You are on page 1of 5

Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 56 (2003) 436–440

Reliability of the Wisconsin Sleep Questionnaire: A French


contribution to international validation
Dan Teculescua,*, Francis Guilleminb,e, Jean-Marc Virionc, Catherine Aubryd,
Bernard Hannharta, Jean-Pierre Michaelya, René Gueguend
a
INSERM Unité 420, BP 184, 54505 Vandœuvre, France
b
EA 3444, School of Public Health, Faculty of Medicine, University H. Poincaré, 3 Avenue de la Foret de Haye, 54500 Vandœuvre, France
c
Centre of Clinical Investigations, INSERM and Department of Clinical Epidemiology and Evaluation, Nancy University Hospital,
5 Avenue de Lattre de Tassigny, 54000 Nancy, France
d
Centre of Preventive Medicine, 7 rue du Doyen Parisot, 54501 Vandœuvre, France
e
Department of Clinical Epidemiology and Evaluation, Nancy University Hospital, 5 Avenue de Lattre de Tassigny, 54000 Nancy, France
Received 10 December 2002; received in revised form 16 December 2002; accepted 6 January 2003

Abstract
We evaluated the reliability of a French version of the Wisconsin Sleep Questionnaire designed to investigate snoring, obstructive
apnoeas, and sleeping problems. The assessment of reliability included the study of internal consistency and the 3 months repeatability of
the questionnaire. The questionnaire was first completed at a Center of Preventive Medicine by a random sample of 122 subjects from the
community. Three months later the same form was mailed and 82 questionnaires were returned (67.2%). No significant differences existed
between responders and nonresponders for anthropometric data or life habits. The internal consistency in each domain was good or
satisfactory (Cronbach’s alpha ⫽ 0.67 to 0.81). The concordance between the answers at a 3-month interval was excellent for questions
on ever snoring, frequency of snoring, gasping/choking during sleep, and breathing stops during sleep (Cohen kappa ⬎ 0.60). The questions on
snoring loudness, a history of sleep apnoea, and excessive daytime sleepiness were fairly reproducible (kappa 0.28 to 0.60). We found no
difference in reproducibility by gender or age. In conclusion, this reliability assessment in a sample of middle-aged subjects from the
community in northeastern France showed satisfactory internal consistency and 3-months reproducibility of the main questions of a French
translation of the Wisconsin Sleep Questionnaire. 쑖 2003 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Sleep questionnaire; Three-month repeatability; Internal consistency; General population

1. Introduction to French again (the Wisconsin University form). We also


performed a pilot study to ensure that the subjects understood
At the time (1996) an epidemiologic study on sleep- correctly the sense of every question [7].
disordered breathing (SDB) was planned in our area, no The reliability of the Wisconsin Sleep Questionnaire
validated French sleep questionnaire was available. For the has not been analyzed yet. Reliability is composed of internal
sake of comparability, we wished to utilize a form largely consistency (the degree to which items exploring various
applied in previous studies. Comparing several such instru- facets of a domain are interrelated) as well as reproducibility,
ments [1–4], we opted for the questionnaire utilized in the the property providing the same result when measuring the
Wisconsin Sleep Cohort Study [3], which seemed com- same level of the phenomenon. We explored here the internal
plete although not too long to fill in. Its validity has been consistency and intraobserver test-retest repeatability [5,6].
documented, and the content has been shown to target sleep Only a few attempts were devoted to the validation of
disorders and consequences appropriately. This content other sleep and breathing questionnaires [8–12]; all of these
should be preserved when using it in a different cultural studies were done in small groups of patients attending
context [5]. diagnostic centers. As such questionnaires are increasingly
Knowing that transcultural adaptation of a field survey used in large field studies [13–15], including non-Anglo-
questionnaire implies a complex work of adaptation to the Saxon samples [16], we felt it necessary to assess the psy-
second language [5,6], we first translated it from English to chometric property as part of the quality of the measurement
French, then retranslated it to English and translated it tool. This article presents the analysis of the 3-month repro-
* Corresponding author. Tel.: ⫹33-03-83-69-39-16; fax: ⫹33-03-83- ducibility and of the internal consistency of the translated
68-39-19. Wisconsin Sleep Questionnaire in a sample of subjects from
E-mail address: Dan.Teculescu@nancy.inserm.fr (D. Teculescu). the general population of northeastern France.
0895-4356/03/$ – see front matter 쑖 2003 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi: 10.1016/S0895-4356(03)00029-5
D. Teculescu et al. / Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 56 (2003) 436–440 437

