You are on page 1of 12

J Neurosurg 64:693-704, 1986

Review Article

The right hemisphere: neuropsychological functions


KENNETH M. HEILMAN, M.D., DAWN BOWERS, PH.D., EDWARD VALENSTEIN, M.D., AND
ROBERT T. WATSON, M.D.
Department of Neurology, College of Medicine, University of Florida, and Veterans Administration
Medical Center, Gainesville, Florida

v" In the past two to three decades, clinicians and neuroscientists have been studying the functions of the
fight hemisphere. Neither hemisphere seems to be dominant in the absolute sense. Each appears to be
specialized and is dominant for different functions. However, most functions require the cooperation of both
hemispheres. When one is damaged, the other can often compensate for the damaged one. Lesions of the left
hemisphere are associated with language (speech, reading, and writing) and praxic disorders, and lesions of the
fight hemisphere can result in visuospatial, attentional, and emotional disorders.
The authors review some of the major behavioral disorders associated with fight hemisphere dysfunction
and concentrate on three major types of disorders - - visuospatial, attentional, and emotional. Although not
all the behavioral defects associated with fight hemisphere damage can be subgrouped under these three types,
they are the ones most often associated with right hemisphere lesions.

KEY WORDS 9right cerebral hemisphere 9 visuospatial disorder 9


attentional disorder 9 psychological function 9 emotional disorder

A
FTER Broca's description ~8 of eight fight-handed behavioral functions. Because the functions of the right
patients who had aphasia associated with left hemisphere are not recognized, surgeons are more likely
hemisphere lesions, the left hemisphere became to operate on the right hemisphere. For example, ven-
known as the dominant (controlling) side. Other 19th tricular shunts are more often placed through the right
and early 20th century neurologists provided evidence hemisphere. Surgeons also feel more at ease when re-
that the left hemisphere also mediated skilled move- moving tumors and epileptic foci from the right hemi-
ment as well as cognitive activities such as reading and sphere association areas because they are less concerned
writing. These reports provide additional evidence for about the residual behavioral defects.
the dominance of the left hemisphere. In the past two to three decades behavioral neurolo-
Clinicians were well aware that lesions of the fight gists, neuropsychologists, neurosurgeons, and neuro-
hemisphere may induce contralateral sensory and mo- physiologists have been studying the functions of the
tor defects. Although there were reports of visuospa- right hemisphere. Neither hemisphere seems to be dom-
tial disorders associated with the fight hemisphere dys- inant in the absolute sense. Each appears to be special-
function, 55 the higher functions of the "non-dominant" ized and is dominant for different functions. However,
fight hemisphere were largely unrecognized. Even today most functions require the cooperation of both hemi-
many clinicians think that, other than sensory or motor spheres. When one is damaged, the other can often
function, the fight hemisphere mediates no important compensate for the damaged one. Left hemisphere le-
sions are associated with language (speech, reading, and
This contribution is the 20th in a series of review articles writing) and praxic disorders, and right hemisphere
selected jointly by the Committee on Graduate Education of
the American Association of Neurological Surgeons and the lesions with visuospatial, attentional, and emotional
Congress of Neurological Surgeons, and the Journal of Neu- disorders.
rosurgery. - - Editor. In reviewing some of the major behavioral disorders

J. Neurosurg. / Volume 6 4 / M a y , 1986 693


K. M. Heilman, et al.

associated with right hemisphere dysfunction, we shall objects and drawing. A defect in spatial-organization
concentrate on three major types of disorders - - vis- performance is referred to as constructional apraxia.
uospatial, attentional, and emotional. Although not all Many assembling and drawing tasks, including block
behavioral defects associated with right hemisphere designs, stick constructions, construction of models,
damage can be subgrouped under these three types, drawing to command, and copying a drawing, have
they are the ones most often associated with right been used to assess constructional praxis. Kleist 56 ini-
hemisphere lesions. tially thought that constructional apraxia was associated
with left parietal dysfunction. Multiple studies have
Visuospatial Disorders since shown that constructional apraxia is more fre-
Almost all human behavior has some spatial com- quent and more severe after right than after left hemi-
ponent. For example, the ability to recognize and name sphere disease (for a review seen Benton8). However,
objects, letters, and printed words relies, at least in part, some investigators have not found differences between
on spatial discriminations. However, most of these ac- patients with right and those with left hemisphere dam-
tivities appear to be mediated by the left hemisphere. age. Others have suggested that there may be qualitative
Benton 8 noted important differences between the iden- differences in the performance of patients with right
tification of objects and their localization in space. and those with left hemisphere damage. Left hemi-
These defects are dissociable; patients with right-sided sphere damage induces an executive-ideation defect,
damage can often identify words, letters, objects, and and right hemisphere damage induces a visuospatial
even faces, but many have difficulty in localizing objects defect.93 Unfortunately, this hemispheric dichotomy
in space. Hughlings Jackson 55 was one of the first to has not been completely supported.
recognize the significance of visuospatial disorders. He Patients with isolated right hemisphere disease not
described a patient who had a tumor in the posterior only can name or recognize words, letters, objects, and
portion of the right hemisphere who lost his way and familiar people, but they also have no difficulty making
could not dress himself. simple visual discriminations. However, when visual
Defective localization in space can be tested using a discriminations become difficult and the patient must
large variety of tasks. For example, Warrington and rely more on spatial skill, 9'65 patients with right hemi-
Rabin 94 presented pairs of cards containing dots in a sphere disease may have defective performance. Pa-
two-dimensional spatial position. The subjects were tients with right hemisphere damage may also have
asked whether the dots were located in the same place difficulty discriminating between incomplete or muti-
on both cards. The performance of patients with right lated figures. 58'92
parietal disease was poorer than the results of those Discriminating and matching unfamiliar faces ap-
with left hemisphere disease or of a control group pears to be different from recognizing familiar faces.
without brain damage. Hannay, eta/., 43 also demon- Although prosopagnosia (inability to recognize familar
strated that right hemisphere-damaged patients have faces) is associated with bilateral lesions, 64 the inability
defective localization. In both of these studies the in- to discriminate unfamiliar faces appears to be associated
vestigators used relative rather than absolute localiza- with right hemisphere lesions. 7 Unlike the patients with
tion, and Benton 8 suggested that the right hemisphere prosopagnosia who have bilateral inferior temporo-oc-
may be more important for allocentric (location in cipital lesions, patients with an inability to discriminate
relation to external cues) than egocentric (location in unfamiliar faces usually have right parietal lesions. That
relation to self) localization. Judgment of direction 9 most patients with prosopagnosia can discriminate un-
and distance (depth perception) 22 may also be impaired familiar faces suggests that the inferior temporo-occip-
with right hemisphere damage. It is nevertheless impor- ital regions are not critical for this type of visuospatial
tant to recognize that there are two types of depth function. Most patients with profound visuospatial dis-
perception - - global and local stereopsis - - and it is orders from right parietal disease also have little diffi-
only the former (which does not depend on binocular culty in recognizing familiar faces.
disparity) that is impaired with right hemisphere lesions. Studies in monkeys led Mishkin and co-workers66 to
Geographic and topographic orientation may be conclude that the striate cortex is the source of two
tested in various manners. For example, Hfcaen, et multisynaptic corticocortical pathways. One courses
al., 44 had patients identify principal geographic land- ventrally, connecting the striate, prestriate, and inferior
marks such as cities and rivers on a map and found temporal areas; the other runs dorsally interconnecting
that those with right hemisphere retrorolandic lesions with the striate, prestriate, and parietal lobe. Although
performed more poorly than did control subjects with the ventral system seems important for recognition of
left hemisphere damage. Although this type of test is objects, the dorsal system appears to influence spatial
easy to perform in the clinic, Benton 8 ascribes to ne- localization. In humans, ventral pathways also appear
glect, rather than to visuospatial disorders, the propen- important in object recognition. However, the dorsal
sity of patients with right hemisphere damage to make system in humans appears to be lateralized, the right
errors on this test. parietal lobe having a critical role in visuospatial proc-
Visuospatial skills are also necessary for building essing.

