You are on page 1of 9

International Journal of Psychology

International Journal of Psychology, 2017


DOI: 10.1002/ijop.12465

Positive discipline, harsh physical discipline, physical


discipline and psychological aggression in five Caribbean
countries: Associations with preschoolers’ early literacy
skills
Elif Dede Yildirim1 and Jaipaul L. Roopnarine2,3
1 Department of Human Development and Family Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia,
MO, USA
2
Department of Human Development and Family Science, Syracuse University, Syracuse, NY,
USA
3 Institute for Graduate Studies and Research, Anton de Kom University of Suriname,

Paramaribo, Suriname

P hysical punishment has received worldwide attention because of its negative impact on children’s cognitive and
social development and its implications for children’s rights. Using UNICEF Multiple Indicator Cluster Surveys 4
and 5 data, we assessed the associations between positive discipline, harsh physical punishment, physical punishment
and psychological aggression and preschoolers’ literacy skills in 5628 preschool-aged children and their caregivers in the
developing nations of Belize, the Dominican Republic, Guyana, Jamaica and Suriname. Caregivers across countries used
high levels of explanations and psychological aggression. There were significant country differences in the use of the
four disciplinary practices. In the Dominican Republic and Guyana, physical punishment had negative associations with
children’s literacy skills, and in the Dominican Republic, positive discipline had a positive association with children’s
literacy skills. Findings are discussed with respect to the negative consequences of harsh disciplinary practices on
preschoolers’ early literacy skills in the developing world.
Keywords: Disciplinary techniques; Literacy; Early childhood.

Two meta-analyses (Gershoff, 2002; Gershoff & & Barker, 2015). In these analyses, we used data from
Grogan-Kaylor, 2016) conducted 15 years apart indi- the UNICEF Multiple Indicator Cluster Surveys (MICS)
cate negative patterns of associations between physical 4 and 5 to examine the associations between positive
punishment and behavioural and mental health outcomes discipline, harsh physical punishment, physical punish-
in children and adults. Other meta-analyses (Ferguson, ment and psychological aggression and children’s early
2013; Paolucci & Violato, 2004) show similar associ- literacy skills in the Caribbean nations of Belize, the
ations but they are less robust. These outcomes aside, Dominican Republic, Guyana, Jamaica and Suriname.
there are gaping holes in the literature on the associations Large-scale and small sample studies show that the
between different forms of discipline and children’s Caribbean region has some of the highest rates (e.g.
cognitive skills during the preschool years in develop- 59.7% of mothers in Guyana and 60% of mothers in
ing countries. Focusing on these links can be useful Trinidad and Tobago spanked children) of parental use
in addressing the effects of harsh parental treatment of of physical punishment in the world (Roopnarine, Jin, &
children in developing countries such as those in the Krishnakumar, 2014; Van der Kooij et al., 2015). Across
Caribbean region where rates of child maltreatment can the Caribbean, adults strongly believe that physical pun-
be high (see Levtov, van der Gaag, Greene, Kaufman, ishment is an appropriate way to address behavioural

Correspondence should be addressed to Elif Dede Yildirim, Department of Human Development and Family Science, University of Missouri,
Columbia, MO 65211, USA. E-mail: dedeyildirime@missouri.edu.
During the preparation of this manuscript the second author was supported by funds made available through a Pearls S. Falk Professorship in the
David B. Falk College of Sport and Human Dynamics at Syracuse University. This support is gratefully acknowledged. Elif Dede Yildirim helped
in conceiving the study, conducted all analyses, wrote the methods and results sections and edited the manuscript. Jaipaul L. Roopnarine helped in
conceiving the study, guided the analyses, wrote the introduction and discussion sections and edited the manuscript.