2. Methods repeated on three categories, collapsing the “rarely”plus


“sometimes” answers on one side, the “several nights per
2.1. Subject recruitment
week” and “almost every night” on the other side, and leav-
The target population were the subjects attending between ing the “at least once a week-pattern irregular” as a the
November 1998 and March 1999, a regional Center of Pre- median (third) category.
ventive Medicine serving a population of about 2 million. Internal consistency was assessed by the Cronbach’s
The subjects were approached in the order of the Center alpha coefficient [20]. According to Nunally, an alpha over
computer-generated list; a written explanation of the study 0.7 indicated a high reliability, over 0.5 a moderate reliability,
was given, stressing the noninvasive character of the proto- and below 0.2 a low reliability [21]. Data processing used
col, and the sleep questionnaire was distributed in person the SAS software version 8.1 (1999).
for self-completion to subjects who volunteered.

2.2. Design of the study 3. Results


Couples of recruited people completed the forms side by Demographic data from subjects who returned (⫽“re-
side, so that he information concerning breathing during sleep spondents”) or did not return (⫽“nonrespondents”) the
was available both from subject and from spouse. Subjects mailed questionnaire were checked and found to be compara-
were included if living in couples, and aged 30 years or ble (Table 1); thus, the group of respondents could be consid-
more. Of 434 subjects invited, 402 (201 couples) agreed to ered representative of the sample to which the questionnaire
participate (participation rate 92.6%), completing the ques- was addressed. As we looked at the prevalence of the main
tionnaire. Three months later, the questionnaire was mailed symptoms in responders, we found habitual snoring in
with a covering letter and a stamped envelope to a random 41.7%; loud snoring in 30.2%; gasping or choking in 5.1%;
subsample of 122 subjects, and was returned by 82 of breathing pauses perceived by the bedpartner in 6.8%; dis-
them (67.2%). No reminder was send to those who did not ruptive movements during sleep in 7.6%; waking up repeat-
answer. The study was approved by the Ethics in Medical edly during the night in 20.7%; difficult to wake up in the
Research Regional Committee. morning in 19.5%; waking up with headaches in 4.9%;
excessive daytime sleepiness in 13.4%, and an obstructed/
2.3. The questionnaire running nose during the night in 12.2%.
For the sake of clarity, the repeatability of the responses
The Wisconsin University Sleep Questionnaire [3], has is presented according to the type of question, with the
been adapted from the Basic Northern Sleep Questionnaire five-category answers collapsed to three, as specified. The
elaborated by Scandinavian authors [2]. The questionnaire repeatability of main questions pertaining to breathing
was first translated to French, checked by experts for ad- during sleep was fair for two questions, good for one, and
equacy of terms, back-translated to English, and finally trans- excellent for three (Table 2), with kappa extreme values of
lated to French again. We used the form utilized in the 0.41 to 0.86. A computation including all the five possible
initial (1992) survey by T. Young et al. [3], consisting of categories would yield somewhat lower figures for kappa,
10 questions on snoring and sleep-disordered breathing, five
questions on disturbed sleep, five questions on personal and
family medical history, and 12 questions on life habits, edu- Table 1
cation, job working hours, gender, anthropometric data, Demographic variables and life habits in responders and
number of children, etc. For each question evaluating the non-responders to a repeat (3-months interval) sleep questionnaire
frequency of a phenomenon, the possible answers were: (1) Responders Nonresponders
rarely; (2) sometimes; (3) at least once a week-pattern irregu- No. subjects 82 40 P—
lar; (4) several (3 to 5) nights per week; (5) every night or Age (years) 43.4 ⫾ 10.5 a
44.2 ⫾ 8.1 .67
almost every night; and (6) do not know. A copy of the Height (cm) 167.1 ⫾ 9.3 165.2 ⫾ 9.3 .28
Weight (kg) 71.3 ⫾ 12.0 73.5 ⫾ 13.5 .39
questionnaire can be obtained from the authors on request.
BMI (kg/m2) 25.5 ⫾ 4.0 26.9 ⫾ 4.5 .08
Neck girth (cm) 36.9 ⫾ 3.7 37.6 ⫾ 4.0 .31
2.4. Statistics Waist girth (cm) 83.8 ⫾ 12.1 86.7 ⫾ 11.8 .22
Hip girth (cm) 99.1 ⫾ 13.8 100.2 ⫾ 7.3 .42
The test/retest repeatability was assessed using the Waist/hip ratio 0.84 ⫾ 0.10 0.87 ⫾ 0.10 .30
Cohen’s kappa statistic [17]; weighted kappa was used for Current smokers (%) 24.7 23.7 .91
Education (school years) 12.9 ⫾ 3.4 11.6 ⫾ 2.9 .12b
questions with multiple-point answers [18]. According to Leisure physical activity 3.6 ⫾ 6.1 5.7 ⫾ 7.7 .45b
Fleiss [19], an agreement of less than 0.40 was considered (hr/week)
as poor, between 0.41 and 0.59 as fair, between 0.60 and 0.74 a
Figures are means ⫾ standard deviations.
as good, and of 0.75 or more as excellent. The analysis was b
Mann–Whitney test; all other comparisons are based on the t
first done on the original five answers categories, and then (Student) test.
438 D. Teculescu et al. / Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 56 (2003) 436–440