694 J. Neurosurg. / Volume 64/May, 1986


The fight hemisphere: neuropsychological functions
Attentional and Intentional Disorders has been recognized that the reticular activating system
When patients with fight parietal disease are asked is critical for arousal, a physiological state of heightened
to draw a picture, they often fail to draw the left side. excitability that prepares the organism for sensory and
This type of defect was thought to reflect a visuospatial motor processing. Whereas bilateral lesions of the retic-
defect and had been termed "visual spatial agnosia. "63 ular activating system induce coma, unilateral lesions
Visuospatial agnosia, or hemispatial neglect, is part of induce contralateral neglect. The reticular activating
the neglect syndrome, which is a group of behavioral system does not have direct connections to the cortex.
disorders in which a subject fails to report, respond to, The two possible mechanisms by which this system
or orient toward novel or meaningful stimuli. The may influence arousal are a polysynaptic pathway to
response failure occurs when the patient has to move a the cortex through the thalamus and a modulating
contralateral limb or move a body part toward or in activity of the thalamic reticular nucleus (for review see
contralateral hemispace. The report failure occurs when Watson, et a/.99). The thalamic reticular nucleus is a
stimuli are presented contralateral to a brain lesion. thin nucleus that envelops the thalamus, projects to the
Although elements of the neglect syndrome can follow thalamic relay nuclei, and inhibits relay to the cortex. 81
left hemisphere lesions, neglect is more frequent and The reticular activating system projects to the thalamic
severe after fight hemisphere lesions. Four major ele- reticular nucleus and inhibits this inhibitory nucleus,
ments of the neglect syndrome will be discussed sepa- allowing increased transmission of sensory information
to the cortex. 84
rately in this review: 1) sensory neglect or hemi-inatten-
tion; 2) extinction to simultaneous stimuli; 3) motor In the human and the ape, the primary sensory cortex
neglect; and 4) hemispatial neglect. projects to modality-specific sensory association areas,
which project to polymodal sensory areas, including the
temporoparietal region. 69 The polymodal sensory areas
Sensory Neglect may be important in modeling (detecting stimulus nov-
For hemi-inattention or sensory neglect to be tested, elty). When a stimulus is novel, multimodal conver-
somatesthetic, visual, and auditory stimuli should be gence areas may have a general inhibitory action on the
presented in a random order to the sides ipsilateral and thalamic reticular nucleus or may activate the mesen-
contralateral to the lesion. Although sensory loss may cephalic reticular formation, thereby providing an
be the most common cause of a failure to report or arousal response after cortical analysis.
respond to a stimulus presented contralateral to the Unlike stimulus novelty, stimulus significance is de-
damaged hemisphere, patients and animals with lesions termined partly by the needs of the organism. Limbic
in locations other than a primary sensory area or sen- system input into the temporoparietal region may pro-
sory projection system may also fail to report or respond vide information about biological needs. Since the fron-
to stimuli. This is termed "sensory neglect." If the lesion tal lobes are critical in goal-oriented behavior and de-
site is unknown, one may be unable to distinguish veloping sets, the frontal lobes may provide input about
hemianesthesia or hemianopsia from severe inatten- goals that are neither stimulus-dependent nor moti-
tion. Unilateral central nervous system lesions, how- vated by an immediate biological need. The frontal
ever, do not produce a contralateral hearing loss. Even lobes and cingulate cortex have strong input into the
patients with a peripheral hearing loss will respond to temporoparietal region. These connections may pro-
unilateral stimuli unless the sound is of low amplitude vide an anatomical substrate by which motivational
and close to the ear. Therefore, when patients do not states may influence stimulus processing. 4599 Cellular
respond to unilateral auditory stimuli, they usually have recordings from the parietal lobe have shown that some
sensory neglect. neurons enhance their activity when an animal is pre-
Several theories have been proposed to account for sented with motivationally significant visual stimuli.
sensory neglect, which has been postulated to be in- The activity of these neurons is spatially selective. Re-
duced by decreased sensory input. T M Brain 17 believed cordings from parietal neurons suggest that the parietal
that the parietal lobes contain the body schema and lobe is subserving attentional processes. 2~
also mediate spatial perception. Parietal lesions, there- In summary, the temporoparietal region receives
fore, caused the patient to fail to recognize only half his polymodal sensory, limbic, and frontal input; can acti-
body and half of space. Neglect in humans and mon- vate the reticular activating system; and has cells that
keys can be induced by lesions in many different brain show increased activity when presented with significant
regions, including the temporoparietal occipital junc- stimuli. Because the temporoparietal region appears to
tion; 25'52 limbic areas, such as the cingulate gyrus; 52'96 be mediating attentional processes, it should not be
and subcortical areas, such as the thalamus 95 and mes- surprising that sensory neglect occurs when the region
encephalic reticular formation. 73'97Heilman and Valen- is ablated.
stein 49 and Watson, et al., 96"99 postulated that sensory In humans, neglect is more frequent and severe after
neglect is an attentional-arousal disorder induced by fight than after left hemisphere lesions. ~7'25The atten-
dysfunction in a corticolimbic reticular formation loop. tional cells found in the parietal lobe of monkeys by
Since the classic work of Moruzzi and Magoun, 68 it Lynch62 and Robinson, eta/., 75 usually have contralat-

