You are on page 1of 13

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/6258160

Goal Shifts Following Reminders of Mortality: Reconciling Posttraumatic


Growth and Terror Management Theory

Article  in  Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin · September 2007


DOI: 10.1177/0146167207303015 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

82 998

5 authors, including:

Suzanne C Segerstrom Daniel R Evans


University of Kentucky University of Washington Seattle
154 PUBLICATIONS   10,712 CITATIONS    23 PUBLICATIONS   873 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Margaret E Kemeny
University of California, San Francisco
117 PUBLICATIONS   15,657 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Margaret E Kemeny on 29 May 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Pers Soc Psychol Bull OnlineFirst, published on June 19, 2007 as doi:10.1177/0146167207303015

Goal Shifts Following Reminders of Mortality:


Reconciling Posttraumatic Growth and Terror
Management Theory
Emily L. B. Lykins
Suzanne C. Segerstrom
Alyssa J. Averill
Daniel R. Evans
University of Kentucky

Margaret E. Kemeny
University of California, San Francisco

Research findings within posttraumatic growth (PTG) crisis, especially one’s own mortality or that of close
and terror management theory (TMT) currently appear others, can lead to “positive psychological change expe-
contradictory. Following confrontations with mortality, rienced as a result of the struggle with highly challeng-
PTG research demonstrates intrinsic goal shifts, whereas ing life circumstances,” a phenomenon known as PTG
TMT suggests extrinsic shifts. The current studies exam- (Calhoun & Tedeschi, 1999, 2001; Tedeschi &
ine factors contributing to these inconsistent results. Calhoun, 2004, p. 1). Positive changes typically include
Study 1 demonstrates that perceived death threat is a greater appreciation for life, a changed sense of prior-
associated with PTG effects. Study 2 illuminates the ities, more intimate relationships with others, a greater
importance of duration of death processing. Study 3 sense of personal strength, recognition of new possibili-
demonstrates that existing goal values and duration and ties for one’s life, and spiritual development (Tedeschi &
type of processing all interact in determining ultimate Calhoun, 1996). PTG has been demonstrated to occur
goal structure, with a match between level of goals and in a variety of different circumstances (Tedeschi &
processing producing the most psychologically advanta- Calhoun, 2004), including cancer (Collins, Taylor, &
geous outcomes. Although previous research suggests Skokan, 1990), bone marrow transplantation (Andry-
that short-term confrontations with death may lead to kowski, Brady, & Hunt, 1993), heart attack (Affleck,
defensiveness, the current studies suggest that encoun- Tennen, Croog, & Levine, 1987), HIV infection
tering death over a longer period or in a manner consis- (Bower, Kemeny, Taylor, & Fahey, 1998), transporta-
tent with goal structure may lead individuals to tion accident (Joseph, Williams, & Yule, 1993), house
transcend defensiveness and maintain intrinsic goals or fire (Thompson, 1985), combat (Elder & Clipp, 1989),
become more intrinsically oriented.
Authors’ Note: The authors acknowledge the late George F. Solomon,
Keywords: posttraumatic growth; terror management; intrinsic principal investigator for Study 1. They thank Peter Grohr for his
and extrinsic goals; earthquake; terrorism assistance with data collection for Study 1 and Andrea Elliot, Natalie
Fantini, Joseph Mayne, Abbey Roach, and Aaron Zink for their assis-
tance with data collection for Study 2. Correspondence concerning
this article should be addressed to Dr. Suzanne C. Segerstrom,

I t is generally agreed that the foreknowledge of death is a


uniquely human characteristic. However, two theories—
posttraumatic growth (PTG) and terror management
Department of Psychology, 115 Kastle Hall, University of Kentucky,
Lexington, KY 40506-0044; e-mail: scsege0@uky.edu.

PSPB, Vol. XX No. X, Month XXXX xx-xx


theory (TMT)—disagree as to the consequences of this DOI: 10.1177/0146167207303015
foreknowledge. PTG findings suggest that facing a life © by the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

Copyright 2007 by Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc..


