, if ex-
often construed as a source trait with a strong bio- traverts are more energetic and lively in some real-
logical basis (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1985). Extraverts world contexts, these positive moods may feed
tend to be outgoing, sociable, and impulsive, into superior intellectual performance. Studies of

Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. Boston University Theology Library, on 21 May 2017 at 22:14:51, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511807947.027
588 MOSHE ZEIDNER AND GERALD MATTHEWS

patients with chronic fatigue syndrome show wide- by uncertainty over the nature of Openness, which
ranging deficits, including considerable impairment has been much debated in the personality literature
of full-scale IQ (Daugherty et al., 1991). Less dra- (e.g., McCrae, 1996). Trapnell (1994), for example,
matic but reliable attentional deficits are found in picks out intellectual competency and liberalism as
individuals whose mood is characterized by low two distinct aspects of Openness, although he pre-
energy, although there is no straightforward rela- fers to relate them to Conscientiousness (see Saucier,
tionship between subjective tiredness and intelli- 1994, for a rebuttal of this view). Intellectual com-
gence test performance (Matthews, 1992). petency may mediate associations between Open-
Finally, there may be a developmental or matu- ness and intelligence test performance. Goff and
rational link between intelligence and personality Ackerman (1992) showed that three constructs were
(Eysenck & Cookson, 1969). These authors suggest mutually intercorrelated: openness, crystallized in-
that around the ages of 13 or 14, there is a tendency telligence, and a measure of 'Typical Intellectual
for the correlation between extraversion and IQ to Engagement" (TIE), which assesses intellectual mo-
change from positive to negative. There seems to tives and interests. In a further study, Rolfhus and
be some uncertainty over the age of the crossover Ackerman (1996) showed that Openness correlated
point. For example, Crookes, Pearson, Francis, and at .67 with TIE and that both measures related to
Carter (1981) found a positive correlation between knowledge of the arts and humanities, although the
extraversion and IQ in 15-16 year old boys and TIE scale was more broadly related to self-report
girls. If the effect is genuine, there are various ex- knowledge. Plausibly, more Open individuals show
planations. According to Eysenck (1994) it may re- greater crystallized intelligence because they are
late to the change from the free and easy atmosphere more motivated to engage in intellectual activities,
at primary school, which accords well with the ex- especially those of a verbal and cultural nature.
traverted temperament, and the more formal atmo- In addition, the relationship between Openness
sphere at secondary school, which agrees better with and intelligence may also relate to its "liberal" el-
the introverted temperament. A second possible ex- ement, that is, willingness to examine social, po-
planation is that more able children become intro- litical and religious values and rejection of tradi-
verted, whereas the less able become extraverted tional conservative views. This link is shown most
(Anthony, 1983). A further possible explanation is clearly by research on authoritarianism, an atti-
that because introverts mature more slowly, intro- tudinal system consisting of a set of interrelated
version in primary school age children is not advan- antidemocratic sentiments, including ethnic preju-
tageous to mental achievement, but by secondary dice, political conservatism, and moralistic rejection
school it becomes an assistance (Anthony, 1983). of the unconventional (Adorno, Frenkel-Brunswick,
Eysenck (1994) suggested that the correlation be- Levinson, & Sanford, 1950). Trapnell (1994) re-
tween introversion and increasing relative success ported a correlation of -.57 between a right-wing
in academic exams is plausible because such success authoritarianism scale and openness. The studies of
is presumably facilitated by private study, an intro- Adorno et al., and those of other researchers show
verted type of behavior. that Authoritarianism scores are negatively associ-
ated with intelligence and have correlations ranging
Openness: Intellect and Authoritarianism between -.20 and -.50.
Openness is characterized by vivid fantasy, artis- One possibility is that only individuals of rela-
tic sensitivity, depth of feeling, behavioral flexibility, tively low intelligence are able to accept the kind
intellectual curiosity, and unconventional attitudes. of ideology represented by an extreme fascist orien-
McCrae (1994) has argued that there is substantial tation (Thompson & Michel, 1972). Another line of
overlap between Openness to experience and In- reasoning points out that both intelligence and au-
tellectual Ability. Ackerman and Heggestad's (1997) thoritarianism are confounded by social class. Also,
meta-analysis established that this factor correlates it may be chiefly education or cultural sophistica-
more strongly with general intelligence than any of tion rather than intelligence per se that reduces au-
the other Big Five factors, although it related more thoritarianism. Cristie (1954) estimated that with
strongly to crystallized than to fluid intelligence. education partialled out the correlation between au-
However, interpretation of the data is complicated thoritarianism and intelligence drops considerably.

Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. Boston University Theology Library, on 21 May 2017 at 22:14:51, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511807947.027
INTELLIGENCE AND PERSONALITY 589

Thus, those in society who are low in personal and Delinquency


social resources (e.g., social status, intelligence, ed- On average, delinquents score eight IQ points
ucation), feel threatened and frustrated and turn to lower than nondelinquents on standard intelligent
authoritarian beliefs as a solution to problems. tests (Lynam, Moffitt, & Stouthamer-Loeber, 1993).
Other evidence also points to the importance of This relation holds up regardless of social class or
socially shaped belief systems. A survey (Argyle, ethnic background. Crime and even aggression may
1961) of American studies of children and students act over a long period of time to depress intellectual
found negative correlations between intelligence functioning (Huesmann, Eron, & Yarmel, 1987). Fur-
and religious beliefs, attitudes, and experiences. The thermore, recent meta-analytic research (Ackerman
correlations are generally higher for religious con- & Heggestad, 1997) reports a modest inverse correla-
servatism (-.15 to -.55) than for attitudes and ex- tion between aggression and g (r = — .19). Wechsler
periences (-.19 to -.27). Similarly, several studies (1958) pointed out that the most outstanding sin-
showed that beliefs in the paranormal predicted low gle feature of the sociopath's test profile is his sys-
intelligence (see Zeidner, 1995, for a review), per- tematic high score on the Performance as compared
haps because higher intelligence may render an indi- with the Verbal part of the scale. Matarrazo (1972)
vidual less vulnerable to the circular and ephemeral listed about 30 studies that generally support this
arguments put forth in defense of paranormal ideas view, although he urges caution in individual diag-
(Zusne & Jones, 1982). Intriguingly, though, recent noses.
studies suggest that due to the antirationalism typ- Researchers have argued over the meaning and in-
ical of many intellectually superior students during terpretation of this negative relationship (Lynam,
the 1960s and the 1970s, the historical relationship Moffitt, & Stouthamer-Loeber, 1993). For one, a
may have changed, and beliefs in the paranormal are delinquent life style may result in lower intellectual
now associated with higher rather than lower intel- functioning. This lowering may be the result of one
ligence (cf. Jones, Russel, & Nickel, 1977). of a variety of factors, including poor school moti-
vation, negative attitudes towards IQ testing, head
Abnormality and Deviance injuries incurred in numerous street or gang fights,
Work on abnormality of personality and intel- or the deleterious effects of drug abuse on the central
ligence has tended to use clinical typologies, al- nervous system. According to Block (1995), high im-
though dimensional models based on normal per- pulsivity accounts for the lower Verbal IQ frequently
sonality structure are becoming more widely used in characterizing serious delinquents. Because effective
other contexts (Matthews, Saklofske et al., in press). test performance requires constrained, slowed, and
Eysenck (1994) believed that research exploring sta- reflective behavior, impulsive and uncontrolled in-
tistical relations between measures of IQ and psy- dividuals are less able to modulate their actions and
chopathology is doomed to failure in view of the reactions, resulting in depressed test performance.
following three factors: the mindless ''blunderbuss'' Moreover, during their education, impulsive and
approach adapted by many researchers in correlat- undercontrolled children may cumulatively register
ing measures of personality and intelligence; the less of the information, reasoning, and logic upon
notorious unreliability of the criterion, that is, di- which verbal tests depend, and their IQ suffers be-
agnosed psychopathology; and problems in statis- cause they are unprepared. It is the unresilient, un-
tical analysis of difference scores. The role of in- dercontrolled child who will be disposed to conduct
telligence in some aspects of abnormality has been disorder, will more frequently miss days in school,
neglected, notably in the case of personality disor- and who will lose the opportunity to develop cog-
ders. Endler and Summerfeldt's (1995) review of the nitive processes that underpin performance on in-
literature concluded that characteristic ways of orga- telligence tests. These behavioral deficits may be
nizing and processing information and experience linked to deficiencies in self-regulation, including
appear to play major roles in the genesis and perpet- lack of self-control, preferences for coping through
uation of personality disorders. The cognitive char- aggression, and distorted appraisals of others' atti-
acteristics of these disorders are important, but there tudes towards oneself (Schwean & Saklofske, 1995;
is little systematic evidence on the role of intelli- Matthews, Schwean et al., in press). Second, a
gence from studies using psychometric tests. low IQ may lead to delinquency as mediated by

Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. Boston University Theology Library, on 21 May 2017 at 22:14:51, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511807947.027
590 MOSHE ZEIDNER AND GERALD MATTHEWS

low scholastic performance and eventual school ization manifest greater intellectual deficit than do
dropout. A third explanation is that the observed patients with recent onset or brief hospitalization.
relationship is spurious, and a third variable (e.g., However, this may be due to changes in the compo-
social class, race, etc.) may cause both low IQ and sition of hospitalized cohorts over time rather than
delinquency. to the effects of hospitalization or processes intrin-
sic to psychosis. Longitudinal studies of hospital-
Schizophrenia ized patients fail to find progressive decline (Hamlin,
Schizophrenic thought and judgment is seriously 1963). That is, chronics may be less bright than
impaired as mental health deteriorates with a grow- acutes because of an association between IQ and re-
ing tendency to confuse wish and reality (Coleman, tention in hospitals and readmission to hospitals.
Butcher, & Carson, 1984). The schizophrenic mind, There is some research to suggest that brighter pa-
although unselectively registering everything in its tients are less often rehospitalized several years af-
field of vision, is unable to distinguish between ter discharge than patients of lower IQ (Heffner,
relevant and irrelevant cues (Williams & Beech, Strauss, & Grisell, 1975). Thus, there appears to be
1997). Decrements in intelligence test performance a prognostic significance for IQ that contributes to
are common but may in part be attributed to the the differences in intelligence between acute and
subject's low motivation to perform, minimal coop- chronic schizophrenics.
eration, and impaired contact with the examiner, The effects of psychosis on intellectual function-
leading to fluctuation of test scores across occasions. ing for children may be more pernicious than for
It is somewhat unclear whether impairment of adults and children suffering from certain personal-
the intellect is a vulnerability factor for schizophre- ity dysfunctions and disturbances, who have been
nia or whether it represents a consequence of psy- reported to suffer from a severe deterioration of
chosis: the direct empirical evidence is conflict- abilities (Sattler, 1988). Children with disintegra-
ing and often confusing. Although the intelligence tive psychosis usually have a period of normal
of severely schizophrenic individuals may be be- development for the first 3-4 years, after which
low average, one is struck by the preservation of profound regression and behavioral disintegration
average or even remarkable intellectual superiority occur, intelligence declines, speech and language
in many schizophrenics, especially those diagnosed abilities deteriorate, and social skills diminish. In ad-
as paranoid. Some studies report (Watson, Herder, dition, children become restive, irritable, anxious,
Kucala, & Hoodecheck-Schow, 1987) substantial de- and overactive. Overall, the prognosis is poor, al-
cline between premorbid assessment and hospital- though more intelligent children may have better
ization, whereas other studies show that intellectual prospects.
decline among schizophrenics may be attributable
to preexisting conditions and is not an inherent fac- Personality Characteristics of the Gifted
tor associated with the onset of schizophrenia. In and intellectually Challenged
one recent study, no significant decay in intelligence The previous section considered the intellectual
scores was observed for schizophrenics whose initial capabilities of individuals with abnormal person-
aptitude test scores were above average, and the ob- alities. We may also look at the personalities of
served decline in test scores was largely attributable people with abnormal intellect. By virtue of their
to a drop in a specific skill - arithmetic performance exceptionally high and low intellectual talents, both
(Watson et al., 1987). Because deterioration is lim- gifted and intellectually challenged individuals are
ited to certain skills, it is best interpreted as a loss often viewed as suffering from special adjustment
in particular intellectual functions rather than as a problems, especially during childhood (cf., Anastasi,
more generalized intellectual decay. 1958).
Some research shows that intelligence deterio-
rates under schizophrenic attack, but, under remis- intellectually Gifted Children
sion, performance on intelligence tests improves Paradoxically, it is often supposed that highly
(Scheartzman & Douglas, 1962). Chronic schizo- intelligent children have special problems in ad-
phrenic inpatients with long histories of hospital- justment. The famous Stanford Gifted Child Study,

Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. Boston University Theology Library, on 21 May 2017 at 22:14:51, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511807947.027
INTELLIGENCE AND PERSONALITY 591

initiated by L. Terman in 1921, systematically also been suggested that personality may develop
investigated the physical, mental, and personal- at a slower rate in these than in normal children.
ity traits of 1,528 gifted children from California Thus, whereas the mentally challenged and nor-
(Mean IQ=151), following them into late adult- mal children of the same mental age are similar in
hood (Terman, 1935; Terman & Oden, 1947; Terman personality characteristics, the mentally challenged
& Oden, 1959). Not only were these children above and regular children of the same chronological age
norms in height and overall health, but they were may differ in personality (Nevo, 1998). In fact, aside
also rated by their teachers as considerably more from those children with clear physiological deficits,
"self confident/' "optimistic/' and "emotionally there is much evidence against the existence of
stable" compared with controls. In adulthood the unique personality characteristics intrinsically asso-
gifted sample proved to be equal or superior to ciated with mental retardation (Nevo, 1998). It is
controls on marital adjustment and showed nor- plausible, however, that poor cognitive performance
mal or below normal incidence of serious person- and repeated failure in school and social settings
ality maladjustment. Data on the personality and may indirectly lead to problems of social adjustment
social characteristics of these 70+ year-old subjects and rejection. This, in turn, may influence both the
are still being gathered and disseminated. Inter- development and expression of certain personality
estingly, two of Terman's subjects, Lee Cronbach traits.
and Robert Sears, grew up to become famous psy-
chologists in their own right and assumed profes- The Psychometric Approach: Conclusions
sorships in psychology at the very university that Correlations between personality and ability con-
sponsored the study! Comparable findings can be structs are often small, although there are substan-
gleaned from the John Hopkins study of mathemat- tial associations between Openness and crystallized
ically gifted youth (Stanley, Keating, & Fox, 1974). intelligence measures. However, closer empirical
The personality profiles of these mathematically links are found in some contexts. Anxiety measures
gifted students suggest social maturity and psycho- related to specific contexts, such as test anxiety, are
logical health and are a far cry from maladjusted moderately predictive of impaired test performance.
by any standards. A study (Mayer, Caruso, Zigler, Both anxiety and extraversion may be shown to pre-
& Dreyden, 1989) of 46 gifted adolescent students dict test performance when situational factors such
attending the Duke University Talent Identification as level of time pressure, stress, or arousal are ma-
Program has also suggested that gifted children are nipulated. Subject groups whose personality is ab-
characterized by optimal levels on several intellect- normal, such as psychotic patients and delinquents,
related personality traits such as high intellectual ab- may also show impairment of intellectual function.
sorption, the tendency to experience pleasure when However, groups of abnormal IQ (high and low)
working on problems, and low degrees of apathy show relatively little sign of dysfunctional person-
or boredom in doing intellectual work. Overall, re- ality, and high IQ appears to be beneficial. It is of-
search provides a highly positive personality pro- ten difficult to discriminate the causal hypotheses
file of the gifted, which is in sharp contrast to the proposed for these various relationships. On one
common stereotype of the highly intelligent child hand, personality may impair or facilitate the in-
as shy, unhealthy, weak, and generally socially mal- tellectual competence associated with IQ, as mani-
adjusted. fested in actual test performance illustrated by some
of the detrimental effects of anxiety, depression,
intellectually Challenged Children schizophrenia, and delinquency. On the other hand,
There is often a tendency to view mentally chal- IQ may influence the person's adjustment to real-
lenged children as a homogeneous group with spe- world demands, and consequently, personality. For
cific personality deficits and problems. For exam- example, the educational difficulties of low-IQ chil-
ple, some research suggests that these children are dren may variously lead to anxiety and conduct
sometimes characterized by more rigid patterns of problems. In addition, personality-IQ relationships
behavior than are those who are at the same men- may be confounded by third variables, such as social
tal age but who are chronologically younger. It has class.

Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. Boston University Theology Library, on 21 May 2017 at 22:14:51, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511807947.027
592 MOSHE ZEIDNER AND GERALD MATTHEWS

THE COGNITIVE SCIENCE PERSPECTIVE cesses. However, there is little work that attempts
to determine whether the two construct types re-
Cognitive science emphasizes processes rather than
late to common neural bases. On the whole, in-
structural factors, whether these processes are neu-
telligence and personality seem to relate to rather
ral, computational, or adaptive. From this perspec-
different psychophysiological indices, as shown by
tive, individual differences dimensions refer to sta-
evoked potential studies, for example (Stelmack &
ble parameters governing the behavior of dynamic
Houlihan, 1995). It is also unclear whether behav-
systems. The first question to be addressed is
ior reflects interactions between distinct sets of neu-
whether ability and personality are associated with
ral processes associated with the two constructs. For
common parameters. For example, is it possible
example, is it possible that the cortical circuits con-
that intelligence and personality factors relate to
trolling performance on an intelligence test are in-
a "clock-speed" parameter influencing rate of in-
fluenced by subcortical circuits linked to personal-
formation processing? In general, the psychomet-
ity? A crude example might be disruption of cortical
ric distinctiveness of the two kinds of dimension
processing caused by fear circuits centered on the
suggests that such overlaps are limited. The sec-
amygdala (LeDoux, 1995), if we may assume that
ond, more subtle question is whether intelligence
chronically fearful individuals show abnormality of
and personality influence common intelligent be-
function in these circuits.
haviors via different parameters. For example, both
One strategy for integrating the two constructs
intelligence and some personality traits may influ-
has been through arousal theory. The electroen-
ence response speed on some performance task, but
cephalogram (EEG) and other psychophysiological
the effect may be mediated by different processes,
measures may provide an index of the functional
say, basic processing speed and strategy choice. This
status of the organism, which integrates outputs
form of overlap may be quite prevalent. Initially, we
from a variety of distinct neural systems (e.g.,
will briefly review the evidence for process-oriented
Mesulam, 1985). There have been occasional at-
relationships between personality and intelligence
tempts to link intelligence to arousal (e.g., Necka,
at the three levels of explanation previously de-
1997), but individual differences in arousal are
scribed: the biological, the cognitive-architectural,
more usually seen as the basis for the personality
and the adaptive-knowledge levels. We will then
traits of extraversion-introversion and neuroticism
outline an integrated cognitive-adaptive framework
(Eysenck & Eysenck, 1985). Eysenck and Eysenck
for interrelating personality and intelligence. It
(1985) suppose that (1) extraverts tend to be less
should be recognized from the outset that it is dif-
aroused than introverts and (2) arousal is linked
ficult to describe the static constructs of differen-
to performance by an inverted-U function (the so-
tial psychology in process-oriented terms. An em-
called Yerkes-Dodson law).
pirical association between, say, intelligence and
information-processing speed, does not necessarily It follows that personality and arousing agents
imply either that intelligence is a direct expression should have interactive effects on performance of
of processing speed or that there is some unitary various tasks, including intelligence tests. Consis-
cognitive entity called intelligence that controls pro- tent with prediction, arousal induced by caffeine
cessing speed (Matthews & Dorn, 1995). and time pressure tends to enhance the test per-
formance of extraverts but impairs introverts' per-
formance (Revelle, Amaral, & Turiff, 1976). How-
Neural Processes ever, other work presents a more complex picture.
The treatment here of the biological level will be Work by Revelle, Humphreys, Simon, & Gilliland
brief. There is much work on the biological bases (1980) demonstrated that when subjects are tested
of both intelligence and personality constructs. in the evening, caffeine benefits introverts, not ex-
Both appear to be partially inherited (Bouchard, traverts, implying that the relationship between
1995; Loehlin, 1992), and both have extensive psy- extraversion and arousal varies with time of day.
chophysiological correlates (Deary & Stough, 1996; Revelle et al. also showed that impassivity was
Matthews & Gilliland, in press), implying that their more predictive than sociability of test performance.
behavioral expressions are influenced by neural pro- Matthews (1985) obtained results similar to those of

Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. Boston University Theology Library, on 21 May 2017 at 22:14:51, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511807947.027
INTELLIGENCE AND PERSONALITY 593

Revelle et al. using a self-report measure of arousal. information-processing tasks, but it is less clear that
However, these data showed that arousal moder- such correlations reflect individual differences in ar-
ated rather than mediated the effect of extraversion: chitecture, as opposed to voluntary strategy. For ex-
the arousal-performance relationship appeared to ample, anxious individuals often behave as though
be qualitatively different in extraverts and intro- they have a functional deficit in attentional capac-
verts. Similarly, Matthews & Amelang (1993) found ity. However, this deficit may reflect diversion of
that associations between EEG alpha and verbal in- capacity onto internal worries rather than a funda-
telligence test performance varied with extraversion. mental lack of resources (Matthews & Wells, in press;
Matthews (1992, 1997b) attributed such effects to Sarason, 1975). Conversely, subjective arousal does
arousal having qualitatively differing effects on at- seem to correlate with total availability of resources,
tentional efficiency in extroverted and introverted a parameter of the architecture (Matthews & Davies,
subjects. in press).
Studies of this kind implicate neural arousal pro-
cesses in both extraversion-introversion and in test INTELLIGENCE. The pioneering studies of Hunt
performance, but their exact theoretical implica- (1978) and Sternberg (1977) provided a major impe-
tions are unclear. The neural processes concerned tus to studies of intelligence and information pro-
are not specified precisely, and so there is no basis cessing. It has been claimed that special importance
for modeling their interaction. Part of the problem attaches to diverse processing constructs, includ-
is that extraversion is most likely associated with a ing elementary processing speed (Neubauer, 1997),
variety of neurologically distinct functions. For ex- working memory (Kyllonen & Chrystal, 1990), and
ample, studies of brainstem-evoked potentials per- strategy use (Ferrara, Brown, & Campione, 1986).
mit extraversion to be linked to some specific brain None of these constructs, however, seem likely to
structures, such as the inferior colliculus (Bullock replace g on psychometric grounds alone (Carroll,
and Gilliland, 1993). It is unlikely, however, that 1993). Instead, as Sternberg (1977) claimed, intelli-
these structures directly influence intelligent be- gence may be associated with a multiplicity of lower
havior. Matthews & Harley (1993) have proposed level components and higher level "metacompo-
a speculative connectionist model that attributes nents" or control processes, and thus there is no
extraversion x arousal interaction effects on cog- single critical process for intelligence. Intelligence
nitive performance to variation in levels of ran- is distributed across a variety of independent pro-
dom noise within a neural net. But, such attempts cessing components at different levels of the archi-
are in their infancy. Although arousal theory may tecture. The observed correlations between intelli-
serve as a useful*heuristic for linking personality gence and a specific task depend on the g loadings
and intelligence, its well-known shortcomings (e.g., of the components controlling individual differ-
Matthews & Amelang, 1993) may limit its utility in ences in performance (Sternberg, 1977). We can
developing neurally based theories. then ask questions about the role of personality sim-
ilar to those posed at the biological level of expla-
Cognitive Processes nation. Are there components associated with both
We can be fairly confident that intelligence is as- intelligence and personality factors? To what extent
sociated with individual differences in the cognitive is intelligent behavior controlled by cognitive pro-
architecture (strictly, the functional architecture, in cesses sensitive to personality as well by ^-loaded
Pylyshyn's, 1984, terms). It is clear that high IQis as- processes?
sociated with greater processing efficiency on some
tasks, whether this advantage is attributed to work- EXTRAVERSION. In previous reviews (Matthews,
ing memory, attentional resources, rate of compu- 1992, 1997b; Matthews & Dorn, 1995), one of us
tation, or some other processing qualities. Reduced has concluded that, as with intelligence, personal-
constraints on processing imposed by architectural ity traits are associated with a diversity of cogni-
features may also translate in superior acquired cog- tive correlates or cognitive patterning. However, in
nitive skills (Anderson, 1992). Like intelligence, per- contrast to intelligence, traits tend to be associated
sonality traits also correlate with performance on with performance deficits as well as enhancements,

Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. Boston University Theology Library, on 21 May 2017 at 22:14:51, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511807947.027
594 MOSHE ZEIDNER AND GERALD MATTHEWS

ously discussed, one interpretation of these interac-


TABLE 26.2. Cognitive Patterning of
tional effects rests on analyses of neural processes
Extraversion-lntroversion: Performance
Characteristics of the Extravert Compared such as arousal theory. Such explanations are, at
with the Introvert the least, incomplete because extraversion x arousal
interactions vary with the information-processing
Characteristics of Extraversion demands of the task. Matthews and Harley (1993)
Superiority in... Divided attention suggested that the typical interaction effect oper-
Resistance to distraction ates on lower-level spreading activation processes.
Retrieval from memory They speculated that the effects of extraversion
Short-term memory
and arousal factors on intelligence test performance
Inferiority in... Vigilance found by Revelle et al. (1980) may be mediated by
Reflective problem solving
Long-term memory "automatic" verbal processes. In other words, prop-
Lower response criterion erties of the architecture may vary to some degree
Little systematic Attentional selectivity across both individuals and contexts. Overall, the
effect on... Reaction-time tasks impression is that intelligence and extraversion are
General intelligence associated with distinctive sets of parameter settings
for the cognitive architecture, although there may
be some overlap with regard to specific parameters.
which may account for the small magnitude of
personality-intelligence correlations previously dis- ANXIETY. Like extraversion, anxiety effects may
cussed. Table 26.2 summarizes the cognitive cor- be characterized in terms of a cognitive patterning
relates of extraversion-introversion. At least some (for reviews see Eysenck, 1992; Wells & Matthews,
of these effects, especially the associations with 1994; Zeidner, 1998). In theory, anxiety may affect
attentional and memory functions, appear suffi- cognitive performance at each of the stages involved
ciently robust to represent individual differences in processing information (i.e., encoding of new in-
in architecture, although there is little direct formation, short- and long-term storage, elaboration
evidence. Superficially, at least, there appears to and processing of encoded material, retrieval of con-
be overlap between some of these cognitive corre- tent from long-term memory, and problem solving).
lates and correlates of intelligence. For example, in- Thus, anxiety may produce a narrowing of atten-
troverts appear more intelligent in their superior tion and increased distractibility, affecting encod-
reflective problem solving, whereas extraverts re- ing of intelligence test information and tasks partic-
semble intelligent individuals in their superior dual- ularly sensitive to distractibility (e.g., digit-symbol
task performance (e.g., Stankov, 1989). At the least, coding). Anxiety may impair the efficiency of short-
then, individual differences in some aspects of per- term storage and tasks such as digit span. The short-
formance may be sensitive to both personality and term memory tasks most sensitive to anxiety tend
intelligence. However, more fine-grained analyses to be those that also require active processing of
tend to suggest that the specific processes sensi- information (working-memory tasks) and may re-
tive to extraversion and intelligence are often dif- quire attentional resources as well as short-term re-
ferent from one another. For example, extraverts' tention. Intelligence tests vary in their demands
dual-task superiority may reflect greater availabil- on attentional resources and working memory, and
ity of resources (Eysenck, 1981), especially for ver- thus impairment in these information-processing
bal tasks. For intelligence, though, the crucial pro- functions does not necessarily result in impairment
cesses appear to be the control processes handling on any given intelligence test (Matthews & Dorn,
the additional complexity associated with dual-task 1995). We would expect emotional disturbance to be
performance (Spilsbury, Stankov, & Roberts, 1990). most detrimental to tests making high demands on
The cognitive correlates of extraversion are highly controlled processing, attention, or working mem-
sensitive to contextual factors such as level of stim- ory, such as complex arithmetic calculations per-
ulation (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1985) and motiva- formed without paper and pencil. Anxiety may also
tional signals (Derryberry & Reed, 1994). As previ- affect students' long term memory, such that highly

Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. Boston University Theology Library, on 21 May 2017 at 22:14:51, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511807947.027
INTELLIGENCE AND PERSONALITY 595

anxious subjects would show a greater retention loss in resource availability (an architectural construct)
over time (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1985). may be influenced by arousal (a neural construct),
Anxiety effects are not limited to deficits. Anxi- for example.
ety is also associated with negative biases in judg- In some respects, though, architectural explana-
ment and with selective attention to threat (Wells & tions may not be just limited but wrong because
Matthews, 1994; Zeidner, 1998). It may facilitate they neglect volitional strategy choice. Individual
performance of easy tasks (Zeidner, 1998). Again, differences in strategy may reflect either the ar-
there is some uncertainty over whether anxiety chitectural components supporting strategy imple-
effects should be linked to the cognitive architec- mentation or the person's choices for dealing with
ture (Matthews & Wells, in press). The cognitive in- task demands. In the latter case, we must invoke
terference hypothesis for anxiety effects (Zeidner, the knowledge-level explanations discussed in the
1998) implies that anxious individuals are not gen- next section. It is often difficult to ascertain which
erally lacking in attentional resources or working- type of explanation is appropriate. Extraverts' de-
memory capacity; they simply allocate it to off-task ficiencies in certain kinds of problem solving may
processing. On the other hand, there may be more be attributed to use of "impulsive exit" strategies
fundamental differences in some specific attentional (Weinman, 1987). One explanation is that the com-
functions such as attentional focusing and disen- putations used in deciding whether a valid solu-
gagement (Derryberry & Reed, 1997). Similarly, the tion has obtained are deficient in extraverts (archi-
nature of attentional bias in anxiety is controversial. tectural explanation). Alternatively, extraverts value
According to Williams et al. (1988), anxiety is associ- speed relative to accuracy and so are at risk for mak-
ated with individual differences in preattentive pro- ing errors on the tasks concerned (knowledge ex-
cesses controlling threat evaluation, which are ap- planation). In the next section, we explore the role
parently a property of the architecture. Matthews of strategy as a construct bridging architectural and
and Wells (in press) point to numerous demonstra- adaptive levels of explanation.
tions that attentional bias effects are sensitive to
contextual and strategic factors. They argue that bias Adaptive Processes
reflects a coping strategy of monitoring for threat. At Pylyshyn's (1984) knowledge level, we are con-
cerned with the processes of intentional adapta-
LIMITATIONS OF INFORMATION-PROCESSING tion to the demands of the external environment
MODELS. There is little dispute that intelligence (cf. Lazarus, 1991). "Adaptation" here refers to the
and personality traits may conjointly influence person's volitional management of real-world op-
many aspects of performance. Even intelligence test portunities and threats and should be distinguished
performance is sensitive to personality when con- from adaptation in the evolutionary sense. Do per-
textual factors such as level of stimulation and sonality and intelligence relate to common adap-
time pressure are taken into account (e.g., Revelle tive goals or to common adaptive outcomes? The
et al., 1980). Cognitive psychological studies have motivational aspect of personality is commonplace:
been invaluable in generating detailed information- Consider the social motivations of extraverts or the
processing models of these findings. However, such concerns with self-protection of the anxious indi-
models are often rather superficial in their explana- vidual, for example. Motivational components of
tions (Matthews, 1997a). The simplest explanation intelligence are less obvious. Motivation involves in-
is that individual difference factors influence multi- vestment of time and energy in the pursuit of per-
ple parameters of the cognitive architecture in such sonal goals. In general, intelligence consists of abili-
a way that intelligence and personality factors re- ties or aptitudes that help people achieve their basic
late to differing cognitive patterning. Such expla- long- and short-term goals or reach certain perfor-
nations may be adequate for explaining the perfor- mance standards. Hence, intelligence is drawn into
mance data, although, in the larger picture, they the domain of goal satisfaction and thus becomes
may require supplementation with neural explana- more linked with general motivational dispositions.
tions. Humphreys and Revelle (1984) and Necka For example, the studies of openness previously de-
(in press) have discussed how individual differences scribed show that intelligent individuals are more

Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. Boston University Theology Library, on 21 May 2017 at 22:14:51, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511807947.027
596 MOSHE ZEIDNER AND GERALD MATTHEWS

likely to have intellectual interests. In this section, by preliminary exposure to uncontrollable aver-
we review the evidence relating to motivational pro- sive stimuli. Thus, people who have been success-
cesses, their control by larger self-regulative systems, ful at using their abilities come to believe in their
and the implementation of control through strategy abilities to control their destiny, and thus the lo-
selection. cus of causality may shape the development of
intelligence. However, we do not really know for
MOTIVATION AND ENVIRONMENTAL CON- sure what the causal direction is. Do attributions
TROL. A fundamental requirement for adaptation affect cognitive test performance, or do less intelli-
is a degree of control over the external environ- gent people tend to rely more on luck and accidental
ment. To achieve environmental control, individu- or external factors than their own abilities?
als spend considerable time and effort assessing en- Achievement motivation designates a general
vironmental contingencies and contextual changes striving to perform one's best when the following
as well as analyzing their own powers to cope and two conditions hold: (a) the quality of one's per-
excel in relevant contexts (cf. Zeidner & Endler, formance is judged in terms of success or failure,
1996). Attribution theory (Weiner, 1973) posits that (b) a relevant standard of excellence applies (Weiner,
an essential motivation is to gain a measure of pre- 1973). It may relate to people's need to seek a sense
dictability and control over events with emphasis of control over their environment, which is often
on gaining an accurate appraisal of one's personal referred to as a motivation for "effectance." Accord-
characteristics. Locus of causality is an attributional ing to achievement motivation theory (Atkinson,
dimension. Individuals with an internal locus of 1964), all individuals have a basic motive to ap-
causality assume that their own behaviors and ac- proach an achievement-related goal and an an-
tions are responsible for the consequences that hap- tagonistic motive to avoid failure. The strengths
pen to them. Externally controlled people, by con- of these motives vary from person to person and
trast, believe that the locus of causality is out of situation to situation. Atkinson (1964) described
their hands and subject to the whims of fate. Inter- in detail how motives and expectancies influence
nal locus of causality, especially for positive events, approach-avoidance conflict between the two op-
has commonly been associated with positive mental posing tendencies, the stronger of the two tenden-
health and more adaptive functioning. Conversely, cies being expressed in action.
those who feel at the mercy of their environment Achievement strivings are a defining character-
and have little control over what happens to them istic of the Conscientiousness (C) trait of the
may have low adaptive capacity. Five-Factor Model (Costa & McCrae, 1992). Some
Intelligence may affect the process of appraising authors have found it useful to distinguish achieve-
personal control by allowing more complex reason- ment striving from the orderliness component of
ing and consideration of alternatives and choice C (Hough, 1992). Achievement motivation may in-
among options, thus affording greater control dur- fluence intellectual development and performance
ing both primary and secondary appraisal (Zeidner, in one of several ways. First, achievement motiva-
1995). Furthermore, people who believe they can tion may determine the level of interest, striving,
control their own lives will put forth the effort and effort that persons invest in the development
to gain competencies and skills, thus enhancing of their intellectual skills throughout all their life
their acquired or crystallized intelligence and abil- experiences prior to the test. Second, achievement
ities. Consistent with these hypotheses, intelligence motivation may help shape the level of attention,
has been reported to be moderately and inversely effort, concentration, and persistence applied in the
correlated with external locus of causality in ju- evaluative or test situations. Recent research, in fact,
nior and high school students (Samuel, 1980). Also, suggests that a student's motivation, as expressed by
some studies (Seligman, 1975) have specifically re- basic interest in different school subjects, is signifi-
lated deficits in mean IQ shown by Black examinees cantly related to cognitive abilities and achievement
to the phenomena of low controllability (learned (Snow, 1989). Third, intelligence may shape the per-
helplessness). Their motivation to cope with envi- son's motives. Thus, a person high in quantitative
ronmental contingencies is drastically diminished ability is likely to develop a motive to achieve in

Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. Boston University Theology Library, on 21 May 2017 at 22:14:51, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511807947.027
INTELLIGENCE AND PERSONALITY 597

math. Furthermore, highly intelligent students de- 1991). Loosely, we can distinguish two aspects of
velop a strong motivation to acquire or develop var- self-regulation. Its more passive, state-oriented
ious intellectual skills tapped by IQ tests and to per- aspect refers to the person's self-representations and
form well in a testing situation. subsumes both self-appraisals and the goals of self-
In fact, some reviews have reported modest pos- regulation. Its more action-oriented aspect (self-
itive relations between need for achievement and agency) refers to beliefs about the coping strategies
intelligence, but others conclude the correlation used to meet those goals. Both aspects have been
is not statistically reliable (see Zeidner, 1995 for linked to intelligence.
review). Achievement motivation research may un-
derestimate the context dependence of motivation. SELF-REPRESENTATIONS: SELF-CONCEPT AND
Successful entrepreneurs who left school with few SELF-ESTEEM. A need for a positive self-concept or
qualifications may be highly motivated to suc- self-regard develops universally. People's self-image
ceed - but not at schoolwork. Achievement striv- (how people come to see themselves) and their self-
ing predicts a variety of indices of occupational per- esteem (to what degree they feel positive about
formance, as does the C trait (Barrick & Mount, themselves) are crucial in determining their per-
1991; Matthews, 1997c). The possibility of nonlin- sonal goals, perceptions, and behaviors. The earliest
ear relationships has also been rather neglected. and most general aspects of the self concept develop
Basic motivation theory predicts that need for in the interaction between the child and parent fig-
achievement will show a linear relation to perfor- ures. Continuing changes in the self-concept should
mance outcomes when measured as cumulative aca- take place as a consequence of later interactions as
demic performance but a curvilinear relationship well. Both theory and past research support the view
when immediate learning performance is measured that a positive self-concept and high self-esteem are
(Snow, 1989). Thus, the optimum immediate learn- related to higher academic ability and attainment.
ing should come from those students with mid- Negative beliefs about the self are associated with
dle positions on need for achievement. Motivation lower ability, scholastic underachievement, and fail-
theory further predicts that only persons of aver- ure (Purkey, 1970). Children who think of them-
age intelligence should be strongly motivated to selves poorly are likely to underestimate their abil-
achieve or avoid failure, depending on the relative ity, anticipate failure, and may well stop trying when
strengths of their motivations for success or moti- difficulties arise.
vation to avoid failure (Weiner, 1972). Hence, per- The causal dynamics in the assumed relation-
formance differences as a function of motivation ship between intelligence and self-concept are am-
may tend to be confined to groups intermediate in biguous. One commonly held view is that the
ability (Atkinson, 1974). Contrary to this hypothe- causality flows from intelligence to self-esteem.
sis, Heckhausen (1967) found that motivation was Accordingly, positive self-concept and adjustment
predictive of IQ only in high-ability groups. Kanfer only reflect past achievement and a person's subjec-
(1990) suggested that the critical moderating factor tive appraisals of his or her own social or educational
is self-perceived ability. Motivation is beneficial only standing and scholastic aptitudes. In fact, a causal
when ability is high, focusing attention on attain- modeling study by Maroyama, Rubin, and Kings-
ing performance goals. Low-perceived ability tends bury, (1981) suggested that self-esteem at age 12
to lead to goal abandonment and performance im- is significantly predicted by ability at age 7. This
pairment. suggests that the causal direction flows from abil-
ity to self-esteem. If correct, then efforts to improve
SELF-REGULATION. A more process-oriented adjustment would have little effect on ability and
analysis of individual differences in motivation and achievement.
adaptation is provided by work on self-regulation.
Self-regulation refers to people's management of SELF-AGENCY. According to current social-cog-
their transactions with the environment under the nitive theory, human action is governed not so
guidance of cybernetic control loops that seek to much by the objective properties of the environ-
align actual and ideal self-states (Carver & Scheier, ment but by the perceived level of personal efficacy

Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. Boston University Theology Library, on 21 May 2017 at 22:14:51, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511807947.027
598 MOSHE ZEIDNER AND GERALD MATTHEWS

to effect changes by productive use of capabilities Unking Adaptation to the Architecture:


and enlistment of sustained effort (Bandura, 1977). The Role of Strategies
The term self-efficacy is used to refer to the belief The research reviewed so far makes some broad
of being able to master challenging demands by predictions about how motivational and self-
means of adaptive action. Self-efficacy can be con- regulative aspects of personality may influence in-
ceptualized as a "can-do" cognition that mirrors a telligence test performance. However, predicting
sense of control over the environment or as an op- effects on specific intelligent behaviors requires in-
timistic view of one's capability to deal with stress tegration of the adaptive and cognitive architectural
and anxiety. Recent research from a social-cognitive levels of explanation via the concept of strategy.
perspective points to a meaningful relationship be- First, we may seek to link the executive functions
tween self-efficacy and academic ability and attain- used in controlling intelligent behavior to self-
ment (Bandura, 1986). Students who view ability as regulation. Perhaps the more intelligent person is
reflecting an acquirable skill foster a resilient sense better able to attain personal goals in consequence.
of self-efficacy. Thus, students who regard intellec- Second, we may investigate how personality factors
tual ability as a skill that can be acquired by gaining influence strategy choice and the consequences of
knowledge and competencies tend to adopt func- these choices for intellectual functioning. This ap-
tional learning goals. These students seek challenges proach provides a fresh perspective on effects of anx-
that provide opportunities to expand their knowl- iety and depression on performance.
edge and competencies. They regard errors as a nat-
ural part of the acquisition process rather than a EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS IN SELF-REGULATION
source of distress. Self-efficacy varies dynamically AND INTELLIGENCE. One aspect of intelligence
with the person's experience of performing the task, relates to cognitive self-regulatory processes ("meta-
depending on the goals the person sets and their per- cognitive" or "executive functions'') that control a
ceived success in attaining them (Kanfer, 1990). person's cognitive functioning. A wide range of ac-
Ego resiliency is a somewhat similar construct: the tivities are included under the rubric of cognitive
capacity of the individual to modulate and moni- self-regulation. These include reflecting on the na-
tor an ever-changing complex of desires and real- ture of the problem at hand, planning activities,
ity constraints effectively (Block & Kremen, 1996). predicting consequences of one's actions, monitor-
A basic premise undergirding this conceptualization ing ongoing activities, checking results of one's ac-
is that the human goal is to be as undercontrolled tions, testing for plausibility, and reflecting on one'
as possible and as overcontrolled as necessary. Thus, actions (Flavell, 1985). Self-regulatory strategies and
ego resiliency refers to the dynamic capacity of an "metacognitive knowledge" are believed to be pur-
individual to modify a characteristic level of ego posely constructed from the knowledge and expe-
control in either direction (i.e., overcontrol or un- rience one has acquired with respect to one's own
dercontrol). This dynamic capacity is viewed as a problem-solving behavior (Flavell, 1985).
function of the demand characteristics of the en- Increased intelligence is often believed to be
vironmental context so as to preserve or enhance associated with benefits in strategy regulation
system equilibration (Block & Kremen, 1996). The (Borkowski & Peck, 1986). The working methods of
ego-resilient person is adaptively attuned to the sur- highly intelligent subjects are often claimed to be
rounding psychosocial environment, and thus he or dominated by well-developed metacognitive strate-
she experiences feelings of zest for life, self-esteem, gies that are highly integrated into their rich cog-
and harmony with others. Ego resiliency has ele- nitive repertoires. The three metacognitive skills
ments of self-efficacy but places less emphasis on di- perhaps most closely aligned with intelligence are
rect environmental control as the route to success- (a) formation of a clear mental representation of
ful adaptation. Ego resiliency, as an index of human learning-performance goals, (b) the capacity to de-
adaptability, might be expected to relate to cognitive vise and revise a plan of action, and (c) the abil-
function. Indeed, Block and Kremen (1996) found a ity to monitor one's behavior to detect mismatches
significant relationship between IQ and a measure between goals and standards and present behaviors
of ego resiliency among male students. (Boekaerts, in press). Other theoretical accounts of

Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. Boston University Theology Library, on 21 May 2017 at 22:14:51, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511807947.027
INTELLIGENCE AND PERSONALITY 599

the link between metacognitive strategies and intel- Schwean et al., in press). Emotional expressions
ligence have been provided by Sternberg (1985) and of stress are closely linked to appraisals and cop-
Snow and Lohman (1984). ing tendencies. There has been much speculation
The person's use of metacognitive strategies but rather little evidence on the relationship be-
should contribute to his or her adaptation to tween intelligence and stress reactions. Intelligence
real-world environments through self-regulation of may be seen as a resource that facilitates personal
the attainment of personally salient goals (Kanfer, growth and adjustment and possibly buffers the
1989). Self-regulation efficiency, that is, the ability effects of environmental stress on stress reactions
to pursue one's goals despite alternative action ten- (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Possibly, intelligence
dencies, has been hypothesized to be an important leads to more positive appraisals through more ac-
variable subsumed under g (Kuhl & Kraska, 1989). curate and flexible assessments of demanding situ-
Thus, whereas more intelligent students are hy- ations. Furthermore, more intelligent persons may
pothesized to use self-regulatory skills (actively set- have better problem-solving skills used in deciding
ting goals, planning, monitoring their performance how to change themselves or their environment
during problem solving, etc.), less intelligent stu- (cf. Payne, 1991). Haan (1977) found that changes
dents rely more on the task and environmental cues in intelligence from early adolescence to middle
than self-regulatory cues to provide them with these adulthood correlate positively with increased us-
structures. These hypotheses have been supported, age of coping mechanisms but correlate negatively
in part, by recent research evidencing that external with defense mechanisms. However, intelligence is
regulation may enhance the learning and perfor- not necessarily beneficial in stressful situations: The
mance of low-ability students. By contrast, self- more intelligent person may be able to work out
regulation may interfere with the self-regulatory ac- negative consequences of a difficult situation that
tivities of high-ability students (Veenman & Elshout, would not occur to a less intelligent person. Cogni-
1995). Unfortunately, researchers have experienced tive resource theory (Fiedler & Garcia, 1987) has set
considerable difficulties distinguishing the con- forth several interesting hypotheses relating leader-
structs used in conceptualizing self-regulation (or ship behavior under stress to individual differences
self-regulated learning) from those employed in in leader intelligence. Accordingly, stress is assumed
more traditional theories of intelligence (Zeidner, to have a distracting effect on performance and lim-
Boekaerts, & Pintrich, in preparation). Researchers its the leader's ability to make use of his or her cog-
are still unclear whether these constructs should be nitive resources. A major hypothesis of this theory
conceptualized as independent constructs, overlap- states that when a leader is under stress, his or her in-
ping constructs, or hierarchically related constructs tellectual abilities will be diverted from the task. As a
(with self-regulation subsumed under the ability do- result, measures of leader intelligence will not corre-
main). Second, a review of the empirical literature late (or even correlate inversely!) with group perfor-
suggests that the actual role of intelligence (with mance under stressful conditions. Intelligent lead-
populations in the normal to gifted range) in strat- ers may be better able than less intelligent leaders
egy regulation is either weak or inconsistent and to foresee the consequences of failure, which makes
is still a matter of debate (Alexander & Schwanen- them less able to function (Fiedler & Garcia, 1987).
flugel, 1994). Studies of personality factors related to stress vul-
nerability, such as anxiety, demonstrate the strat-
STRESS, MOTIVATION, AND STRATEGY USE. egy choices that may influence intelligent behav-
Current transactional perspectives on emotion ior under stress (Matthews & Wells, 1996). Wells &
(Lazarus, 1991) see stress as maladaptation to the Matthews' (1994) model of attention and emo-
perceived demands of the environment. Stress arises tion, the Self-Regulative Executive Function (S-REF)
when the perceived demands of the environment model, describes how choice of coping strategy is
tax or exceed the individual's perceived capacity influenced by self-knowledge in states of distress.
to cope with these demands (Lazarus & Folkman, Matthews and Wells (in press) discuss how the
1984). Stress may also be seen as a consequence of model interrelates knowledge level and architectural
ineffective or misguided self-regulation (Matthews, concerns. At the knowledge level, the S-REF model

Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. Boston University Theology Library, on 21 May 2017 at 22:14:51, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511807947.027
600 MOSHE ZEIDNER AND GERALD MATTHEWS

states that coping is influenced by self-regulatory than to observed performance effectiveness. The
goals, such as self-protection from threats (gen- anxious individual may be compared with a car hav-
eralized anxiety), reevaluation of personal failure ing a trailer hooked up to it. Additional acceleration
(depression), maintenance of mental or physical is needed to reach a given level of speed compared
health (panic disorder), and avoidance of con- with a car without a trailer. However, deficits in
tamination (obsessive-compulsive disorder). As the active strategy use have been observed in trait-
transactional theory of stress specifies (Lazarus & anxious subjects and neurotic subjects (Mueller,
Folkman, 1984), coping also derives from individ- 1992), implying that anxious individuals may some-
ual evaluations of the demands of the situation and times have difficulty applying compensatory effort
of personal ability to implement accessible coping adaptively. In addition, the task-focused coping of
strategies. The model also describes the cognitive ar- anxious individuals may be applied to the "wrong"
chitecture that supports self-regulation and coping: task. Selective attention to threat in anxiety may
An executive system integrates intrusions from "au- be a consequence of using the task-focused coping
tomatic' ' processing with plans for coping retrieved strategy of monitoring for threat (see Matthews
from a stable store of self-knowledge. & Harley, 1996, for a computational model). This
In states of distress, the coping strategy selected "hypervigilance" for threat (Eysenck, 1992) may
is often "emotion-focused": The person may choose distract from the overt task at hand.
to replace task-focused goals by self-regulatory goals
that generate worry and self-scrutiny (Matthews, STRATEGIES AND ADAPTATION. In summary,
Schwean et al., in press). In other words, cognitive the knowledge or adaptive level brings together
interference reflects maladaptive self-regulation. In several interrelated constructs: motivation, self-
the case of depression, there is considerable evidence regulation, and stress processes such as appraisal
that lack of task motivation contributes to perfor- and coping. Intelligence and personality may over-
mance deficit (Johnson & Magaro, 1987). The in- lap in terms of adaptive goals and outcomes. Adap-
tellectual performance of depressives is impaired by tive goals are perhaps primarily an attribute of per-
self-focus of attention (Strack, Blaney, Ganellen, & sonality, but achievement-related goals appear to
Coyne, 1985) and improved by a distracting sec- relate to intelligence also. Personality and intelli-
ondary task. Foulds (1952) showed that depressives' gence may interact also as influences on adaptive
deficit on the Porteus Maze intelligence test was success. Both may be linked to constructs such as
eliminated when they also had to repeat digits back self-esteem, self-efficacy, and coping, though there
to the experimenter. This result is inconsistent with is a shortage of evidence. Understanding the adap-
a simple resource-deficit explanation, which would tive processes involved requires an understanding of
suppose that the extra task would increase the re- how self-regulation is implemented by the cognitive
quirement for resources and disadvantage the de- architecture through selecting and executing strate-
pressives further. Similarly, studies of driver stress gies. Research should aim to specify the executive
have shown that negative emotions are most dam- and coping routines involved in both adaptive and
aging to single-task driving; stress-vulnerable drivers information-processing terms. Such research is in its
appear to be able to mobilize sufficient effort to initial stages, but some progress has been made in re-
deal with dual-task situations (Matthews, Sparkes, & lating the performance consequences of anxiety and
Bygrave, 1996). depression to choice and implementation of coping
The role of coping in anxiety is more controversial strategies.
because there is no clear overall motivational deficit.
Indeed, Eysenck & Calvo (1992) argued that anxious The Longer View: A Cognitive-Adaptive
individuals may be motivated to compensate for Framework
their perceived deficit in performance through in- The previous section showed how explaining the
creased compensatory effort. Anxious subjects may effects of anxiety on intellectual function within ex-
counter cognitive interference resulting from emo- perimental settings requires analysis of both the in-
tion focus with increased task focus. Hence, anxiety formation-processing routines involved and their
may relate more strongly to processing efficiency adaptive significance. Matthews (1997d, in press)

Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. Boston University Theology Library, on 21 May 2017 at 22:14:51, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511807947.027
INTELLIGENCE AND PERSONALITY 601

has developed a more general cognitive-adap-


/•"" Adaptation/Trait
tive framework that integrates the different lev-
Arousal
els of explanation over longer time periods. In it functions^. "Knowledge"
part, individual differences reflect the relatively Genes - • Neural / 1 Skills
- Motivations
- Efficacy beliefs
stable influences of genes and early learning,
feeding forward into neural systems controlling
nonspecific arousal (biological level of expla- '
r \
\ Information/^
v•
• Emotions

Early ' processing


nation) and into information-processing func-
learning
tions (cognitive architecture level). This tool kit Real-world
of elementary functions provides the basis for adaptive
behaviors
subsequent skill acquisition: real-world adap-
tation depends on somewhat context-bound skills FIGURE 26.2. A cognitive-adaptive framework for personality

(remembering conversations) rather than on basic and ability (Matthews, in press).


functions (short-term memory).
The right-hand side of Figure 26.2 illustrates the are supported both by acquired skills built on the
dynamic interplay at the adaptive level of explana- platform provided by the cognitive architecture and
tion. "Knowledge" refers to the package of psycho- by knowledge. For example, extraverts' processing
logical functions involved in self-regulation, which functions equip them to learn how to converse ef-
are, of course, discriminated in more fine-grained fectively, to deal with challenging social encounters,
models (e.g., Wells & Matthews, 1994). On the one and to act rapidly. At the knowledge level, extraverts
hand, successful adaptation reflects the application have confidence in their own abilities, which feeds
of the skills the individual is predisposed to acquire. into more effective coping in demanding environ-
Ensuing real-world success then feeds into congru- ments (Matthews, 1997b, in press). Conversely, in-
ent self-knowledge - motivation and confidence in troverts are adapted to environments characterized
application of skill - which in turn is likely to en- by infrequent stimuli, such as working as a writer
hance actual skill. On the other hand, adaptation or artist, within which they may capitalize on skills
is partially driven by knowledge itself, given that for sustained attention, reflective problem solving,
people may have unrealistic beliefs about the per- and tolerance of monotony. Intelligence is perhaps
sonal competencies. Self-beliefs and motivations in- more strongly influenced by feed-forward from
fluence the environments to which people choose basic neural and cognitive processing functions
to expose themselves, and, hence, the skills they than is personality and could be identified with the
have the opportunity to learn. Normally, there will acquired reasoning skills used for solving intelli-
be some positive feedback between the triangle of gence test problems, influenced by processing-level
components to the extent that people gravitate to- constructs such as speed of information processing.
wards environmental niches that suit their skills and The cognitive-adaptive approach provides a fresh
interests. perspective on relationships between personality
Table 26.3 shows how the three individual dif- and intelligence. The two kinds of individual differ-
ference constructs, g, E, and N, may relate to the ence factors are most sharply discriminated at the
cognitive-adaptive framework. Ability and per- adaptive level, at which each dimension represents
sonality traits are conceptualized as bundles of cognitive fitness for a different type of environment.
computationally independent processing functions However, the skills supporting different adaptations
that jointly support adaptation of the individual use some common processing building blocks. For
to specific informational environments. Intelligent example, a good short-term memory may be used to
individuals are adapted to novelty and complexity cultivate skills in fluent but nonintellectual conver-
(cf. Sternberg, 1985), extraverts to social and informa- sation, an extraverted skill, or for more intellectual
tionally demanding environments, and more neu- problem-solving skills related to g. In addition,
rotic individuals to environments in which threats specific real-world environments may sometimes
are subtle or disguised, and thus hypervigilance for relate to multiple adaptations. For example, training
threat (Eysenck, 1992) is required. These adaptations environments tend to possess both novelty and

Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. Boston University Theology Library, on 21 May 2017 at 22:14:51, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511807947.027
602 MOSHE ZEIDNER AND GERALD MATTHEWS

TABLE 26.3. Relationship between Three individual Difference Factors and Components of the
Cognitive-Adaptive Framework

Anxiety/
intelligence Extraversion Neuroticism
Arousal ? Cortico—recticular Autonomic arousal ? (1)
functions arousability
Sensitivity to reward Sensitivity to
signals? punishment signals?
Information Processing speed (+) Dual task (+) Working memory (-)
processing Working memory (+) Short-term memory (+) Attentional
Executive functions (+) Response criterion (—) resources (—)
Vigilance (+) Judgment (negative bias)
Problem solving (-) Selective attention
Arousal-dependent effects (negative bias)
Skills Intellectual skills Conversation Awareness of danger
- handling complexity Rapidity of action Safety behaviors
— handling novelty Social encounters More "substantive"
- transfer across decision making (1)
context
Knowledge Achievement Social motivation Self-protective
motivation Self-efficacy motivations
Positive affect
Self-efficacy? Task-focused coping Low Self-efficacy
Intellectual interest Negative affect
Emotion-focused
coping
Domain-specific
knowledge (2)
Real-world Academic and High-pressure Covert threats (4)
adaptation educational occupations (3)
environments Dating and mating
1. Relates to state anxiety only.
2. Knowledge referring to specific sources of threat: e.g., social or physical threat.
3. Introverts adapted to activities offering little external stimulation: e.g., literary and scientific work.
4. Emotionally stable individuals adapted to overtly threatening environments.

cognitive complexity (congruent with g) and po- It is readily apparent that an individual who is
tential information overload (congruent with E). high in both intelligence and some personality fac-
Hence, job performance during training relates not tor such as Neuroticism or Extraversion will think,
just to g but also to E, although E does not relate feel, and act differently from another person who
to job performance in general (Barrick & Mount, has high intelligence but is very low in N or E,
1991). In other words, personality may sometimes respectively. The different information processing
relate to the context-bound skills or practical intelli- characteristics of extraverts and introverts may not
gence (Sternberg, 1985) required for specific environ- only underlie behavioral differences but also suggest
ments. Extraverts function more intelligently than the kinds of environments (e.g., vocational, social,
introverts in the environments congruent with the interpersonal) they are most likely to seek out and
trait. However, the underlying cognitive basis for function best in. More traditional views of intelli-
this practical intelligence is not reasoning ability gence may be somewhat limited in describing the in-
but the acquired information-management skills re- terface between personality and intelligence and in
quired for handling overload and social encounters. describing how these two broad domains contribute

Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. Boston University Theology Library, on 21 May 2017 at 22:14:51, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511807947.027
INTELLIGENCE AND PERSONALITY 603

to a description of human behavior and individual improving Research Design, Measurement,


differences. and Analysis
Further work is needed on the interrelations be-
DIRECTIONS FOR FUTURE RESEARCH tween different individual difference constructs and
the underlying processes to which they relate (Snow,
The integration of research and assessment in 1995). Various advances in design and analysis are
the ability, personality, and motivational domains ripe for application to the personality-intelligence
would appear to be an important goal for individual interface such as modern confirmatory factor anal-
differences research in the 21st century. To achieve ysis, structural equation modeling, and multilevel
this goal, we conclude this chapter by high- modeling techniques (Boyle et al., 1995). Research
lighting several potentially useful directions for would also benefit from using modern scaling tech-
future research on the personality-intelligence in- niques such as univariate or multivariate Item Re-
terface recently suggested by Zeidner (1997). sponse Theory (IRT) models in constructing affec-
tive assessment instruments. IRT technology has
More Refined Conceptualization been available for some time now, but few individ-
and Taxonomies of Constructs ual difference researchers have taken advantage of
More thought needs to be given to the conceptu- modern test theory in constructing unidimensional
alization of the personality-intelligence constructs scales (Most & Zeidner, 1995).
and domains of discourse. Even quite loose provi- Within the psychometric approach, there is scope
sional classification structures may help guide ex- for testing for nonlinear relations as well as for mul-
ploration and provide a useful framework to which tiplicative functions introducing both linear and
to pin individual data as they accumulate. The grow- quadratic functions of intelligence and motivational
ing consensus about the multidimensional nature predictors of performance.
of the ability domain (Carroll, 1993), together with
more recent advances in the taxonomy of personal- Clearer Specification of the Meaning and
ity (McCrae & Costa, 1997) and conative aptitudes Nature of the interaction between
(cf. Corno, 1997), provides some solid ground for es- intelligence and Personality
tablishing personality-intelligence relations. Much work needs to be done on modeling the
To that end, it may be useful to employ a complexity of personality-ability interactions while
facet-analytic approach to the investigation of the maintaining clarity and testability of hypotheses
personality-intelligence interface by constructing a (Snow, 1995). Personality-intelligence interactions
matrix with ability constructs (/') represented by may take many forms and may reflect different hy-
rows and personality constructs (k) represented by potheses about particular causal mechanisms and
columns and the entire two-dimensional matrix types of interactive effects and mechanisms pre-
(/' x k), or Cartesian space, representing the do- sumed to be operative. We need more longitudi-
main of discourse for any future integrative attempt. nal research modeling the dynamic transactions be-
A third facet, area of application (school, occupa- tween personality and intellectual variables over
tion, military) may be added to form a three-faceted time via such procedures as structural equation
cubic model. Furthermore, we need more theory- modeling.
driven research to allow us to make focused pre-
dictions of the relationship between intelligence Identifying Key Bridging Concepts
and personality. Thus, the optimal approach is to The psychometric approach tends to provide
look at well-established theories concerning perfor- personality traits that are sharply discriminated
mance data and articulate focused hypotheses to from intelligence. Perhaps a more productive search
test, arrange experimental conditions to test deduc- would be to examine a gray area between intelli-
tions from these theories, use appropriate measures gence and general personality, the domain of "cog-
to test hypotheses, and interpret results cautiously nitive" or "intellectual" styles (Messick, 1996). A va-
in the light of the theories in question (Eysenck, riety of stylistic concepts may provide a useful bridge
1994). between affect and cognition and a means by which

Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. Boston University Theology Library, on 21 May 2017 at 22:14:51, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511807947.027
604 MOSHE ZEIDNER AND GERALD MATTHEWS

to conceptualize the intersection and crossroads be- telligence variables in the process of psychodiagno-
tween personality, conation, and intelligence do- sis and decision making. More research is needed
mains. One of the difficulties in past research has on the considerations practitioners bring to bear in
been the application of inappropriate measurement making decisions based on the integration between
models. Most measures of style have inappropri- affective and ability constructs.
ately followed the ability factor model and have As previously discussed, intelligence may relate to
yielded scores that are unipolar and value-directed one's current level of organization and integrative
rather than bipolar and value-differentiated. In fact, functioning. Hence, intelligence may play an impor-
styles should be concerned with not "how much," tant role in the prognosis of maladjusted individ-
but "how/' One of the important contributions uals. In fact, there is some research to support the
psychometricians can make to our field is through claim that intelligence is a significant predictor of
improved measurement of stylistic variables as a success in psychological treatment and therapy, al-
potential bridge between personality and intelli- though not all studies substantiate the link (Zeidner,
gence variables. Recent research suggests that in- Matthews, & Saklofske, 1998).
telligence would be most closely and organically
related to personality variables that reflect typical
ways of dealing with information, and it might be CONCLUSIONS
useful to have another careful and methodologi- This review suggests a rather complex recipro-
cally sounder look at both old and new stylistic cal relationship between intelligence and a vari-
variables. Several stylistic variables, such as "mental ety of personality factors in which cognitive and
self-government" (Sternberg & Grigorenko, 1995), affective-motivational variables dynamically im-
"prudence" (Haslam & Baron, 1994), "wisdom" pact upon each other in the course of development
(Maciel, Heckhausen, & Baltes, 1994), "mindful- and day-to-day behavior. On one hand, personal-
ness" (Brown & Langer, 1990), and "intellectual ab- ity dispositions and motivational factors may in-
sorption" (Mayer, Caruso, Agiler, & Dreyden, 1989) fluence intellectual functioning. It is important to
provide contemporary and dynamic examples of distinguish the effects personality may have on per-
bridging concepts linking personality and intelli- formance, in the short term, from effects on com-
gence. petence in the longer term. There is solid empirical
evidence for various negative emotional and moti-
ln-Depth Research in Practical vational states tending to impair intellectual perfor-
and Clinical Settings mance to a moderate degree, especially when the
Personality and intelligence variables are often task demands attention or working memory. It is
used jointly for decision-making purposes in var- likely that both loss of functional resources and mo-
ious practical domains. By necessity, the greatest tivational processes contribute to intellectual im-
amount of integration of cognitive and personality pairment in both emotional disorders and in sub-
variables takes place in professional and applied set- clinical stress states. Conversely, strategies driven by
tings. Research models such as Cronbach and Snow's motivational factors such as self-efficacy and need
(1977) Aptitude x Treatment Interaction attempted to achieve may serve to maintain or enhance per-
to demonstrate empirically how characteristics of formance. Some of these effects may be context-
the individual such as intelligence and personal- dependent in that they are contingent upon stress
ity (e.g., extraversion, anxiety) interacted with dif- or arousal. It is less clear whether personality factors
ferent possible treatments (e.g., therapies, instruc- affect basic competence in addition to the person's
tional strategies, teacher characteristics) to produce performance on specific occasions. Certain person-
particular outcomes. The clinical, vocational, coun- ality traits, such as poor self-concept and aggres-
seling and school psychologist performs formal and sion, may act over extended periods of time to de-
informal ATIs each time a client is seen for assess- press intellectual functioning by reducing a person's
ment and diagnosis. However, we know very little motivation to acquire and develop specific intellec-
about how practitioners and clinicians conduct the tual skills. Psychotic conditions and personality dis-
integration between personality, conative, and in- orders too may affect both temporary performance

Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. Boston University Theology Library, on 21 May 2017 at 22:14:51, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511807947.027
INTELLIGENCE AND PERSONALITY 605

and underlying competence. We have used a cog- the skills acquired to handle the context concerned.
nitive science framework to suggest that cognitive, Personality assessment tells us something about the
adaptive, and (possibly) neural explanations may all situations in which the individual will behave more
be necessary in understanding the processes control- or less "intelligently".
ling these effects. Current trends in individual differences research
On the other hand, intelligence may impact upon allow for the hope of an integrated understanding
personality and affective states through encouraging of the ways that "each person is like all other
more positive cognitions of personal competence. people, some other people, and no other people"
Thus, individuals high in intellectual functioning (Revelle, 1995). A truly integrative science of per-
are frequently shown to be better adjusted, both sonality would help clear from our path some of the
socially and emotionally, than their less intelligent debris of disciplinary provincialism. However, there
counterparts. High intelligence can aid a student in is a difference between simple eclecticism and an
learning the realities of general and school culture integrated model that forms the basis for a unified
and the physical world as part of an integrated learn- approach to, and rapprochement with, the domains
ing process and help the person acquire more so- of conation intelligence. Such an approach would
cially desirable traits. Overall, intelligence may serve not simply look at a person from intellectual and
as a personal resource that can facilitate personal motivational or affective perspectives but instead
growth and adjustment and also serve as a buffer integrate data from these perspectives. A true inte-
against the crippling effects of psychological stress gration would certainly move us a step forward in
and disease (cf. Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Lazarus, understanding both normal as well as pathological
1991). Conversely, more or less veridical appraisals states, and this appears to be a major challenge for
of negative outcomes on intellectual tasks may con- individual differences research in the 21st century.
tribute to the development of negative affectivity.
From a practical point of view, personality vari-
ables seldom bear such a sizable impact on intel- REFERENCES
lectual performance as to invalidate intelligence as- Ackerman, P. L.; & Heggestad, E. D. (1997). Intelligence,
sessments or test scores as a whole. The impact of personality, and interests: Evidence for overlapping
various personality and conative factors affecting traits. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 219-245.
performance (e.g., anxiety, motivation) may in fact Adorno, T. W., Frenkel-Brunswick, E., Levinson, D. J., &
be viewed as key aspects of the individual's global Sanford, R. N. (1950). The authoritarian personality. New
intellectual capacity (Matarazzo, 1972; Wechsler, York: Harper & Row.
1944). Arguably, individuals who do poorly on in- Alexander, J. M., & Schwanenflugel, P. J. (1994). Strategy
regulation: The role of intelligence, metacognitive attri-
telligence tests because of the debilitating effects butions, and knowledge base. Developmental Psychology,
of certain personality factors (e.g., high test anx- 30, 709-723.
iety, low motivation) may also do poorly on the Anastasi, A. (1958). Differential psychology. New York:
criterion measure of performance - and for much Macmillan.
the same reasons. The cognitive-adaptive frame- Anastasi, A. (1988). Psychological testing (4th ed.). New York:
work presented in this chapter emphasizes both in- Macmillan.
formation processing and the expression of individ- Anderson, M. (1992). Intelligence and development: A cogni-
tive theory. Oxford: Blackwell.
ual differences in real-world skills. It leaves open
Anthony, W. S. (1973). The development of extroversion,
causal questions concerning the origin of individ- of ability, and of the relation between them. British Jour-
ual differences in feed-forward from genes and feed- nal of Educational Psychology, 43, 223-227.
back from person-situation interactions. Plausibly, Argyle, M. (1961). Religious behavior. Chicago: Free Press.
it may be the dynamic interplay between biologi- Atkinson, J. W. (1974). An introduction to motivation. Prince-
cally based predispositions and the varying opportu- ton, NJ: Van Nostrand.
nities provided by the environment that determine Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action:
A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliff, NJ: Prentice
personality and intelligence and relationships be-
Hall.
tween them. Specifically, personality may relate to Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory
context-bound practical intelligence supported by of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191-215.

Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. Boston University Theology Library, on 21 May 2017 at 22:14:51, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511807947.027
606 MOSHE ZEIDNER AND GERALD MATTHEWS

