You are on page 1of 7

REVIEW PAPER

Creativity and Mental Illness: Is There a Link?


1
Charlotte Waddell, MSc, MD, CCFP, FRCpc

Objective: To critically assess the scientific evidence for associating creativity with mental illness.
Method: MEDLINE and secondary literature searches identified 29 studies and 34 review articles on creativity
and mental illness. All studies were critically evaluated. Reviews were also assessed.
Results: Of 29 studies that evaluated possible associations between creativity and mental illness, 15 found no
evidence to link creativity and mental illness, 9 found positive evidence, and 5 had unclear findings. Most studies
used flawed methodologies with weak (case series or case control) designs. There were no randomized or
prospective cohort studies. Adequate criteria for determining causal association were not met. In 34 selective
reviews, despite mixed evidence, many authors asserted that creativity and mental illness were positively or
causally associated.
Conclusions: There is limited scientific evidence to associate creativity with mental illness. Despite this, many
authors promoted a connection. Explanations for this contradiction are explored, and social and research
implications are discussed.

(Can J Psychiatry 1998;43:166-172)

Key Words: creativity, mental illness

reativity and mental illness have long been popularly methodological problems as well as social and research im-
C associated. Until recently, few studies have examined
this purported association using scientific methods. A spate
plications are discussed.

ofrecent review articles, as well as memoirs ofmental illness What Is Creativity?


by prominent, creative individuals, have coincided with sci- In the diverse literature on creativity, many authors have
entific interest in possible associations between creativity and suggested definitions of creativity. Becker equated creativity
mental illness. These studies, reviews, and memoirs have with genius or intellectual giftedness (1). Richards suggested
garnered media attention and reinforced popular views that that intelligence was necessary but not sufficient for creativity
creativity and mental illness are positively associated. (2). Storr defined creativity as a dynamic of normal drives to
play (3). Independence was mentioned by Andreasen and
Despite the interest in linking creativity and mental illness,
Glick (4), and fluency and flexibility were cited by Jamison
there have been no recent comprehensive, critical reviews of as essential to creativity (5).
the literature. This review examines the studies on creativity
and mental illness and critically assesses the scientific evi- Rothenberg defined creativity as the ability to simultane-
dence for an association. Reviews are also assessed, and ously conceive opposites or antitheses (6). Ludwig com-
mented that creativity required both unconventionality and a
drive to communicate (7-9). Both Richards and Ludwig
noted that evaluations of novel or creative products were
Manuscript received March 1997, revised and accepted May 1997. always embedded in social and political contexts (2,7-9).
IDepartment of Psychiatry, McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario. Weisberg stressed that creativity required hard work and
Address for correspondence: Dr C Waddell, Department of Psychiatry,
McMaster University, 1200 Main Street West, Hamilton, ON L8N 325
collaboration (10).

In the recent scientific literature, most authors have de-


Can} Psychiatry, Vol 43, March 1998 fined creativity as either processes or products that are both
166
March 1998 Creativity and Mental U1ness 167