J, Neurosurg. / Volume 64/May, 1986 695


K. M. Heilman, et al.

eral receptive fields, but some of these neurons have entation. The mechanism underlying obscuration, how-
bilateral receptive fields. That is, some respond to stim- ever, is not known.
uli presented in the right or the left visual half-fields. Several investigators have suggested that extinction
To account for a hemispheric asymmetry of attention and perhaps obscuration result from suppression or
in humans, we suggest that the temporoparietal regions reciprocal inhibition. In the case of cerebral damage,
of the human brain also have attentional or compara- the normal hemisphere inhibits the damaged hemi-
tor neurons, but that the cells in the right hemisphere sphere more than the damaged hemisphere inhibits the
are more likely to have bilateral receptive fields than normal one. Consequently, stimuli contralateral to the
are cells in the left hemisphere. Thus, cells in the left damaged hemisphere are not perceived when the nor-
hemisphere would be activated predominantly by novel mal side is stimulated. The physiological mechanisms
or significant stimuli in the right hemispace or hemi- that induce this reciprocal inhibition are unknown.
field, but cells in the right hemisphere would be acti- Each association cortex may project not only to the
vated by novel or significant stimuli in either visual ipsilateral but also to the contralateral thalamic reticular
field or either side of the hemispace, or both. If this nucleus. Unlike the ipsilateral connections, which are
were the case, right hemisphere lesions would more inhibitory, the contralateral projections may be facili-
often cause inattention than would left hemisphere tatory.
lesions. When the left hemisphere is damaged, the right Another explanation for extinction, the limited atten-
can attend to ipsilateral stimuli, but the left cannot tion theory, specifies that under normal circumstances
attend to ipsilateral stimuli after right-sided damage. bilateral simultaneous stimuli are processed simulta-
Support for this hypothesis has been provided by both neously, with each hemisphere processing the contra-
physiological5~and imaging studies. TM lateral stimulus. However, a damaged hemisphere may
be unable to attend to contralateral stimuli, making the
Extinction to Simultaneous Stimulation organism inattentive to those stimuli. As the organism
recovers, it becomes capable of attending to contralat-
Most patients improve after initial sensory neglect. eral stimuli. This improvement may be mediated by
At first they ignore stimuli presented to the side opposite the normal ipsilateral hemisphere. The normal hemi-
the lesion, but they eventually become able to detect sphere, however, may have a limited attentional capac-
and lateralize these stimuli correctly. When given bilat- ity. Therefore, with bilateral simultaneous stimulation,
eral stimulation, however, they often fail to report the the normal hemisphere's attentional mechanism is oc-
stimulus presented to the side contralateral to the lesion. cupied with the contralateral stimulus and may be
This phenomenon, first noted by Loeb 6~to occur in the unable to attend to the ipsilateral stimulus.
tactile modality, has been termed "extinction to double
simultaneous stimulation. ''6 It may also occur in the
auditory and visual modalities.6 Motor Neglect (Hemiakinesia)
The methods used to test for extinction are similar An aroused animal can selectively attend to a stim-
to those used to test for sensory neglect. If the patient ulus and either act or not act. The decision is based not
responds normally to unilateral stimulation, bilateral only on the nature of the stimulus but also on the state
simultaneous stimulation may be used. Unilateral stim- of the organism. The physiological state in which one
uli should be randomly interspersed with bilateral si- prepares for an action is called "intention." A disorder
multaneous stimuli. The lesions causing extinction are of intentional processes leads to akinesia.
often in the same areas as lesions that cause sensory Akinesia is behaviorally defined as the inability to
neglect. However, certain forms of extinction may also initiate a movement in an aroused organism in which
occur after lesions of the corpus callosum, 85 and even the lack of movement cannot be attributed to a defect
left-sided extinction has been reported to follow left in the motor unit or pyramidal system. Akinesia may
hemisphere lesions. 82 also be expressed as less forceful movements, move-
Different mechanisms may underlie extinction in ment of decreased amplitude (hypometria), and slower
patients with callosal lesions, sensory defects, or hemi- movement (bradykinesia). Some patients have in-
spheric lesions. Such mechanisms are poorly under- creased contralateral limb akinesia when they must use
stood, but several explanatory hypotheses have been their ipsilateral extremities simultaneously. This has
advanced. Because patients with partial deafferentation been termed "motor extinction. ''s9
may have extinction, several authors have postulated a Lesions of the dorsolateral frontal lobe, especially the
sensory mechanism to explain sensory neglect and sen- region of the frontal eye field (Brodmann's area) may
sory extinction from parietal lesions.6 Psychophysical induce contralateral akinesia. 98 The connections of the
methods have been used to demonstrate that in normal frontal eye fields are important in understanding the
subjects sensory threshold increases on one side when possible role of the dorsolateral frontal lobe in inten-
the opposite side is stimulated (obscuration). ~~If this tion. 99 It has reciprocal connections with unimodal and
obscuration phenomenon occurs in a patient with an polymodal posterior sensory association cortices23 and
elevated threshold caused by an afferent lesion, it would is an area of sensory convergence. ~2 The dorsolateral
appear similar to extinction in patients without deaffer- frontal lobe has reciprocal connections with medial

696 J. Neurosurg. / Volume 6 4 / M a y , 1986


The right hemisphere: neuropsychological functions
(nonspecific) thalamic nuclei. Projections to the mes- an organism for action and thereby reduce reaction
encephalic reticular formation,57 as well as nonrecipro- times? 9 Because patients with fight hemisphere lesions
cal projections to the caudate nucleus, also exist. Last, have been shown to have reduced behavioral evidence
the dorsolateral frontal lobe also receives input from of activation,54it has been postulated that in the human
the limbic system, mainly from the anterior cingulate the right hemisphere may dominate in mediating the
gyrus.3 intention (activation) process? ~ That is, the left hemi-
Its connections with neocortical sensory association sphere prepares the fight extremities for action, and the
and sensory convergence areas may provide the frontal fight prepares both. Therefore, with left-sided lesions,
lobe with information about external stimuli that may left limb akinesia is minimal, but with fight-sided le-
call the organism to action. The limbic connections sions, there is severe left limb akinesia. In addition,
(anterior cingulate gyrus) may provide the frontal lobe because the right hemisphere is more involved than the
with motivational information. Connections with the left in activating the right extremities with fight hemi-
mesencephalic reticular formation may be important sphere lesions, there is more ipsilateral hypokinesia than
in arousal, and connections with the basal ganglia and with left hemisphere lesions.
premotor regions may be related to motor preparation. That the fight hemisphere dominates mediation of
Physiological studies support the hypothesis that the activation or intention (physiological readiness to re-
frontal lobe mediates intentional activity. 38 Lesions in spond) has been demonstrated in normal subjects. They
many of the regions that connect with the frontal lobe show more activation (measured behaviorally by reac-
may also induce contralateral akinesia, including poly- tion times) with warning stimuli delivered to the fight
modal sensory cortex (temporoparietal regions)9~ and hemisphere than with warning stimuli delivered to the
supplementary motor area cingulate regions (Meador, left hemisphere. That is, warning stimuli projected to
et al., in preparation). Neglect or akinesia may also the fight hemisphere reduced reaction times of the right
accompany lesions of the "nonspecific" intralaminar hand more than warning stimuli projected to the left
thalamic nuclei, 98 which project both to the frontal hemisphere reduced reaction times of the right hand.
lobes and to the neostriatum. Akinesia may also result Warning stimuli projected to the fight hemisphere re-
from lesions of the basal ganglia and ventral thalamus duced reaction times even more than did warning stim-
(ventralis lateralis and ventralis anterior). 9~ The basal uli projected directly to the left hemisphere. These
ganglia project to the ventral thalamus, and this "mo- results support the hypothesis that the fight hemisphere
tor" portion of the thalamus is also gated by the nucleus dominates intention (activation, preparation)? ~
reticularis thalami. The reticular nucleus of the thala-
mus may be inhibited by the mesencephalic reticular Hemispatial Neglect
formation during an arousal or orienting response and When patients with hemispatial neglect are asked to
be inhibited by the frontal lobes during a motor set.99 perform a variety of behavioral tasks, they neglect the
Much evidence suggests the importance of dopami- hemispace contralateral to their lesion. For example,
nergic neurons in the mediation of intention. Marked when asked to draw a picture of a flower, they may
defects of intention have long been known to be prom- draw only half of the flower. When asked to bisect a
inent in patients with Parkinson's disease, which is line, they may quarter it instead, and they may fail to
characterized pathologically by degeneration of ascend- cross out lines distributed over a page.
ing dopaminergic neurons. In animals, unilateral le- The lesions associated with hemispatial neglect are
sions in these pathways cause unilateral motor neglect. similar to those associated with motor and sensory
Patients with fight hemisphere lesions have a more neglect. Although hemianopsia may enhance the symp-
severe contralateral limb akinesia than do patients with toms of hemispatial neglect, hemianopsia alone cannot
left hemisphere lesions (Coslett and Heilman, in prep- entirely account for the deficit. First, some patients with
aration). Howes and Boiler54demonstrated that patients hemispatial neglect are not hemianopsic.63 Second,
with fight hemisphere lesions had slower reaction times hemianopsic patients without neglect do not perform
of the ipsilateral hand than did patients with left hemi- differently from nonhemianopsic patients in the visual
sphere lesions. The slowing of reaction time was not as discrimination of bisected lines. TM
dependent on lesion size as on location. Right hemi- The defective performance cannot be explained by a
sphere lesions appeared to induce the most profound visual half-field defect, but it may be a hemispatial
slowing. These investigators did not mention whether defect. Hemispace is a complex concept because it can
the patients with ipsilateral slowing had unilateral ne- be defined according to eye, head, or trunk position.
glect. In monkeys, no hemispheric asymmetries in the With the eyes and head facing directly forward, the
production of the neglect syndrome have been noted. hemispaces defined by these three standards are con-
However, monkeys with lesions inducing neglect had gruent. But if the eyes are directed to the far right, for
slower ipsilateral reaction times than did monkeys with example, the left visual field falls in large part in the
equal-sized lesions that did not induce neglect (Valen- fight hemispace, as defined by the head and body
stein, et al., in preparation). midline. Similarly, if the head and eyes are turned far
It has been shown that warning stimuli may prepare to the right, left head and eye hemispace can both be