Table 2 Table 3
Repeatability of answers pertaining to sleep-disordered breathing Repeatability of answers on sleep problems and daytime sleepiness
during sleep 95%
95% Confidence
Confidence Question Kappa interval
Question Kappaa interval Please check if you have one of these
Have you ever been told that you snore? 0.86 0.72 0.99 sleeping problems:
According to what others have told you, please 0.69 0.53 0.84 Difficulty getting to sleep 0.51 0.35–0.66
estimate how often you snore? Wake up, with hard time getting back to sleep 0.53 0.38–0.68
How loud have others said your snoring is? 0.41 0.17 0.65 Wake up repeatedly during the night 0.43 0.26–0.61
Have you had the feeling or awareness that you 0.45 0.24 0.66 Wake up too early in the morning 0.53 0.37–0.70
have been snoring? Not feel rested during the day 0.28 0.11–0.45
Do you gasp, choke, or make snorting 0.80 0.60 0.99 Very difficult to wake up in the morning 0.53 0.35–0.70
sounds during sleep? Nightmares, disturbing dreams 0. 35 0.14–0.55
Do you seem to have momentary periods during 0.75 0.54 0.97 “Restless legs” 0.50 0.28–0.71
sleep when you stop breathing or breathe Excessive daytime sleepiness 0.52 0.35–0.70
abnormally? Nasal obstruction or discharge at night 0.45 0.24–0.66
a
The results correspond to “simple” kappas for questions with binary Fall asleep or doze watching TV or reading 0.60 0.41–0.75
answers and “weighted” kappas for questions with multiple-point answers,
collapsed to three categories.
4. Discussion
for example, 0.57 instead of 0.69 for snoring frequency, or Crosscultural adaptation of a questionnaire is much more
0.50 instead of 0.75 for breathing pauses during sleep. We than a simple translation [6]: the novel version must reflect
addressed the problem of possible differences in reproduc- in depth the true significance of the questions in the new
ibility by sex or by age. For example, the reproducibility of language [6,22]. Adaptation of an English questionnaire to
snoring frequency yielded a kappa of 0.65 for males and one of the Latin group languages is reputed to raise difficult
of 0.69 for females (difference nonsignificant). Subjects problems [23]. In the present study, we followed the recom-
aged more than 50 years had a slightly lower reproducibility mended sequence: forward translation to French; evaluation
when compared with younger subjects (kappa 0.65 vs. 0.70), of the preliminary translation; backward translation to
but again, the difference was not significant. We also evalu- English; and final translation to French [24]. In the pilot study
ated the distribution of concordance of the extreme catego- mentioned above [7], the nonresponses were infrequent (2%
ries of response. In males, 64.3% of nonsnorers remained or less, with the exception of hypertension = 12.0%). The
nonsnorers after 3 months, while 86.7% of habitual snorers responses “do not know” were frequent for certain items,
remained habitual snorers. In females, the proportions were such as “how many years do you think you have been snor-
85.7 and 66.7%, respectively. The same analysis according to ing,” for a parental history of narcolepsy and for a parental
age yielded 78.9% concordance for nonsnoring, and 78.6% history of sleep apnoea.
concordance for habitual snoring in subjects older than 50 In studies by previous authors, the interval between two
years; in subjects less than 50 years old, concordance for administrations of the questionnaire varied between very
nonsnoring was 66.7%, and concordance for habitual snoring large limits [8,25]. We followed the recommendation of
was 85.7%. Most questions on sleep problems and excessive Bailar [26] and adopted an interval of 3 months for our
daytime sleepiness yielded fair repeatable answers (eight repeatability study; this is reputed to minimize recall bias
questions) with three exceptions: question 13e “not feel [27,28]. The questionnaire was self-completed in both
rested during the day” and question 13g “nightmares or phases of our study. The only difference was that at the
disturbing dreams” were poorly repeatable; “falling asleep first phase it was distributed in person, while at the second
watching TV” showed good repeatability (Table 3). The it was mailed. Thus, both administrations used the same
kappa values ranged between 0.28 and 0.60. technique [29].
The reproducibility of the question on current smoking For reasons alluded to in the introduction, a direct com-
was excellent (kappa 0.91). Concerning the medical family parison between our results and those of previous validation
history, our subjects obviously poorly knew two conditions: studies [8–12] is impossible for several reasons. First, it is
narcolepsy (30 and 33% missing values for the mother and very difficult to compare two different questionnaires.
the father, respectively), and sleep apnoea in parents. For the Second, previous work was done in small groups of patients
latter condition, the missing values were more frequent in attending specialized laboratories, while the present one
males (49% for both parents) compared to the females (29% involved a larger sample of (presumably) healthy subjects
for mothers, 27% for fathers). from the community. Third, any comparison is meaning-
The reliability coefficient showed good to high internal less until the test/retest interval is standardized. We under-
consistency throughout. The alpha statistic was 0.67 for took our study being aware of the fact that measures of
questions on snoring, 0.81 for questions on apnoeas, and 0.76 validity of questionnaires used in patients cannot be general-
for questions on sleep disturbance and daytime sleepiness. ized for measurement in general population, as stressed by
D. Teculescu et al. / Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 56 (2003) 436–440 439