original and worthwhile, as denoted by public recognition or individuals. Citations in primary sources were also searched.
awards. Creativity in the arts, science, and public leadership Studies were critically evaluated applying currently accepted
have all been studied, but no universal or psychometrically scientific standards. Reviews were also assessed.
standardized definitions of creativity have been accepted.
Results
What is Mental Illness?
There is copious literature on the nature of mental illness, The search yielded 29 empirical studies on creativity and
and many authors have critiqued psychiatric concepts and mental illness. Fourteen early studies surveyed creative or
definitions. Foucault, a French philosopher and sociologist, gifted people but did not use standardized definitions of
suggested that "madness" was socially constructed to scape- mental illness. Fifteen later studies used standardized defini-
goat and control people whom society found deviant (11). tions of mental illness: 10 assessed mental illness in creative
Foucault's thought was echoed in many later feminist, socio- or gifted people, 1 assessed creativity in gifted people and
logical, and philosophical critiques of psychiatry, including psychiatric patients, and 4 assessed creativity in people with
critiques that suggested "madness" did not exist apart from mental illness. There were also 34 reviews (including letters,
social constructs (12-15). books, and case studies) on creativity and mental illness.
These studies and reviews are summarized.
Most recent authors studying creativity and mental illness
have not debated philosophical definitions. Rather, they have Early Studies
used standardized definitions of mental illness derived from Fourteen early studies assessed mental illness in creative
the American Psychiatric Association's Diagnostic and Sta- or gifted people but did not use standardized or contemporary
tistical Manuals of Mental Disorders (DSM) (16-18). A definitions ofmental illness. First, starting in 1925, Terman's
variety of mental illnesses have been studied in connection group monitored health and social outcomes in 1000 intellec-
with creativity, with most emphasis placed on mood disor- tually gifted children over a 30-year period (28). While not
ders. all the gifted children became eminent, they had lower rates
Popular Links Between Creativity and Mental Illness of mental illness than the general population.
There is a long history ofassociating creativity and mental In 1926 Ellis's survey of 1020 eminent British people
illness in western European cultures, starting with Aristotle, found rates ofschizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and depression
who equated insanity with genius, and culminating in the similar to the general population (29). Nicolson's 1947 sur-
"mad genius" controversy ofthe last 2 centuries (1,2,4-9,19). vey of 32 British authors found that only 6% were "insane"
Many authors have described famous, creative individuals (30). In 1949 Juda reported that the majority of 294 gifted
who reportedly had mental illnesses (5,6,9,19). Recent ac- German artists and scientists surveyed did not have mental
counts in popular media have touted a link between creativity disorders (31).
and various forms of mental illness (20-22). Drevdahl and Cattell assessed 153 American writers and
The popularity ofasserting connections between creativity artists using personality tests in 1958 (32). Writers and artists
and mental illness has also been fueled by recent memoirs of did not have greater psychopathology and were more intelli-
mental illness by prominent gifted individuals. Campbell, a gent, adventurous, and mature than average Americans. In
Canadian medical scientist, grappled with manic-depression 1962 Goertzel and Goertzel found evidence ofabove-average
(23). Styron, an American writer, battled alcohol and depres- rates of depression and suicide in the biographies of 400
sion (24). Millett, an American feminist, struggled with eminent people (33).
manic-depression (and with psychiatrists) (25). Mays, a Ca- Torrance, in 1965, reported that although creative children
nadian art critic, suffered debilitating bouts of depression had less psychopathology, they suffered more sanctions and
(26). Manic-depression coloured the life of Jamison, an hostility and worked alone more than less creative peers (34).
American psychiatric researcher (27). MacKinnon, also in 1965, found that creativity in architects
In summary, much recent psychiatric and popular litera- was associated with unusual profiles on personality tests,
ture has enthusiastically promoted an association between including courage and independence (35).
creativity and mental illness. This enthusiasm, however, has Noreik and Odegard surveyed 12 000 highly educated
not always been balanced with scientific evidence. Norwegians in 1966 and found psychiatric admission rates
comparable to the general population (36). Barron's 1968
Method study of 66 American writers found that while most were
"troubled" according to psychological tests, most also had
The literature was searched with MEDLINE using the
greater "ego strength" than average Americans (37).
following terms: creativity, mental illness, depression,
manic-depression, schizophrenia, and alcohol. The search McNeil's 1971 study of 10 "highly creative" Danish adop-
was restricted to human, English-language studies from this tees found more "constitutional psychopathy" compared with
century that assessed creativity and mental illness in the same less creative comparison subjects (38). Martindale's 1972
168 The canadian Journal of Psychiatry Vol 43, No 2

