You are on page 1of 13

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

2003, Vol. 85, No. 4, 723–735 0022-3514/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.4.723

Relationships Between Personality Structure, Structure of Word Meaning,


and Cognitive Ability: A Study of Cultural Mechanisms of Personality
Aaro Toomela
University of Tartu

Native-born Estonian men (N ⫽ 912), 17– 68 years old, participated in a study on relationships between
personality characteristics, dominant structure of word meaning (“everyday concepts” thinking or
“scientific concepts” thinking), and level of cognitive ability. Individuals who primarily used everyday
concepts thinking or who possessed relatively low levels of cognitive ability did not reveal a coherent Big
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Five personality structure, whereas individuals who primarily used scientific concepts thinking or
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

possessed high levels of cognitive ability did. Thus, personality may be shaped by a cultural factor—
word meaning structure. Earlier studies, which seem to support the idea that Big Five personality
structure is a biologically determined human universal, suffer from serious sampling problems and
insufficient data analyses.

Five-factor theory postulates that five personality traits—Neu- Britt, 1993). Heritability studies have not taken that possibility into
roticism, Extraversion, Openness, Agreeableness, and Conscien- account.
tiousness—are endogenous and biologically determined (McCrae Factors that contribute to the emergence of a personality dimen-
& Costa, 1996, 1999; but see Saucier & Goldberg, 1996, who, like sion and to variation in a personality dimension may be both
many others, understand five-factor models as descriptive summa- intraindividual and environmental. Intraindividually, cognitive
ries rather than endogenous traits). Indeed, some have said that the processes such as thinking, memory, and perception may contrib-
issue of the structure of personality traits has been resolved in ute to the structure and expression of personality dimensions.
personality psychology. As a result, if certain samples fail to reveal There is clearly a need for research that examines the relationship
a five-factor model structure, then the premise of universality or between personality and cognition (Endler, 2000). Several studies
adequacy of the translation and administration of the personality have already demonstrated that personality dimensions are weakly
instrument must be questioned (McCrae, 2000).
but significantly correlated with intelligence (e.g., Allik & Realo,
The idea that major personality dimensions are determined by
1997; Beier & Ackerman, 2001; Paunonen & Ashton, 2001). It is
biological factors is supported by studies of the heritability of
noteworthy that the correlation between two personality do-
personality dimensions. Studies have shown that about 20%–55%
mains—Psychoticism and Neuroticism, as measured by the Ey-
of the total variation in personality dimensions is attributable to
genetic sources (e.g., Jang, McCrae, Angleitner, Riemann, & senck Personality Questionnaire—may be different at different
Livesley, 1998; Pedersen, Plomin, McClearn, & Friberg, 1988). ability levels (Austin et al., 2002). Austin et al. (2002) speculated
The same heritability studies also show that a substantial amount that personality structure might change with ability level. Orthog-
of variation is due to nongenetic factors. In addition, heritability onal dimensions of Psychoticism and Neuroticism may exist
studies probably overestimate the role of genetic factors and un- among the most able, whereas the less able are better described by
derestimate the role of environment (e.g., Funder, 2001; Joseph, a personality structure with nonorthogonal dimensions. Thus, the
2001). Moreover, there is much evidence that human biology is structure of personality may indeed be related to cognitive factors.
shaped profoundly by the social world and vice versa (Cacioppo, The most obvious environmental factor that may shape person-
Berntson, & Sheridan, 2000). Finally, heritability studies try to ality is culture. Studies in more than 20 cultures with languages
“explain” variation in a personality dimension statistically. There from many different language families, however, seem to suggest
are, however, two kinds of variation in psychological constructs. that the five-factor personality trait structure is a human universal.
One is variation in the presence or absence of a construct. The What may vary between cultures is the mean level of a personality
other is variation in the degree of expression of the existing trait (e.g., Costa, Terracciano, & McCrae, 2001; McCrae, 2000,
dimension. Several studies have demonstrated that some persons 2001; McCrae & Costa, 1997). According to other authors, the
may not have particular traits at all (e.g., Baumeister & Tice, 1988; number of factors may be larger than five in some cultures, but the
content of the basic five factors remains the same (e.g., Blumberg,
2001; Cheung et al., 2001). These studies, however, are method-
ologically and conceptually flawed.
This work was supported by Estonia Science Foundation Grant 5388. I First, matched factor structures do not guarantee equivalence
thank Jüri Allik and Aleksander Pulver for sharing their data and knowl-
across cultures because factors may differ in the proportion of
edge, Lawrence T. White for advice, and Kristina Seepter and Mango Meus
for help with data collection and scoring.
variance accounted for in different cultures, different items within
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Aaro a factor may have different loadings, means and variances may
Toomela, Department of Special Education, University of Tartu, Salme 1a, differ across cultures, factors can assume a different meaning
Tartu 50103, Estonia. E-mail: aaro.toomela@ut.ee within the context of questions or items not included in the

723
724 TOOMELA

analysis, and some relevant items may not be included in the factor structure of word meaning. Nevertheless, some researchers have
analysis (Marsella, Dubanoski, Hamada, & Morse, 2000). suggested that different persons may understand words describing
Second, most studied samples are not representative of the personality differently (e.g., Block, 1995; Johnson, 1997). Even
culture. Of 26 cultures studied, only two samples were approxi- more, language may not only be a repository of the set of individ-
mately representative (McCrae, 2001). Most studies use very non- ual differences but language may also constrain descriptions and
representative samples of undergraduates or participants with ex- perceptions of personality (Saucier & Goldberg, 1996).
ceptionally high levels of education, which are the most common Personality psychology on the whole seems to be unaware of the
samples in personality research (Endler & Speer, 1998; Mallon, abundant evidence that the structure of word meaning may be
Kingsley, Affleck, & Tennen, 1998). Such samples are at least systematically different in different people because of the way in
partly Westernized (Poortinga & van Hemert, 2001) so that many which word meanings are constructed. This dimension, first de-
college students may be culturally understood as strangers in their scribed by Lev Vygotsky (Vygotsky, 1934/1996), can be under-
own lands (Marsella et al., 2000). Thus, problems arise when one stood as a boundary to word meaning. I have called such a
leaps from studies of student populations in industrial countries to boundary a topographical constraint (cf. Toomela, 2000, 2003), to
claims of universality of factors and then to national comparisons exclude certain forms of function of a system or restrict processes
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

based on mean levels of self-reported traits (Bock, 2000). to the possibilities compatible with boundaries. In human cultures
Third, replications of the five-factor structure in different cul- of today, the most common types of word meanings are so-called
tures are methodologically biased. The authors of the translations “everyday concepts” (or “complexes”) and “scientific concepts”
of Estonian and Finnish versions of the NEO Personality Inventory (Vygotsky, 1934/1996).
(NEO-PI; Costa & McCrae, 1985), for example, have explicitly Words of the everyday concept type encode information on the
stated their goals: “the factor structure of the developed tests must basis of perceptual attributes of word referents and on the basis of
correspond as closely as possible to the original factor structure of observation of events in everyday life; word meanings are tied to
the NEO-PI [italics added]” (Pulver, Allik, Pulkkinen, & Hä- immediate sensory experiences. Word meaning is, thus, con-
mäläinen, 1995, p. 111). Similar goals seem to characterize other strained by the characteristics and contexts of external referents.
translations of the Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO- As an everyday concept, the expression “efficient person,” for
PI-R) as well. If the primary goal of the researchers is to replicate example, refers to observable external behaviors like “a person
the structure of the original inventory, the result cannot be taken as who built a house, bought an expensive car, and runs a big
reliable evidence for a “human universal.” The result may just as company.” Words of the scientific concept type encode informa-
easily point to a “researchers’ universal.” tion in the language; the structure of word meaning is abstract and
Finally, and perhaps most importantly, culture in all cross- hierarchical, its structure is formally logical, and it does not
cultural studies on the structure of personality has been understood depend on the immediate reflection of reality. “Efficient person” in
in a very specific way. Cultural psychology has two theoretical this case would mean, “A person acting or functioning compe-
wings, sociologically oriented and psychologically oriented (Toma- tently, a person who causes desired effects.” A researcher who
sello, 1996). The sociologically oriented wing emphasizes the builds a novel theory would not be “efficient” in everyday concept
distributed and collective nature of human cognition. In this ap- boundaries because building a theory is often not reflected in
proach, culture is usually defined as a political border/country, external behavior. For example, writing a book after 25 years of
nation, or ethnicity (cf. Toomela, in press). Nearly all cultural observation and thinking (as Charles Darwin did) would not be
studies of personality are representatives of the sociological wing. labeled “efficient” in everyday concept boundaries. The same act,
The psychologically oriented wing emphasizes the role of the however, would refer to extreme efficiency in scientific concept
individual; it assumes that to participate meaningfully in cultural boundaries, where it is understood that an enormous amount of
activities, individuals must bring to the process certain social and information was systematized in Darwin’s The Origin of Species.
cognitive skills. As personality psychology is a psychology of Another important idea is that everyday and scientific structures
individual differences, a sociological conceptualization of culture of word meanings are hierarchically related to each other. A
may be inappropriate for understanding the contribution of culture scientific structure is a hierarchical system of words; the words
to individual personality. that form the initial, lower levels of the hierarchy refer directly to
Taken together, human personality is shaped by cognitive pro- external objects and phenomena. For example, happiness and
cesses as well as culture. The next question is: What specific anger are first encoded in terms of external events and only later
cultural factors shape personality in important ways? Culture, change into scientific concepts through their connection with a
following the cultural– historical school of psychology, is under- common denominator “emotion.”
stood here as “socially shared information that is coded in sym- Space limitations do not allow a thorough discussion, but it is
bols” (Toomela, 1996, p. 298). In this article it is argued that one enough to know that these two types of word meaning structures
of the most influential factors that shapes personality is the struc- have been differentiated in developmental psychology (e.g., Kikas,
ture of word meaning. One reason for this hypothesis is that 1998, 2003; Luria, 1979; Nelson, 2003; Toomela, 2003), in cul-
personality is usually studied with verbal measures— question- tural psychology (e.g., Luria, 1974, 1979; Toomela, 1996, in
naires (Endler & Speer, 1998; Mallon et al., 1998). More gener- press), and in neuropsychology (e.g., Funnell, 2001; Luria, 1973,
ally, the most influential personality accounts today rest on the 1979; Toomela, Tomberg, Orasson, Tikk, & Nômm, 1999).
lexical hypothesis, according to which all important aspects of It is important to note that the structure of word meaning is not
human personality are coded in language (e.g., Digman, 1996; directly related to vocabulary; the same words can encode infor-
McCrae & Costa, 1996, 1999; Saucier & Goldberg, 1996). Costa mation in both ways. Similarly, the particular words in a person’s
and McCrae’s lexical approach to personality ignores the possi- vocabulary do not tell us how the meanings of the words are
bility that there may be profound individual differences in the constructed. If we ask a person to rate himself or herself on the
RELATIONSHIPS BETWEEN PERSONALITY AND WORD MEANING 725