Young [30]. The subjects studied by us belonged to families asked each subject, for each question, “what do you mean
attending a Preventive Medicine Center. We were satisfied by this?” it came out that one-third of the subjects meant
to obtain a return rate of 67% for the mailed form and to “angor pectoris,” while the other two-thirds meant “acute
find no significant difference in anthropometry, smoking pharyngitis”! A careful translation process is particularly
habits, education, and leisure physical activities between important for investigating subjective perceptions by the
responders and nonresponders. We obtained excellent or individual of his/her health status, less so for physical symp-
good agreement for four of the important questions on toms. Most of the above-mentioned steps have been imple-
breathing during sleep: “have you ever been told you snore”; mented during the development of the French version of
“please estimate how often you snore”; “do you gasp, choke, the Wisconsin Sleep Questionnaire.
or make snorting sounds…” and “do you seem to have periods In conclusion, in this sample of middle-aged subjects of
you stop breathing…” The agreement was fair for the two both sexes from a French community, we found excellent or
remaining questions (“loudness of snoring” and “have you good reproducibility at a 3-month interval for most questions
had the feeling that you have been snoring?”). of a French translation of the Wisconsin University Sleep
The questions on other sleep problems were somewhat Questionnaire, with no significant differences according to
less repeatable. The kappa values were in the fair range for gender or age. No valid conclusions could be reached on
eight (out of 11 questions), poor for two questions, and good the reproducibility of the familial history of narcolepsy or
for one question. In respect to the missing values, it should sleep apnoea because of the small number of positive an-
be mentioned that we found no difference by sex, with one swers. The internal consistency of the French version of the
notable exception: daughters seem to have a better recollec- Wisconsin Sleep Questionnaire proved to be satisfactory.
tion of the presence/absence of sleep apnoea in their parents, The reliability of this version should be tested in samples
compared to sons. If we exclude overestimation by daugh- with a larger age and socioeducational range. Similar
ters, this means that information of familial factors in sleep- “breathing during sleep” and “sleep-quality” questionnaires
disordered breathing should preferably be obtained from are increasingly used in large-scale field surveys [30]. En-
daughters, rather than sons. glish versions were utilized in the Wisconsin Sleep Cohort
Finally, the internal consistency of the principal chapters Study [3], in the Nurses Health Study [14], and in the Sleep
of the questionnaire (snoring, apnoeas, and sleep distur- Heart Health Study [13,15]. A translation of the Wisconsin
bances including daytime sleepiness) was found to be satis- Sleep Questionnaire has recently been used in a large survey