assessment ofthe biographies of42 English and French poets (42), and none used clearly reproducible measures ofcreativ-
suggested that 15% were "psychotic" and half had "some" ity.
psychopathology (39).
Mental Illness in Creative People: Case-Control Studies
Lucas and Stringer's 1972 study of 84 architecture stu-
Four recent case-control studies assessed creative people
dents in London found only 1 was "severely disturbed" (40).
using standardized or reproducible definitions of mental ill-
Finally, in 1979, Schou described the impact of lithium pro-
ness. First, in 1974, Andreasen and Canter assessed 15 Ameri-
phylaxis in 24 artists with bipolar mood disorder (41). Artistic
cans attending a writers' workshop, compared with 15
productivity was increased in 12, unchanged in 6, and reduced
"non-creative" controls, who were matched for socioeco-
in 6.
nomic status, age, and gender (48). They found evidence of
These 14 early studies were often cited later as demonstrat- lifetime (DSM) psychiatric diagnoses on unblinded inter-
ing high rates ofmental disorders in creative or gifted people. views, mostly mood disorders and alcoholism, in signifi-
In 11 of these studies, however, either high rates of mental cantly more writers than controls (73% versus 20%).
disorders were not clearly found (28-32,34-37,40), or crea- Using object sorting tests, Andreasen and Powers then
tive people did better when mental illness was treated (41).
compared the 15 writers with 32 patients who were hospital-
Methodologically, these early studies shared several flaws:
ized with either mania or schizophrenia (49). Writers were
none used standardized or reproducible definitions of mental
more like manic patients than schizophrenic patients, al-
illness or creativity, none used randomized selection or
though differences were not significant.
blinded assessments, and most did not use comparison groups
or prospective designs. In 1984 Andreasen expanded her sample to 30 writers who
were compared, using unblinded interviews, with 30 "non-
Mental Illness in Creative People: Case Series creative" controls matched for age, gender, socioeconomic
Six recent case series assessed creative or eminent people status, and intelligence (50). Writers had significantly higher
using standardized or clearly reproducible measures of men- (DSM) lifetime prevalence rates than controls for all mood
tal illness. Jamison described 47 British writers and artists in disorders (80% versus 30%), for bipolar disorder (43% versus
1989: 38% had received treatment for mood disorders at some 10%), and for alcoholism (30% versus 7%).
time in their lives (42). Ludwig, in 1990, surveyed biogra- Finally, Ludwig assessed 59 writers and 59 matched con-
phies of 34 American writers, artists, and musicians and trols using a "lifetime scale" to measure creativity in "every-
found that alcohol abuse impaired creativity in 75% (43). day" accomplishments (51). Writers had a 56% lifetime
Ludwig later examined the biographies of 1005 eminent prevalence ofdepression (versus 14% in controls) and higher
people in academic, business, artistic, social, activist, mili- creativity scores.
tary, and athletic fields, looking for evidence ofeither psychi- Three of these studies found significantly higher rates of
atric difficulty or creativity (44). Writers, actors, artists, and mental disorders in creative subjects compared with controls
musicians had the most psychiatric difficulty, while public (48,50,51), and 1 study had unclear findings (49). These
officials, scientists, and military and business personnel had studies, however, shared several methodological flaws: sub-
the least. Most creativity was found among musicians, scien- jects and controls were not identified randomly or assessed
tists, and artists. blindly, all used retrospective data, and creativity was actu-
In 1994 Schildkraut's group assessed 15 American artists' ally measured in only 1 of the studies (51). One study ap-
biographies: 50% had suffered either depression or cy- peared to be a preliminary study (48) that was later
clothymia, 33% had abused alcohol, and 40% had received incorporated into a larger study (50).
psychiatric treatment at some time in their lives (45). Creativity in Gifted and Mentally III People
Also in 1994, Post assessed 291 biographies of eminent One recent case-control study assessed creativity in both
males in music, art, writing, science, and political leadership gifted people and psychiatric patients. In 1983 Rothenberg
(46). He found high rates of psychiatric disorders in writers used timed word-association tests to measure creativity in 12
and artists. In a later, expanded assessment of writers' biog- Nobel laureates compared with 18 patients, who were hospi-
raphies, Post found high rates ofboth depression and alcohol- talized with various psychiatric (DSM) diagnoses (52). He
ism (47). then compared both groups with 113 "high" and "low" crea-
tivity university students, who were sorted according to re-
Four of these studies found higher than expected rates of
cords of achievement in the arts and sciences.
mental disorders in creative or eminent people (42,45-47),
1 found that mental disorder hindered creativity (43), and 1 Creativity scores were highest in Nobel laureates, fol-
had unclear findings (44). Methodologically, these studies lowed in descending order by "high" and "low" creativity
shared several flaws: none used control or comparison students, then psychiatric patients. Differences among the
groups, none identified or assessed subjects randomly or groups were statistically significant, suggesting to the author
blindly, all were retrospective, only I used living subjects that mental illness was not associated with creativity, al-
March 1998 Creativity and Mental Illness 169