© 2017 International Union of Psychological Science


2 DEDE YILDIRIM AND ROOPNARINE

and other difficulties in children (Anderson & Payne, care by avoiding harsh methods of discipline during daily
1994; Leo-Rhynie & Brown, 2013). Moreover, parents socialisation practices are more likely to use explana-
in Jamaica, Guyana and Dominica expect children to tions and reasoning with their children (Baumrind, 1996;
obey and show unilateral respect to them (e.g. Durbrow, Rohner & Khaleque, 2005). This approach to parenting
1999; Wilson, Wilson, & Berkeley-Caines, 2003), and lays the foundation for the encouragement of two-way
there is a fair amount of denigration of children in the verbal exchanges and cognitive collaboration between
form of blaming and name calling (e.g. “hardened” mean- parent and child (e.g. book reading, telling stories) that
ing stubborn, troublesome, lazy) in Trinidad and Tobago are essential for early literacy skills development (Mor-
and Dominica (Durbrow, 1999; Roopnarine & Jin, 2016). rison & Cooney, 2002). By contrast, harsh disciplinary
However, later studies show that a majority of moth- practices, such as physical punishment and psychological
ers (70%) and fathers (68%) in a sample of families in aggression, can serve to distance children from their par-
Trinidad and Tobago engaged in low levels of physical ents, undermine trust and confidence in children and sup-
control with preschool-aged children (Roopnarine, Yang, press meaningful language and social exchanges between
Krishnakumar, & Davidson, 2013). parents and children during the vulnerable preschool
A few studies have also examined associations years. The early childhood literature from the developed
between physical punishment and childhood outcomes world indicates negative associations between harsh par-
in Caribbean families. Severity of maternal physical enting practices and children’s language and social skills
punishment had an inverse direct relation with prosocial (see Berlin et al., 2009; Paolucci & Violato, 2004; Shu-
behaviours in preschool-aged children in Guyana (Roop- mow, Vandell, & Posner, 1998; Straus & Paschall, 2009).
narine et al., 2013) and had a direct positive association The Caribbean nations of Belize, the Dominican
with behavioural difficulties in children in Trinidad and Republic, Guyana, Jamaica and Suriname were chosen
Tobago (Roopnarine, Krishnakumar, Narine, Logie, & for this investigation because of their ethnic, linguistic
Lape, 2014). Guyanese children whose mothers dis- and religious diversity. Guyana is English-speaking
played high levels of warmth and low levels of control and Suriname is Dutch-speaking, and both have
were rated by their teachers as having better social and large African Caribbean and Indo Caribbean popula-
cognitive skills than children whose mothers displayed tions. The Dominican Republic is Spanish-speaking,
high levels of warmth and moderately high levels of con- with a large mixed-ethnic population, and Jamaica
trol during parenting (Roopnarine et al., 2013). Another is English-speaking, with a relatively homogeneous
study showed that caretaker rejection mediated the link African Caribbean population. Within this diverse ethnic
between physical punishment and social adjustment in mosaic, families embrace Christian, Hindu, Islamic,
older children in St. Kitts (Rohner, Kean, & Cournoyer, Rastafari and Orisha religious practices (Roopnarine &
1991). Jin, 2016). The five countries are also at different levels
In as much as these studies advance our understanding of economic development based on Human Development
of parenting practices in the Caribbean region, they have Indices and all have pockets of poverty in rural and urban
some major drawbacks. Most employed small samples areas (UNDP, 2016). From a risk and resilience perspec-
of families mainly within the English-speaking countries, tive (Rutter & Sroufe, 2000), an examination of different
narrowly focused on the associations between physical disciplinary practices in these diverse nation states can
punishment and children’s social skills or behavioural dif- help to establish whether consistency exists in associa-
ficulties (e.g. Roopnarine et al., 2014), and used global tions between less and more severe modes of discipline
indices of physical punishment without differentiating and children’s early literacy skills across the world.
between more severe modes of physical punishment from The present study was guided by propositions in
normative spanking (e.g. Anderson & Payne, 1994). Find- interpersonal-acceptance rejection theory (Rohner &
ings on some ethnic groups in the United States indicate Khaleque, 2005) and parenting models and literacy
that normative physical punishment such as spanking had development in children (see Bus, van Ijzendoorn, & Pel-
negligible effects on children’s academic performance legrini, 1995; Isbell, Sobel, Lindauer, & Lowrence, 2004;
and cognitive skills (see Paolucci & Violato, 2004), but Mol & Bus, 2011; Morrison & Cooney, 2002). Interper-
others report its long-term adverse effects on young chil- sonal acceptance-rejection theory underlines the benefits
dren’s receptive language skills (e.g. MacKenzie, Nicklas, of parental warmth and the negative consequences of
Waldfogel, & Brooks-Gunn, 2013). parental rejection and hostility for childhood develop-
This study extends prior work on disciplinary prac- ment, whereas models of early literacy development
tices in developing nations by examining the associations stress the importance of sensitive parental engagement
between positive discipline, harsh physical punishment, with children through language-rich activities. Accord-
physical punishment and psychological aggression and ingly, this study explored the following questions: (a)
young children’s literacy skills in the Caribbean nations Are there differences in the use of positive discipline,
of Belize, the Dominican Republic, Guyana, Jamaica and harsh physical punishment, physical punishment and
Suriname. In the main, parents who employ sensitive psychological aggression across Belize, the Dominican