Likert scale of a personality inventory item (e.g., “I am efficient level of education is closely related to cognitive ability. Moreover,
and effective at my work,” from the Competence facet of the word meaning structure and cognitive ability are theoretically
Conscientiousness dimension in the NEO-PI-R), the answer related constructs. Space limitations do not allow a full discussion
strongly agree may derive from very different conceptual struc- of this relationship (see Toomela, 2003, in press; Vygotsky, 1929/
tures and refer to very different ideas. 1994, 1934/1996, for discussions). Briefly, a word meaning struc-
This study is the first direct empirical test of the hypothesis that ture can be understood as a set of mental “tools” used for all kinds
the dominant structure of word meaning is related to different of mental operations. Cognitive ability, in turn, can be understood
structures of personality. Two specific hypotheses were tested. as one’s level of mastery in using those tools. More complex tools
First, it has been demonstrated that high intellectual ability is allow more sophisticated and effective operations. But it is possi-
related to adaptive traits (Austin et al., 2002). Therefore, it was ble to use simple tools for complex operations. It is also possible
hypothesized that persons with different types of dominant word to use complex tools inefficiently and ineffectively. Thus, simple
meaning structure will exhibit different mean levels of personality tools— everyday concepts—sometimes allow complex operations
traits. Specifically, individuals who possess a predominantly sci- but usually are associated with relatively ineffective cognitive
entific concept type of word meaning, who encode information
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

performance. More complex scientific concepts, in turn, can be


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

both about external events and unobservable mechanisms and used inefficiently, but normally are associated with relatively high
ideas behind external events, will be less neurotic because of their levels of cognitive performance. As a result, personality may also
ability to predict and understand the world better. be related to differences in cognitive ability. In this study, I
Second, and more importantly, it was hypothesized that the hypothesize that both word meaning structure and cognitive abil-
basic structure of personality itself may be different in persons ity—as theoretically related, complementary facets of cognition—
with different types of dominant word meaning structures. Such are related to differences in personality dimension levels and
differences may be expressed in the number of personality factors,
personality structure.
but not necessarily. On the one hand, persons with a predominantly
scientific word meaning structure have at their disposal mental
tools to organize their perception of their own personality into a Method
few general hierarchical dimensions. If that were the only impor-
tant aspect of word meaning structure, one would expect the Participants
number of factors to be higher among persons with everyday
conceptual structures because they lack mental tools for creating A sample of physically and mentally healthy adults was constructed to
hierarchical categories of personality dimensions. On the other represent all educational levels. Participants were 912 male members of the
hand, scientific conceptual structures are more differentiated than Estonian military— 407 military recruits, 204 noncommissioned officers,
everyday structures. This is because of the hierarchical relationship and 191 officers. The data were collected in 1999 and 2000. At that time,
between the two types of word meanings. If a whale, for example, university students did not serve in the military before graduation (and
some did not serve at all). Therefore, an additional 62 undergraduate
is encoded as a “mammal” in a scientific hierarchical structure,
students were studied. Finally, 48 participants were members of a volun-
one’s knowledge that whales look and behave like a fish does not
tary paramilitary organization, the Estonian Defense League (Kaitseliit).
disappear. Rather, a more differentiated understanding of a whale All participants were native Estonians, both by nationality and ethnicity,
emerges. As a result, one might find fewer distinguishable factors and all test materials were administered in the Estonian language. At the
in self-report personality measures among persons with dominant time of the study, recruits were selected for the army only on the basis of
everyday conceptual structure because their descriptions of per- medical examinations; no psychological tests were used.
sonality may be less differentiated. Thus, opposite tendencies may Forty-two participants were excluded from the final sample because they
lead to the same number of factors despite different dominant word failed to complete all questionnaire items. The final sample comprised 870
meaning structures. The content of factors, however, would be participants with a mean age of 24.64 years (SD ⫽ 8.14), range 17– 68
substantially different. years. Younger persons were relatively overrepresented in the sample (the
What should distinguish personality structures in individuals median age was 21 years). There were 255 participants with primary
with different types of dominant conceptual structures is the struc- education (9 years or less), 498 participants with secondary education (12
ture itself. It was predicted that in persons with predominantly years), 62 first- or second-year university students, and 55 participants with
a university degree (Bachelor of Science or Bachelor of Arts in nearly all
everyday conceptual structures, the structure of personality does
cases).
not correspond to usual five-factor solutions. It was also predicted
that the five-factor structure of personality as described by Costa
and McCrae (1992) will be found only in persons with a predom- Test Materials and Procedure
inantly scientific structure of word meaning. The rationale for this
hypothesis comes from studies that have found that scientific word Personality inventory. Personality was measured with the Estonian
meaning structures characterize individuals with high levels of version of the NEO-PI-R (Costa & McCrae, 1992). The Estonian NEO-
PI-R (Kallasmaa, Allik, Realo, & McCrae, 2000) was developed on the
Westernized formal schooling (e.g., Cole, 1996; Luria, 1974). As
basis of the Estonian NEO-PI (Pulver et al., 1995). The Estonian version of
the NEO-PI-R findings are based primarily on highly educated
the NEO-PI-R demonstrates high levels of Cronbach alphas of both domain
samples, it is possible that the five-factor structure is “universal” and facet scales (.87–.93 and .56 –.85, respectively), and retest reliabilities
only for that particular stratum of society. are acceptable (across a 2-year testing interval, reliabilities were .67–.86
If these hypotheses about the relationships between dominant and .48 –.81 for domain and facet scales, respectively). The Estonian
type of word meaning structure and personality are supported by NEO-PI-R also reveals five factors as expected, and the extracted factors
the data, an important question emerges. Dominant type of word show reasonable congruence with the original inventory. After Procrustes
meaning structure is closely related to level of education—and rotation (the transformation of a factor matrix to approximate a target
726 TOOMELA

factor matrix defined by the analyst), an excellent fit to the American target not statistically different from a normal distribution (Kolmogorov–
was obtained for all five factors.1 Smirnov d ⫽ .014, ns), ␹2(20, N ⫽ 870) ⫽ 23.12, p ⫽ .28. It
Structure of word meaning. I constructed an original test according to appears that the structure of word meaning varies across individ-
suggestions by Luria (1979) to measure dominant type of word meaning uals and the present sample is representative of the possible
structure. Three complementary measures of word meaning structure were
variation in the population.
used. The first part of the test comprised definitions of eight concepts. Half
of the concepts were concrete (car, hospital), and half of the concepts were It was hypothesized that the structure of word meaning is related
abstract (republic, revolution). The second part comprised nine word pairs; to the level of formal education. Indeed, correlation between years
the participant was asked to describe the most important similarity of the of education and the number of hierarchical answers on the Struc-
concepts. Word pairs varied in the transparency of their similarity—words ture of Word Meaning test was highly significant (r ⫽ .46, p ⬍
referring to items from the same category in one extreme (dog– cat) and .0001).
words referring to objects in complementary relationships in the other Mean levels of personality dimensions. Participants were di-
extreme (head– hat). The third part comprised nine triplets of words. The vided into five groups according to their number of hierarchical
task was to indicate which two words out of three “go together,” and to answers on the Structure of Word Meaning test. The first group
state explicitly why the chosen two words go together.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

consisted of participants who scored more than one standard


All 26 answers were coded into two categories, everyday concepts
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