though mental health was not actually assessed in the Nobel Discussion
and university groups.
In summary, of 29 studies, 15 negated (28-32,
Creativity in People With Mental Illness: Case Series 34-37,40,41,43,52,55,56), and 9 supported (33,38,39,42,
Two recent case series assessed creative accomplishments 45-47,50,51) an association between mental illness and crea-
in people with mental illness. In 1983 Karlsson surveyed tivity. Five studies suggested that creativity was hindered by
171 000 Icelanders' records and found higher than expected severe mental illness (41,43,52,54,55), 4 studies had unclear
educational and creative achievement among people who had findings (44,49,53,54), and 1study (48) was incorporated into
been hospitalized for psychosis but not among people who a later study (50).
had been hospitalized for alcoholism (53). In 1988 Akiskal Methodological problems with the studies on the pur-
and Akiskal reported on 750 psychiatric patients: artistic ported association between creativity and mental illness will
creativity was more frequent with mild, bipolar mood disor- be discussed. A contradiction will be described between the
ders but less frequent with severe disorders (54). Both studies limited scientific evidence and the resounding enthusiasm for
were retrospective, and neither used comparison groups or linking creativity and mental illness. Possible explanations
randomized or blinded assessments. for this contradiction will be ventured, and social and research
implications will be reviewed.
Creativity in People With Mental Illness: Case-Control
Studies Methodological Problems
Two recent case-control studies assessed creativity in peo- All studies used case-series or case-control designs. Four-
ple with mental disorders using standardized definitions of teen early studies did not use standardized measures ofeither
mental illness. In 1988 Richards' group hypothesized that mental illness or creativity. While 15 recent studies used
mild mood disorders like cyclothymia might confer greater standardized measures ofmental illness, only 4 (51,52,55,56)
creativity than either good health or severe disorders such as actually measured creativity, none selected subjects ran-
manic-depression (55). They compared 33 manic-depressive domly, only 3 used prospective designs (52,55,56), and only
and cyclothymic patients with 11 healthy relatives of the 2 rated subjects blindly (55,56). Six did not use living subjects
patients and 33 controls who were either well or had an (44-47,53,54).
unrelated psychiatric diagnosis. They used a "lifetime scale"
to measure peak originality in various endeavours. Mild The quality of research evidence in the health sciences,
mood disorders were associated with higher creativity, including psychiatry, is commonly ranked in order from
whereas severe illness was associated with lowest creativity, strongest to weakest: randomized clinical trials, prospective
although differences between groups were not statistically cohort studies, case-control studies, case series, and expert
significant. opinion (79).

In 1993 Funk's group hypothesized that impulsivity might The 29 empirical studies of creativity and mental illness
confer greater creativity (56). They assessed 19 boys with used weaker (case-control or case series) designs. There were
attention deficit with hyperactivity disorder compared with 34 reviews, yet these comprise the weakest form of evidence
21 well controls matched for age, socioeconomic status, and (expert opinion), and none met current critical standards for
intelligence. Creativity, assessed using drawing completion literature reviews (80). Many authors apparently suggested
tests, was not significantly higher in subjects (on or off that mental illness and creativity were positively associated
medication). based on finding higher than expected rates of mental illness
in selected populations in whom creativity was usually not
Neither study supported a strong association between (and mental illness was not always) measured.
mental disorder and creativity. Both studies used cross-sec-
tional designs with matched controls, blinded raters, and clear Since many authors implied that creativity and mental
measures of both creativity and mental illness. illness were causally associated (2,4,5,9,19,20,22,45,
47,50,67,68,70), the following epidemiological criteria for
Reviews determining causation apply (79). Was there evidence from
experimental studies in humans? Was the association strong
In addition to empirical studies, 34 review and discussion
and consistent among studies? Were the temporal relation-
papers (including books, letters, and case studies) examined
ships correct? Was there a dose-response gradient? Did the
purported links between creativity and mental illness
association make epidemiological and biological sense? Was
(1,2,4-9,19-22,57-78). Most were selective reviews that
the association specific and analogous to a previously proven
mixed scientific evidence with journalistic or literary evi-
causal association? In the studies on creativity and mental
dence. Only 11 reviews incorporated methodological criti-
illness, these criteria were not met.
cisms (1,2,4-6,9,19,59,62,64,69). Most authors
enthusiastically promoted an association between creativity There were other methodological problems with the litera-
and mental illness, and many implied that the association was ture on creativity and mental illness. Early studies did not use
causal. There were no metaanalyses. standardized definitions of mental illness, which made com-
170 The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry Vol 43, No 2