J. Neurosurg. / Volume 6 4 / M a y , 1986 697


K. M. Heilman, et al.
in the right hemispace of the body. There is evidence normal pointing hand with their eyes closed to an
to suggest that head or body hemispace, or both, is imaginary point directly in front of them, they pointed
important in determining the symptoms of hemispatial toward right hemispace. 46 In addition, when directional
neglect45 (Coslett, et al., in preparation). For example, reaction times were measured, patients with hemispatial
when patients with left hemispatial neglect are asked to neglect were slower initiating a response toward left
bisect a line, their performance is poorer in left body than right hemispace) 3 These results cannot be ac-
hemispace than it is in right body hemispace even when counted for by inattention, representation disorders, or
using a strategy that ensures that the line is seen in the gaze defects; therefore, they provide evidence for a
normal right visual field. Similarly, independent of hemispatial or directional akinesia. Although these stud-
visual field and body hemispace, lines are more poorly ies support a directional or hemispatial akinesia, or
bisected in left head hemispace than in right head both, they do not refute alternative hypotheses. Atten-
hemispace. tion, intention, gaze, and memory (representations) are
The abnormal performance of patients in contralat- all closely linked functions.
eral space suggests that brain mechanisms relating to Lesions in the right hemisphere induce hemispatial
the opposite hemispace have been disturbed. It suggests neglect more often than do those in the left hemisphere.
that each hemisphere is responsible not only for receiv- The neglect induced by right hemisphere lesions is also
ing stimuli from the contralateral visual field and for more severe, t.33 The mechanism underlying this asym-
controlling the contralateral limbs, but also for atten- metry is unknown; however, the right hemisphere may
tion and intention in contralateral hemispace independ- be able to intend and attend in and toward both hemi-
ent of which visual field the stimulus enters or which spatial fields, but the left may be able to attend and
hand is used. 45 The postulate that each hemisphere intend only in and toward the right hemispatial field.
attends to and intends in contralateral hemispace has With left hemisphere lesions, therefore, the right hemi-
been supported by studies of normal subjectsJ 6 sphere will attend and intend in and toward ipsilateral
Several neuropsychological mechanisms could ac- (fight) hemispace. However, with right hemisphere le-
count for the hemispatial defect associated with neglect. sions, the left hemisphere attends and intends in and
Bisiach and Luzzatti ~3proposed a representational map toward the right hemispace; the left hemispace is ne-
hypothesis similar to Brain's body schema hypothesis.17 glected.
They asked two patients with fight hemisphere damage
to describe from memory a familar scene, the main Affeetive Disorders
square in Milan, from two different spatial perspectives Babinski 2 noted that patients with right hemisphere
(one facing the cathedral and the other facing away disease often appear indifferent or euphoric. Goldstein 4~
from the cathedral). Regardless of the patients' orienta- noted that patients with left hemisphere lesions and
tion, left-sided details were omitted. On the basis of the aphasia often showed a profound depression, which he
findings from these two patients and from a second termed the "catastrophic reaction." These clinical ob-
study, TM these investigators postulated that the mental servations were also seen by Gainotti 3~who studied 160
representation of the environment is structured topo- patients with lateralized brain damage.
graphically and is mapped across the brain. That is, the
visual image conjured up in the mind may be split Affective Comprehension
between the two hemispheres (like the projection of a Auditory-Verbal Comprehension. Patients with
real scene). With right hemisphere damage, there is a right hemisphere lesions might have a defect in the
representational disorder for the left half of this image. comprehension of affect. Developing an appropriate
The representation map postulated by Bisiach and co- emotion depends, in part, on comprehending speech.
workers ~3'~4may be hemispatially organized so that left Speech can simultaneously carry at least two different
hemispace is represented in the right hemisphere and types of information - - the linguistic content (what is
right hemispace is represented in the left hemisphere. said) and the affective content (how it is said). The
Hemispatial neglect may be associated with a gaze linguistic content is conveyed by a complex code that
defect which might prevent patients with hemispatial requires semantic and phonemic decoding. In contrast,
neglect from fully exploring the neglected side of the the affective content is conveyed by the pitch, tempo,
line. Partial support for the gaze hypothesis is provided and tonal contours of speech 7~ and consequently re-
by De Renzi, et al., 28 who demonstrated that patients quires a different type of processing.
with right hemisphere lesions and neglect have eye In the vast majority of persons (particularly fight-
deviation to the right. handed individuals), the left hemisphere is clearly more
Patients with hemispatial neglect may have a hemi- adept than the right in decoding the linguistic content
spatial attentional deficit. Although the line is "seen" of speech. Until about a decade ago little was known
in the normal visual field, the line is in neglected hemi- about how affective intonations were being mediated.
space and therefore is not attended to. Patients with To determine whether the fight hemisphere is more
neglect may also have a hemispatial or directional aki- adept than the left in decoding the affective components
nesia. 53 When patients were asked to point with their of speech, we read sentences with semantically neutral