2 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

sexual assault and abuse (Burt & Katz, 1987), bereave- The first difference between these traditions is the
ment (Calhoun & Tedeschi, 1989-1990), and divorce emphasis on mortality per se. As PTG has been demon-
(Wallerstein, 1986), among others. Many, though not strated in circumstances both including and not includ-
all, circumstances promoting PTG include reminders of ing direct reminders of mortality (e.g., cancer vs.
or the potential for personal mortality. divorce), an important initial question is what role
PTG can be characterized as a shift toward intrinsic death threat or reminders of mortality play in PTG.
goals, which are oriented toward building meaningful Reminders of mortality in the TMT tradition play a
and lasting resources and satisfying essential human causal role in demonstrated effects, as these reminders
needs for autonomy, relatedness, competence, or are under the experimental control of the investigator.
growth (e.g., build closer relationships, help the world However, in naturalistic studies of PTG, reminders of
be a better place) and away from extrinsic goals, which mortality can coexist with changes in resources,
are oriented toward building only transient resources lifestyle, or energy level. The determination of what role
and satisfying superficial needs (e.g., be attractive, make perceived mortality plays in PTG may be an important
more money) (Kasser, 2002; Kasser & Ryan, 1996). first step in understanding the differences between the
On the other hand, experimental reminders of death PTG and TMT literatures.
lead to extrinsic shifts in tests of TMT (Arndt, Solomon, A second difference between the PTG and TMT tra-
Kasser, & Sheldon, 2004; Dechesne et al., 2003; Kasser ditions is the duration of mortality reminders. The nat-
& Sheldon, 2000). TMT posits that humans possess an urally occurring confrontations with death in the PTG
instinctive drive for self-preservation but also have an literature tend to be on a longer term (typically days to
understanding of the inevitability of death, which can years) than the experimental confrontations of death in
produce existential anxiety (Greenberg, Pyszczynski, & TMT (typically seconds to minutes). Currently, the
Solomon, 1986; S. Solomon, Greenberg, & Pyszczynski, effect of length of mortality reminders on goal value has
1991). Humans manage this anxiety by living in accord not yet been investigated empirically.
with a cultural worldview in which their lives are imbued A third potential difference between the PTG and
with worldview-consistent meaning, permanence, and TMT traditions is the type of processing associated with
stability (Pyszczynski, Greenberg, & Solomon, 1999). mortality reminders in the two literatures. Cozzolino,
The mortality salience (MS) hypothesis of TMT suggests Staples, Meyers, and Samboceti (2004) compared a TMT
that reminding people of their mortality (MS induction) manipulation, which typically elicits more defensive pro-
should increase their need for the structure and meaning cessing such as distraction and optimistically biased
provided by their personal worldview and their need for appraisals (Pyszczynksi et al., 1999), with a death-reflection
self-esteem. Experimental evidence suggests that induc- manipulation in which participants focused on their own
tion of MS promotes worldview defense in the form of death in a manner consistent with PTG (Bower et al.,
greater emphasis placed on personally and culturally val- 1998), including an imagined real death, a life review,
ued, but extrinsic, goals such as physical attractiveness, and the opportunity to take the perspective of others.
materialism, and wealth (Arndt et al., 2004; Kasser & Cozzolino et al. found that those who were initially
Sheldon, 2000; Routledge, Arndt, & Goldenberg, 2004). extrinsically oriented and engaged in death reflection
PTG and TMT suggest that reminders of mortality lead took fewer raffle tickets in return for their participation
to intrinsic and extrinsic goal value, respectively. This arti- than if they processed death more generally and
cle is an attempt to resolve this theoretical paradox. It is abstractly in the mortality-salience condition. These find-
likely that one or more major experiential or method- ings suggest that type of processing of death-related stim-
ological differences in these two research traditions uli affects behavior associated with selfishness and greed,
account for the different results obtained. PTG research which in turn suggests changes in goal structure.
typically examines naturalistic challenges, including con- However, although Cozzolino et al. investigated the
frontations with death (i.e., cancer, heart attacks, HIV dis- influence of type of death processing, they did so on a
ease) that are typically uncontrollable and occur over cross-sectional basis, which did not allow for an exami-
weeks, months, or years, whereas TMT research typically nation of the influence of duration of processing.
experimentally manipulates death reminders (e.g., presen- The present studies used both naturalistic and exper-
tations of death-related words) on a single occasion. Thus, imental paradigms to investigate the influences of mor-
the current research will focus on examining what types of tality threats and reminders, the passage of time, and
experiences may prompt TMT- or PTG- consistent out- type of processing on intrinsic and extrinsic goal value.
comes, which will help develop a better understanding of In general, we hypothesized that mortality threats and
the mechanisms underlying the different consequences of reminders are associated with more intrinsic goal value,
confronting death, which are associated with significantly but only when these threats and reminders are prolonged
different psychological outcomes. and when thoughts about them are reflective.
Lykins et al. / GOAL SHIFTS FOLLOWING REMINDERS OF MORTALITY 3

STUDY 1 Procedure
Participants were approached by an investigator at their
The influence of perceived death threat on goal shifts
workplace. The purposes of the study, the procedure, and
consistent with PTG was investigated in the context of
the planned follow-up waves were explained. To ensure a
the Northridge earthquake, which struck the Los
sample of people without severe mental health problems,
Angeles area in the early morning hours of January 17,
participants were excluded if, previous to the earthquake,
1994, and measured 6.7 on the Richter scale. The expe-
they had had a psychiatric hospitalization or were taking
rience of the earthquake could justifiably have reminded
psychiatric medication or were currently experiencing dis-
affected individuals of their own mortality: Fifty-seven
tress so severe that they were not able to function in their
people died as a direct result of the earthquake, includ-
normal activities. Participants were given questionnaires to
ing 16 crushed in a single apartment building that col-
be completed and returned in the mail. Some participants
lapsed. More people died as an indirect result of the
had been transferred to a nearby VAMC at Time 2 and
earthquake; for example, cardiac deaths in the area
were followed up at that site. Time 1 data were collected
doubled on the day of the earthquake (Kloner, Leor,
11-24 days after the earthquake; Time 2 data were col-
Poole, & Perritt, 1997). Thousands of people were
lected 53-67 days after the earthquake.
injured (Coffey, 1994).
In a sample of hospital employees who experienced
the Northridge earthquake in one of the Los Angeles Measures
areas most heavily damaged by the earthquake, we
Demographic information. Participants were asked
hypothesized that surviving the earthquake would lead
to report their gender, age, and ethnic background.
to intrinsic goal shifts consistent with PTG literature, as
their experience would include many long-term
Life Goals Inventory. At Time 2, participants were
reminders of their own mortality. We further hypothe-
asked to indicate how important 16 specific goals were to
sized that the degree of mortality threat experienced
them before and after the earthquake on the Life Goals
during the earthquake would be associated with the
Inventory (Kemeny, 1995). Responses were on a 7-point
degree of intrinsic goal shift, such that greater threat
scale with endpoints 1 = not at all and 7 = extremely. The
would lead to larger shifts.
16 goals were divided into two categories: intrinsic (9
items; e.g., cultivate close friendships, give and receive
Method love, develop myself as a person, do creative work; α =
.89) and extrinsic (7 items; e.g., be attractive, achieve
Participants acknowledgement and praise, have means or possessions,
career advancement; α = .83). Following Kasser and Ryan
Participants were employees of the Sepulveda
(1996), difference scores were calculated by subtracting
Veterans Affairs Medical Center (VAMC) in clinical,
the extrinsic scale mean from the intrinsic scale mean so
administrative, and support services. The Sepulveda
positive values indicate the relative importance of intrinsic
VAMC is located less than 2 km from the epicenter of
over extrinsic goals. The magnitude of the difference score
the Northridge earthquake and sustained severe dam-
relays information as to how much intrinsic goals are
age. Most employees lived near the hospital in the San
prioritized over extrinsic goals (Kasser & Ryan, 1996).
Fernando Valley, and most experienced damage to
their homes and possessions in addition to the damage
Mortality threat. At Time 1, participants were asked,
to their workplace and local infrastructure (roads, elec-
“At any time during the earthquake did you think you
tric, gas, and water; G. F. Solomon, Segerstrom, Grohr,
might die?” and responded on a 7-point Likert-type
Kemeny, & Fahey, 1997). The participants for this
scale with endpoints 1 = not at all and 7 = absolutely.
study were 74 employees who completed question-
Participants reported a wide range of mortality threat.
naires at two waves after the earthquake. Mean age
Mean response to this item was 3.0 (SD = 2.3), with a
was 45 years (range = 24 to 66). The sample was 63%
range from 1 to 7. Approximately one third of the sam-
female and 30% minority. These participants were a
ple responded at or above the scale midpoint.
subset of 113 participants who participated at Time 1.
Those who continued at Time 2 tended to be slightly
older than those who did not continue (M = 38 years), Results
t(111) = 1.77, p < .10. However, there were no differ-
ences in gender or ethnicity or in any of the relevant Mortality Threat and Changes in Goal Value
study variables between those who continued and those Changes in goal value are shown in Table 1. There
who did not. was no effect of the earthquake on goal value generally,
4 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