© 2017 International Union of Psychological Science


DISCIPLINARY PRACTICES AND LITERACY SKILL 3

Republic, Jamaica, Guyana and Suriname? and (b) What Dominican Republic (n = 3968) and Guyana (n = 555)
are the degrees of associations between positive disci- from MICS 5 surveys. The sample was limited to fam-
pline, harsh physical punishment, physical punishment ilies who had children under age 5 and to those that
and psychological aggression and children’s literacy completed the MICS household survey child discipline
skills across the five countries? From a biodevelopmental section and child development surveys. We excluded 97
perspective, the focus on literacy skills as an outcome children whose caregivers completed the child survey for
acknowledges its importance as an important indicator at least one more child in the same household. Table 1
of later academic functioning (Shonkoff, 2010). Because presents descriptive information on the families. Across
sociodemographic factors such as maternal education, countries, 49.8% of children were female, with an overall
economic status, preschool enrolment, children’s age, average age of 47.86 months (SD = 6.81). Approximately
marital status, child sex and area of residence are all asso- 29% of caregivers were between 20 and 24 years of age,
ciated with children’s cognitive skills (e.g. Burger, 2010; 29% were between 25 and 29 years of age, 17.9% were
Votruba-Drzal, Miller, & Coley, 2015), these variables between 30 and 34 years of age and 10% were 40 years
were entered as controls in the analyses. or older. Two thirds of caregivers were married, 32.7% of
Based on prior research on the use of harsh disci- caregivers completed primary school and 41.2% of care-
plinary practices in the Caribbean (Roopnarine et al., givers completed secondary school. Among all, 28.7%
2014), we expect the use of physical modes of pun- of households fell into the lowest wealth index quintile,
ishment and psychological aggression to be high and 21.3% were in the second lowest quintile, 18.7% were
positive discipline to be low across Caribbean countries in the middle quintile, 16.9% were in the fourth quintile
with little between-country variation. Taking into consid- and 14.4% of the households were in the highest quintile.
eration associations between positive and harsh parenting More than half of the families resided in rural areas.
practices and children’s language skills (MacKenzie
et al., 2013; Tamis-LeMonda, Shannon, Cabrera, & Measures
Lamb, 2004) and cognitive ability (Straus & Paschall,
2009), we expect a positive association between positive Caregiver discipline
discipline and children’s literacy skills and negative asso-
ciations between harsh physical punishment, physical To measure disciplinary practices, caregivers com-
punishment and psychological aggression and children’s pleted questionnaires on the focal child. Caregivers were
literacy skills, irrespective of country. asked a series of questions on the use of different disci-
plinary practices within the past month. Following prior
work (see Ferguson, 2013; Paolucci & Violato, 2004)
METHODS and our interest in differentiating the influence of differ-
ent forms of discipline on childhood development, we
Sample focused on positive discipline, harsh physical punish-
ment, physical punishment and psychological aggression.
Data for the present study were obtained from MICS As noted earlier, the use of psychological aggression is
4 and 5. MICS is a global household initiative that fairly common in Caribbean countries and its impact on
collects individual and household level data from low- childhood development is largely undetermined in devel-
and middle-income countries (UNICEF, 2015). MICS oping countries.
conducted home interviews with mothers and other care- Positive discipline was assessed using three binary
givers aged 15–59 with children under 5 years of age. items: “took away privileges, forbade something child
Detailed data are collected on maternal and child health, liked or did not allow him/her to leave house,” “ex-
marital union history, early childhood development, child plained why behaviour was wrong” and “gave child some-
discipline and labour, pre- and post-natal health, immuni- thing else to do.” Across the countries, 45.8% of moth-
sation history and child mortality. Five waves of surveys ers or other adults in the household took away privi-
have been conducted: MICS 1 (1995), MICS 2 (2000), leges, 71.7% explained why the behaviour was wrong and
MICS 3 (2005–2007), MICS 4 (2009–2012) and MICS 27.3% gave child something else to do to teach child the
5 (2012–2014). Data sets became publicly available right behaviour in the past month. A composite measure
after the dissemination of survey results at the country was created by summing up the three items (range 0–3).
level. The available MICS data sets are de-identified and Harsh physical punishment was distinguished from
do not require Institutional Review Board approval for more normative physical punishment such as spanking
use. Our sample consisted of families and children from with hand (see Paolucci & Violato, 2004). It was mea-
five Caribbean countries1 : Belize (n = 309), Jamaica sured using five binary items that included: “shook child,”
(n = 321) and Suriname (n = 475) from MICS 4, and the “hit child on the bottom or elsewhere on the body with
something like a belt, hairbrush, stick or other hard
1 Barbados and St. Lucia were not included due to small sample sizes. object,” “hit or slapped child on the face, head or ears,”

© 2017 International Union of Psychological Science


4 DEDE YILDIRIM AND ROOPNARINE

TABLE 1
Sample characteristics by country

Variables Belize Dominican Republic Guyana Jamaica Suriname

Child age (months), mean (SD) 48.08(6.65) 48.03(6.86) 48.14(6.72) 47.38(6.30) 46.33(6.70)
Children’s sex (female), % 49.8 50.1 50.6 52.3 48.8
Mothers’ age, %
15–19 1.4 3.2 1.6 1.4 2.6
20–24 27.5 29.7 30.1 22.4 24.4
25–29 33.0 28.5 29.1 32.0 27.9
30–34 15.8 17.6 20.1 17.3 20.5
35–39 12.7 11.6 11.6 14.6 10.7
40–44 6.5 6.0 4.7 9.5 10.0
45–49 3.1 3.5 2.8 2.7 4.0
Marital status (married), % 82.8 72.1 89.8 49.3 72.3
Area of residence (urban), % 40.1 65.7 20.7 46.1 38.7
Mothers’ education, %
Primary 45.3 30.6 16.8 73.2 33.5
Secondary 48.5 37.3 69.9 22.4 47.8
Household Wealth Index Quantiles, %
Poorest (0–19%) 19.4 27.9 28.8 20.9 46.1
Second (20–39%) 20.4 21.5 20.7 26.5 17.1
Middle (40–59%) 22.3 19.0 17.8 21.2 13.5
Fourth (60–79%) 22.0 17.2 14.4 19.9 11.8
Richest (80–100%) 15.9 14.3 18.2 11.5 11.6