698 J. Neurosurg. / Volume 64/May, 1986


The right hemisphere: neuropsychological functions
content (such as "The boy went to the store.") in four without captions. Patients with right hemisphere disease
different emotional intonations (happy, sad, angry, in- performed better with captions.
different) to patients with right temporoparietal infarc- We gave a variety of affective visual tasks to patients
tions and to those with aphasia and left temporoparietal with left or fight hemisphere lesions as well as to neu-
infarctions. The patients' task was to identify the emo- rological patients without hemispheric disease. 26 We
tional tone of the speaker. Patients with fight hemi- asked our subjects to discriminate between a pair of
sphere lesions performed worse on this task than did neutral faces (saying, for instance, "Are these two faces
those with left hemisphere lesions; suggesting that the the same person or two different people?"); to name
fight hemisphere is more critically involved in process- the emotion expressed by a face (happy, sad, angry,
ing the affective intonations of speech. 47 Further sup- indifferent); to select a "target" emotion from a multi-
port for the hypothesis that the two hemispheres process ple-choice array of faces (saying, for instance, "Point to
different aspects of speech (semantic content vs affec- the happy face."); and to determine whether two pic-
tive intonations) has been drawn from dichotic listening tures of the same person's face expressed the same or a
studies with normal adults. 42 different emotion.
The neuropsychological defect underlying the im- Compared with other groups, patients with right
paired ability of patients with right hemisphere disease hemisphere disease were markedly impaired in their
to identify affective intonations in speech is not entirely ability to discriminate between pairs of neutral faces, as
clear. The defect may be related to a cognitive inability has been previously discussed. Although both left and
to denote and classify affective stimuli. It could also right hemisphere-damaged groups had difficulty nam-
be related to an inability to discriminate perceptually ing and selecting emotional faces, there was a trend for
between affective intonations in speech. In a subse- patients with right hemisphere disease to perform more
quent study, we88 replicated our previous finding and poorly on these two tasks than patients with left hemi-
attempted to determine whether patients with fight sphere disease. In addition, patients with right hemi-
hemisphere disease could, in fact, discriminate between sphere disease were more impaired in making same/
affective intonations of speech without having to clas- different discriminations between emotional faces.
sify or denote these intonations. Patients were required When performance across these various affective
to listen to identical pairs of sentences spoken in either tasks was covaried for neutral facial discrimination (a
the same or different emotional tones. The patients did visuospatial nonemotional task), differences between
not have to identify the emotional intonation, but had the two groups disappeared. This finding suggests that
to tell whether the intonations associated with the sen- the defect underlying poor discrimination of neutral
tences sounded the same or different. Patients with right faces observed in the group with right hemisphere dis-
hemisphere disease performed more poorly on this task ease might also cause their inability to recognize and
than did patients with left hemisphere disease, which discriminate between emotional faces. However, the
suggests that the perceptual discrimination between poor facial discrimination by the group with right hemi-
affectively intoned stimuli was impaired in the patients sphere disease did not entirely correlate with their abil-
with fight hemisphere damage. ity to recognize and discriminate between emotional
Emotional messages may be conveyed by proposi- faces. Retrospective review of the data revealed that
tional speech. Graves, et al., 4~ visually presented emo- about one-third of the patients with right hemisphere
tional and nonemotional words to aphasic patients with disease performed poorly on both the neutral facial
left hemisphere lesions. Their task was to read the discrimination task and the emotional faces task,
words. Unlike speech, which can be affectively intoned, whereas about one-third performed well on both. The
the written word carries no prosody. Nevertheless, remaining patients with right hemisphere disease, how-
Graves and co-workers found that emotional words ever, performed relatively well on neutral facial discrim-
were read better than were nonemotional words. These ination but poorly on the emotional faces task.
authors also studied normal adults with emotional and We therefore believe that the right hemisphere might
nonemotional words tachistoscopically presented to the be important for perceiving faces and facial expressions.
left and right visual half-fields. In the left visual half- These processes may be either interdependent (one
field (right hemisphere) there was a relative superiority must perceive faces before perceiving the emotions
for the recognition of emotional words over nonemo- expressed by the faces), or they may be independent
tional words. but share the same or a contiguous anatomical locus.
Cicone, et al., 24 also presented emotional faces to pa-
Visual-Nonverbal Comprehension. The develop- tients with right and left hemisphere damage and again
ment of an appropriate emotional state also depends found that right hemisphere-damaged patients had im-
on perceiving and comprehending visual stimuli, such paired ability to recognize emotional faces. We 15 have
as facial expression and gesture. Gardner, et al.,35 found examined a patient who had a large right hemisphere
that patients with right hemisphere disease and those tumor in the posterior region that did not appear to
with left hemisphere disease were equally impaired in involve cortical structures. It did involve subcortical
selecting the most humorous of a group of cartoons association fibers, some of which may have been com-

J. Neurosurg. / Volume 6 4 / M a y . 1986 699


K. M. Heilman, et al.

missural fibers that course to and from the left hemi- less facially expressive than left hemisphere-injured sub-
sphere. This patient performed flawlessly on the neutral jects.
and emotional facial discrimination task, but had great Occasionally, patients who cannot move their face
difficulty in naming and selecting named emotional normally on command can have pathological outbursts
faces. His test performance suggested that the ability to of crying or laughing. Usually these emotional outbursts
recognize or match faces can be dissociated from the are not under volitional control, and the patients will
ability to recognize emotional faces. A right hemisphere often tell the examiner that their facial expression is not
disconnection from the left hemisphere may have always consistent with their mood. Most patients with
caused his inability to name emotional faces or to point pathological laughing or crying have bilateral subcorti-
to a face named by the examiner. cal lesions. 72 Most observers have proposed that this
laughing or crying may be induced by disinhibition of
Affective Expression a subcortical region where these behaviors are pro-
grammed. Sackeim, eta/., TM retrospectively analyzed
Verbal Expression. Previous research with normal such cases and noted that, although most of these
and brain-damaged subjects has suggested that there are patients have bilateral lesions, those with pathologi-
hemispheric differences in the ability to comprehend cal laughing have larger lesions on the right side and
affectively intoned speech. A hemispheric difference those with pathological crying have larger lesions on the
may also exist in the ability to express various emotions left side.
in speech. Patients with left hemisphere disease are
often aphasic and thus have difficulty using proposi-
Affective Memory
tional speech. They are impaired in the ability to convey
emotional content with a semantic-propositional con- In one of the few studies of affective memory,
text. However, as noted by Hughlings Jackson (see Wechsler ~~176presented right and left hemisphere-dam-
Taylor87), some aphasic patients can, by varying tempo, aged patients with two stories - - one designed to elicit
pitch, and timbre, convey a rich variety of emotional an affective response, the other a neutral response. For
feeling, despite their inability to produce propositional example, the emotional story told of a kind man "who
speech. Patients with left hemisphere lesions who have was very sick and his doctors were unable to cure him.
severe nonfluent aphasia and limited ability to express He sent for his wise men who told him he would get
propositional speech not only can affectively intone well if he wore the shirt of a truly happy man." Im-
words and sounds but also may be very fluent when mediately after a story was read to the patients, they
using emotional words, especially expletives. were asked to reproduce it verbally. The score was based
We attempted to determine whether patients with on the total units recalled (similar to logical memory
right hemisphere disease can express emotionally in- subtest of the Wechsler Memory Scale). The patients
toned speech 88 by asking them to say semantically neu- with left hemisphere lesions made fewer errors on the
tral sentences (such as "The boy went to the store."), emotionally charged story than did patients with right
and to use a happy, sad, angry, or indifferent tone of hemisphere disease; there were no differences in their
voice. These patients were severely impaired. Typically, performance on the neutral story. However, it was also
they spoke sentences in a flat monotone and often noted that even left hemisphere-damaged subjects,
denoted the target affect (for example, "The boy went compared with normal controls, had more difficulty
to the store, and was sad."). Ross and MesulamTM de- with the emotionally charged stories than with neutral
scribed two patients who could not express affectively stories. In normal subjects, affectively charged facts are
intoned speech but could comprehend affective speech. recalled better than neutral stories.
R o s s 77 has also described patients who could not com-
prehend affective intonations but could repeat affec- Underlying Affect
tively intoned speech. He has postulated that right The studies thus far have suggested that patients
hemisphere lesions may disrupt affective speech (in with fight hemisphere disease have more difficulty than
terms of comprehension, repetition, and production) in those with left hemisphere disease in comprehending
the same manner that left hemisphere lesions disrupt and expressing affectively intoned speech as well as in
propositional speech. comprehending emotional facial expressions. Patients
Facial Expression. Buck and D u f f y 19 studied the with right hemisphere disease may also have more diffi-
ability of fight and of left hemisphere-damaged patients culty in comprehending or remembering emotionally
to show emotional facial expressions in response to charged speech. These perceptual, cognitive, and ex-
viewing slides such as those of familiar people, an pressive deficits must underlie and account for the
unpleasant scene (such as a starving child or a crying different emotional reactions of patients with fight ver-
woman), and unusual pictures (such as double expo- sus left hemisphere lesions (indifference vs catastrophic
sures). The patients' faces were videotaped as they reaction), as previously described by clinical investi-
viewed the slides and were later judged. The results gators. 32,4~ Alternatively, these perceptual, cognitive,
showed that fight hemisphere-damaged patients were and expressive deficits may be independent of and not