Barrick, M. R.; & Mount, M. K. (1991). The Big Five person- Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1986). Personality stabil-
ality dimensions and job performance: a meta-analysis. ity and its implications for clinical psychology. Clinical
Personnel Psychology, 44, 1-26. Psychology Review, 1, 144-149.
Beech, A., & Williams, L. (1997). Investigating cognitive Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEOpersonality
processes in schizotypal personality and schizophrenia. inventory and NEO five factor inventory. Professional man-
In G. Matthews (Ed.), Cognitive science perspectives on per- ual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.
sonality and emotion. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science. Crookes, T. G., Pearson, P. R., Francis, L. J., & Carter, M.
Block, J. (1995). On the relation between IQ, impulsivity, (1981). Extraversion and performance on Raven's Pro-
and delinquency: Remarks on the Lynam, Moffitt, and gressive Matrices in 15—16 year old children. British Jour-
Stouthamer-Loeber (1993) interpretation. Journal of Ab- nal of Educational Psychology, 51, 109-111.
normal Psychology, 104, 395-398. Cronbach, L. J., & Snow, R. E. (1977). Aptitudes and instruc-
Block, J., & Kremen, A. M. (1996). IQ and ego-resiliency: tional methods. New York: Irvington.
Conceptual and empirical connections and separate- Daugherty, S. A., Henry, B. E., Peterson, D. L, Swarts, R. L.,
ness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 349— Bastien, S., & Thomas, R. S. (1991). Chronic fatigue syn-
361. drome in Northern Nevada. Reviews ofInfectious Diseases,
Boekaerts, M. (in press). Self-regulated learning at the junc- 13, pp. 539—44 (supplement).
tion of cognition and motivation. European Psychologist Deary, I. J., & Stough, C. (1996). Intelligence and inspec-
Borkowski, J. G., & Peck, V. A. (1986). Causes and con- tion time: Achievements, prospects and problems. Amer-
sequences of metamemory in gifted children. In R. ican Psychologist, 51, 599-608.
Sternberg & J. Davidson (Eds.), Conceptions of giftedness Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (1994). Temperament and
(pp. 182-200). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University attention: Orienting toward and away from positive and
Press. negative signals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
Bown, J., & Langer, E. (1990). Mindfulness and intelli- ogy, 66, 1128-1139.
gence: A comparison. Educational Psychologist, 25, 305— Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (1997). Motivational and
335. attentional components of personality. In G. Matthews
Boyle, G. J., Stankov, L., & Cattell, R. B. (1995). Measure- (Ed.), Cognitive science perspectives on personality and emo-
ment and statistical models in the study of personal- tion. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.
ity and intelligence. In D. H. Saklofske & M. Zeidner Endler, N. S., & Summerfeldt, L. J. (1995). Intelligence,
(Eds.), International handbook of personality and intelli- personality, psychopathology, and adjustment. In D. H.
gence (pp. 417—446). New York: Plenum. Saklofske & M. Zeidner (Eds.), International handbook
Brody, N. (1992). Intelligence (2nd ed.). New York: Academic of personality and intelligence (pp. 249-284). New York:
Press. Plenum.
Bullock, W. A., & Gilliland, K. (1993). Eysenck's arousal Eysenck, H. J. (1994). Personality and intelligence: Psycho-
theory of introversion—extraversion: A converging mea- metric and experimental approaches. In R. J. Sternberg
sures investigation. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- & P. Ruzgis (Eds.), Personality and intelligence (pp. 3-31).
chology, 64, 113-123. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities. New York: Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, M. W (1985). Personality and in-
Cambridge University Press. dividual differences: A natural science approach. New York:
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1981). Attention and self- Plenum.
regulation: A control-theory approach to human behavior. Eysenck, H. J., & Cookson, D. (1969). Personality in pri-
Berlin: Springer-Verlag. mary school children - Ability and achievement. British
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. E (1991). A control-process Journal of Educational Psychology, 39, 109-130.
perspective on anxiety. In R. Schwarzer & R. A. Wick- Eysenck, M. W. (1981). Learning, memory and personal-
land (Eds.), Anxiety and self focused attention (pp. 3—8). ity. In H. J. Eysenck (Ed.), A model for personality. Berlin:
London: Harwood Publishers. Springer-Verlag.
Cattell, R. B. (1971). Abilities: Their structure, growth and ac- Eysenck, M. W. (1982). Attention and arousal: Cognition and
tion. New York: Houghton Mifflin. performance. New York: Springer-Verlag.
Christie, R. (1954). Authoritarianism re-examined. In R. Eysenck, M. W. (1992). Anxiety: The cognitive perspective.
Christie and M. Jahoda (Eds.), Studies in the scope and Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
method of "The Authoritarian Personality." New York: Eysenck, M. W., & Calvo, M. G. (1992). Anxiety and perfor-
Free Press. mance: The processing efficiency theory. Cognition and
Coleman, J. C, Butcher, J. N., & Carson, R. C. (1984). Ab- Emotion, 6, 409-434.
normal psychology (7th ed.). Scott, Foresman, & Co.: NY. Ferrara, R. P., Brown, A. L, & Campione, J. C. (1986). Chil-
Corno, L. (1997, July). Conative individual differences in dren's learning and transfer of inductive reasoning rules:
learning. Paper presented at the Second Spearman Semi- Studies of proximal development. Child Development, 57,
nar on Intelligence and Personality, Plymouth, England. 1087-1099.

Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. Boston University Theology Library, on 21 May 2017 at 22:14:51, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511807947.027
INTELLIGENCE AND PERSONALITY 607

Fiedler, F. E., & Garcia, J. E. (1987). New approaches to ef- between individual differences and information process-
fective leardership: Cognitive researches and organizational ing. Psychological Review, 91, 153-184.
performance. New York: Wiley. Hunt, E. B. (1978). Mechanics of verbal ability. Psychological
Flavell, J. H. (1985). Cognitive development. Englewood Review, 85, 109-130.
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Jensen, A. (1980). Bias in Mental Testing. New York: Free
Foulds, G. A. (1952). Temperamental differences in Press.
maze performance II: The effect of distraction and of Johnson, M. H., & Magaro, P. A. (1987). Effects of mood and
electroconvulsive therapy on psychomotor retardation. severity on memory processes in depression and mania.
British Journal of Psychiatry, 43, 33-41. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 28-40.
Goff, M., & Ackerman, P. L. (1992). Personality-intelli- Jones, W. H., Rusell, D. W., & Nickel, T. (1977). Belief in the
gence relations: Assessment of typical intellectual en- paranormal scale: An objective instrument to measure
gagement. Journal ofEducational Psychology, 84, 537-552. belief in magical phenomena and causes. JSAS Catalog of
Gray, J. A. (1982). The neuropsychology of anxiety: An inquiry selected documents in psychology, 7, 100.
into the functions of the septo—hippocampal system. Oxford: Kanfer, R. (1989). Conative processes, dispositions, and be-
Oxford University Press. havior: Connecting the dots within and across para-
Grigorenko, E. L., & Sternberg, R. J. (1995). In D. Saklofske, digms. In R. Kanfer, P. L. Ackerman, & R. Cudeck (Eds.),
& M. Zeidner (Eds.). (1995). Thinking styles. International Abilities, motivation, and methodology (pp. 375-388).
handbook of personality and intelligence (pp. 205—229). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
New York: Plenum. Kanfer, R. (1990). Motivation and individual differences in
Gustafsson, J. (1997, July). On the hierarchical structure of learning: An integration of developmental, differential
personality and ability. Paper presented at the 2nd Spear- and cognitive perspectives. Learning and Individual Dif-
man Seminar, Plymouth, England. ferences, 2, 221-239.
Haan, N. (1977). Coping and defending: Processes of self- Kuhl, J., & Kraska, K. (1989). Self-regulation and meta-
environmental organization. New York: Academic Press. motivation: Computational mechanisms, development,
Hamlin, R. M. (1963). The stability of intellectual function- and assessment. In R. Kanfer, P. L. Ackerman, & R.
ing in chronic schizophrenia. Journal ofNervous and Men- Cudeck (Eds.), Abilities, motivation, and methodology
tal Disease, 136, 360-364. (pp. 343-374). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hartlage, S., Alloy, L. B., Vazquez, C, & Dykman, B. (1993). Kyllonen, P. (1997, July). Learning, cognition, personality,
Automatic and effortful processing in depression. Psy- and emotion. Paper presented at the 2nd Spearman
chological Bulletin, 113, 247-278. Seminar, Plymouth, England.
Haslam, N., & Baron, J. (1994). Intelligence, personality, Kyllonen, P. C, & Christal, R. E. (1990). Reasoning ability
and prudence. In R. J. Sternberg & P. Ruzgis (Eds.), Person- is (little more than) working-memory capacity?! Intelli-
ality and intelligence (pp. 32—60). New York: Cambridge gence, 14, 389-433.
University Press. Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York:
Heckhausen (1967). The anatomy of achievement motivation. Oxford University Press.
New York: Academic Press. Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Coping and adapta-
Heffner, P. A., Strauss, M. E., & Grisell, J. (1975). Rehospi- tion. In W. D. Gentry (Ed.), The handbook of behavioral
talization of schizophrenics as a function of intelligence. medicine (pp. 282-325). New York: Guilford.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 84, 735—736. LeDoux, J. E. (1995). In search of an emotional system in
Hembree, R. (1988). Correlates, causes, effects and treat- the brain: Leaping from fear to emotion and conscious-
ment of test anxiety. Review of Educational Research, 58, ness. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences
47-77. (pp. 1049-1062). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Hofstee, W. (1997, July). Conceptual issues in the relation be- Loehlin, J. C. (1992). Genes and environment in personality
tween personality and intelligence. Paper presented at the development. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Second Spearman Seminar, Plymouth, UK. Lynam, D., Moffitt, T., & Stouthame-Loeber, L. M.
Hope, D. A., & Heimberg, R. G. (1990). Dating anxiety. In (1993). Explaining the relation between IQ and delin-
H. Leitenberg (Ed.), Handbook of social and evaluative anx- quency: Class, race, test motivation, school failure, or
iety (pp. 217-246). New York: Plenum Press. self control? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 187-
Hough, L. M. (1992). The "Big Five" personality variables 196.
construct confusion: Description versus prediction. Hu- Maciel, A. G., Heckhausen, J., & Baltes, P. B. (1994). A
man Performance, 5, 139-155. life-span perspective on the interface between person-
Huesman, L. R., Eron, L. D., & Yarmel, P. W. (1987). Intel- ality and intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg & P. Ruzgis
lectual functioning and aggression. Journal of Personality (Eds.), Personality and intelligence (pp. 61—103). New York:
and Social Psychology, 52, 218-231. Cambridge University Press.
Humphreys, M. S., & Revelle, W. (1984). Personality, mo- Maroyama, G., Rubin, R. A., & Kingsbury, G. G. (1981).
tivation and performance: A theory of the relationship Self-esteem and educational achievement: Independent

Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. Boston University Theology Library, on 21 May 2017 at 22:14:51, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511807947.027
608 MOSHE ZEIDNER AND GERALD MATTHEWS

constructs with a common cause. Journal of Personality Matthews, G., & Harley, T. A. (1996). Connectionist models
and Social Psychology, 40, 962-975. of emotional distress and attentional bias. Cognition and
Matarazzo, J. D. (1972). Wechsler's measurement and ap- Emotion, 10, 561-600.
praisal of adult intelligence (5th ed.). Baltimore: Williams Matthews, G., Saklofske, D. H., Costa, P. T, Jr., Deary, I. J., &
& Wilkins. Zeidner, M. (1998). Dimensional models of personality:
Matthews, G. (1986). The effects of anxiety on intellectual A framework for systematic clinical assessment. European
performance: When and why are they found? Journal of Journal of Psychological Assessment, 14, 36-49.
Research in Personality, 20, 385-401. Matthews, G., Schwean, V. L., Campbell, S. E., Saklofske,
Matthews, G. (1992). Extraversion. In A. P. Smith & D. M. D. H., & Mohamed, A. A. R. (1999). Personality, self-
Jones (Eds.), Handbook of human performance. Vol. 3: State regulation and adaptation: A cognitive-social frame-
and trait, pp. 95—126. London: Academic. work. In M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.),
Matthews, G. (1997a). An introduction to the cognitive sci- Handbook of self-regulation. New York: Academic.
ence of personality and emotion. In G. Matthews (Ed.), Matthews, G., & Wells, A. (1999). The cognitive science
Cognitive science perspectives on personality and emotion. of attention and emotion. In T. Dalgleish & M. Power
Amsterdam: Elsevier Science. (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 171—191),
Matthews, G. (1997b). Extraversion, emotion and perfor- New York: Wiley.
mance: A cognitive-adaptive model. In G. Matthews Matthews, G., Sparkes, T. J., & Bygrave, H. M. (1996).
(Ed.), Cognitive science perspectives on personality and emo- Stress, attentional overload and simulated driving per-
tion (pp. 339-342) Amsterdam: Elsevier Science. formance. Human Performance, 9, 77—101.
Matthews, G. (1997c). The Big Five as a framework for per- Mayer, J. D., Caruso, D. R., Zigler, E. & Dreyden, J. I. (1989).
sonality assessment. In N. Anderson & P. Herriot (Eds.), Intelligence and intelligence-related traits. Intelligence,
International handbook of selection and appraisal (2nd ed.), 13, 119-133.
pp. 175-200. London: Wiley. McCrae, R. R. (1987). Creativity, divergent thinking, and
Matthews, G. (1997d). Intelligence, personality and infor- openness to experience. Journal of Personality and Social
mation processing: An adaptive perspective. In W. Tomic Psychology, 52, 1258-1265.
& J. Kingsma (Eds.), Advances in cognition and educational McCrae, R. R. (1994). Openness to experience: Expanding
practice. Volume 4: Reflections on the concept of intelligence, the boundaries of Factor V. European Journal ofPersonality.
pp. 475-492. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. 13, 39-55.
Matthews, G. (1999). Personality and skill: A cognitive- McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T, Jr. (1997). Personality trait
adaptive framework. In P. L. Ackerman, P. C. Kyllonen, structure as a human universal. American Psychologist, 53,
& R. D. Roberts (Eds.), The future of learning and in- 509-516.
dividual differences research: Processes, traits, and content Messick, S. (1996). Bridging cognition and personality in ed-
(pp. 251-273) Washington, DC: American Psychologi- ucation: The role of style in performance and development.
cal Association. Research Report, Educational Testing Service.
Matthews, G., & Amelang, M. (1993). Extraversion, arousal Mesulam, M. M. (1985). Principles of behavioral neurology.
theory and performance: A study of individual differ- Philadelphia: F. A. Davis.
ences in the EEG. Personality and Individual Differences, Mischel, W. (1968). Personality and assessment. New York:
14, 347-364. John Wiley.
Matthews, G., & Davies, D. R. (in press). Arousal and vig- Morris, L. W., & Liebert, R. M. (1969). Effects of anxiety
ilance: The role of task factors. In R. B. Hoffman, M. E on timed and untimed intelligence tests: Another look.
Sherrick, & J. S. Warm (Eds.), Integrating perception across Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 33, 240-
psychology. Perspectives on motivation, choice, and individ- 244.
ual differences (pp. 113-144), Washington, DC: American Most, B., & Zeidner, M. (1995). Constructing personality
Psychological Association. and intelligence test instruments: Methods and issues. In
Matthews, G., & Dorn, L. (1995). Personality and in- D. Saklofske & M. Zeidner (Eds.). International handbook
telligence: Cognitive and attentional processes. In D. of personality and intelligence (pp. 475—503). New York:
Saklofske & M. Zeidner (Eds.), International handbook Plenum.
of personality and intelligence (pp. 367—396). New York: Mueller, J. H. (1992). Anxiety and performance. In A. P.
Plenum. Smith & D. M. Jones (Eds.), Handbook of human perfor-
Matthews, G., & Gilliland, K. (1999). The personality theo- mance (3rd ed.) (pp. 127-160). London: Academic Press.
ries of H. J. Eysenck and J. A. Gray: A comparative review. Necka, E. (1997). Attention, working memory and arousal:
Personality and Individual Differences, 26, 583-626. Concepts apt to account for the process of intelligence.
Matthews, G., & Harley, T. A. (1993). Effects of extraversion In G. Matthews (Ed.), Cognitive science perspectives on per-
and self-report arousal on semantic priming: A connec- sonality and emotion. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.
tionist approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- Neubauer, A. C. (1997). The mental speed approach to the
ogy, 65, 735-756. assessment of intelligence. In W. Tomic & J. Kingsma

Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. Boston University Theology Library, on 21 May 2017 at 22:14:51, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511807947.027
INTELLIGENCE AND PERSONALITY 609

(Eds.), Advances in cognition and educational practice. Schwartzman, A. E., & Douglas, V. I. (1962). Intellectual
Volume 4: Reflections on the concept of intelligence. loss in schizophrenics. Part I. Canadian Journal ofPsychol-
Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. ogy, 16, 1-10.
Nevo, B. (1998). Human intelligence. Tel-Aviv: Open Univer- Schwean, V. L., & Saklofske, D. H. (1995). A cognitive-social
sity Press (in Hebrew). description of exceptional children. In D. H. Saklofske
Nunnally, J. C. (1978). Psychometric theory. New York: & M. Zeidner (Eds.), International handbook ofpersonality
McGraw-Hill. and intelligence (pp. 185-204). New York: Plenum.
Payne, R. (1991). Individual differences in cognition and Siegman, A. W. (1956). The effect of manifest anxiety on
the stress process. In C. L. Cooper & R. Payne (Ed.), a concept formation task, a nondirected learning task,
Personality and stress: Individual differences in the stress pro- and on timed and untimed intelligence tests. Journal of
cess. New York: John Wiley. Consulting Psychology, 20, 176-178.
Purkey, W. W. (1970). Self-concept and school achievement. Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Helplessness. San Francisco:
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Freeman.
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1984). Computation and cognition: Toward Seipp, B. (1991). Anxiety and academic performance: A
a foundation for cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT meta-analysis of findings. Anxiety Research, 4, 27--41.
Press. Snow, R. (1989). Cognitive-conative aptitude interac-
Rawlings, D., & Carnie, D. (1989). The interaction of tions in learning. In R. Kanfer, P. L. Ackerman, &
EPQ extraversion with WAIS subtest performance under R. Cudeck (Eds.), Abilities, motivation, and methodology
timed and untimed conditions. Personality and Individual (pp. 435-474). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Differences, 10, 453-458. Snow, R. (1995). Foreword. In D. H. Saklofske & M. Zeidner
Revelle, W. (1995). Personality processes, Annual Review of (Eds.), International handbook of personality and intelli-
Psychology, 46, 295-328. gence (pp. 11-15). New York: Plenum.
Revelle, W., Amaral, P. & Turriff, S. (1976). Introversion/ Snow, R., & Lohman, D. F. (1984). Toward a theory of cog-
extraversion, time stress, and caffeine: Effects on verbal nitive aptitude for learning from instruction. Journal of
test performance. Science, 192, 149—150. Educational Psychology, 76, 347—376.
Rolfhus, E. L., & Ackerman, P. L. (1996). Self-report knowl- Snow, R. E., Killonen, P. C, & Marshalek, B. (1984). The
edge: At the crossroads of ability, interest and personal- topography of ability and learning correlations. In. R. J.
ity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 174-188. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intel-
Revelle, W., Humpheys, M. S., Simon, L., & Gilliland, K. ligence (Vol. 2). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
(1980). The interactive effects of personality, time of day, Spielberger, C. D., Gonzales, H. P., Taylor, C. J., Algaze, B.,
and caffeine: A test of the arousal model. Journal of Ex- & Anton, W. D. (1978). Examination stress and test anx-
perimental Psychology: General, 109, 1-31. iety. In C. D. Spielberger & I. G. Sarason (Eds.), Stress and
Robinson, D. L. (1985). How personality relates to intel- anxiety (Vol. 5, pp. 167-191). New York: Wiley.
ligence test performance: Implications for a theory of Stankov, L. (1989). Attentional resources and intelligence:
intelligence, aging research, and personality assessment. A disappearing link. Personality and Individual Differences,
Personality and Individual Differences, 6, 203-216. 10, 957-968.
Saklofske, D. H., & Kostura, D. D. (1990). Extraversion- Spilsbury, G., Stankov, L., & Roberts, R. (1990). The effect
intraversion and intelligence. Personality and Individual of a test's difficulty on its correlation with intelligence.
Differences, 11, 547-551. Personality and Individual Differences, 11, 1069-1077.
Saklofske, D. H., & Zeidner, M. (Eds.). (1995). Interna- Tett, R. P., Jackson, D. N., & Rothstein, M. (1991). Person-
tional handbook of personality and intelligence. New York: ality measures as predictors of job performance: A meta-
Plenum. analytic review. Personnel Psychology, 44, 703-742.
Samuel, W. (1980). Mood and personality correlates of IQ Trapnell, P. D. (1994). Openness versus intellect: A lexical
by race and sex of subject. Journal ofPersonality and Social left turn. European Journal of Personality, 8, 273-290.
Psychology, 38, 993-1004. Stanley, J. C, Keating, D. P., & Fox, L. H. (1974). Mathemat-
Sarason, I. G. (1975). Test anxiety, attention, and the gen- ical talent: Discovery, description, development. Baltimore:
eral problem of anxiety. In C. D. Spielberger & I. G. John Hopkins University Press.
Sarason (Eds.), Stress and anxiety (Vol. 1, pp. 165-210). Stelmack, R. M., & Houlihan, M. (1995). Event-related
New York: Hemisphere/Halstead. potentials, personality, and intelligence. In D. H.
Sarason, I. G. (1980). (Ed.). Test anxiety: Theory, research and Saklofske & M. Zeidner (Eds.), International handbook
applications. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. of personality and Intelligence (pp. 349—366). New York:
Saucier, G. (1994). Trapnell versus the lexical factor: Much Plenum.
ado about nothing? European Journal of Personality, 8, Strack, S., Blaney, P. H., Ganellen, R. J., & Coyne,
291-298. J. C. (1985). Pessimistic self-preoccupation, performance
Sattler, J. M. (1988). Assessment of children. (3rd ed.). San deficits and depression. Journal of Personality and Social
Diego: Author. Psychology, 49, 1076-1085.

Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. Boston University Theology Library, on 21 May 2017 at 22:14:51, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511807947.027
610 MOSHE ZEIDNER AND GERALD MATTHEWS

Sternberg, R. J. (1977). Intelligence, information process- Weiner, B. (1972). Theories of motivation: From mechanisms
ing, and analogical reasoning. The componential analysis to cognition. London: Markham.
of human ability. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Weiner, B. (1973). Theories of motivation. Chicago: Rand
Sternberg, R. J. (1985). Beyond IQ: A triarchic theory of McNally.
human intelligence. New York: Cambridge University Wells, A., & Matthews, G. (1994). Attention and emotion: A
Press. clinical perspective.: Hillsdale, NJ: LEA.
Sternberg, R. J., & Ruzgis, P. (Eds.). (1994). Personality and Willerman, L. (1979). The psychology of individual and group
intelligence. New York: Cambridge University Press. differences. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman & Co.
Terman, L. M. (Ed.). (1935). Mental and physical traits of Weinman, J. (1987). Non-cognitive determinants of per-
a thousand gifted children. Volume I. Stanford: Stanford ceptual problem-solving strategies. Personality and Indi-
University Press. vidual Differences, 8, 53-58.
Terman, L. M., & Oden, M. H. (1947). The gifted child Williams, J. M. G., Watts, F. N., MacLeod, C, & Mathews,
grows up. In L. M. Terman (Ed.). Genetic studies of genius A. (1988). Cognitive psychology and emotional disorders.
(Vol 4). Stanford: Stanford University Press. Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Thompson, R. C, & Michel, J. B. (1972). Measuring au- Zeidner, M. (1995). Personality trait correlates of intelli-
thoritarianism: A comparison of the F and D scales. Jour- gence. In D. Saklofske & M. Zeidner (Eds.), International
nal of Personality, 40, 180-190. handbook of personality and intelligence (pp. 299-319).
Thorndike, R. L. (1982). Applied psychometrics. Boston: New York: Plenum.
Houghton Mifflin. Zeidner, M. (1997, July). Personality, conation, and intelli-
Veenman, M. V. J., & Elshout, J. J. (1995). Differential ef- gence: Directions for future research. Paper presented at
fects of instructional support on learning in simulation the 2nd Spearman Seminar, Plymouth, England.
environments. Instructional Science, 22, 363-383. Zeidner, M. (1998). Test anxiety: The state of the art. New
Watson, C. G., Herder, J., Kucala, T, & Hoodecheck-Schow, York: Plenum.
E. (1987). Intellectual deterioration and personality de- Zeidner, M., Boekaerts, M., & Pintrich, P. (in press). Self-
compensation in schizophrenia. Journal of Clinical Psy- regulation: Current perspectives and directions for fu-
chology, 43, 447-455. ture research. In M. Boekaerts, P. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner
Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1992). On traits and tempera- (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation. New York: Academic
ment: General and specific factors of emotional experi- Press.
ence and their relation to the five-factor model. Journal Zeidner, M., & Endler, N. (Eds.), Handbook of coping: Theory,
of Personality, 60, 441-476. research, applications. New York: Wiley.
Wechsler, D. (1944). The measurement of adult intelligence Zeidner, M., Matthews, M., & Saklofske, D. H. (1998). Intel-
(3rd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins. ligence and mental health. Encyclopedia ofMental Health.
Wechsler, D. (1950). Cognitive, conative, and nonintellec- New York: Academic Press.
tive intelligence. American Psychologist, 5, 78-83. Zusne, L., & Jones, W. H. (1982). Anomalistic psychology: A
Wechsler, D. (1958). The measurement and appraisal of adult study of extraordinary phenomena, of behavior, and experi-
intelligence (4th ed.). Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins. ence. Hillsdale, NJ: LEA.

Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. Boston University Theology Library, on 21 May 2017 at 22:14:51, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511807947.027

You might also like