“hit or slapped child on the hand, arm or leg” and “beat 10.” Across the countries, 31.2% of children identified at
child up, that is, hit him/her over and over as hard as least 10 letters, 22.1% read at least four words and 44.5%
one could.” Approximately 17.6% of mothers or anyone recognised symbols of all numbers. These items are simi-
else in the household shook the focal child, 17% hit child lar to those used in standardised assessments of children’s
with belt, brush or stick, 2.6% hit or slapped child on the early language skills. A composite score was created by
face, head or ears, 17.4% hit or slapped child on the hand, summing caregiver’s responses across the literacy skills
arm or leg and less than 1% beat child up as hard as one items (range 0–3).
could in the past month. Responses to the five items were
summed to form a total score of harsh physical punish-
ment (range 0–5). Covariates
Psychological aggression was assessed with two The following variables were used as controls: moth-
binary items: “shouted, yelled at or screamed at child” ers’ highest level of education (1 = at least secondary,
and “called child dumb, lazy or another name like that.” 0 = not), area of residence (1 = urban, 0 = rural), chil-
Roughly 56% of mothers or other caregivers in the dren’s age (in months) and sex (1 = male, 0 = female),
household shouted, yelled or screamed at child, whereas
preschool enrolment (1 = enrolled, 0 = not enrolled),
7.5% of caregivers in the household called child dumb,
mothers’ age (in years) and marital status (1 = married,
lazy or another name in the past month (range 0–2).
0 = not) and household wealth index. A composite mea-
Physical punishment was assessed using a binary item
sure of household wealth index was created using the
asking whether caregivers in the household “spanked, hit
source of drinking water, type of toilet facility, number
or slapped child on the bottom with bare hand.” This
of rooms for sleeping, type of main material for floor and
item reflected the normative use of physical punishment
roof, access to electricity and household assets (see Sun,
(Ferguson, 2013). Across the five countries, 39.6% of
Liu, Chen, Rao, & Liu, 2016).
caregivers in the household spanked, hit or slapped the
focal child on bottom with bare hand.
Analytical techniques
Children’s literacy skills
We first assessed between country differences in the pro-
Children’s literacy skills were assessed using care- portion of caregivers who used positive discipline, harsh
givers’ reports on three binary items: “child can identify physical punishment, physical punishment and psycho-
or name at least 10 letters of the alphabet,” “child can read logical aggression. Weighted chi-square tests within the
at least four simple, popular words” and “child knows the R packages of “Weights” (Pasek, 2016) were used to
name and recognises the symbol of all numbers from 1 to take into account the two-stage stratified cluster sampling

© 2017 International Union of Psychological Science


DISCIPLINARY PRACTICES AND LITERACY SKILL 5

utilised by MICS to obtain national level data. Follow-up physical punishment than those in Belize (χ2 = 123.68,
pairwise chi-square tests with Bonferroni adjustment p < .005), Guyana (χ2 = 223.53, p < .005), Jamaica
(p < .005) were conducted between countries on the pro- (χ2 = 411.62, p < .005) and Suriname (χ2 = 294.27,
portion of caregivers who used positive discipline, harsh p < .005). Another trend that stood out was that care-
physical punishment, physical punishment and psycho- givers in Suriname were more likely to shout, yell at or
logical aggression. scream at children than caregivers in Belize (χ2 = 121.10,
Next, a set of ordinal logistic regression analyses p < .005), Dominican Republic (χ2 = 162.03, p < .005),
were conducted to assess the associations between pos- Guyana (χ2 = 87.23, p < .005) and Jamaica (χ2 = 30.42,
itive discipline, harsh physical punishment, physical p < .005), and to call children dumb, lazy or another
punishment and psychological aggression and chil- name than caregivers in Belize (χ2 = 41.47, p < .005),
dren’s literacy skills across countries. For each country, Dominican Republic (χ2 = 295.81, p < .005), Guyana
children’s literacy skills were entered as the outcome (χ2 = 73.55, p < .005) and Jamaica (χ2 = 35.12,
variable, and positive discipline, harsh physical punish- p < .005).
ment, physical punishment and psychological aggression
were entered as predictor variables. Children’s sex and
age, preschool education, mothers’ age, marital status, Disciplinary practices and children’s literacy
maternal education, household wealth index and area skills
of residence were entered as control variables in all
models. All regression models were assessed within the The ordinal logistic regression models produced a mixed
Mplus 7.4 software (Muthén & Muthén, 2014) that can pattern of associations and the odds ratios, estimates, stan-
handle complex survey sample designs while permit- dard errors and p-values are indicated for each country
ting the addition of children’s sample weights to the separately in Table 3.
MICS data. Robust maximum likelihood estimates were
used and missing data (<1% at the country level) were
addressed by using full information maximum likelihood Belize
estimates.
Children’s age, household wealth and preschool enrol-
ment were positively associated with children’s liter-
RESULTS acy skills. Harsh physical punishment was marginally
significantly associated with children’s literacy skills.
Country-level differences in modes of discipline There were no significant associations between positive
discipline, harsh physical punishment, physical punish-
Table 2 presents the results from the weighted chi-square ment and psychological aggression and children’s literacy
tests on the proportion of caregivers in each country who skills.
engaged in positive discipline, harsh physical punish-
ment, physical punishment and psychological aggression.
Across countries, a majority of caregivers reported using Dominican Republic
explanations as a disciplinary practice (from 88.6% in
Preschool enrolment, children’s age, mother’s age and
Belize to 67.9% in the Dominican Republic). At the same
household wealth index were positively, and caregivers’
time, the use of psychological aggression was moderately
age was negatively associated with children’s literacy
high (e.g. for shouting from 82.9% in Suriname to 45.5%
skills. Positive discipline was positively, and physical
in Belize). There were significant differences between
punishment was negatively associated with children’s
countries across all disciplinary practices (see Table 2).
literacy skills. Harsh physical punishment and psycho-
For instance, caregivers in Belize were more likely to
logical aggression were not significantly associated with
use explanations than those in the Dominican Republic
children’s literacy skills.
(χ2 = 57.97, p < .005), Guyana (χ2 = 14.53, p < .005),
Jamaica (χ2 = 10.81, p < .005) and Suriname (χ2 = 8.41,
p < .005), whereas caregivers in the Dominican Republic Guyana
were less likely to redirect children compared to those
in Belize (χ2 = 150.47, p < .005), Guyana (χ2 = 116.39, Children’s age, preschool enrolment and household
p < .005), Jamaica (χ2 = 47.26, p < .005) and Suriname wealth index were positively associated with children’s
(χ2 = 260.05, p < .005). In terms of harshness, caregivers literacy skills. Physical punishment was negatively asso-
in Jamaica were more likely to use physical punishment ciated with children’s literacy skills. There were no sig-
than those in Guyana (χ2 = 46.53, p < .005) and the nificant associations between positive discipline, harsh
Dominican Republic (χ2 = 57.00, p < .005). By contrast, physical punishment and psychological aggression and
caregivers in Dominican Republic were less likely to use children’s literacy skills.