factory, confirming previous results in patients [4,10,12], and in Poland [16]; it would be interesting to compare the repro-
the preliminary report of Goudge et al. in a mixed group of ducibility and internal consistency of the present work
patients and healthy subjects [31]. A good to high Cronbach’s to translations of the Wisconsin Sleep Questionnaire to
alpha coefficient is the reflection of the degree of coherence other languages.
of items within one domain in exploring the concept, in our
case snoring, obstructed breathing, or sleeping problems. It
was usually interpreted as items being complementary of Acknowledgments
each other so as to explore comprehensively all related
aspects of this domain. However, recent literature [32] The authors are indebted to the staff of the Vandœuvre
has stressed the fact that a high internal consistency could also Preventive Medicine Center for their help in subject recruit-
be the reflection of redundancy in measurement of the under- ment, to Mr. Bruno Phelut for his help with the computer
lying phenomenon, thus leading to reject items that would files, to Ms. Aline Berthelin for manuscript processing, and
be too highly correlated to other scale items. Therefore, an to the subjects of the study for their willing participation.
alpha value within the range of 0.7 to 0.8 can be considered They wish to acknowledge the constructive criticism of the
a scale balanced for reliability. two anonymous reviewers whose comments substantially
The translation of questionnaires may result in a new improved the initial version of this article.
questionnaire that does not exactly reflect the original mea-
sure. Indeed, a change in culture may not convey the same
meaning to words sentences or context. Although translators References
may reliably obtain semantic equivalence, idioms, and other
forms of equivalence like conceptual or experiential adapta- [1] Kapuniai MS, Andrew DJ, Crowell DH, Pearce JW. Identifying sleep
apnoea from self-reports: a validation study. Sleep 1988;11:430–6.
tion may loose the original concept intended to measure. [2] Partinen M, Gislason T. Basic Nordic Sleep Questionnaire (BNSQ):
Thus, it is recommended to use adequate and qualified a quantitated measure of subjective sleep complaints? J Sleep Res
translators, to apply forward and backward translations 1995;4(supp 1):150–5.
successively, to submit a preliminary version to a multidisci- [3] Young T, Palta M, Dempsey J, Skatrud J, Weber S, Badr S. The
plinary expert committee, and to conduct a pretest in commu- occurrence of sleep-disordered breathing among middle-aged adults.
N Engl J Med 1993;328:1230–5.
nity or patient groups [5]. For example, we translated English [4] Netzer NC, Stoohs RA, Netzer CM, Clark K, Strohl KP. Using the
“angina” into the French “angine,” with agreement of the Berlin Questionnaire to identify patients at risk for the sleep apnoea
experts. When, however, in the pilot study, the investigator syndrome. Ann Intern Med 1999;131:485–91.
440 D. Teculescu et al. / Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 56 (2003) 436–440