pari sons with current population data difficult. Most early Lowry, a writer, described alcoholism: "the agonies ofthe
studies also did not measure creativity but simply included drunkard find their most accurate poetic analogue in the
subjects based on eminence or giftedness. agonies ofthe mystic who has abused his powers" (82, p 23).
Recent studies did use standardized criteria to assess men- Millett, a feminist, wrote: "During depression the world
tal disorders, but they did not always define or measure disappears. Language itself. One has nothing to say. Nothing"
creativity clearly, thus making it difficult to compare studies. (25, p 283). Vedder, a rock singer, said: "Ifit hadn't been for
Since most studies used unblinded assessments of retrospec- music, I would have shot myself in front of that classroom"
tive data, selection biases were likely significant. No studies (83, p 93).
assessed creativity in community samples for comparison.
Mays, an art critic, said that having depression was like
Few studies adequately assessed confounding variables, such
living in "a clearing bounded by thickets roamed by the
as socioeconomic status or intelligence.
killing black dogs" (26, p xi). Jamison, a psychiatric re-
Arguably, quantitative scientific standards may be too searcher, described manic-depression: "So why would I want
reductionistic to apply to studies on creativity and mental anything to do with this illness? Because I honestly believe
illness. No one, however, applied rigorous qualitative meth- that as a result of it I have felt more things, more deeply; had
ods to complement the quantitative ones. Many authors sug- more experiences, more intensely; loved more and been more
gested that creativity and mental illness were associated, but loved" (27, p 218).
most did not discuss possible neurobiological or social
Clearly, some mental disorders, especially milder ones,
mechanisms to explain this purported association.
may enhance creativity in some individuals; for instance,
Implications for Future Research hypomania may be enjoyable and may enhance creativity
more than depression or mania (55,84). This, however, does
While definitive studies were lacking, some methodologi- not prove a general association between creativity and mental
cal improvements would help clarify whether creativity and illness.
mental illness are positively associated.
In another explanation for the persisting popular links
Larger prospective studies involving randomly selected between creativity and mental illness, Becker commented
living subjects, matched controls, and raters blinded to histo- that the need to romantically link genius and madness may be
ries of illness or creativity would reduce the recall biases rooted in society's need to regard both as "deviant" (1).
inherent in retrospective or biographical studies. Creativity, Kessel added, "Ifgeniuses disturb society, ifthey misfit, then
as well as mental illness, should be defined and measured this is another characteristic they share with the mentally ill.
using standardized instruments such as Richards' scale (55). Yet it is a dangerous conjunction to make because the reasons
Knowledge about creativity and mental illness in repre- lying behind the two types of disturbance are so different"
sentative community samples is needed for comparison. Dif- (69, p 203).
ferent types ofcreativity (scientific, artistic, business) should
be clarified. While randomized prospective studies would be Sontag noted that nonrational metaphors can also domi-
difficult to mount, even naturalistic, prospective studies nate perceptions of illness: "The romantic view is that illness
would be an improvement on the existing work. exacerbates consciousness. Once that illness was TB; now it
is insanity" (85, p 36). Sontag explained this phenomenon as
Evidence from several different studies, all using random- a "sublimated spiritualism: a secular, ostensibly scientific
ized and prospective methodologies and showing a strong way of affirming the primacy of 'spirit' over matter" (85, p
association between creativity and mental illness, is needed 55-56). Perhaps both mental illness and creativity have be-
before a causal relationship can be suggested. come metaphors for nonrational or spiritual needs that are
sublimated in our rational, scientific age.
Contradictions in the Literature
Kuhn commented that scientists were often slow to accept
Enthusiasm for associating creativity and mental illness
their own rational evidence when it contradicted popular
exceeds the scientific evidence and persists despite evidence
theories (86). He suggested that scientific "revolutions" arose
to the contrary. What explains this contradiction? One possi-
when evidence accumulated and finally overwhelmed popu-
bility is that the eloquence of creative or gifted people who
lar theory, a nonrational phenomenon essential to scientific
comment on their own mental health problems attracts atten-
change. The contradiction between the enthusiasm and the
tion and compels us to spuriously connect their creativity with
lack ofevidence for linking creativity and mental illness may
their problems.
epitomize Kuhn's thesis.
For instance, James, a philosopher and psychologist, wrote
about depression: "So much for melancholy in the sense of Conclusions
incapacity for joyous feeling. A much worse form of it is
positive and active anguish, a sort of psychical neuralgia This review examined the existing scientific evidence for
wholly unknown to healthy life" (81, p 129). associating creativity and mental illness. The evidence was
March 1998 Creativity and Mental Ulness 171