TABLE 1: Reported Changes in Goal Importance Before and After the Northridge Earthquake

Before Earthquake After Earthquake

Mean importance 4.93 (0.99) 4.92 (0.89)


Intrinsic mean 5.08 (1.19) 5.31 (0.97)
Extrinsic mean 4.63 (1.11) 4.41 (1.07)
Mean difference score 0.45 (0.81) 0.90 (0.97)

NOTE: Standard deviations in parentheses.

so pre-event and current mean goal value reported at explained by a lack of influence of mortality threat in
Time 2 were almost identical before and after the earth- PTG outcomes.
quake, F(1, 73) = 0.05, ns, η = .03. However, intrinsic The question of whether people can accurately recall
goals were reported to be more valued after the earth- their previous goal values after they have experienced a
quake than before, F(1, 73) = 6.48, p < .05, η = .29, and traumatic event must be addressed. It is possible that
extrinsic goals were reported to be less valued, F(1,73) after mortality threat, people devalue their prior intrin-
= 8.52, p < .05, η = .32. Consequently, there was a sig- sic goals in order to perceive goal shift where none has
nificant perception of goal shift, as represented by the actually occurred. One remedy would be to assess goals
intrinsic-extrinsic difference, from before to after the before and after a traumatic event; however, most such
earthquake, F(1, 73) = 24.53, p < .05, η = .50. events are difficult to predict and therefore to study in a
Reports of a change in goal value from before the prospective design. Another remedy is to compare goal
earthquake to 8 weeks after the earthquake, that is, per- shifts occurring after mortality threat to those occurring
ceived goal shift at Time 2, was significantly related to after a benign event for which people should not be
the perception that one’s life was at risk during the earth- motivated to devalue their prior selves, as changes in the
quake as reported at Time 1. Thoughts of more certain mortality threat group can be separated out from goal
death during the earthquake predicted a greater perceived shifts normally reported over time. This approach was
goal shift (r = .38, p < .001). People who feared most for employed in Study 2, in which the influence of duration
their lives during the earthquake were later most likely to of mortality threat was also investigated.
endorse increased value placed on intrinsic goals (r = .24,
p < .04) and, to a lesser degree, decreased value placed on
extrinsic goals (r = –.19, p < .11). STUDY 2

Discussion The influence of duration of mortality reminders was


investigated by examining perceived goal shifts occur-
Following the Northridge earthquake, hospital ring over a 6-week period in a sample of midwestern
employees reported shifts in their personal goals away undergraduate students who either vicariously experi-
from extrinsic goals such as “please people, be liked, enced the terrorist attacks of September 11, 2001, or
popular” and “have means or possessions” and toward were completing a normal semester 3 years later.
intrinsic goals such as “maintain close relationships with Mortality reminders associated with the terrorist
relatives” and “accomplish things in life.” In addition, attacks of 9/11, which killed almost 3,000 people, were
more certain thoughts of dying during the earthquake, experienced widely, albeit indirectly. Sixty-nine percent
recalled in the days afterward, predicted greater intrin- of American households were estimated to have
sic goal shifts 6 weeks later. Thus, a reminder of one’s watched prime-time television on the day of the attacks,
death on the magnitude of surviving an earthquake when most programming focused on the terrorist
appears to lead to intrinsic goal shifts. That greater per- attacks (Kissell, 2002), and American adults were esti-
ceived mortality threat during the earthquake predicted mated to have watched approximately 8.1 hr of televi-
greater perceived intrinsic goal shifts over 8 weeks sug- sion coverage of the attacks by 3 to 5 days after the
gests that mortality threat does play a role in PTG. attacks (Schuster et al., 2001). This coverage included
Although this finding does not dispute the additional scenes of airplanes crashing into the World Trade
role of changes in other areas such as resources, Center, the wreckage of the planes that crashed into the
lifestyle, or energy level, this study does demonstrate Pentagon and the Pennsylvania countryside, and the
that the magnitude of PTG outcomes is influenced by extreme fear and distress experienced by those surviving
the magnitude of mortality threat. Thus, the differences the attacks. The extensive media coverage of the terror-
between the PTG and TMT literatures cannot simply be ist attacks following 9/11, as well as messages about the
Lykins et al. / GOAL SHIFTS FOLLOWING REMINDERS OF MORTALITY 5