700 J. Neurosurg. / Volume 64/May, 1986


The fight hemisphere: neuropsychological functions
contribute to the different patterns of emotional reac- subjects. In 1924, Maranon (see Fehr and Stern 3~ in-
tivity after unilateral hemispheric lesions. duced physiological arousal by administering sympath-
To learn more about mood, we36 administered the omimetic drugs to normal subjects. He stated that
Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI) most of his subjects reported feeling "no emotions,"
to patients with unilateral hemisphere lesions. The pa- although many reported that they experienced "as if"
tients were matched for severity of cognitive (such as feelings. When Maranon induced an affective memory
intelligence quotient) and motor defects (such as motor that was not strong enough to produce an emotion in
tapping). The MMPI has been widely used as an index the normal state, emotional reactions then occurred if
of underlying affective experiences, and the completion there was concomitant pharmacological arousal. Simi-
of this inventory does not require the perception or larly, Schachter 8~ aroused normal subjects pharmaco-
expression of affectively intoned speech or the percep- logically, placed them in a stressful situation, and found
tion of facial expression. Patients with left hemisphere both subjective and objective evidence of emotional
disease showed a marked increase on the depression states. Pharmacological arousal alone did not produce
scale of this inventory, whereas patients with right such emotional states. Likewise, the stressful situation
hemisphere disease did not. This finding suggests that alone produced less emotion when the subjects were
the differences in emotional reactions of patients after not pharmacologically aroused. Maranon's and Schach-
right versus left hemisphere disease cannot be attributed ter's studies both support the hypothesis that, in order
entirely to difficulties in perceiving or expressing affec- to experience an emotion, one must have the appropri-
tive stimuli. Equally important, the difference in "de- ate cognitive state plus a certain degree of arousal.
pression" between the left and right hemisphere-dam- Arousal depends on the brain-stem reticular formation,
aged groups appears unrelated to differences in the nonspecific thalamic nuclei, and certain changes me-
severity of cognitive or motor defects. diated by the hypothalamus. The hypothalamus is
strongly influenced by the limbic system which, in turn,
Pathophysiology Underlying the Indifference has considerable input from the neocortex. An emotion
Response thus depends on varied anatomical structures, including
Cannon 21 regarded the thalamus as an important cortical systems for producing the appropriate cognitive
central structure responsible for mediating emotions. It set, limbic structures for activating the brain stem and
can be stimulated either by peripheral sensory input thalamic activating centers as well as for controlling
or by cortical impulses. He proposed that thalamic hypothalamic output, the hypothalamus for regulating
activity could excite both the cortex and the viscera. endocrine and autonomic responses, and the brain stem
Cortical activation induces the conscious emotional and thalamic activating systems for producing cortical
state; the visceral changes occur simultaneously and arousal.
serve adaptive purposes. Bard 4 suggested that the hy- Studies in our laboratory have suggested that patients
pothalamus was, in fact, the major effector of emotional with fight hemisphere disease have difficulty in com-
expression, since it regulates both the endocrine system prehending affectively intoned speech and affective fa-
and the autonomic nervous system. Papez, TM in turn, cial expressions. Such deficits might therefore interfere
proposed that the limbic system, which has important with the development of an appropriate cognitive set
connections with the hypothalamus, cortex, and thala- (thought to be an essential component of emotion),
mus, is important in regulating emotion. thereby resulting in emotional flattening in patients
The concept of cortical activation or arousal was with fight hemisphere disease. In addition, patients with
central to Cannon's theory? ~ Subsequent research led fight hemisphere disease and the indifference reaction
to a better understanding of the physiology of arousal. might be inadequately aroused. To determine whether
Berger ~l noted that the electroencephalographic pattern such patients had normal arousal, we48 stimulated the
of behavioral arousal is decreased amplitude and in- normal side of patients with right or left hemisphere
creased frequency. This was termed "electroencepha- disease with an electrical stimulus and simultaneously
lographic desynchronization." Desynchronization also recorded galvanic skin responses. The galvanic skin
occurs during emotional states. 6~As we discussed, stim- response is a measure of peripheral sympathetic activity
ulation of nonspecific thalamic nuclei or meseneephalic which correlates well with other central measures of
reticular formation induces behavioral arousal and elec- arousal, such as the electroencephalogram. We found
troencephalographic desynchronization. 68 In addition, that patients with fight hemisphere disease and the
stimulation of certain cortical areas like the frontal or indifference reaction had dramatically smaller arousal
temporoparietal regions activates the mesencephalic re- responses (galvanic skin response) than aphasic patients
ticular formation 31 and elicits an arousal response. 83 with left hemisphere disease or control patients without
The limbic system, which has strong input into the hemispheric lesions.
reticular formation, is another pathway by which cor- Morrow, et al., 67 have presented neutral and emo-
tical stimulation can produce arousal. 49'96 tionally loaded stimuli to fight or left hemisphere-
The relationship between arousal and the "experi- damaged patients. These investigators found that the
ence" of emotion has been studied in normal human patients with right hemisphere disease had reduced

J. Neurosurg. / Volume 64/May, 1986 701


K . M . H e i l m a n , e t al.

galvanic skin responses to both neutral and emotional "spatial agnosia" ("inattention") in patients with cere-
stimuli. These findings provide further evidence that bral lesions. Brain 79:68-93, 1956
the indifference reaction is associated with hypoarousal. 6. Bender MB: Disorders in Perception, with Particular
Reference to the Phenomena of Extinction and Displace-
Patients with the indifference reaction resulting from ment. Springfield, Ill: Charles C Thomas, 1952
right hemisphere disease typically have unilateral 7. Benton AL: The neuropsychology of facial recognition.
neglect. 27'32'49 As we have discussed, unilateral neglect Am Psychol 35:176-186, 1980
is an attention-arousal-activation defect caused by dys- 8. Benton AL: Visuoperceptual, visuospatial, and visuo-
function in a corticolimbic-reticular loop. 45 Since ne- constructive disorders, in Heilman KM, Valenstein E
glect occurs more often after fight hemisphere lesions, (eds): Clinical Neuropsychology, ed 2. New York: Ox-
ford University Press, 1985, pp 151-186
we have also proposed and provided evidence for the
9. Benton AL, Hamsher K, Varney NR, et al: Contributions
postulate that the right hemisphere may be dominant to Neuropsychologicai Assessment: A Clinical Manual.
for mediating an attention-arousal response. 48'5~ New York: Oxford University Press, 1983
It is therefore possible that the fight hemisphere may 10. Benton AL, Levin HS: An experimental study of "ob-
have a special role in mediating emotional behavior. scuration." Neurology 22:1176-1181, 1972
Not only can the fight hemisphere process affective 11. Berger H: Ober das Electrenkephalogramm des Men-
stimuli and program emotional behavior, but it also schen. Arch Psychiatr Nervenkr 99:555-574, 1933
12. Bignall K_E, Imbert M: Polysensory and cortico-cortical
appears to have a special relationship to those subcor- projections to frontal lobe of squirrel and rhesus mon-
tical structures important for mediating cerebral arousal keys. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 26:206-215,
and activation. 1969
13. Bisiach E, Luzzatti C: Unilateral neglect of representa-
tional space. Cortex 14:129-133, 1978
Conclusions 14. Bisiach E, Luzzatti C, Perani D: Unilateral neglect,
In this review, we have presented evidence that the representational schema and consciousness. Brain 102:
right hemisphere has an important role in mediat- 609-618, 1979
15. Bowers D, Heilman KM: Dissociation between the proc-
ing visuospatial skills, attention, and emotions. Several
essing of affective and nonaffective faces: a case study.
theories have been advanced as to anatomical and J Clin Neuropsychol 6:367-379, 1984
physiological substrates underlying right versus left 16. Bowers D, Heilman KM, Van Den Abell T: Hemispace-
hemisphere dominance. For example, functions like VHF compatibility. Neuropsychologia 19:757-765, 1981
attention and emotion rely in part on cortical-sub- 17. Brain WR: Visual disorientation with special reference
cortical connections, and it has been proposed that the to lesions of the right cerebral hemisphere. Brain 64:
right hemisphere has more cortical-subcortical and cor- 244-271, 1941
18. Broca P: Sur la facult6 du langage articul& Bull Soc
ticolimbic projections than does the left hemisphere, Anthropol Paris 6:337-393, 1865
which has more corticocortical projections. 29 Whereas 19. Buck R, Duffy RJ: Nonverbal communication of affect
the left hemisphere has been thought of as an analyti- in brain-damaged patients. Cortex 16:351-362, 1980
cal or serial processor, the right has been thought of as 20. Bushnell MC, Goldberg ME, Robinson DL: Behav-
a Gestalt (holistic) simultaneous and parallel processor. ioral enhancement of visual responses in monkey cere-
Based on these psychological constructs, it has been bral cortex. I. Modulation in posterior parietal cortex re-
lated to selected visual attention. J Neurophysiol 46:
postulated that corticocortical connections of the two 755-772, 1981
hemispheres are different, the right being more diffusely 21. Cannon WB: The James-Lange theory of emotion: a
organized than the left. critical examination and an alternative theory. Am J
Unfortunately, the anatomical and physiological Psychol 39:106-124, 1927
basis for dominance is not clearly understood. The 22. Carmon A, Bechtoldt HP: Dominance of the right cere-
anatomical asymmetries demonstrated by Geschwind bral hemisphere for stereopsis. Neuropsychologia 7:
and Levitsky 37 and Galaburda, et al., 34 are an exciting 29-39, 1969
23. Chavis DA, Pandya DN: Further observations on corti-
beginning, but further studies are needed. cofrontal connections in the rhesus monkey. Brain Res
117:369-386, 1976
References 24. Cicone M, Wapner W, Gardner H: Sensitivity to emo-
tional expressions and situations in organic patients.
1. Albert ML: A simple test of visual neglect. Neurology Cortex 16:145-158, 1980
23:658-664, 1973 25. Critchley M: The Parietal Lobes. New York: Hafner,
2. Babinski J: Contribution &l'6tude des troubles mentaux 1966
darts l'h6mipl6gie organique c6r6brale (anosognosie). 26. DeKosky ST, Heilman KM, Bowers D, et al: Recogni-
Rev Neurol 22:845-848, 1914 tion and discrimination of emotional faces and pictures.
3. Bateydier C, Maugui6re F: The duality of the cingulate Brain Lang 9:206-214, 1980
gyrus in monkey. Neuroanatomical study and functional 27. Denny-Brown D, Meyer JS, Horenstein S: The signifi-
hypothesis. Brain 103:525-554, 1980 cance of perceptual rivalry resulting from parietal lesion.
4. Bard P: Emotion: I. The neuro-humoral basis of emo- Brain 75:434-471, 1952
tional reactions, in Murchison C (ed): Handbook of 28. De Renzi E, Colombo A, Faglioni P, et al: Conjugate
General Experimental Psychology. Worcester, Mass: gaze paresis in stroke patients with unilateral damage.
Clark University Press, 1934 An unexpected instance of hemispheric asymmetry.
5. Battersby WS, Bender MB, Pollack M, et al: Unilateral Arch Neurol 39:482-486, 1982