© 2017 International Union of Psychological Science


6 DEDE YILDIRIM AND ROOPNARINE

TABLE 2
Proportion of endorsement of positive discipline, harsh physical punishment, physical punishment, and psychological aggression items
by country

Total Belize Dominican Guyana Jamaica Suriname


Variables (n = 5628) (n = 309) Republic (n = 3968) (n = 555) (n = 321) (n = 475) χ2 (df = 4)

Positive discipline
Took away privileges, forbade something 45.8% 53.7% 45.7% 40.1% 44.2% 49.1% 22.93*
child liked or did not allow him/her to
leave house
Explained why behaviour was wrong 71.7% 88.6% 67.9% 77.8% 78.9% 80.8% 134.12*
Gave child something else to do 27.3% 50.0% 19.9% 40.3% 36.3% 53.2% 361.49*
Physical discipline
Spanked, hit or slapped child on the 39.6% 47.4% 36.8% 34.4% 58.1% 52.1% 104.03*
bottom with bare hand
Harsh physical discipline
Shook child 17.6% 12.3% 15.8% 20.1% 25.2% 28.0% 29.44*
Hit child on the bottom or elsewhere on 17.0% 24.0% 15.8% 16.9% 22.9% 17.7% 32.00*
the body with something like a belt,
hairbrush, stick or other hard object
Hit or slapped child on the face, head or 2.6% 3.9% 2.1% 3.3% 1.6% 6.3% 26.69*
ears
Hit or slapped child on the hand, arm or 17.4% 30.1% 9.5% 31.8% 48.4% 37.1% 666.38*
leg
Beat child up, that is hit him/her over and 0.9% 1.3% 0.6% 0.2% 1.6% 4.0% 26.46*
over as hard as one could
Psychological aggression
Shouted, yelled at or screamed at child 55.6% 45.5% 52.2% 55.8% 65.9% 82.9% 164.20*
Called child dumb, lazy, or another name 7.5% 8.1% 5.0% 7.0% 9.7% 26.7% 240.20*
like that

*p<. 001.

TABLE 3
Estimates for predicting children’s literacy skills by country

Belize Dominican Republic Guyana Jamaica Suriname


OR Est. SE OR Est. SE OR Est. SE OR Est. SE OR Est. SE

Child age 1.07 .07*** .02 1.06 .06*** .01 1.09 .08*** .02 1.08 .08** .03 1.06 .05*** .02
Children’s sex .85 −.17 .22 1.02 .02 .11 .81 −.22 .24 .83 −.18 .31 .57 −.57** .23
Preschool enrolment 2.62 .97*** .31 2.86 1.05*** .11 2.02 .70** .27 2.66 .98 .77 1.46 .38 .29
Mothers’ age .86 −.15 .09 .90 −.11*** .04 1.08 .08 .09 .96 −.04 .11 .97 −.03 .08
Marital status .73 −.32 .35 .97 −.03 .10 .46 −.77* .40 .98 −.02 .29 1.31 .27 .32
Area of residence .91 −.10 .34 1.17 .16 .11 1.62 .48* .27 1.89 .64** .30 1.32 .28 .27
Mothers’ education 1.10 .09 .29 .88 −.13 .11 1.21 .19 .23 2.42 .89** .46 1.84 .61* .33
Household wealth index 1.37 .31** .16 1.69 .52*** .07 1.31 .27** .13 1.21 .19 .23 1.56 .44** .20
Positive discipline 1.14 .13 .12 1.14 .13** .06 1.05 .05 .12 1.08 .08 .18 1.00 .00 .13
Psychological aggression 1.02 .02 .22 1.00 .00 .11 1.26 .23 .22 .90 −.11 .26 .74 −.31 .20
Physical punishment .89 −.12 .26 .71 −.35*** .11 .58 −.55** .26 1.09 .09 .33 .68 −.39 .26
Harsh discipline .77 −.26* .15 .94 −.06 .07 .88 −.13 .14 1.01 .01 .18 .86 −.16 .16

*p < .10. **p < .05. ***p < .001. OR = Odds Ratio.