continued threat of terrorism, likely served to remind 2004, the final sample of 95 participants was 92%
many Americans of their own mortality. female and 91% Caucasian, with a mean age of 19.04
Preliminary evidence seems to support the experience years (range = 18 to 44). The samples from 2001 and
of increased intrinsic goal value and PTG in Americans 2004 with complete data had no significant age or
following the terrorist attacks of September 11, 2001. minority participation differences, though the 2004
Americans reported higher levels of character strengths sample did have a lower proportion of males than the
such as spirituality, hope, kindness, leadership, love, 2001 sample, χ2(1) = 4.58, p < .05.
gratitude, and teamwork after the attacks of 9/11 than
before (Peterson & Seligman, 2003) and reported the
initiation of prosocial or altruistic behaviors, intensifi- Procedure
cation of the quest for value or meaning and relational
investment, and increased feelings of patriotism (Yum Participants completed questionnaires in small groups
& Schnuck-Hamlin, 2005). at two time points. In 2001, the first assessment took
Study 2 allows for an examination of the influence of place during the week of October 15, 2001, approxi-
duration of mortality reminders and additionally offers mately 5 weeks after the September 11 attacks. The sec-
broader generalizability to life events. Because these ond assessment took place 6 weeks later, during the
participants experienced the 9/11 attacks vicariously, week of November 26, 2001, which was approximately
they did not experience mortality threat to the degree 11 weeks after the attacks. In 2004, the first assessment
that the earthquake sample did. Such reminders are took place during the week of October 18, 2004,
more likely to occur to the average person than are approximately 8 weeks after the initiation of the semes-
direct experiences, so evidence of goal shifts following ter. The second assessment took place 6 weeks later,
indirect confrontations with mortality are more applic- during the week of November 29, 2004, approximately
able to daily life. 14 weeks after the semester’s initiation. All measures
We hypothesized, this time in a between-groups were administered at both time points.
design, that a sample that had recently experienced the
9/11 terrorist attacks (the 2001 sample) would report Measures
greater intrinsic goal shifts than a sample that had not
experienced a recent attack (the 2004 sample). We Demographic information. Participants were asked
further hypothesized that these effects would be more to report their gender, age, and ethnic background.
evident with passage of time since the attacks.
Life Goals Inventory. At Time 1 and Time 2, partic-
ipants were asked to complete the Life Goals Inventory
Method (Kemeny, 1995), in which they were asked to indicate
how important the same 16 goals used in Study 1 were
Participants to them both currently and before a designated time
The participants were 225 undergraduate students at point. In 2001, that time point was “before the terror-
a large midwestern university who participated for par- ist attacks of September 11, 2001.” In 2004, that time
tial course credit. At the first of two time points (Time 1), point was “before the beginning of the current semes-
110 students participated in 2001, and 115 students par- ter.” Alpha reliabilities were acceptable for all time
ticipated in 2004. In 2001, the initial sample size frames at all time points (intrinsic, .79 to .84.; extrin-
decreased from N = 110 at Time 1 to n = 92 at Time 2 sic, .73 to .79).
(16% attrition). In 2004, the initial sample size decreased
from N = 115 to n = 95 (17% attrition). In 2001, there
Data Analysis
were no significant differences in age or gender between
the individuals who completed the study and those who To use a sample with a roughly equivalent experience
dropped out. However, a higher proportion of minorities with the 9/11 terrorist attacks, individuals who knew a
dropped out of the study, χ2(1) = 11.16, p = .001. In close friend, relative, or acquaintance who was injured
2004, there were no significant differences in age, gender, or killed (n = 22, with 11 knowing someone who was
or minority status between those who did and those who killed) were excluded from all analyses. Repeated-
did not complete the study. measure ANOVAs were conducted for the intrinsic—
The demographic characteristics of the final samples extrinsic difference scores, using the Time 1 pre-event
in 2001 and 2004 are as follows: In 2001, the final sample retrospective, Time 1 current, and Time 2 current val-
of 92 participants was 82% female and 96% Caucasian, ues for each participant. Group (2001 vs. 2004 sample)
with an average age of 18.41 years (range = 18 to 25). In was included as a between-subjects factor.
6 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

Results
2.5
Validity of Retrospective Reports of Goal Value

Mean Difference Score


2
We employed two comparisons to assess the validity
of retrospective reports of goal value prior to a threaten- 1.5
ing event. First, we calculated the differences between ret-
rospective reports of pre-event goal value at two time 1
points, 5 and 11 weeks after the event in 2001 and 8 and
14 weeks after the event in 2004. If motivational factors 0.5
affect retrospective reports, then there may be changes in
motivation and retrospective report between those two 0
Before Time 1 Time 2
time points. Conversely, lack of change would tend to
support accurate reporting. Test-retest reliability was Time Point
high, with correlations between retrospective reports at
Time 1 and Time 2 of .73 for intrinsic goals, p < .001, 2001 2004
and of .73 for extrinsic goals, p < .001. Second, we com-
pared retrospective reports by the sample asked to report Figure 1 Mean Difference Scores as a Function of Group and Time
on goal shifts after the terrorist attacks to those by the Point in Study 2
NOTE: Higher scores represent more value attached to intrinsic over
sample asked to report on goal shifts after starting col- extrinsic goals.
lege. If motivational factors affect retrospective reports,
the former sample should devalue their pre-event goals
relative to the latter. A repeated-measures ANOVA with
goal type (within; extrinsic, intrinsic), time of retrospec- of the groups did not significantly differ at baseline (see
tive report (within; Time 1 pre-event, Time 2 pre-event), above) or at Time 1, t(149) = 1.84, ns, though they sig-
and group (between; 2001, 2004) was conducted to nificantly differed at Time 2, t(148) = 2.50, p = .01.
examine these motivational factors. Overall goal values
were consistent across time, F(1, 149) = 1.16, ns, η = .09, Discussion
did not vary across time by group, F(1, 149) = 1.66, ns,
η = .10, and did not vary by group and goal type across A sample recently exposed to the 2001 terrorist
time, F(1, 149) = 2.31, ns, η = .12. These analyses demon- attacks perceived greater intrinsic goal shift over time as
strate that the retrospective reports of pre-event goal compared with a parallel control sample assessed in
value were not affected by motivational factors, as the 2004. Although the experimentally controlled, short-
reported pre-event goal values were consistent at Times 1 term confrontations with death in TMT may trigger
and 2 and did not vary by group or group by goal value defensive processing, this study suggests that confronting
at Times 1 and 2. Thus, for all analyses, the Time 1 pre- more real-life and personal reminders of one’s mortality
event goal values will be used as the “before” value, due may eventually promote psychological growth, as demon-
to its temporal proximity to the pre-event time frame. strated in goal shifts, though it may not promote such
growth immediately, as group differences were demon-
strated at Time 2 but not at Time 1.
Changes in Goal Value
Studies 1 and 2 demonstrate that confronting more
From an initial equivalent pre-event difference score, real-life and personal reminders or threats toward per-
t(149) = –1.58, ns, difference scores became more sonal mortality can produce perceived intrinsic goal
intrinsic over time in the combined sample, F(2, 296) = shifts consistent with PTG, suggest that degree of mor-
20.24, p < .001, η = .35. However, this was qualified by tality threat is associated with degree of perceived
a significant group by time interaction, F(2, 296) = intrinsic goal shift, and suggest that duration of pro-
11.18, p < .001, η = .26, in which the 2001 group cessing does play an important role in goal shifts.
showed more intrinsic goal shift (difference scores of However, these studies used traditional PTG paradigms
0.61 pre-event, 1.93 at Time 1, and 1.72 at Time 2) in which people who were not randomly assigned to
than did the 2004 group (difference scores of 1.09 pre- degree of mortality threat retrospectively reported
event, 1.40 at Time 1, and 1.08 at Time 2). Figure 1 changes in their goals. Furthermore, the influence of
shows the relative value of goals for the 2001 and 2004 type of death-related processing has not yet been
groups: The group that was indirectly exposed to mor- addressed. Thus, Study 3 used an experimental para-
tality threat reported a larger shift toward intrinsic digm to examine the joint influences of duration of pro-
goals than did the control group. The difference scores cessing, type of processing, and pre-existing goal values
Lykins et al. / GOAL SHIFTS FOLLOWING REMINDERS OF MORTALITY 7

in determining the effects of reminders of mortality on the full study, mean age was 19.1 years (range = 18 to 29).
perceived goal shifts. The sample was 80% female and 10% minority.