(coded as 0) or scientific concepts (or “hierarchical” concepts, coded as 1).


deviation below the sample mean. The second group consisted of
The criteria for coding were those proposed by Luria (1979). An answer participants who scored more than one standard deviation above
was coded as an everyday concept when the definition, description of the mean. The remaining participants (who scored between ⫹/⫺
similarity, or definition of commonality described (a) sensory attributes of one standard deviation) were divided into three groups of roughly
objects (e.g., car and bicycle go together because both have round wheels; equal size. These groups are referred to as H1 (1– 6 hierarchical
door and window go together because both are rectangular), (b) observa- answers), H2 (7–9 hierarchical answers), H3 (10 –12 hierarchical
tions of external everyday activities (e.g., school is where children go for answers), H4 (13–15 hierarchical answers), and H5 (16 –25 hier-
learning), (c) observations of everyday situations and connections of ob- archical answers). H1 represents a relatively pure group with a
jects in everyday situations (e.g., potato and carrot go together because
predominantly everyday concept type of word meaning structure,
both grow in the field), (d) the function of the object was described (e.g.,
car is for driving from one place to another), (e) sharing of parts was
and H5 represents a relatively pure group with a predominantly
described (e.g., cat and dog are similar because both have sharp teeth), (f) scientific concept type of word meaning structure. The other
no answer was given. No answer was coded as an everyday concept groups represent relatively mixed word meaning structures.
because some items are not answerable when one lacks scientific (hierar- Mean levels of personality dimensions for participants in the
chical) word meanings. For example, the similarity of “a horse” and “a five word meaning structure groups are shown in Figure 1. An
rider” is not coded in everyday concepts, so a hierarchical response (e.g., analysis of variance (ANOVA) revealed significant main effects
“both are living beings/animals”) is required. When a person whose dom- due to group, F(4, 865) ⫽ 10.74, p ⬍ .0001, and personality
inant structure of word meaning is of everyday concepts is required to domain, F(4, 3460) ⫽ 432.51, p ⬍ .0001, as well as a significant
answer such questions, nothing comes to mind. It should be noted that all
Group ⫻ Domain interaction, F(16, 3460) ⫽ 12.21, p ⬍ .0001.
participants answered at least two items in each of the three parts. There-
Inspection of Figure 1 reveals that the mean differences between
fore, failure to respond cannot be attributed solely to a lack of motivation.
An answer was coded as a scientific (or hierarchical) concept when (a) word meaning structure groups follow a systematic pattern: The
the relationship between words was defined hierarchically (e.g., knife and higher the number of hierarchical answers, the lower the level of
bread are similar because they are both physical objects, drives and opens Neuroticism and the higher the level of all other personality
go together because both are verbs), (b) a word was related to a hierarchi- domains. Post hoc analyses (Scheffé test) revealed that the level of
cally higher level concept (e.g., hospital is a medical institution, revolution Neuroticism was significantly lower in the H5 than in the H1
is a kind of change that is characterized by sudden reorganization of a group ( p ⬍ .0001); levels of Extraversion, Openness, Agreeable-
system). ness, and Conscientiousness were significantly higher in the H5
Responses were coded by two assistants. In addition, protocols of 50
than in the H1 group ( p ⬍ .04 in all cases). Mean scores of
randomly selected participants were simultaneously coded by both assis-
adjacent groups were not significantly different in any of the
tants. Interrater agreement was high; when adjusted for chance, Cohen’s
␬ ⫽ .91. Doubtful cases were coded after discussion between the assistants. personality dimensions. Thus, it appears that the dominant type of
A participant’s dominant structure of word meaning was characterized by word meaning structure affects mean levels of all personality
the sum of all item scores (maximum 26). domains.
Cognitive ability. Cognitive ability was measured with the Estonian Factor structure of the NEO-PI-R. A series of factor analyses
Cognitive Ability Scale (Pulver, 1999). The Cognitive Ability Scale con- were conducted to determine if personality structure is less coher-
tains 36 questions designed to measure verbal, arithmetical–logical, and ent in those participants who provided a small number of hierar-
spatial abilities. The sum of all correct answers on the Cognitive Ability chical responses to the Structure of Word Meaning test and more
Scale provides a measure of the participant’s general level of cognitive coherent in those participants who provided a large number of
ability.
hierarchical responses.
First, the number of extracted factors was analyzed for the entire
Results sample and for the five word meaning structure subgroups. Two
reliable statistical methods for determining the number of factors
Word Meaning Structure Groups to retain—Horn’s parallel analysis (based on mean eigenvalues
Structure of word meaning. The maximum possible range of
scores on the test is 0 –26; in the studied sample, scores between 1 1
NEO-PI-R data for this study, collected as part of the Estonian test
and 25 were observed (M ⫽ 11.17, SD ⫽ 4.66). The distribution construction project, were provided by Jüri Allik, leader of the Estonian
of scores on the Structure of Word Meaning test was unimodal and NEO-PI-R development team.
RELATIONSHIPS BETWEEN PERSONALITY AND WORD MEANING 727
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Figure 1. Mean levels of personality dimensions in different word meaning structure groups. N ⫽ Neuroticism;
E ⫽ Extraversion; O ⫽ Openness to Experience; A ⫽ Agreeableness; C ⫽ Conscientiousness.

of 50 random data sets) and Cattell’s scree test (cf. Zwick & the Estonian version, created with a sample similar to that of the
Velicer, 1986)—were performed. For the entire sample and all original, replicates the original structure very well (Kallasmaa et
subgroups (with the exception of H1), five factors were suggested al., 2000).
by both the scree test and the parallel analysis. In the case of H1, In the present study, the sample was selected to represent the
six factors should be retained by the scree test and four factors by entire range of differences in word meaning structure because
the parallel analysis. So, in almost all cases a five-factor solution different subgroups may exhibit different personality structures.
is the best. The results shown in Table 1 support this hypothesis. In the least
The first five factors accounted for 63.4% of the variance in the hierarchical word meaning structure group, H1, the pattern of
entire sample. In the subgroups, five factors accounted factor loadings does not correspond to the usual five-factor solu-
for 59.3%, 63.3%, 65.4%, 64.6%, and 66.6% of the variance in H1, tion, with the exception of the Neuroticism factor. In the most
H2, H3, H4, and H5, respectively. It should be noted that the hierarchical group, H5, the highest loadings of facet scales, with
proportion of the total variance accounted for by the five factors only three exceptions (N5, O4, and O6), are on the “correct”
systematically increases with the number of hierarchical answers. factors. N5 also loaded on the Conscientiousness factor in the
This result is in agreement with the hypothesis that an everyday Estonian NEO-PI-R (Kallasmaa et al., 2000).
concept word meaning structure may be associated with a less It is noteworthy that, as the number of hierarchical answers on
coherent personality structure. a word meaning structure test increases, the factor structure be-
Finally, the pattern of factor loadings was analyzed for the entire comes closer and closer to the original structure of the NEO-PI-R.
sample and all subgroups. In Table 1, the results of a principal- The relatively chaotic Extraversion, Agreeableness, and Consci-
components analysis with a varimax rotation are shown for the entiousness factors in H1 stabilize in H2 and remain stable in the
entire sample and for the five word meaning structure groups remaining subgroups. The least stable Openness factor becomes
separately. relatively stable in H3 and remains stable in the remaining groups.
In the entire sample, Factor 1 is a Neuroticism–Conscien- The pattern of loadings of Neuroticism facets changes systemati-
tiousness factor; Factor 4 is a relatively pure Neuroticism factor cally in an interesting fashion. Neuroticism is the only factor in
with two Openness facets loading on that factor too; Factor 2 is an which all facets load on the same factor in H1. In the remaining
Extraversion factor; Factor 3 is an Agreeableness factor; and groups, there is always a factor on which all facets of Neuroticism
Factor 5 is an Openness factor (with two facets loading on the load together (with the exception of H4, in which one of six facets
Neuroticism factor instead of on the Openness factor). Overall, the does not load on the same factor). In H2, H3, and H4 Neuroticism
results for the entire sample are not substantially different from the and Conscientiousness load on the same factor. In addition, in H3
original NEO-PI-R with one exception: Neuroticism is split be- and H4 Neuroticism facets load simultaneously on two factors, one
tween two factors, one that is shared with Conscientiousness and shared with Conscientiousness and the other being relatively pure
another that is relatively pure Neuroticism. Although the original Neuroticism.
NEO-PI produces a simple structure, it should be noted that, in the
original version, Neuroticism and Conscientiousness scales were
Cognitive Ability Groups
strongly and negatively correlated (⫺.53; Costa & McCrae, 1992,
Appendix F). At first glance, it appears that the Estonian version of Cognitive ability. The maximum possible range of scores on
the NEO-PI-R may be flawed, but it has been demonstrated that the Cognitive Ability Scale is 0 –36; in the studied sample, scores
728 TOOMELA