Jamaica Suriname

Children’s age, mothers’ education and area of res- Children’s age and household wealth index were pos-
idence were positively associated with children’s liter- itively associated with children’s literacy skills. Chil-
acy skills. Positive discipline, harsh physical punish- dren’s sex was negatively associated with children’s lit-
ment, physical punishment and psychological aggression eracy skills, indicating that girls showed higher lev-
were not significantly associated with children’s literacy els of literacy skills compared to boys. There were
skills. no significant associations between positive discipline,

© 2017 International Union of Psychological Science


DISCIPLINARY PRACTICES AND LITERACY SKILL 7

harsh physical punishment, physical punishment and positive discipline and those in the Dominican Repub-
psychological aggression, and children’s literacy skills. lic were less likely to use positive methods of discipline
than those in the other four countries. It was also the case
that caregivers in Suriname were more likely to use psy-
DISCUSSION chological aggression than those in the other countries.
In terms of physical punishment, caregivers in Jamaica
Physical punishment has received worldwide attention stood apart from those in the other countries. While it is
because it places children at increased risk for maltreat- not immediately apparent to us what may account for the
ment and because of its implications for children’s rights country-level differences in disciplinary practices, stud-
(Gershoff & Grogan-Kaylor, 2016). Drawing on UNICEF ies have confirmed high levels of endorsement and use
MICS 4 and 5 data, we determined between-country dif- of physical punishment in Jamaica (e.g. Brown & John-
ferences in the use of positive discipline, harsh physical son, 2008), and high levels of physical punishment and
punishment, physical punishment, psychological aggres- psychological aggression (29.8% with boys and 34.3%
sion and the associations between these four disciplinary with girls) were recorded across ethnic groups in Suri-
practices and preschool-aged children’s literacy skills in name (Van der Kooij et al., 2015; van der Kooij et al.,
the developing Caribbean nations of Belize, the Domini- 2017). Along with firmly entrenched beliefs about the use
can Republic, Guyana, Jamaica and Suriname. Using of physical punishment and psychological aggression by
large, representative samples from diverse ethnic back- parents to address childhood difficulties in the Caribbean,
grounds in these countries, our analyses provided a further socioeconomic conditions and limited knowledge about
appropriate parenting practices may have also contributed
assessment of the degrees of associations between harsh
to variations in caregivers’ greater use of harsh discipline
and non-harsh disciplinary practices and children’s liter-
across countries.
acy skills in a part of the developing world where phys-
Although studies have established links between phys-
ical punishment is seen as normative (Roopnarine & Jin,
ical punishment and children’s academic performance in
2016).
general, few have focused on harsh disciplinary prac-
Unlike previous assertions about the widespread use
tices and young children’s literacy skills in the devel-
of physical punishment and psychological aggression
oping world. Early literacy skills provide the founda-
with children in Caribbean countries such as Curacao,
tion for children’s language development during the early
Suriname, Guyana, Barbados, Trinidad and Tobago and
schooling process (Mol & Bus, 2011) and harsh disci-
Jamaica (Anderson & Payne, 1994; Brown & Johnson,
plinary practices can severely limit attempts for parents to
2008; Klein, Meyboom de-Jong, & de Bruijin, 2013; engage in language-rich activities (e.g. book reading, sto-
Rohner et al., 1991; Van der Kooij et al., 2015), these data rytelling, general conversations) with children. In Guyana
showed more diverse use of disciplinary practices. Across and the Dominican Republic, physical punishment had
the five linguistically and ethnically diverse Caribbean negative associations with children’s literacy skills. There
countries, 46% of caregivers took privileges away and was also a tendency for harsh physical punishment to
fully 72% used explanations with children. At the same be negatively associated with children’s literacy skills in
time, 40% spanked, hit or slapped their preschool-aged Belize. These findings are in line with those of a grow-
children and 56% yelled or screamed at the child. These ing body of studies in the developed world such as the
findings tend to confirm the simultaneous use of positive United States (Lee, Altschul, & Gershoff, 2013; Dede
and harsh methods of attending to childhood behavioural Yildirim & Roopnarine, 2015; Roopnarine et al., 2014)
difficulties that have been suggested by other studies that point to the negative associations between physi-
(e.g. Roopnarine et al., 2013). This approach to childrea- cal discipline and children’s cognitive and social skills
ring does not fall neatly into well-established parenting during the preschool years. A few studies have shown
styles typologies (e.g. authoritative, authoritarian or per- direct associations between severity of physical punish-
missive) (Baumrind, 1967; Baumrind, 1996) or interper- ment and children’s behaviours in Guyanese families (e.g.
sonal acceptance-rejection theory (Rohner & Khaleque, Roopnarine et al., 2013) and in families in other cultural
2005) that espouses levels of warmth at one end and con- groups around the world (e.g. Dede Yildirim & Roopnar-
trol/rejection at the other as its core tenets. It is likely that ine, 2015; Lee et al., 2013), and some are beginning to
the use of a combination of positive and harsh methods demonstrate the long-term consequences of physical pun-
of discipline is a result of adaptations to rearing children ishment on children’s language development in particu-
in difficult ecological niches that are plagued by neigh- lar (MacKenzie et al., 2013). That positive discipline was
bourhood infrastructure deficiencies, persistent economic associated with better literacy skills is not surprising given
hardship and personal challenges. that explanations and redirection involve two-way lan-
There was a good deal of variation in the use of differ- guage exchanges between caregivers and children. These
ent methods of discipline across countries. A few trends exchanges can only serve to boost emerging literacy skills
are notable. Caregivers in Belize were more likely to use (see Bus et al., 1995; Mol & Bus, 2011).