Procedure
STUDY 3
Three groups of participants were randomly assigned
A midwestern student sample reported on current goal to one of three conditions, each of which involved 6
values both before and after completing 6 days of pro- total days of participation. In the first sessions, each
cessing in one of three topic conditions: death reflection, group of participants was seated in a common room,
mortality salience, or control (dental pain). The death- received information about the study, and signed an
reflection manipulation was designed to capture the key informed consent form. Participants then spent 10 min
elements of near-death experience, contains elements engaged in a processing task that consisted of answering
related to PTG, and may lead to shifts toward intrinsic open-ended essay questions that comprised the experi-
goals (Cozzolino et al., 2004), whereas the mortality- mental manipulation. The participants also completed
salience manipulation was consistent with typical MS the demographic questionnaire, the Life Goals Inventory,
inductions that have resulted in shifts toward extrinsic and additional filler questionnaires.
goals (Routledge et al., 2004). We hypothesized that type Members of the death-reflection group were pre-
of processing would influence perceived goal values and sented a vivid half-page scenario in which they were
goal shifts such that on Day 1, the participants in the asked to imagine themselves caught in a building fire,
death-reflection condition would report increased intrin- frantically attempting to escape, but eventually suc-
sic goal value and lower extrinsic goal value than those in cumbing to smoke and fire. They were then asked to
the mortality-salience condition, with those in the control describe the thoughts and emotions they felt while
condition reporting values between the two death-related imagining the scenario, discuss how they would handle
groups. In addition, we hypothesized that duration of the final moments, describe the life they had led up to
processing and type of processing would interact over the that point, and discuss how they think their family and
6-day processing period. Those participants in the death- friends would react if the event actually occurred. The
reflection condition would experience early intrinsic goal mortality-salience group members were asked to simply
shift whereas those in the mortality-salience condition describe the thoughts, feelings, and emotions that the
would report early extrinsic goal shift, but as a function thought of their own death aroused in them, as well as
of repeated mortality reminders, those in the mortality- describing what they think will happen to them physi-
salience condition would also report intrinsic goal shift cally as they die. The dental-pain condition participants
by Day 6. Those in the control condition would report were asked a parallel set of questions related to dental
minimal or no shift in goal values. Finally, we predicted pain, describing the thoughts, feelings, and emotions
that Day 1 goal values would interact with duration of that the thought of dental pain aroused in them, as well
processing and type of processing such that relatively as describing what they think will happen to them phys-
intrinsic individuals would experience less positive ically as they experience dental pain.
change than relatively extrinsic individuals over the pro- On the next 4 consecutive days (Days 2 to 5), partici-
cessing period because they already value intrinsic goals pants completed their respective 10-min processing task,
(Cozzolino et al., 2004). submitting their responses daily through the Internet. On
Days 2 and 4, the death-processing group was asked to
write about their thoughts and emotions and how they
Method would handle the final moments; the mortality-salience
group was asked to write about the thoughts, feelings,
Participants
and emotions that the thought of their own death
The participants were 149 undergraduate students at aroused in them; and the dental-pain group was asked to
a large midwestern university who participated for par- write about the thoughts, feelings, and emotions that the
tial course credit. Participants were eligible for the study thought of dental pain aroused in them. On Days 3 and
if they were 18 years of age and were able to read and 5, the death-processing group was asked to write about
write in English. The initial sample size decreased from the life they had led up to the point of their death and
N = 149 at Day 1 to n = 103 at Day 6 (31% attrition). how their family and friends would react; the mortality-
There were no significant differences in age, gender, or salience group was asked to write about what will hap-
minority status between the individuals who completed pen to them physically after death; and the dental-pain
the study and those who dropped out or were terminated group was asked to write about what will happen to
from the study. In the sample of individuals completing them physically as they experience dental pain.
8 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

TABLE 2: Content Differences Among Groups: Day 1

Content Category Death Reflection Mortality Salience Dental Pain p (F) <

Positive affect .09 (.16)a .09 (.14)a .03 (.08)a .13


Negative affect .47 (.19)a .39 (.19)a .17 (.19)a .08
Physical reality .06 (.13)a .55 (.18)b .62 (.25)b .001
Physical reaction .04 (.11)a .05 (.13)a .42 (.32)b .001
Life reflection .41 (.16)a .05 (.11)b .00 (.00)b .001
Life goals .10 (.16)a .16 (.21)b .00 (.00)a .001
Life regrets .13 (.18)a .03 (.08)b .00 (.00)b .001
Thoughts of others .55 (.13)a .25 (.20)b .01 (.07)c .001
Selfish thoughts of others .03 (.08)a .06 (.12)a .00 (.00)a .03
Religion .17 (.18)a .21 (.21)a .00 (.00)b .001
Personal mortality .05 (.11)a .28 (.16)b .01 (.08)a .001
Universal mortality .01 (.04)a .11 (.18)b .00 (.00)a .001

NOTE: Means not sharing subscripts are significantly different after Bonferroni correction (α = .004).

On the final day of the study (Day 6), participants .78 to .99. Following discussion of disagreements, individ-
completed their final 10-min processing task, using the ual raters then coded the remaining responses. Following
same prompts as on Day 1. They also again completed Cozzolino et al., all content codings were transformed into
the Life Goals Inventory and additional filler question- proportions (content category coding matches/total content
naires. After submitting their answers, participants were coding matches) for each participant to account for indi-
presented with online debriefing information. Student vidual differences in the length of responses. Distributions
participants received research credits commensurate of these proportions were skewed and therefore were
with the number of days they completed, but they were square-root transformed to improve normality.
dropped from the study if they failed to submit their
answers by midnight on any active study day. Results