[5] Guillemin F, Bombardier C, Beaton D. Cross-cultural adaptation of [17] Cohen J. Weighted kappa: nominal scale agreement with provision for
health-related quality of life measures: literature review and proposed scaled disagreement or partial credit. Psychol Bull 1968;70:213–20.
guidelines. J Clin Epidemiol 1993;46:1417–32. [18] Fleiss JL. Statistical methods for rates and proportions. 2nd ed. New
[6] Guillemin F. Cross-cultural adaptation and validation of health status York: J. Wiley; 1981.
measures. Scand J Rheumatol 1995;24:61–3. [19] Fleiss JL, Cohen J. The equivalence of weighted kappa and the in-
[7] Teculescu D, Hannhart B, Cornette A, Montaut-Verient B, Virion traclass coefficient as measures of reliability. Educ Psychol Meas
J-M, Michaely J-P. Prevalence of habitual snoring in a sample of 1973;33:613–9.
French males. Role of “minor” nose-throat abnormalities. Respiration [20] Cronbach LJ. Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests.
2001;68:365–70. Psychometrika 1951;16:297–334.
[8] Cuda D, de Benedetto M, Graziuso M, Leante M. Replicabilità test- [21] Nunally JC. Bernstein IH. Psychometric theory. 3rd ed. New York:
retest dei dati anamnestici correlati alla roncopathia chronica. Acta McGraw Hill; 1994.
Otorhinolaryngol Ital 1990;10:529–37. [22] Berkanovic E. The effect of inadequate language translation on His-
[9] Cormick W, Olson LG, Hensley MJ. Nocturnal hypoxaemia and qual- panics’ responses to health surveys. Am J Public Health 1980;
ity of sleep in patients with chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. 70:1273–6.
Thorax 1986;41:846–54. [23] Brislin RW. The wording and translation of research instruments. In:
[10] Kump K, Whalen C, Tishler PV, Browner I, Ferrette V, Strohl KP, Lonner WJ, Berry JW, editors. Field methods in cross-cultural re-
Rosenberg C, Redline S. Assessment of the validity and utility of a search. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications; 1986. p. 137–64.
sleep-symptom questionnaire. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 1994; [24] Del Greco L, Wallop W, Eastridge L. Questionnaire development: 3;
150:735–41. Translation. Can Med Assoc J 1987;136:817–8.
[11] Douglass AB, Bornstein R, Nino-Murcia G, Keenan S, Miles L, Zar- [25] Westerdahl J, Anderson H, Olsson H, Ingvar C. Reproducibility of a
cone V, Guilleminault C, Dement WC. The Sleep Disorders self-administered questionnaire for assessment of melanoma risk. Int
Questionnaire. I: creation and multivariate structure of the SDQ. Sleep J Epidemiol 1996;25:245–51.
1994;17:160–7. [26] Bailar BA. Recent research in the re-interview procedures. J Am Stat
[12] Maislin G, Pack A, Kribbs N, Smith PL, Schwartz AR, Kline LR, Assoc 1968;63:41–63.
Schwab RJ, Dinges DF. A survey screen for prediction of apnea. [27] Drews CD, Greenland S. The impact of differential recall on the
Sleep 1995;18:158–66. results of case–control studies. Int J Epidemiol 1990;19:1107–12.
[13] Quan SF, Howard BV, Iber C, Kiley JP, Nieto FJ, O’Connor GT, [28] Raphael K. Recall bias: a proposal for assessment and control. Int J
Rappoport DM, Redline S, Robbins J, Samet JM, Wahl PW. The Epidemiol 1987;16:167–70.
Sleep Heart Health Study: design, rationale, and methods. Sleep [29] Van Der Gulden JW, Jansen IW, Verbeek AL, Kolk JJ. Repeatability
1997;20:1077–85. of self-reported data on occupational exposure to particular com-
[14] Hu FB, Willet WC, Colditz GA, Ascherio A, Speizer FE, Rosner B, pounds. Int J Epidemiol 1993;22:284–7.
Hennekens OH, Stampfer MJ. Prospective study of snoring and risk [30] Young T. Epidemiology as a research tool in breathing disorders of
of hypertension in women. Am J Epidemiol 1999;150:806–16. sleep. In: Saunders NA, Sullivan CE, editors. Sleep and breathing.
[15] Newman AB, Nieto FJ, Guidry U, Lind BK, Redline S, Shahar E, New York: M. Dekker; 1994. p. 311–36.
Pickering TG, Quan SF. Relation of sleep-disordered breathing to [31] Goudge R, Goh N, Barnes M, Howard M, Worsnop C. Validation of
cardiovascular risk factors—The Sleep Heart Health Study. Am J a sleep apnoea symptom questionnaire. Am J Respir Crit Care Med
Epidemiol 2001;154:50–9. 2001;163:A933 [abstract].
[16] Zielinski J, Zgierska A, Polakowska M, Finn L, Kurjata P, Kupsc W, [32] Silverstein AB. Agreement between a short form and the full scale
Young T. Snoring and excessive daytime somnolence among Polish as a function of the correlation between them. J Clin Psychol
middle-aged adults. Eur Respir J 1999;14:946–50. 1989;45:929–31.

You might also like