© 2017 International Union of Psychological Science


8 DEDE YILDIRIM AND ROOPNARINE

Despite inherent difficulties (e.g. method variance, Durbrow, E. H. (1999). Cultural processes in child competence:
underreporting in the case of physical punishment) How rural Caribbean parents evaluate their children. In A.
associated with total reliance on parental reports as S. Masten (Ed.), Cultural processes in child development:
opposed to observations of parental behaviours and the The Minnesota symposia on child psychology. (Vol. 29, pp.
97–121). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
lack of standardised assessments of children’s literacy
Ferguson, C. J. (2013). Spanking, corporal punishment and
skills, this study provides additional insights into the
long term outcomes: A meta-analytic review of longitudinal
use of different modes of discipline and children’s early studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 33, 196–208.
cognitive functioning in five developing Caribbean coun- Gershoff, E. T. (2002). Corporal punishment by parents and
tries. These results begin to question the widespread associated child behaviors and experiences: A meta-analytic
use of physical means of disciplining children in the and theoretical review. Psychological Bulletin, 128,
Caribbean in that there was more varied use of positive 539–579.
and harsh approaches in attending to childhood difficul- Gershoff, E. T., & Grogan-Kaylor, A. (2016). Spanking and
ties across countries. However, these findings bring to the child outcomes: Old controversies and new meta-analyses.
fore the negative consequences of physical punishment Journal of Family Psychology, 30(3), 453–469. https://doi
on preschool children’s literacy skills in developing .org/10.1037/fam0000191
Caribbean countries where children face multiple risk Isbell, R., Sobel, J., Lindauer, L., & Lowrence, A. (2004). The
effects of storytelling and story reading on the oral lan-
factors (e.g. neighbourhood violence; lack of parenting
guage complexity and story comprehension of young chil-
resources). Information derived from more careful exam- dren. Early Childhood Education Journal, 32(3), 157–163.
ination of what mechanisms mediate and moderate the Klein, B.,. A. A., Meyboom de-Jong, B., & de Bruijin, J. (2013).
associations between different methods of discipline and Child abuse: A common problem in curacao. West Indian
childhood outcomes in developing nations can be helpful Medical Journal, 62, 127–134.
in constructing policies and programmes that advance Lee, S. J., Altschul, I., & Gershoff, E. T. (2013). Does warmth
the rights and welfare of children. moderate longitudinal associations between maternal
spanking and child aggression in early childhood? Develop-
Manuscript received January 2017 mental Psychology, 49, 2017–2028 https://doi.org/10.1037/
Revised manuscript accepted September 2017 a0031630
Leo-Rhynie, E., & Brown, J. (2013). Child rearing practices
in the Caribbean in the early childhood years. In C. Logie
REFERENCES
& J. L. Roopnarine (Eds.), Issues and perspectives in early
Anderson, S., & Payne, M. (1994). Corporal punishment in childhood education in the Caribbean. La Romaine, Trinidad
elementary education: Views of Barbadian school children. and Tobago: Caribbean Publishers.
Child Abuse and Neglect, 18, 377–386. Levtov, R., van der Gaag, N., Greene, M., Kaufman, M., &
Baumrind, D. (1967). Child care practices anteceding three Barker, G. (2015). State of the world’s fathers. Washington,
patterns of preschool behavior. Genetic Psychology Mono- DC: Prumondo, Rutgers, Save the Chilldren, Sonke Justice
graphs, 75, 43–88. Centre, and the Engage Alliance.
Baumrind, D. (1996). Parenting: The discipline controversy MacKenzie, M. J., Nicklas, E., Waldfogel, J., & Brooks-Gunn,
revisited. Family Relations, 45, 405–414. J. (2013). Spanking and child development across the first
Berlin, L. J., Malone, P. S., Ayoub, C. A., Ispa, J., Fine, M., decade of life. Pediatrics, 21, e1118–e1125. https://doi.org/
Brooks-Gunn, J., … Bai, Y. (2009). Correlates and conse- 10.1542/peds.31013-1227
quences of spanking and verbal punishment for low income Mol, S., & Bus, A. G. (2011). To read or not to read: A
White, African American, and Mexican American toddlers. meta-analysis of print exposure from infancy to early adult-
Child Development, 80, 1403–1420. hood. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 267–296.
Brown, J., & Johnson, S. (2008). Child rearing and child partic- Morrison, F. J., & Cooney, R. R. (2002). Parenting and aca-
ipation in Jamaican families. International Journal of Early demic achievement: Multiple paths to early literacy. In J. G.
Years Education, 16, 31–41. Borkowski, S. L. Ramey, & M. Bristol-Power (Eds.), Par-
Burger, K. (2010). How does early care and education affect enting and the child’s world: Influences on academic, intel-
cognitive development? An international review of the effects lectual, and social-emotional development. (pp. 141–160).
of early interventions for children from different social back- Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
grounds. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 25, 140–165. Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2014). Mplus user’s guide.
Bus, A. G., van Ijzendoorn, M. H., & Pellegrini, A. D. (1995). (7th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.
Joint book reading makes for success in learning to read: A Paolucci, E. O., & Violato, A. (2004). A meta-analysis of the
meta-analysis on intergenerational transmission of literacy. published research on the affective, cognitive, and behavioral
Review of Educational Research, 64, 1–21. effects of corporal punishment. Journal of Psychology, 138,
Dede Yildirim, E., & Roopnarine, J. L. (2015). The mediating 197–221.
role of maternal warmth in the relationship between harsh Pasek, J. (2016). Package “Weights”. R package. Available from
parental practices and externalizing and internalizing behav- https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/weights/weights.pdf
iors in Hispanic American, African American, and European Rohner, R. P., Kean, K. J., & Cournoyer, D. E. (1991). Effects
American families. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority of corporal punishment, perceived caretaker warmth, and
Psychology, 21, 430–439. cultural beliefs on the psychological adjustment of children