Measures Content Differences Among Groups


Demographic information. Participants were asked One-way ANOVAs with Bonferroni correction (α =
to report their gender, age, and ethnic background. .004) revealed significant differences among the three
experimental groups in all categories of response content
Life Goals Inventory. On Days 1 and 6, participants except positive affect, negative affect, and selfish thoughts
were asked to complete the Life Goals Inventory of others. Table 2 shows the group means of the square-
(Kemeny, 1995), in which they were asked to indicate root transformed variables on Day 1. To summarize, the
how important the same 16 goals used in Studies 1 and dental-pain group had the most references to physical
2 were to them currently. Alpha reliabilities were reality and physical reactions; both death-topic groups
acceptable for all time frames at all time points (intrin- had more references to religion than the dental-pain
sic, .67 on Day 1 and .83 on Day 6; extrinsic, .75 on group; the mortality-salience group had more references
Day 1 and .74 on Day 6). to mortality, both personal and universal, and life goals
than the other two groups; and the death-reflection group
Content Coding had more references to life reflection, life regrets, and
thoughts of others than the other two groups. These pat-
Based on Cozzolino et al. (2004), responses to the pro-
terns were maintained at Day 6, with the exceptions that
cessing tasks on Days 1 and 6 were coded using 12 cate-
the death-reflection group had more references to life
gories: positive affect; negative affect; physical reality of
goals than the dental-pain group, and the difference in life
dying or dental pain (e.g., pain); physical reactions to
regrets between death reflection and mortality salience no
thinking about death or dental pain (e.g., increased heart
longer reached Bonferroni-corrected significance.
rate, “stomach dropped”); life reflections or summary; life
goals; life regrets; thoughts of others; selfish thoughts of
Goal Differences Among Groups
others; religious references; personal mortality (e.g., “I
know I am going to a better place”); and universal mortal- At Day 1, for the total sample, there were no signifi-
ity (“Death is natural or inevitable”). Initially, two raters cant group differences in intrinsic or extrinsic goal value
coded each of 45 responses, yielding alpha reliabilities of or in goal value difference scores. For the sample that
Lykins et al. / GOAL SHIFTS FOLLOWING REMINDERS OF MORTALITY 9

completed through Day 6, there were no significant dif-


6
ferences on Day 1 between groups in their extrinsic goal

Goal Change
4
value or in the difference scores. The groups did differ
2
significantly in their intrinsic goal value, F(2, 102) = 3.37,
0
p < .05, η = .24, with the dental-pain group members
−2
reporting higher value attached to intrinsic goals (M =
−4
53.52) than either death-processing group (death reflec-
−6
tion M = 50.09; mortality salience M = 50.55). Death Mortality Dental pain
On Day 6, there were no significant differences reflection salience
between groups in their goal value difference score Experimental Group
after controlling for Day 1 goal value, F(2, 89) = 0.04,
High initial value (+1 SD)
ns, η = .03. However, there was an interaction between Low initial value (−1 SD)
group and initial goal value, F(2, 89) = 3.92, p < .05,
η = .28. Figure 2 shows the results from regression
Figure 2 Predicted Changes in Goals From Day 1 to Day 6 in
models run for each group, with low initial values Study 3 for Participants High and Low in Initial Goal
equal to –1 SD and high initial values equal to +1 SD. Emphasis by Experimental Group
First, there was evidence for regression toward the
mean; those with high initial values tended to decrease, Discussion
whereas those with low initial values tended to
increase. However, this tendency differed significantly Duration and type of processing and pre-existing goal
among groups. Participants with high initial values values are all important to the type of outcomes observed
decreased the least in the death-reflection group and or perceived following a prolonged confrontation with
most in the mortality-salience group; participants with reminders of personal mortality. Although no reliable
low initial values increased the most in the mortality- group differences were found in difference scores at Day 1
salience group and least in the death-reflection group. or in goal shifts occurring from Day 1 to Day 6, individu-
The impression that the differences in goal change were als who reported more intrinsic goals at Day 1 appeared
driven by the two death-topic groups is confirmed by a most able to maintain their intrinsic goal orientation
significant interaction between initial goal value and across time in the death-reflection condition, whereas indi-
these two groups, F(1, 61) = 7.21, p < .05, η = .33; the viduals with higher extrinsic goals at Day 1 appeared to
interaction was not significant when restricted to either experience the most intrinsic goal shifts in the mortality-
death-topic group and the dental-pain group. salience condition. Thus, some combinations of pre-existing
Analysis of intrinsic and extrinsic goals separately goal values and processing type are more psychologically
suggested that this effect was due to their combined advantageous across time t
influence on the difference score, because the initial han others.
value by group interaction was not significant for either Death reflection led to the most intrinsic goal value for
one: intrinsic, F(2, 89) = 1.43, ns, η = .17; extrinsic, F(2, individuals who were already intrinsically oriented and
89) = 2.03, ns, η = .20. the least for those who were already extrinsically oriented.
Following confrontations with mortality, processing
focused on the quality of one’s life to date (e.g., reflection,
Initial Goals and Content Changes
regrets) may reinforce the value of intrinsic goals for those
The possibility that content changes mediated these who already value them but may also reinforce extrinsic
differences was explored in the two death-topic groups. goal values for those who already value them. In contrast,
After Bonferroni correction (α = .004) and controlling rehearsal of one’s own theory of what happens to oneself,
for initial content, there were several main effects of one’s body, or humans generally following death, which was
group at Day 6 such that the death-reflection group was the content of the processing conducted by the mortality-
lower in physical reality and higher in life reflection and salience group, may actually lead to more intrinsically ori-
thoughts of others. There were no significant interac- ented goals over time for individuals who possess more
tions between initial content and group; the largest extrinsic goals initially.
effect was for life reflection (p < .01). However, con- The experimental groups maintained their content dif-
trolling for life reflection at Day 1 and Day 6 did not ferences over time, suggesting that extrinsically oriented
affect the initial goal value by group interaction, F(1, individuals are not simply changing the nature of their
59) = 7.64, p < .05, η = .35. Therefore, this particular processing over time to produce intrinsic goal shifts. This
content category did not account for the effect of group suggests that the psychological outcomes of prolonged
and initial goal value on later goal value. processing of death reminders may be influenced by the
10 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