Table 1
Factor Loadings of NEO-PI-R Scales After Varimax Rotation, for Entire Sample and for Different Word Meaning Structure Groups

Word meaning structure (H) groupb

Entire sampleb H1 H2
(N ⫽ 870) (N ⫽ 158) (N ⫽ 158)
NEO-PI-R
scalesa 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5

Neuroticism (N)

N1 ⴚ53 ⫺20 ⫺01 ⴚ64 10 ⴚ72 ⫺04 ⫺11 18 02 ⴚ78 ⫺20 ⫺01 21 18
N2 ⴚ51 ⫺17 ⫺24 ⴚ62 00 ⴚ76 ⫺17 13 ⫺17 ⫺14 ⴚ82 ⫺06 ⫺16 22 06
N3 ⴚ55 ⫺31 ⫺04 ⴚ62 06 ⴚ85 ⫺06 ⫺07 ⫺10 01 ⴚ79 ⫺25 ⫺01 25 12
N4 ⴚ51 ⫺29 04 ⴚ60 04 ⴚ79 00 ⫺10 ⫺07 09 ⴚ71 ⫺29 ⫺01 23 17
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

N5 ⴚ74 13 ⫺08 ⫺26 07 ⴚ60 03 ⫺14 24 ⫺37 ⴚ80 07 ⫺03 ⫺20 ⫺05
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

N6 ⴚ67 ⫺39 ⫺04 ⫺34 ⫺14 ⴚ55 ⴚ53 ⫺06 ⫺14 ⫺13 ⴚ70 ⴚ40 ⫺14 ⫺07 ⫺19

Extraversion (E)

E1 28 70 26 16 15 44 41 36 35 ⫺19 37 63 29 02 ⫺03
E2 12 72 04 29 ⫺13 48 14 36 32 ⫺32 29 59 ⫺06 ⫺14 ⫺23
E3 45 59 ⫺32 16 19 31 13 77 ⫺02 ⫺07 38 63 ⫺29 ⫺00 38
E4 43 64 ⫺11 03 13 16 39 61 20 12 38 60 ⫺03 01 24
E5 12 75 ⫺12 ⫺02 12 ⫺08 53 49 33 11 12 72 ⫺04 09 03
E6 07 72 13 34 16 37 38 19 58 ⫺32 10 72 21 ⫺28 ⫺03

Openness to Experience (O)

O1 ⴚ40 17 02 11 61 ⫺36 01 09 71 15 ⫺37 24 11 ⴚ44 32


O2 17 ⫺08 06 08 74 08 01 13 14 77 02 ⫺12 11 ⫺18 79
O3 07 33 17 01 67 08 35 09 65 26 ⫺11 37 39 ⫺17 50
O4 ⫺09 11 ⫺01 59 26 28 ⴚ51 ⫺03 41 25 ⫺01 06 01 ⴚ77 08
O5 24 07 ⫺18 20 72 10 ⫺04 62 11 52 23 03 ⫺06 ⫺00 76
O6 ⫺15 06 09 61 30 29 ⫺16 ⴚ42 47 ⫺16 03 ⫺01 ⫺04 ⴚ66 05

Agreeableness (A)

A1 22 22 55 28 13 31 47 ⫺18 ⫺05 ⫺09 37 17 39 ⫺20 18


A2 26 ⫺22 65 05 ⫺06 38 19 ⴚ43 00 02 34 ⫺29 64 ⫺06 02
A3 16 36 65 06 15 10 65 ⫺12 21 ⫺19 15 23 65 04 08
A4 11 ⫺18 63 23 ⫺15 52 09 ⫺34 05 03 29 ⫺39 51 ⫺16 ⫺09
A5 ⫺04 ⫺39 53 ⫺13 ⫺13 05 34 ⴚ61 02 ⫺13 10 ⫺30 55 ⫺11 ⫺39
A6 ⫺01 25 64 ⫺24 26 ⫺15 70 04 18 ⫺11 ⫺10 14 71 19 10

Conscientiousness (C)

C1 74 39 04 17 12 59 44 28 ⫺11 07 64 41 20 21 35
C2 75 20 10 ⫺04 09 48 40 ⫺00 26 25 68 26 20 16 29
C3 73 17 34 ⫺06 09 28 72 ⫺11 ⫺04 20 64 17 49 20 19
C4 74 37 04 ⫺17 09 28 70 33 ⫺10 14 57 31 22 37 27
C5 79 29 13 11 08 55 54 05 08 19 71 31 28 18 27
C6 78 ⫺08 16 08 ⫺02 46 39 01 ⫺35 03 76 ⫺00 10 33 18

Note. Decimal points are omitted; loadings greater than or equal to 兩0.40兩 are printed in bold. NEO-PI-R ⫽ Revised NEO Personality Inventory.
a
Facet names of personality domains: N1: anxiety; N2: angry hostility; N3: depression; N4: self-consciousness; N5: impulsiveness; N6: vulnerability; E1:
warmth; E2: gregariousness; E3: assertiveness; E4: activity; E5: excitement-seeking; E6: positive emotion; O1: fantasy; O2: aesthetics; O3: feelings; O4:
actions; O5: ideas; O6: values; A1: trust; A2: straightforwardness; A3: altruism; A4: compliance; A5: modesty; A6: tender-mindedness; C1: competence;
C2: order; C3: dutifulness; C4: achievement striving; C5: self-discipline; C6: deliberation. b Factors are reported in order of extraction.

between 1 and 35 were observed (M ⫽ 20.43, SD ⫽ 7.69). The on the Cognitive Ability Scale was highly significant (r ⫽ .54, p ⬍
distribution of scores was significantly different from a normal .0001).
distribution (Kolmogorov–Smirnov d ⫽ .051, p ⬍ .05), ␹2(31, Mean levels of personality dimensions. Participants were di-
N ⫽ 870) ⫽ 126.08, p ⬍ .0001, but the overall shape of the vided into five groups according to their number of correct an-
distribution was roughly bell-shaped. swers on the Cognitive Ability Scale. The first group consisted of
It was hypothesized that the structure of word meaning is related participants who scored more than one standard deviation below
to cognitive ability. Indeed, correlation between the result of the the sample mean. The second group consisted of participants who
Structure of Word Meaning test and the number of correct answers scored more than one standard deviation above the mean. The
RELATIONSHIPS BETWEEN PERSONALITY AND WORD MEANING 729

Word meaning structure (H) groupb

H3 H4 H5
(N ⫽ 217) (N ⫽ 185) (N ⫽ 152)

1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5

Neuroticism (N)

ⴚ79 ⫺18 ⫺02 ⫺37 02 ⫺36 ⫺21 ⫺00 ⴚ75 03 ⫺33 ⴚ77 ⫺10 02 ⫺28
ⴚ73 ⫺17 ⫺21 ⫺36 ⫺04 ⫺29 ⫺11 ⫺25 ⴚ76 ⫺09 ⫺37 ⴚ71 ⫺24 ⫺01 ⫺24
ⴚ76 ⫺26 ⫺03 ⴚ41 ⫺03 ⴚ40 ⫺36 ⫺03 ⴚ69 08 ⫺38 ⴚ62 ⫺03 03 ⴚ47
ⴚ70 ⫺19 07 ⴚ46 ⫺05 ⫺35 ⫺33 11 ⴚ71 06 ⫺30 ⴚ62 ⫺01 ⫺19 ⴚ48
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ⴚ48 20 ⫺16 ⴚ55 07 ⴚ68 21 ⫺17 ⫺37 09 ⴚ71 ⴚ42 ⫺07 06 01


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ⴚ45 ⫺31 ⫺06 ⴚ63 ⫺27 ⴚ63 ⫺37 04 ⴚ45 ⫺09 ⴚ49 ⴚ57 08 ⫺09 ⴚ49

Extraversion (E)

06 70 32 24 23 24 72 32 21 12 24 22 29 14 71
33 71 17 11 ⫺09 ⫺07 76 08 24 ⫺04 17 20 02 ⫺10 77
22 59 ⫺28 41 22 38 66 ⫺23 19 22 44 27 ⫺26 10 63
02 65 ⫺04 47 07 48 68 ⫺11 10 ⫺00 33 17 ⫺02 08 69
05 78 ⫺08 12 11 08 73 ⫺09 02 24 09 12 ⫺16 ⫺04 80
38 69 14 06 22 01 74 18 32 16 ⫺03 17 09 24 74