702 J. Neurosurg. / Volume 6 4 / M a y , 1986


The fight hemisphere: neuropsychological functions
29. Eidelberg D, Galaburda AM: Inferior parietal lobe. Di- Neurology 22:660-664, 1972
vergent architectonic asymmetries in the human brain. 50. Heilman KM, Van Den Abell T: Right hemisphere
Arch Neurol 41:843-852, 1984 dominance for attention: the mechanism underlying
30. Fehr FS, Stern JA: Peripheral physiological variables and hemispheric asymmetries of inattention (neglect). Neu-
emotion: the James-Lange theory revisited. Psychol Bull rology 30:327-330, 1980
74:411-424, 1970 51. Heilman KM, Van Den Abell T: Right hemispheric
31. French JE, Hern~ndez-Pe6n R, Livingston R: Projec- dominance for mediating cerebral activation. Neuro-
tions from the cortex to cephalic brainstem (reticular psychologia 17:315-321, 1979
formation) in monkeys. J Neurophysiol 18:74-95, 52. Heilman KM, Watson RT, Valenstein E, et al: Locali-
1955 zation of lesions in neglect, in Kertesz A (ed): Localiza-
32. Gainotti G: Emotional behavior and hemispheric side tion in Neuropsyehology. New York: Academic Press,
of the lesion. Cortex 8:41-55, 1972 1983, pp 471-492
33. Gainotti G, Messerli P, Tissot R: Qualitative analysis 53. Heilman KM, Watson RT, Valenstein E: Neglect and
of unilateral spatial neglect in relation to laterality of related disorders, in Heilman KM, Valenstein E (eds):
cerebral lesions. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 35: Clinical Neuropsychology, ed 2. New York: Oxford Uni-
545-550, 1972 versity Press, 1985, pp 243-293
34. Galaburda AM, LeMay M, Kemper TL, et al: Right-left 54. Howes D, Boller F: Simple reaction time: evidence for
asymmetries in the brain. Structural differences between focal impairment from lesions of the right hemisphere.
the hemispheres may underlie cerebral dominance. Sci- Brain 98:317-332, 1975
ence 199:852-856, 1978 55. Jackson JH: Case of large cerebral tumour without optic
35. Gardner H, Ling PK, Flamm L, et al: Comprehension neuritis and with left hemiplegia and imperception. R
and appreciation of humorous material following brain Ophthalmol Hosp Rep 8:434-444, 1876
damage. Brain 98:399-412, 1975 56. Kleist K: Kriegsverletzungen des Gehirns im ihrer Be-
36. Gasparrini WG, Satz P, Heilman KM, et al: Hemispheric deutung ffir die Hirnlokalisation und Hirnpathologie, in
asymmetries of affective processing as determined by von Schjerning O (ed): Handbuch der Arztlichen Erfah-
the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory. J rung im Weltkriege, 1914/1918. Leipzig: Barth, 1923,
Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 41:470-473, 1978 Vol 4
37. Geschwind N, Levitsky W: Human brain: left-right 57. Kuypers HGJM, Lawrence DG: Cortical projections to
asymmetries in temporal speech region. Science 161: the red nucleus and the brain stem in the rhesus monkey.
186-187, 1968 Brain Res 4:151-188, 1967
38. Goldberg ME, Bushnell MC: Behavioral enhancement 58. Lansdell H: Effect of extent of temporal lobe ablations
of visual responses in monkey cerebral cortex. II. Mod- on two lateralized deficits. Physiol Behav 3:271-273,
ulation in frontal eye fields specifically related to sac- 1968
cades. J Neurophysiol 46:773-787, 1981 59. Lansing RW, Schwartz E, Lindsley DB: Reaction time
39. Goldberg ME, Robinson DL: Visual responses of neu- and EEG activation under alerted and nonalerted con-
rons in monkey inferior parietal lobule: the physiologic ditions. J Exp Psychol 58:1-7, 1959
substrate of attention and neglect. Neurology 27:350, 60. Lindsley D: The role of nonspecific reticulo-thalamo-
1977 (Abstract) cortical systems in emotion, in Black P (ed): Physiolog-
40. Goldstein K: Language and Language Disturbances. ical Correlates of Emotion. New York: Academic Press,
Aphasic Symptom Complexes and Their Significance for 1970, pp 147-188
Medicine and Theory of Language. New York: Grune 61. Loeb J: Die elementaren Storfingen einfacher Functi-
& Stratton, 1948 onen nach oberfl/ichlicher, umschriebener Verletzung
41. Graves R, Landis T, Goodglass H: Laterality and sex dif- des Grosshirns. Pflugers Arch 37:51-56, 1885
ferences for visual recognition of emotional and non- 62. Lynch JC: The functional organization of posterior pa-
emotional words. Neuropsychologia 19:95-102, 1981 rietal association cortex. Behav Brain Sci 3:485-534,
42. Haggard MP, Parkinson AM: Stimulus and task factors 1980
as determinants of ear advantages. Q J Exp Psychol 23: 63. McFie J, Piercy MF, Zangwill OL: Visual-spatial agnosia
168-177, 1971 associated with lesions of the right cerebral hemisphere.
43. Hannay HJ, Varney NR, Benton AL: Visual localization Brain 73:167-190, 1950
in patients with unilateral brain disease. J Neorol Neu- 64. Meadows JC: The anatomical basis of prosopagnosia. J
rosurg Psychiatry 39:307-313, 1976 Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 37:489-501, 1974
44. Hrcaen H, de Ajuriaguerra J, Massonnet J: Les troubles 65. Meier MJ, French LA: Lateralized deficits in complex
visuo-constructifs par 16sion pari&o-occipitale droit. visual discrimination and bilateral transfer of reminis-
Role des perturbations vestibulaires. Encephale 40: cence following unilateral temporal lobectomy. Neuro-
122-179, 1951 psychoiogia 3:261-272, 1965
45. Heilman KM: Neglect and related disorders, in Heilman 66. Mishkin M, Ungerleider LG, Macko KA: Object vision
KM, Valenstein E (eds): Clinical Neuropsychology. New and spatial vision: two cortical pathways. TINS 6:
York: Oxford University Press, 1979, pp 268-307 414-417, 1983
46. Heilman KM, Bowers D, Watson RT: Performance on 67. Morrow L, Vrtunski PB, Kim Y, et al: Arousal responses
hemispatial pointing task by patients with neglect syn- to emotional stimuli and laterality of lesion. Neuropsy-
drome. Neurology 33:661-664, 1983 chologia 19:65-71, 1981
47. Heilman KM, Scholes R, Watson RT: Auditory affective 68. Moruzzi G, Magoun HW: Brain stem reticular forma-
agnosia. Disturbed comprehension of affective speech. tion and activation of the EEG. Electroencephalogr Clin
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 38:69-72, 1975 Neurophysiol 1:455-473, 1949
48. Heilman KM, Schwartz HD, Watson RT: Hypoarousal 69. Pandya DM, Kuypers HGJM: Cortico-cortical connec-
in patients with the neglect syndrome and emotional tions in the rhesus monkey. Brain Res 13:13-36, 1969
indifference. Neurology 28:229-232, 1978 70. Papez JW: A proposed mechanism of emotion. Arch
49. Heilman KM, Valenstein E: Frontal lobe neglect in man. Neurol Psychiatry 38:725-743, 1937