“match” between type of goal structure (deep for intrinsic, more conflicted relationships with friends and romantic
shallow for extrinsic) and type of processing done (deep partners (Kasser, 2002; Kasser & Ryan, 1996;
for death reflection, shallow for mortality salience). Schmuck, Kasser, & Ryan, 2000; Sheldon, Ryan, Deci,
Unlike Cozzolino et al. (2004), we did not find effects & Kasser, 2004). In addition, individuals valuing
of condition in our cross-sectional data analysis, and the extrinsic over intrinsic goals may adopt contingencies of
longitudinal patterns suggested here are almost com- self-worth, make more frequent social comparisons,
pletely opposite. However, we examined perceived goal and diminish time available for intrinsic goals (Sheldon
values at two time points, whereas Cozzolino et al. exam- et al., 2004). Thus, maintaining an intrinsic goal orien-
ined a behavioral outcome. It is possible that behavioral tation (as intrinsically oriented individuals did best in
changes dissociate from goal values, at least after acute the death-reflection condition) or making an intrinsic
experimental manipulation, or that altruistic behavior is shift in goal valuation (as did the extrinsically oriented
unrelated to generally intrinsic and extrinsic goal values individuals in the mortality-salience condition) across
as measured by the Life Goals Inventory. Furthermore, time will likely maintain or lead to new, qualitatively
the content analysis patterns are generally consistent with positive effects. As intrinsic goal shifts appear to lead to
Cozzolino et al. but do not match exactly. Thus, future the building of important resources (i.e., close relation-
work needs to examine how outcome type and content ships, feelings of competence), it is difficult to imagine
type affect the results of death reflection. that these effects would be transient. Thus, long-term
changes in goal value appear possible as a link between
confrontations with death and PTG.
GENERAL DISCUSSION Of course, Study 3 suggests that psychologically
advantageous goal shifts or the maintenance of health-
PTG and terror management findings appear contra- ier goal values will only occur following processing that
dictory, with PTG suggesting that confrontations with is matched to an individual’s goal value structure over
death lead to intrinsic goal shift and TMT suggesting time, which suggests that the current findings may be
that they lead to extrinsic goal shift. The present studies used in an intervention framework. Encouraging intrin-
demonstrate that differences in duration of those con- sically oriented individuals who are confronting death
frontations and the type of processing that occurs after- to reflect on life, process life regrets, and think of others
ward are important factors in determining the type of may promote PTG through continued valuation of
goal change that results. Studies of PTG typically intrinsic goals. Alternately, encouraging individuals
include long-term reminders of death, which appear to with more extrinsic goals to process their theory of
promote maintenance of intrinsic goals or intrinsic goal what happens following death as well as current life
shifts. Alternately, studies in TMT typically include very goals may help promote PTG over time through intrin-
short-term reminders of death, which here led to no dis- sic goal shifts. However, a mismatch between level of
cernable group differences but in other studies have processing and goal values may lead to a devaluation of
elicited behavior consistent with extrinsic goal values intrinsic goals, or extrinsic shifts. Very traumatic events
such as greed and vanity (Cozzolino et al., 2004; or reminders of one’s mortality create tremendous psy-
Routledge et al., 2004). Although individual goal valu- chosocial needs. Thus, it is quite important to ensure
ation and type of processing influence goal outcomes, that clinicians are helping clients face death in a manner
the main factor influencing differences between PTG that will have the most psychologically beneficial out-
and TMT results may be the influence of time, as longer comes, and the current research suggests that goal val-
duration of processing was related to outcomes consis- ues and type of processing must be considered before
tent with PTG across all studies. The current research beginning an intervention, though death-reflection pro-
suggests that when people encounter death over a cessing may be feasible for all individuals, initially or
longer period of time or in a manner consistent with once their goals begin to become more intrinsically ori-
their goal structure, they move to transcend their defen- ented. The promotion of PTG is a reasonable therapeu-
siveness, maintain or become more intrinsically ori- tic goal; for example, it mediates the physiological
ented, and may end up healthier in the long term. benefit of cognitive-behavioral therapy in breast cancer
Research on the correlates of intrinsic and extrinsic patients (Cruess et al., 2000; McGregor et al., 2004).
goals demonstrates that the relative centrality of intrinsic Although this series of studies resolves much of the
goals is positively related to well-being, adjustment, and PTG versus TMT paradox, there are important ques-
self-actualization and is negatively related to anxiety. In tions remaining. In the absence of information on pro-
contrast, the relative focus on extrinsic goals is related cessing type in Studies 1 and 2, the three-way
to lower levels of happiness, higher susceptibility to psy- interaction of processing type, initial goal value, and
chological disorders in adolescents, and less loving and duration could only be examined in Study 3. In
Lykins et al. / GOAL SHIFTS FOLLOWING REMINDERS OF MORTALITY 11