Openness to Experience (O)

06 23 14 ⴚ41 61 ⴚ40 12 ⫺03 ⫺29 59 ⴚ42 04 ⫺15 63 03


⫺08 ⫺04 02 20 72 26 ⫺01 16 06 71 02 05 21 78 ⫺04
⫺05 34 12 13 64 03 32 17 ⫺19 62 09 ⫺21 17 68 29
60 19 03 ⫺10 09 ⫺17 05 ⫺17 29 46 ⫺20 40 15 33 33
17 13 ⫺14 08 72 28 15 ⫺07 11 72 08 36 ⫺19 74 ⫺04
47 ⫺04 16 ⫺10 51 ⫺31 11 ⫺05 51 44 ⴚ46 49 01 20 19

Agreeableness (A)

31 12 68 19 11 05 22 56 33 07 09 18 59 18 29
04 ⫺27 63 29 ⫺10 24 ⫺25 65 02 ⫺15 15 06 68 ⫺08 ⫺30
09 33 77 10 15 09 23 70 04 06 22 23 56 19 33
36 ⫺16 64 ⫺01 ⫺09 06 ⫺30 67 ⫺00 ⫺15 03 21 69 ⫺27 ⫺13
⫺28 ⴚ44 39 ⫺02 ⫺18 03 ⴚ65 36 ⫺05 ⫺09 ⫺05 ⫺21 58 07 ⴚ42
⫺29 17 71 14 15 01 ⫺01 62 ⫺15 34 ⫺13 ⫺24 62 27 29

Conscientiousness (C)

38 35 06 69 15 72 38 07 28 06 61 37 ⫺04 07 47
10 18 16 74 01 84 13 03 ⫺03 06 84 01 ⫺05 ⫺09 17
03 13 32 77 07 72 00 22 15 ⫺01 79 03 25 03 19
⫺04 36 02 78 ⫺03 81 27 01 ⫺00 17 76 04 ⫺02 05 39
29 29 12 75 02 79 23 05 25 03 76 24 08 04 32
37 ⫺17 14 68 07 77 ⫺20 19 17 ⫺05 76 26 08 ⫺06 ⫺05

remaining participants (who scored between ⫹/⫺1 standard devi- .0001, as well as a statistically significant Group ⫻ Domain
ation) were divided into three groups of roughly equal size. These interaction, F(16, 3460) ⫽ 17.61, p ⬍ .0001.
groups are referred to as CA1 (1–12 correct answers), CA2 (13–18 Inspection of Figure 2 reveals that the mean differences between
correct answers), CA3 (19 –24 correct answers), CA4 (25–28 cognitive ability groups follow a systematic pattern: The higher the
correct answers), and CA5 (29 –35 correct answers). cognitive ability, the lower the level of Neuroticism and the higher
Mean levels of personality dimensions for participants in the the level of all other personality domains with the exception of the
five cognitive ability groups are shown in Figure 2. An ANOVA Agreeableness dimension. Post hoc analyses (Scheffé test) re-
revealed significant main effects due to group, F(4, 865) ⫽ 4.65, vealed that the level of Neuroticism was significantly lower in the
p ⬍ .002, and personality domain, F(4, 3460) ⫽ 443.17, p ⬍ CA5 than in the CA1 group ( p ⬍ .0001); levels of Extraversion,
730 TOOMELA
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Figure 2. Mean levels of personality dimensions in different cognitive ability groups. N ⫽ Neuroticism; E ⫽
Extraversion; O ⫽ Openness to Experience; A ⫽ Agreeableness; C ⫽ Conscientiousness.

Openness, and Conscientiousness were significantly higher in the Thus, again, there is a clear shift toward “desired” five-factor
CA5 than in the CA1 group ( p ⬍ .003 in all cases); there were no structure with increasing level of cognitive ability. At the same
differences between groups in the mean levels of Agreeableness time, however, the factor solutions at different cognitive ability
( p ⬎ .43 in all cases). Thus, it appears that the cognitive ability levels do not replicate exactly the factor solutions in different
level affects mean levels of all personality domains with the dominant word meaning structure groups. The most notable dif-
exception of Agreeableness. ferences between cognitive ability groups and word meaning struc-
Factor structure of the NEO-PI-R. To test the possibility that ture groups are in the number of extracted factors in different
differences in personality structure may be related to cognitive groups, in the proportion of the total variance accounted by five
ability, a series of factor analyses were conducted on the person- factors in different groups, and in the patterns of factor loadings,
ality data. First, the number of extracted factors was analyzed for especially with the Agreeableness factor, which becomes similar to
the five cognitive ability subgroups. Horn’s parallel analysis “desired” factor structure “earlier” with word meaning structure
(based on mean eigenvalues of 50 random data sets) and Cattell’s (H2) than in cognitive ability level (CA3).
scree test were performed. For the CA1, CA2, and CA3, five
factors were suggested by both the scree test and the parallel Discussion
analysis. In the case of CA4, four factors should be retained by
Relationships Between Personality and Word Meaning
both tests. In CA5, three factors should be retained by the scree test
and four factors by the parallel analysis. So, it appears that the
Structure
five-factor solution is the best only for participants with relatively As noted earlier, personality may be shaped by intraindividual
low or average level of cognitive ability. The first five factors processes and by culture. Culture can be defined in many ways,
accounted for 62.0%, 62.3%, 64.2%, 67.1%, and 63.9% of the each suitable for its own purposes. In this article, I have selected
variance in the CA1, CA2, CA3, CA4, and CA5, respectively. an approach to culture that allows for a direct connection between
Finally, the pattern of factor loadings was analyzed for all culture and intraindividual psychological processes. Culture is
subgroups. In Table 2, the results of a principal-components anal- understood here as socially shared information that is coded in
ysis with a varimax rotation are shown for the five cognitive ability symbols (Toomela, 1996). This definition, following the tradition
groups separately. of the cultural– historical school of psychology (e.g., Vygotsky,
In the two lowest cognitive ability groups, CA1 and CA2, the 1929/1994, 1934/1996), encompasses individual and cultural as-
pattern of factor loadings does not correspond to the usual five- pects of mind in one phenomenon that simultaneously reflects both
factor solution; Neuroticism and Conscientiousness facet scales the cultural (in the form of symbols) and the individual (in the
load on the intended factor whereas different Extraversion, Open- form of word meanings). Theoretically, words have two comple-
ness, and Agreeableness facet scales load on different factors. In mentary roles. On the one hand, words are used for communica-
addition, in CA2 and CA3, Neuroticism and Conscientiousness tion, for social interaction—that makes words cultural. On the
facet scales load on the same factor. In the highest levels of other hand, words can be used intraindividually as “psychological
cognitive ability, CA4 and CA5, the highest loadings of facet tools,” as tools for guiding thinking, memory, perception, emotion,
scales, are on the “correct” factors, with six exceptions for the CA4 attention, and perhaps also personality.
(N5, N6, O4, O6, and A5) and two exceptions for the CA5 (N5 and One fundamental fact about word meaning is that the structure
C1). of word meaning—how information is coded with words— can
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Table 2
Factor Loadings of NEO-PI-R Scales After Varimax Rotation, for Different Levels of Cognitive Ability