unconvincing based on a critical review of 29 empirical References


studies: most did not support an association between creativ-
I. Becker G. The mad genius controversy. London: Sage Publications; 1978.
ity and mental illness, and most had methodological flaws. 2. Richards R. Relationships between creativity and psychopathology: an evaluation
and interpretation of the evidence. Genetic Psychology Monographs
Despite this limited evidence, many authors promoted a 1981; I03:261-324.
positive association in 34 review articles on creativity and 3. Storr A. The dynamics of creation. New York: Atheneum;J985.
mental illness. The enthusiasm for associating creativity and 4. Andreasen N, Glick I. Bipolar affective disorder and creativity: implications and
clinical management. Compr Psychiatry 1988;29:207-17.
mental illness was not supported by the evidence. 5. Jamison K. Manic-depressive illness, creativity and leadership. In: Goodwin F,
Jamison K, editors. Manic-depressive illness. New York: Oxford University
Is there any harm in linking creativity and mental illness Press; 1990. p 332-67.
despite limited scientific evidence? Eardley commented: 6. Rothenberg A. Creativity and madness. Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins University
Press; 1990.
"Society treats artists . . . poorly enough to dissolve the 7. Ludwig A. Reflections on creativity and madness. Am J Psych other
hardiest among humans ... to put up with all this and still get 1989;43:4-14.
by, the artist may actually be in some way more stable, 8. Ludwig A. Culture and creativity. Am J Psychother 1992;46:454-69.
9. Ludwig A. The price of greatness. New York: The Guilford Press; 1995.
mentally or emotionally, than the norm" (87, p A15). 10. Weisberg R. Creativity: beyond the myth of genius. New York: Freeman and
Company; 1993.
We may do more harm than good to the cause ofcreativity 11. Foucault M. Madness and civilization. Toronto: Random House; 1965.
ifwe inadvertently convey the idea that creativity and mental 12. Chesler P. Women and madness. New York: Avon Books; 1973.
13. Penfold S, Walker G. Women and the psychiatric paradox. Montreal: Eden Press;
illness are both forms of deviance. We may do more harm 1983.
than good to the cause of alleviating mental illness if we 14. Szasz T. The myth of mental illness. Revised edition. New York: Harper and Row;
romanticize mental illness and trivialize its impact by associ- 1974.
15. Sass L. Madness and modernism. Boston: Harvard University Press; 1992.
ating it with creativity. We may do more harm than good if 16. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
we fail to note that mental illness impedes creativity or if disorders (DSM-III). 3rd ed. Washington (DC): American Psychiatric Press; 1980.
promoting an association between creativity and mental ill- 17. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders (DSM-III-R). 3rd ed. Revised. Washington (DC): American Psychiatric
ness takes precedence over either encouraging creativity or Press; 1987.
reducing mental illness. 18. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders (DSM-IV). 4th ed. Washington (DC): American Psychiatric Press; 1994.
Despite the gap between enthusiasm and solid evidence, 19. Jamison K. Touched with fire. Toronto: Maxwell MacMillan Canada; 1993.
20. Jamison K. Manic-depressive illness and creativity. Sci Am 1995;Feb:62-7.
debates about creativity and mental illness have merit. Much 21. Charbonneau L. Some calm for an unquiet mind. The Medical Post 1995 Nov
debate appears motivated by a desire to destigmatize mental 7:65.
illness. The debates may also ultimately encourage more 22. Gutin 1. Mental illness: going over the creative edge. The Globe and Mail 1996