addition, work examining the influence of all three fac- Burt, M. R., & Katz, B. L. (1987). Dimensions of recovery from rape:
Focus on growth outcomes. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 2,
tors in naturally occurring confrontations with death is 57-81.
necessary, as the only study examining the influence of Calhoun, L. G., & Tedeschi, R. G. (1989-1990). Positive aspects of
all three factors simultaneously was an experimental critical life problems: Recollections of grief. Omega, 20, 265-272.
Calhoun, L. G., & Tedeschi, R. G. (1999). Facilitating posttraumatic
study. However, the current studies do point to the growth: A clinician’s guide. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
influences of all three factors and interactions between Calhoun, L. G., & Tedeschi, R. G. (2001). Posttraumatic growth: The
the factors that may have clinical importance in long- positive lessons of loss. In R. A. Neimeyer (Ed.), Meaning recon-
struction and the experience of loss (pp. 157-172). Washington,
term psychological outcomes. The literature would ben- DC: American Psychological Association.
efit from future studies examining such questions as Coffey, Shelby, III (Ed.). (1994). 4:31–Images of the 1994 Los
whether different types of mortality reminders automat- Angeles Earthquake. Los Angeles: Los Angeles Times.
Collins, R. L., Taylor, S. E., & Skokan, L. A. (1990). A better world
ically trigger different types of processing, whether or a shattered vision? Changes in life perspectives following vic-
people with different goal-value structures are naturally timization. Social Cognition, 8, 263-285.
drawn to different types of processing, why a match Cozzolino, P. J., Staples, A. D., Meyers, L. S., & Samboceti, J. (2004).
Greed, death, and values: From terror management to transcen-
between goal values and processing type is more advan- dence management theory. Personality and Social Psychology
tageous than a mismatch, and how long goal shifts fol- Bulletin, 30, 278-292.
lowing mortality reminders are maintained. Further Cruess, D. G., Antoni, M. H., McGregor, B. A., Kilbourn, K. M.,
Boyers, A. E., Alferi, S. M., et al. (2000). Cognitive-behavioral
work using both experimental and naturalistic methods stress management reduces serum cortisol by enhancing benefit
may help illuminate why duration of processing appears finding among women being treated for early stage breast cancer.
to lead to beneficial outcomes in naturally occurring Psychosomatic Medicine, 62, 304-308.
Dechesne, M., Pyszczynski, T., Arndt, J., Ransom, S., Sheldon, K. M.,
confrontations with death such as those examined in van Knippenberg, A., et al. (2003). Literal and symbolic immor-
Studies 1 and 2. tality: The effect of evidence of literal immortality on self-esteem
Confrontations with mortality are a fact of life. With striving in response to mortality salience. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 84, 722-737.
the extensive access to news outlets in our society, the Elder, G. H., Jr., & Clipp, E. C. (1989). Combat experience and emo-
goal to stay informed about world events can induce tional health: Impairment and resilience in later life. Journal of
frequent reminders of mortality. However, the current Personality, 57, 311-341.
Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., & Solomon, S. (1986). The causes and
research suggests that reminders of our own mortality consequences of a need for self-esteem: A terror management
can lead to beneficial outcomes such as intrinsic goal theory. In R. F. Baumeister (Ed.), Public self and private self
shifts over time, with the caveat that an individual’s (pp. 189-212). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Joseph, S., Williams, R., & Yule, W. (1993). Changes in outlook fol-
goal-value structure and the type of processing may lowing disaster: The preliminary development of a measure to
need to be matched with one another. Furthermore, assess positive and negative responses. Journal of Traumatic
when reminders of mortality reach the quality of Stress, 6, 271-279.
Kasser, T. (2002). The high price of materialism. Cambridge, MA:
trauma, as in life-threatening disease or disaster, intrin- MIT Press.
sic goal-structure maintenance or intrinsic shifts may Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1996). Further examining the American
produce PTG by building up personal and relational dream: Differential correlates of intrinsic and extrinsic goals.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 2, 280-287.
resources that the individual can draw upon in times Kasser, T., & Sheldon, K. M. (2000). Of wealth and death:
of need. Intrinsic goals may allow for growth, instead Materialism, mortality salience, and consumption behavior.
of immobility or defensiveness, to occur following a Psychological Science, 11, 348-351.
Kemeny, M. E. (1995). The Life Goals Inventory. Unpublished man-
confrontation with the inevitability of mortality. uscript.
Kissell, R. (2002). Auds show 9/11 apathy. Viewers again gravitate
towards CBS documentary. Retrieved November 25, 2005, from
http://www.variety.com/index.asp?layout=story&articleid=VR11
17872705&categoryid=14&cs=1
REFERENCES Kloner, R. A., Leor, J., Poole, W. K., & Perritt, R. (1997). Population-
based analysis of the effect of the Northridge earthquake on car-
Affleck, G., Tennen, H., Croog, S., & Levine, S. (1987). Causal attri- diac death in Los Angeles County, California. Journal of the
bution, perceived control, and recovery from a heart attack. American College of Cardiology, 30, 1174-1180.
Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 5, 339-355. McGregor, B. A., Antoni, M. H., Boyers, A., Alferi, S. M., Blomberg,
Andrykowski, M. A., Brady, M. G., & Hunt, J. W. (1993). Positive B. B., & Carver, C. S. (2004). Cognitive-behavioral stress man-
psychological adjustment in potential bone marrow transplant agement increases benefit finding and immune function among
recipients: Cancer as a psychosocial transition. Psycho-Oncology, women with early-stage breast cancer. Journal of Psychosomatic
2, 261-276. Research, 56, 1-8.
Arndt, J., Solomon, S., Kasser, T., & Sheldon, K. M. (2004). The urge Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2003). Character strengths before
to splurge revisited: Further reflections on applying terror man- and after September 11. Psychological Science, 14, 381-384.
agement theory to materialism and consumer behavior. Journal of Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., & Solomon, S. (1999). A dual-process
Consumer Psychology, 14, 225-229. model of defense against conscious and unconscious death-related
Bower, J. E., Kemeny, M. E., Taylor, S. E., & Fahey, J. L. (1998). thoughts: An extension of terror management theory.
Cognitive processing, discovery of meaning, CD 4 decline, and Psychological Review, 106, 835-845.
AIDS-related mortality among bereaved HIV-seropositive men. Routledge, C., Arndt, J., & Goldenberg, J. L. (2004). A time to tan:
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 979-986. Proximal and distal effects of mortality salience on sun exposure
12 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

intentions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 24, 91-159).
1347-1358. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Schmuck, P., Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). Intrinsic and extrin- Tedeschi, R. G., & Calhoun, L. G. (1996). The Posttraumatic Growth
sic goals: Their structure and relationship to well-being in German Inventory: Measuring the positive legacy of trauma. Journal of
and U.S. college students. Social Indicators Research, 50, 225-241. Traumatic Stress, 9, 455-471.
Schuster, M. A., Stein, B. D., Jaycox, L. H., Collins, R. L., Marshall, Tedeschi, R. G., & Calhoun, L. G. (2004). Posttraumatic growth:
G. N., Elliott, M. N., et al. (2001). A national survey of stress Conceptual foundations and empirical evidence. Psychological
reactions after the September 11, 2001, terrorist attacks. The New Inquiry, 15, 1-18.
England Journal of Medicine, 345, 1507-1512. Thompson, S. C. (1985). Finding positive meaning in a stressful event
Sheldon, K. M., Ryan, R. M., Deci, E. L., & Kasser, T. (2004). The and coping. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 6, 279-295.
independent effects of goal contents and motives on well-being: Wallerstein, J. S. (1986). Women after divorce: Preliminary report
It’s both what you pursue and why you pursue it. Personality and from a 10-year follow-up. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry,
Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 475-486. 56, 65-77.
Solomon, G. F., Segerstrom, S. C., Grohr, P., Kemeny, M., & Fahey, J. Yum, Y.-O., & Schnuck-Hamlin, W. (2005). Reactions to 9/11 as a
(1997). Shaking up immunity: Psychological and immunologic changes function of terror management and perspective taking. The Journal
after a natural disaster. Psychosomatic Medicine, 59, 114-127. of Social Psychology, 145, 265-268.
Solomon, S., Greenberg, J., & Pyszczynski, T. (1991). A terror man-
agement theory of social behavior: The psychological functions of Received February 6, 2006
self-esteem and cultural worldviews. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Revision accepted January 21, 2007

View publication stats

You might also like