Cognitive ability (CA) groupb

CA1 (N ⫽ 160) CA2 (N ⫽ 173) CA3 (N ⫽ 232) CA4 (N ⫽ 160) CA5 (N ⫽ 145)
NEO-PI-R
scalesa 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5
Neuroticism (N)
N1 ⴚ82 04 ⫺11 09 08 ⴚ53 01 10 ⴚ62 08 ⴚ76 ⫺27 04 26 17 ⫺28 ⫺24 ⫺17 07 ⴚ79 ⫺18 ⫺16 ⫺06 ⫺00 ⴚ80
N2 ⴚ67 08 ⫺19 ⫺30 ⫺34 ⴚ55 ⫺26 ⫺11 ⴚ60 ⫺04 ⴚ73 ⫺18 ⫺22 28 19 ⫺31 ⫺12 ⫺24 01 ⴚ77 ⫺16 ⫺03 ⫺33 ⫺06 ⴚ77
N3 ⴚ80 ⫺06 ⫺17 ⫺25 ⫺09 ⴚ57 04 ⫺04 ⴚ66 08 ⴚ76 ⫺33 ⫺07 32 14 ⫺36 ⫺38 ⫺06 10 ⴚ70 ⫺30 ⫺27 ⫺08 ⫺05 ⴚ75
N4 ⴚ79 ⫺09 ⫺09 ⫺12 01 ⴚ54 09 ⫺02 ⴚ58 ⫺00 ⴚ75 ⫺30 05 31 08 ⫺28 ⫺36 01 ⫺08 ⴚ74 ⫺24 ⫺31 ⫺04 ⫺05 ⴚ71
N5 ⴚ61 ⫺09 ⫺14 24 ⫺20 ⴚ67 ⫺18 24 ⫺19 ⫺17 ⴚ83 12 ⫺11 ⫺06 08 ⴚ71 11 ⫺14 ⫺03 ⫺37 ⴚ54 19 ⫺20 13 ⴚ48
N6 ⴚ47 ⫺10 ⴚ68 ⫺16 ⫺15 ⴚ70 11 ⫺33 ⫺23 ⫺02 ⴚ74 ⴚ42 ⫺01 13 ⫺09 ⴚ57 ⫺33 05 ⫺11 ⴚ56 ⫺38 ⫺20 02 ⫺23 ⴚ75
Extraversion (E)
E1 24 19 57 43 03 47 ⫺19 60 28 ⫺03 26 67 32 ⫺01 22 22 69 37 13 26 13 71 30 14 24
E2 44 31 18 48 ⫺28 34 ⴚ17 41 41 ⫺04 19 76 13 ⫺21 ⫺08 08 69 13 ⫺18 37 04 80 05 ⫺09 08
E3 32 74 27 10 03 58 ⴚ51 36 10 07 37 66 ⫺27 08 16 41 65 ⫺21 24 30 24 48 ⫺36 19 48
E4 11 49 39 34 04 60 ⫺30 45 14 03 30 74 ⫺03 16 11 47 62 ⫺06 12 15 38 62 ⫺14 09 26
E5 ⫺02 43 53 37 ⫺24 29 ⴚ43 57 13 18 09 76 ⫺12 ⫺08 02 02 70 ⫺06 10 08 19 75 ⫺19 10 07
E6 42 16 29 60 ⫺12 20 ⫺23 67 41 ⫺20 13 74 20 ⫺22 20 ⫺06 66 18 18 41 ⫺12 74 09 15 18
Openness to Experience (O)
O1 ⫺17 19 07 71 12 ⫺38 ⫺11 50 01 23 ⴚ41 02 ⫺01 ⫺35 45 ⴚ48 16 ⫺00 64 ⫺06 ⴚ51 02 ⫺07 64 ⫺07
O2 ⫺06 27 12 13 78 09 04 05 ⫺04 81 13 06 08 ⫺02 72 09 ⫺14 07 82 11 18 01 17 77 ⫺05
O3 00 06 31 61 19 11 ⫺00 70 ⫺06 29 ⫺12 15 22 ⫺05 74 04 25 08 76 ⫺09 ⫺06 47 11 50 16
O4 42 ⫺05 ⫺38 28 33 ⫺15 ⫺02 ⫺02 51 37 03 18 01 ⴚ65 07 ⫺21 21 09 28 49 ⫺03 34 08 47 00
O5 09 67 08 07 50 17 ⫺18 13 05 80 27 17 ⫺27 ⫺20 62 07 09 ⫺01 81 14 15 ⫺05 ⫺10 74 27
O6 23 ⫺25 ⫺12 47 18 ⫺17 17 12 66 00 03 ⫺05 ⫺05 ⴚ77 14 ⴚ40 05 16 39 48 ⫺36 20 04 40 23
Agreeableness (A)
A1 18 ⫺24 45 04 45 31 23 34 31 05 34 24 56 ⫺12 09 ⫺02 10 66 12 30 10 25 66 13 19
A2 33 ⴚ69 23 ⫺11 15 21 66 03 06 01 26 ⫺29 61 18 11 24 ⫺24 68 ⫺11 ⫺07 21 ⫺03 76 ⫺03 08
A3 ⫺04 ⫺21 67 33 03 17 31 69 05 00 23 26 67 ⫺09 09 02 18 77 10 23 06 18 56 27 32
A4 27 ⫺37 02 09 54 14 77 06 21 ⫺01 24 ⫺17 63 ⫺04 ⫺16 10 ⫺26 66 ⫺21 25 ⫺17 ⫺11 71 ⫺11 18
A5 ⫺11 ⴚ67 22 02 01 ⫺09 52 ⫺05 ⫺12 ⫺38 ⫺04 ⴚ48 43 05 ⫺07 07 ⴚ57 39 ⫺03 ⫺09 06 ⴚ44 59 ⫺08 ⫺25
RELATIONSHIPS BETWEEN PERSONALITY AND WORD MEANING

A6 ⫺24 ⫺12 71 14 02 24 29 58 ⫺22 ⫺04 ⫺28 11 71 13 15 ⫺01 12 68 34 ⫺14 09 11 58 41 ⫺19


Conscientiousness (C)
C1 40 23 69 09 10 82 ⫺08 28 12 06 74 41 07 06 13 64 36 ⫺04 09 37 53 22 ⫺02 10 64
C2 35 04 60 16 ⫺04 70 28 20 ⫺01 12 66 23 09 19 28 83 11 03 ⫺07 ⫺00 73 20 01 ⫺00 33
C3 18 ⫺24 80 ⫺02 04 71 30 33 ⫺07 ⫺01 65 17 30 21 21 82 ⫺02 26 01 ⫺04 60 10 23 07 44
C4 11 29 81 ⫺13 09 78 ⫺09 24 ⫺05 04 56 45 16 28 27 82 28 ⫺00 ⫺02 04 79 24 ⫺09 14 27
C5 43 ⫺02 69 10 15 80 07 25 18 04 75 33 11 09 21 83 21 01 03 14 65 22 09 02 55
C6 47 02 43 ⫺31 01 77 19 ⫺03 02 10 78 ⫺11 17 15 14 75 ⫺19 13 ⫺08 26 67 ⫺13 25 ⫺02 37

Note. Decimal points are omitted; loadings greater than or equal to 兩0.40兩 are printed in bold. NEO-PI-R ⫽ Revised NEO Personality Inventory.
a
Facet names of personality domains: N1: anxiety; N2: angry hostility; N3: depression; N4: self-consciousness; N5: impulsiveness; N6: vulnerability; E1: warmth; E2: gregariousness; E3: assertiveness;
E4: activity; E5: excitement-seeking; E6: positive emotion; O1: fantasy; O2: aesthetics; O3: feelings; O4: actions; O5: ideas; O6: values; A1: trust; A2: straightforwardness; A3: altruism; A4: compliance;
731

A5: modesty; A6: tender-mindedness; C1: competence; C2: order; C3: dutifulness; C4: achievement striving; C5: self-discipline; C6: deliberation. b Factors are reported in order of extraction.
732 TOOMELA

vary. The two most common types of word meaning structure are number of hierarchical responses increased, the factor structure
so-called everyday concepts, which organize information around more and more closely resembled the original factor structure of
everyday activities and the immediate sensory environment, and the NEO-PI-R. In the most hierarchical group, the original struc-
scientific concepts, which encode information hierarchically in the ture was replicated quite well.
language and is relatively distanced from an individual’s immedi- Several explanations are possible. First, the Estonian version of
ate environment (e.g., Nelson, 2003; Toomela, 2003; Vygotsky, the NEO-PI-R may not be adequately translated. This clearly is not
1934/1996). It was hypothesized that individual differences in the case, as the Estonian adaptation replicates the structure of the
word meaning structure are related both to mean levels of person- original fairly well (Kallasmaa et al., 2000). Moreover, in the
ality dimensions and, more importantly, to the structure of person- present study the original structure of the NEO-PI-R was repli-
ality. Findings of the study were in full agreement with the cated in the subsample of highly educated participants. Similarly
hypotheses. educated samples were used to create the original and translated
Structure of word meaning, dimension of individual differences. versions of the NEO-PI-R.
The results of this study demonstrate that the structure of word Second, the subsamples in our study may have been too small
meaning is a true dimension of individual differences. A test was (between 152 and 217 participants in each subsample) to get
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