J. Neurosurg. / Volume 64/May, 1986 703


K. M. Heilman, et al.

71. Paul H: Principien der Sprachgeschichte, ed 4. Nie- 86. Sprague JM, Chambers WW, Stellar E: Attentive, affec-
meyer, 1909 tive, and adaptive behavior in the cat. Sensory depriva-
72. Poeck K: Pathophysiology of emotional disorders asso- tion of the forebrain by lesions in the brain stem results
ciated with brain damage, in Vinken PJ, Bruyn GW in striking behavioral abnormalities. Science 133:
(eds): Disorders of Higher Nervous Activity. Handbook 165-173, 1961
of Clinical Neurology, Vol 3. Amsterdam: North-Hol- 87. Taylor J (ed): Selected Writings of John Hughlings
land, 1969, pp 343-367 Jackson. London: Hodder & Stoughton, 1932, Vol 2
73. Reeves AG, Hagamen WD: Behavioral and EEG asym- 88. Tucker DM, Watson RT, Heilman KM: Discrimination
metry following unilateral lesions of the forebrain and and evocation of affectively intoned speech in patients
midbrain in cats. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol with right parietal disease. Neurology 27:947-950,
30:83-86, 1971 1977
74. Reivich M, Alavi A, Gur RC: Positron emission tomo- 89. Valenstein E, Heilman KM: Unilateral hypokinesia and
graphic studies o f perceptual tasks. Ann Neurol 16 motor extinction. Neurology 31:445-448, 1981
(Suppl):61-66, 1984 90. Valenstein E, Heilman KM, Watson RT, et al: Nonsen-
75. Robinson DL, Goldberg ME, Stanton GB: Parietal as- sory neglect from parietotemporal lesions in monkeys.
sociation cortex in the primate: sensory mechanisms and Neurology 32:1198-1201, 1982
behavioral modulations. J Neurophysioi 41:910-932, 91. Velasco F, Velasco M: A reticulothalamic system me-
1978 diating proprioceptive attention and tremor in man.
76. Rosenberger P: Discriminative aspects of visual hemi- Neurosurgery 4:30-36, 1979
inattention. Neurology 24:17-23, 1974 92. Warrington EK, James M: An experimental investiga-
77. Ross ED: The aprosodias. Functional-anatomic organi- tion of facial recognition in patients with unilateral
zation of the affective components of language in the cerebral lesions. Cortex 3:317-326, 1967
right hemisphere. Arch Neurol 38:561-569, 1981 93. Warrington EK, James M, Kinsbourne M: Drawing
78. Ross ED, Mesulam MM: Dominant language functions disability in relation to laterality of cerebral lesion. Brain
of the right hemisphere? Prosody and emotional gestur- 89:53-82, 1966
ing. Arch Neurol 36:144-148, 1979 94. Warrington EK, Rabin P: Perceptual matching in pa-
79. Sackeim HA, Greenberg MS, Weiman AL, et al: Hemi- tients with cerebral lesions. Neuropsyehologia 8:
spheric asymmetry in the expression of positive and 475-487, 1970
negative emotions. Neurologic evidence. Arch Neurol 95. Watson RT, Heilman KM: Thalamic neglect. Neurology
39:210-218, 1982 29:690-694, 1979
80. Schachter S: The interaction of cognitive and physiolog- 96. Watson RT, Heilman KM, Cauthen JC, et al: Neglect
ical determinants of emotional state, in Berkowitz L after cingulectomy. Neurology 23:1003-1007, 1973
(ed): Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. New 97. Watson RT, Heilman KM, Miller BD, et al: Neglect
York: Academic Press, 1970, Vol 1 after mesencephalic reticular formation lesions. Neurol-
81. Scheibel ME, Scheibel AB: The organization of the ogy 24:294-298, 1974
nucleus reticularis thalami: a Golgi study. Brain Res 1: 98. Watson RT, Miller BD, Heilman KM: Nonsensory ne-
43-62, 1966 glect. Ann Neurol 3:505-508, 1978
82. Schwartz AS, Marchok PL, Kreinick CJ, et al: The 99. Watson RT, Valenstein E, Heilman KM: Thalamic ne-
asymmetric lateralization of tactile extinction in pa- glect. Possible role of the medial thalamus and nucleus
tients with unilateral cerebral dysfunction. Brain 102: reticularis thalami in behavior. Arch Neurol 38:
669-684, 1979 501-506, 1981
83. Segundo JP, Naguet R, Buser P: Effects of cortical 100. Wechsler AF: The effect of organic brain disease on
stimulation on electrocortical activity in monkeys. J recall of emotionally charged versus neutral narrative
Neurophysiol 18:236-245, 1955 texts. Neurology 23:130-135, 1973
84. Singer W: Control of thalamic transmission by cortico-
fugal and ascending reticular pathways in the visual Manuscript received July 5, 1985.
system. Physiol Rev 57:386-420, 1977 Accepted in final form November 4, 1985.
85. Sparks R, Geschwind N: Dichotic listening in man after Address reprint requests to: Kenneth M. Heilman, M.D.,
section of neocortical commissures. Cortex 4:3-16, Box J-236, Department of Neurology, J. Hillis Miller Health
1968 Center, University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida 32610.

704 J. Neurosurg. / Volume 6 4 / M a y , 1986

You might also like