© 2017 International Union of Psychological Science


DISCIPLINARY PRACTICES AND LITERACY SKILL 9

in St. Kitts, West Indies. Journal of Marriage and the Family, Straus, M. A., & Paschall, M. J. (2009). Corporal punishment
53, 681–693. by mothers and development of children’s cognitive ability:
Rohner, R. P., & Khaleque, A. (Eds.) (2005). Handbook for the A longitudinal study of two nationally representative age
study of parental acceptance and rejection. (4th ed.). Storrs, cohorts. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma, 18,
CT: Rohner Research Publications. 159–183. https://doi.org/10.1080/10926770903035168
Roopnarine, J. L., & Jin, B. (2016). Family socialization prac- Sun, J., Liu, Y., Chen, E., Rao, N., & Liu, H. (2016). Fac-
tices and childhood development in Caribbean cultural com- tors related to parents’ engagement in cognitive and
munities. In J. L. Roopnarine & D. Chadee (Eds.), Caribbean socio-emotional caregiving in developing countries: Results
psychology: Indigenous contributions to a global disci- from multiple indicator cluster 3. Early Childhood Research
pline. (pp. 71–96). Washington, DC: American Psychologi- Quarterly, 36, 21–31.
cal Association. Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Shannon, J. D., Cabrera, N. J., & Lamb,
Roopnarine, J. L., Jin, B., & Krishnakumar, A. (2014). Do M. E. (2004). Fathers and mothers at play with their 2-and
Guyanese mothers’ levels of warmth moderate the associa- 3-year-olds: Contributions to language and cognitive devel-
tion between harshness and justness of physical punishment opment. Child Development, 75, 1806–1820.
and preschoolers’ prosocial behaviours and anger? Interna- UNDP (United Nations Development Programme) (2016).
tional Journal of Psychology, 49(4), 271–279. https://doi Caribbean human development report. New York: UNDP.
.org/10.1002/ijop.12029 United Nations Children Fund (2015). Monitoring the situation
Roopnarine, J. L., Krishnakumar, A., Narine, L., Logie, C., of children and women for 20 years: The multiple indica-
& Lape, M. (2014). Relationships between parenting prac- tor cluster surveys (MICS) 1995–2015. New York: UNICEF.
tices and preschoolers’ social kills in African, Indo, and Available from http://mics.unicef.org/publications/reports-
mixed-ethnic families in Trinidad and Tobago: The mediat- and-methodological-papers
ing role of ethnic socialization. Journal of Cross-Cultural Van der Kooij, I. W., Nieuwendam, J., Bipat, S., Boer, F., Lin-
Psychology, 45(3), 362–380. https://doi.org/10.1177/ dauer, R. J., & Graafsma, T. L. (2015). A national study on
0022022113509894 the prevalence of child abuse and neglect in Suriname. Child
Roopnarine, J. L., Yang, Y., Krishnakumar, A., & Davidson, Abuse & Neglect, 47, 153–161. https://doi.org/10.1016/j
K. L. (2013). Parenting practices in Guyana and Trinidad .chiabu.2015.03.019
and Tobago: Connections to preschoolers’ social and cog- van der Kooij, I. W., Nieuwendam, J., Moerman, G., Boer,
nitive skills. Interamerican Journal of Psychology, 47(2), F., Lindauer, R. J. L., Roopnarine, J. L., & Graafsma, T.
313–328. L. G. (2017). Perceptions of corporal punishment among
Rutter, M., & Sroufe, L. A. (2000). Developmental psy- Creole and Maroon professionals and community members
chopathology: Concepts and challenges. Development and in Suriname. Child Abuse Review, 26(4), 275–288.
Psychopathology, 12(3), 265–296. Votruba-Drzal, E., Miller, P., & Coley, R. L. (2015). Poverty,
Shonkoff, J. P. (2010). Building a new biodevelopmental frame- urbanicity, and children’s development of early academic
work to guide the future of early childhood policy. Child skills. Child Development Perspectives, 10(1), 3–9. https://
Development, 81(1), 357–367. https://doi.org/10.1111/j doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12152
.1467-8624.2009.01399.x Wilson, L. C., Wilson, C. M., & Berkeley-Caines, L. (2003).
Shumow, L., Vandell, D. L., & Posner, J. K. (1998). Harsh, Age, gender and socioeconomic differences in parental
firm, and permissive parenting in low-income families: Rela- socialization preferences in Guyana. Journal of Comparative
tions to children’s academic achievement and behavioral Family Studies, 34, 213–227.
adjustment. Journal of Family Issues, 19(5), 483–507.
https://doi.org/10.1177/019251398019005001

© 2017 International Union of Psychological Science

You might also like