constructed to measure which type of word meaning structure, reliable results in the factor analysis. This explanation is unlikely,
everyday concept or scientific concept, is dominant for an indi- as sample sizes above 150 and five or more variables per factor
vidual. In the studied sample, nearly the entire range of possible (there were six in this study) are entirely acceptable (Guadagnoli &
scores was observed (possible range 0 –26, observed range 1–25). Velicer, 1988; Velicer & Fava, 1998). In addition, it has been
Test scores in the studied sample were distributed normally. demonstrated that, when sample sizes are small, Type I errors in
The dominant type of word meaning structure was closely factor analysis (i.e., observed variables are incorrectly identified as
related to level of education, a finding that agrees with many measuring a component) are extremely rare. When errors occur,
cultural studies (cf. Cole, 1996; Luria, 1974; Toomela, in press). they are usually Type II errors (i.e., observed variable is incor-
Persons with higher levels of education are more likely to possess rectly not identified as measuring a component; Guadagnoli &
the scientific concept type of word meaning structure. Velicer, 1988). In the present study, in only one subsample 1 facet
Most studies in personality psychology use undergraduate stu- in 30 did not load beyond |0.40| on some factor. That is, almost all
dents and other highly educated samples (Endler & Speer, 1998; facet scales measured at least one component in every subsample.
Mallon et al., 1998). Our finding that the everyday concept type of In the least hierarchical subgroups, it is unlikely that the loadings
thinking is underrepresented in educated samples demonstrates on “wrong” factors were Type I errors.
that studies must use more representative (of the entire adult Third, the least hierarchical group may have exhibited an inco-
population) samples if they wish to examine a phenomenon that is herent factor structure because of some unidentified problem re-
or may be related to the structure of word meaning. Our results lated to the composition of that particular subsample. This was not
demonstrate that personality is one such phenomenon. the case, however, because the factor structure changed very
Structure of personality in individuals with different dominant systematically in accordance with an increase in the dominance of
structures of word meaning. We conducted a series of factor scientific concept type of word meaning structure. The remaining
analyses with data from the Estonian version (Kallasmaa et al., four groups were ordered exactly as predicted with respect to
2000) of the NEO-PI-R (Costa & McCrae, 1992). When the entire factor structure and mean levels of personality domains.
sample was entered in the analysis, five factors emerged, which Fourth, and most plausibly, our results differ from earlier studies
closely resembled the structure of the original analysis. because the studied sample successfully represented the maximum
A very different picture emerged when the sample was divided possible range of variation in word meaning structure. Indeed,
into five groups according to the number of hierarchical answers most samples used to create and replicate the Big Five structure in
on the word meaning structure test. Theoretically, differences in different cultures/languages have not adequately represented the
word meaning structure should produce differences in the number population (McCrae, 2001). Even when samples were representa-
of factors and/or the coherence of factors. The number of factors tive, they did not include sufficient variability in word meaning
may not differ between dominant word meaning structure types structures or even its marker, level of education. The original
because two opposite tendencies are involved. On the one hand, NEO-PI-R studies used samples that overrepresented individuals
scientific concepts are organized hierarchically, which should lead from high socioeconomic status groups and underrepresented in-
to a decrease in the number of factors. On the other hand, scientific dividuals with low levels of education (cf. Costa & McCrae, 1992,
concepts are more detailed and differentiated in content, which pp. 39 – 40).
should lead to an increase in the number of factors. Indeed, it was It should be noted that the sample in this study was also not
found that in all groups, with the exception of the least hierarchical perfectly representative of the larger population. It included only
word meaning structure group, a five-factor solution was the best. men with some connection to military or paramilitary organiza-
In the least hierarchical group, four or six factors should have been tions. One subsample was representative of a particular population
retained. of men—recruits together with undergraduate students represented
A hierarchical scientific concept type of word meaning structure the entire population of healthy 19 –21-year-old Estonian men. In
should be related to a highly coherent factor structure because such a worst-case scenario, our findings about the relationships between
individuals prefer to organize information into hierarchical do- word meaning structure and personality structure can only be
mains of knowledge. Everyday concepts are not organized hierar- generalized to young, healthy Estonian men. Even in this most
chically and, therefore, should be related to a less coherent factor restricted case, however, it should be evident that the Big Five
structure. This is exactly what we found. In the least hierarchical structure of personality is not universal to every culture and to
group, only the Neuroticism factor was clearly defined; as the every healthy man.
RELATIONSHIPS BETWEEN PERSONALITY AND WORD MEANING 733

Mean levels of personality dimensions. In this study, differ- ability groups, the highest proportion of variance accounted for by
ences in the mean level of major personality dimensions were five factors was found in CA4. Finally, there were differences in
found to depend on word meaning structure. It was found that all the patterns of factor loadings, especially with the Agreeableness
personality dimensions were systematically related to word mean- factor that becomes similar to the “desired” factor structure already
ing structure: The everyday concept type of word meaning struc- in H2 group of word meaning structure, but only in the CA3
ture was related to high levels of Neuroticism and low levels of all cognitive ability group.
other personality dimensions. Results of factor analyses, however, Altogether, it can be concluded that personality, as measured by
suggested that only the differences in Neuroticism can be mean- the NEO-PI-R, changes systematically with the dominant type of
ingfully interpreted. All other dimensions did not have a stable word meaning structure and with level of cognitive ability. Nev-
factor structure in all subgroups. ertheless, there are notable differences in exactly how personality
As hypothesized, individuals with a dominant scientific concept changes depending on these two cognitive factors. Theoretically,
type of word meaning, who encode information both about exter- word meaning structure describes mental “tools” and cognitive
nal events and unobservable mechanisms and ideas behind exter- ability can be understood as mastery of using those tools. Which
nal events, reported lower levels of Neuroticism. So-called “sci- tools are available for an individual and how well a person has
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

entific thinking” may enhance one’s ability to predict and acquired the skill to use those tools should both affect all aspects
understand the world. This finding is in agreement with other of mind that are related to the tools. The results of this study are
findings, which demonstrate that one’s intellectual ability is related in agreement with our theoretical model—personality is described
to adaptive traits (Austin et al., 2002). by using mental tools (i.e., words), and personality is different
depending on which tools are available for a person and how those
Alternative Explanations tools are used.
Some other alternatives. It is also possible that changes in
This study clearly demonstrates that personality structure and personality, as measured by the NEO-PI-R, are not directly caused
mean levels of personality dimensions, as measured by the NEO- by differences in dominant type of word meaning structure or
PI-R, may be very different in different individuals from the same cognitive ability. It might also be possible that people who score
sociologically defined “culture.” It may be possible, however, that “low” on the word meaning structure test or who score low on a
variation in personality is not related to word meaning structure test of cognitive ability are careless, uncooperative, and/or insuf-
but rather to some other human characteristic. The most plausible ficiently motivated to provide valid data to the researcher, com-
alternative is cognitive ability, which is tightly related to word pared with people who score “high” on the word meaning test or
meaning structure. high on a test of cognitive ability. This explanation, however, does
Relationships between personality and cognitive ability. First, not fully correspond to the data. Carelessness may be expressed in
the studied sample included nearly the entire range of possible the amount of variance accounted for by five factors; low levels of
scores on the Cognitive Ability Scale (possible range 0 –36, ob- cognitive ability and low levels of hierarchical thinking were,
served range 1–35). Second, dominant type of word meaning indeed, related to smaller amounts of “explained” variance. Nev-
structure was, indeed, related to level of cognitive ability. Partic- ertheless, the individuals in H1 and CA1 provided responses to the
ipants who used hierarchical thinking were more likely to demon- NEO-PI-R that were structured. Neuroticism facets clearly loaded
strate a high level of cognitive ability. on the same factor in these groups that were most different from
Next, it was found that cognitive ability is related to mean levels the supposedly universal five-factor structure of personality. If
of personality domains. Levels of Neuroticism were negatively responses had been given carelessly, there should have been no
correlated with cognitive ability, whereas levels of Extraversion, structure for any of the personality domains. But responses were
Openness, and Conscientiousness were positively correlated with structured in all studied groups; the structure was just different
cognitive ability. Agreeableness was not related to cognitive abil- from the “universal” structure. It is not plausible to think that
ity but was related to word meaning structure. Thus, both cognitive “careless” individuals responded carefully and systematically to
ability and word meaning structure are related to mean levels of Neuroticism items but responded carelessly to all other items.
personality dimensions, but not in exactly the same way. There are other factors that psychological researchers have
The factor structure of the NEO-PI-R also changed systemati- found to be related to individual psychological differences; level of
cally with different levels of cognitive ability. At higher levels of education and socioeconomic status are perhaps the best two
cognitive ability, the factor structure corresponded more closely to examples. In the case of such variables, it is important to make a
the original NEO-PI-R structure. Again, the data point to an clear distinction between descriptions of where individuals have
inescapable conclusion: The five-factor structure of Neuroticism, been (or what kind of life they live) and who these individuals are.
Extraversion, Openness, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness is An educational system helps individuals to develop skills and
not a human universal. It appears that cognitive ability and word knowledge; socioeconomic status, however, results from individ-
meaning structure are differently related to personality structure as ual differences in skills and knowledge. In this study it was found
measured by the NEO-PI-R. There were differences in the number that educational level is related to dominant type of word meaning
of factors that should be extracted (four or six in H1 and five in structure (and also to cognitive ability). Education in this context
H2–H5; five in CA1–CA3, but only four in CA4, and only three or can be understood as a primary cultural institution that induces
four in CA5). There were also differences in the proportion of total development of word meaning structure and cognitive ability.
variance accounted for by the five factors in different groups. In Education also causes changes in personality—not directly, but
the case of the word meaning structure groups, the variance ac- through changes in mental tools and skills in using those tools. It
counted for by the five factors increased systematically with an follows that, theoretically, there is a causal chain from (a) educa-
increase in the number of hierarchical responses; in the cognitive tion to (b) change in word meaning structure and cognitive effi-
734 TOOMELA

ciency to (c) change in personality. The results of this study Johnson, J. A. (1997). Units of analysis for the description and explanation
strongly support such a model, although the hypothesis can only be of personality. In R. Hogan, J. Johnson, & S. Briggs (Eds.), Handbook
tested directly with a large scale longitudinal study. The causal of personality psychology (pp. 73–93). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
chain can be extended to include socioeconomic status as a product Joseph, J. (2001). Separated twins and the genetics of personality differ-
of differences in cognitive efficiency and personality. ences: A critique. American Journal of Psychology, 114, 1–30.
Kallasmaa, T., Allik, J., Realo, A., & McCrae, R. R. (2000). The Estonian
References version of the NEO-PI-R: An examination of universal and culture-
specific aspects of the five-factor model. European Journal of Person-
Allik, J., & Realo, A. (1997). Intelligence, academic abilities, and person- ality, 14, 265–278.
ality. Personality and Individual Differences, 23, 809 – 814. Kikas, E. (1998). The impact of teaching on students’ definitions and
Austin, E. J., Deary, I. J., Whiteman, M. C., Fowkes, F. G. R., Pedersen, explanations of astronomical phenomena. Learning and Instruction, 8,
N. L., Rabbitt, P., et al. (2002). Relationships between ability and
439 – 454.
personality: Does intelligence contribute positively to personal and
Kikas, E. (2003). Constructing knowledge beyond senses: Worlds too big
social adjustment? Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 1391–
and too small to see. In A. Toomela (Ed.), Cultural guidance in the
1411.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

development of the human mind (pp. 211–227). Westport, CT: Ablex


Baumeister, R. F., & Tice, D. M. (1988). Metatraits. Journal of Person-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Publishing.
ality, 56, 571–598.
Luria, A. R. (1973). Osnovy neiropsikhologii [Fundamentals of neuropsy-
Beier, M. E., & Ackerman, P. L. (2001). Current-events knowledge in
chology]. Moscow: Izdatel’stvo MGU.
adults: An investigation of age, intelligence, and nonability determi-
nants. Psychology and Aging, 16, 615– 628. Luria, A. R. (1974). Ob istoricheskom razvitii poznavatel’nykh processov.
Block, J. (1995). A contrarian view of the five-factor approach to person- Eksperimental’no-psikhologicheskoje issledovanije [On historical devel-
ality description. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 187–215. opment of cognitive processes]. Moscow: Nauka.
Blumberg, H. H. (2001). The common ground of natural language and Luria, A. R. (1979). Jazyk i soznanije [Language and consciousness].
social interaction in personality description. Journal of Research in Moscow: Izdatel’stvo Moskovskogo Universiteta.
Personality, 35, 289 –312. Mallon, S. D., Kingsley, D., Affleck, G., & Tennen, H. (1998). Method-
Bock, P. K. (2000). Culture and personality revisited. American Behavioral ological trends in Journal of Personality: 1970 –1995. Journal of Per-
Scientist, 44, 32– 40. sonality, 66, 672– 685.
Britt, T. W. (1993). Metatraits: Evidence relevant to the validity of the Marsella, A. J., Dubanoski, J., Hamada, W. C., & Morse, H. (2000). The
construct and its implications. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- measurement of personality across cultures. American Behavioral Sci-
chology, 65, 554 –562. entist, 44, 41– 62.
Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., & Sheridan, J. F. (2000). Multilevel McCrae, R. R. (2000). Trait psychology and the revival of personality and
integrative analyses of human behavior: Social neuroscience and the culture studies. American Behavioral Scientist, 44, 10 –31.
complementing nature of social and biological approaches. Psycholog- McCrae, R. R. (2001). Trait psychology and culture: Exploring intercul-
ical Bulletin, 126, 829 – 843. tural comparisons. Journal of Personality, 69, 819 – 846.
Cheung, F. M., Leung, K., Zhang, J. X., Sun, H. F., Gan, Y. Q., Song, McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1996). Toward a new generation of
W. Z., & Xie, D. (2001). Indigenous Chinese personality constructs: Is personality theories: Theoretical contexts for the five-factor model. In
the Five-Factor model complete? Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychol- J. S. Wiggins (Ed.), The five-factor model of personality (pp. 51– 87).
ogy, 32, 407– 433. New York: Guilford Press.
Cole, M. (1996). Cultural psychology. A once and future discipline. Cam- McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1997). Personality trait structure as a human
bridge, MA: Belknap Press. universal. American Psychologist, 52, 509 –516.
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1985). The NEO personality inventory McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1999). A five-factor theory of personality.
manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. In A. Lawrence & O. P. J. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of personality:
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory Theory and research (pp. 139 –153). New York: Guilford Press.
(NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI). Professional Nelson, K. (2003). Making sense in a world of symbols. In A. Toomela
manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. (Ed.), Cultural guidance in the development of the human mind (pp.
Costa, P. T., Terracciano, A., & McCrae, R. R. (2001). Gender differences
139 –162). Westport, CT: Ablex Publishing.
in personality traits across cultures: Robust and surprising findings.
Paunonen, S. V., & Ashton, M. C. (2001). Big Five predictors of academic
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 322–331.
achievement. Journal of Research in Personality, 35, 78 –90.
Digman, J. M. (1996). The curious history of the five-factor model. In J. S.
Pedersen, N. L., Plomin, R., McClearn, G. E., & Friberg, L. (1988).
Wiggins (Ed.), The five-factor model of personality (pp. 1–20). New
Neuroticism, Extraversion and related traits in adult twins reared apart
York: Guilford Press.
and reared together. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55,
Endler, N. S. (2000). The interface between personality and cognition.
950 –957.
European Journal of Personality, 14, 377–389.
Endler, N. S., & Speer, R. L. (1998). Personality psychology: Research Poortinga, Y. H., & van Hemert, D. A. (2001). Personality and culture:
trends for 1993–1995. Journal of Personality, 66, 622– 669. Demarcating between the common and the unique. Journal of Person-
Funder, D. C. (2001). Personality. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, ality, 69, 1033–1060.
197–221. Pulver, A. (1999). Vaimsete võimete skaala VVS2. Unpublished manual.
Funnell, E. (2001). Evidence for scripts in semantic dementia: Implications Tartu, Estonia: University of Tartu, Department of Psychology.
for theories of semantic memory. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 18, 323– Pulver, A., Allik, J., Pulkkinen, L., & Hämäläinen, M. (1995). A Big Five
341. personality inventory in two non-Indo-European languages. European
Guadagnoli, E., & Velicer, W. F. (1988). Relation of sample size to the Journal of Personality, 9, 109 –124.
stability of component patterns. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 265–275. Saucier, G., & Goldberg, L. R. (1996). The language of personality:
Jang, K. L., McCrae, R. R., Angleitner, A., Riemann, R., & Livesley, W. J. Lexical perspectives on the five-factor model. In J. S. Wiggins (Ed.), The
(1998). Heritability of facet-level traits in a cross-cultural twin sample: five-factor model of personality (pp. 21–50). New York: Guilford Press.
Support for a hierarchical model of personality. Journal of Personality Tomasello, M. (1996). The child’s contribution to culture: A commentary
and Social Psychology, 74, 1556 –1565. on Toomela. Culture and Psychology, 2, 307–318.
RELATIONSHIPS BETWEEN PERSONALITY AND WORD MEANING 735

Toomela, A. (1996). How culture transforms mind: A process of internal- sampling on factor pattern recovery. Psychological Methods, 3, 231–
ization. Culture and Psychology, 2, 285–305. 251.
Toomela, A. (2000). Stages of mental development: Where to look? Vygotsky, L. S. (1994). The problem of the cultural development of the
Trames: A Journal of the Humanities and Social Sciences, 4, 21–52. child. In R. van der Veer & J. Valsiner (Eds.), The Vygotsky reader (pp.
Toomela, A. (2003). Development of symbol meaning and the emergence 57–72). Oxford, England: Blackwell. (Originally published 1929)
of the semiotically mediated mind. In A. Toomela (Ed.), Cultural Vygotsky, L. S. (1996). Myshlenije i rech [Thinking and speech]. Moscow:
guidance in the development of the human mind (pp. 163–209). West- Labirint. (Originally published 1934)
port, CT: Ablex Publishing. Zwick, W. R., & Velicer, W. F. (1986). Comparison of five rules for
Toomela, A. (in press). Culture as a semiosphere: On the role of culture in determining the number of components to retain. Psychological Bulle-
culture-individual relationship. In I. E. Josephs & J. Valsiner (Eds.), tin, 99, 432– 442.
Dialogicality in development. Westport, CT: Ablex Publishing.
Toomela, A., Tomberg, T., Orasson, A., Tikk, A., & Nômm, M. (1999).
Paradoxical facilitation of a free recall of nonwords in persons with Received May 31, 2002
traumatic brain injury. Brain and Cognition, 39, 187–201. Revision received February 24, 2003
Velicer, W. F., & Fava, J. L. (1998). Effects of variable and subject Accepted March 7, 2003 䡲
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

You might also like