Dec 7;Sect 0:6.
research on preventing and treating mental illnesses and on 23. Campbell M. Not always on the level. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press;
supporting creativity. 1988.
24. Styron W. Darkness visible: a memoir of madness. New York: Vintage Books;
Art provides a "path to the sacred and spiritual, even in a 1990.
profane and fragmented world" (45, p 486). We need to 25. Millet K. The loony-bin trip. New York: Simon and Schuster; 1990.
26. Mays J. In the jaws of the black dogs: a memoir of depression. Toronto: Viking
encourage creativity and creative people in our midst. We Press; 1995.
also need to find more effective ways to prevent, treat, and 27. Jamison K. An unquiet mind. New York: Alfred A. Knopf; 1995.
28. Terman L, Cox C, Oden M, Burks B, Jensen D. Genetic studies ofgenius (Volumes
destigmatize mental illnesses ofall kinds. We need both good 1-5). Stanford: Stanford University Press; 1925-1959.
art and good science. Science and art, creativity and mental 29. Ellis H. A study of British genius. New York: Houghton Miftlin; 1926.
illness may all be trivialized, however, and we may do more 30. Nicolson H. The health of authors. Lancet 1947;2:709-14.
31. Juda A. The relationship between highest mental capacity and psychic abnormali-
harm than good to those "afflicted" with either creativity or ties. Am J Psychiatry 1949;106:296-307.
mental illness ifwe promote romantic associations that over- 32. Drevdahl J, Cattell R. Personality and creativity in artists and writers. J Clin
PsycholI958;14:107-12.
look our own best evidence. 33. Goertzel V, Goertzel M. Cradles of eminence. Boston: Little, Brown; 1962.
34. Torrance E. Rewarding creative behavior: experiments in classroom creativity.
New Jersey: Prentice-Hall; 1965.
35. MacKinnon D. Personality and the realization of creative potential. Am Psychol
Clinical Implication 1965;20:273-81.
36. Noreik K, Odegard O. Psychoses in Norwegians with a background of higher
• There is little evidence to link creativity and mental illness. education. Br J Psychiatry 1966;112:34-55.
37. Barron F. Creative writers. In: Albert R, editor. Genius and eminence. 2nd ed.
Limitation New York: Pergamon Press; 1992. p 315-24.
38. McNeil T. Prebirth and postbirth influence on the relationship between creative
• This was a single-author review of the literature. Definitive ability and recorded mental illness. J Pers 1971;39:391-406.
studies are lacking. 39. Martindale C. Father's absence, psychopathology, and poetic eminence. Psychol
Rep 1972;31:843-7.
40. Lucas C, Stringer P. Interaction in university selection, mental health and aca-
demic performance. Br J Psychiatry 1972;120:189-95.
41. Schou M. Artistic productivity and lithium prophylaxis in manic-depressive
Acknowledgements illness. BrJ Psychiatry 1979;135:97-103.
42. Jamison K. Mood disorders and patterns ofcreativity in British writers and artists.
The author thanks Russell Joffe, MD, George McLauchlin, MSc, Psychiatry 1989;52:125-34.
and Dan Offord, MD for their comments on previous drafts of this 43. Ludwig A. Alcohol input and creative output. British Journal of Addiction
manuscript. 1990;85:953-63.
172 The canadian Journal of Psychiatry \/.,! .£3, No 2

44. Ludwig A. Creative achievement and psychopathology: comparison among pro- 64. Prentky R. Creativity and psychopathology. New York: Praeger; 1980.
fessions. Am J Psychother 1992;46:330-56. 65. DeLong G, Aldershof A. Association of special abilities with juvenile manic-de-
45. SchildkrautJ, Hirshfeld A, Murphy J. Mind and mood in modern art, II: depressive pressive illness. In: Programs and Abstracts. Twelfth Annual Meeting ofthe Child
disorders, spirituality, and early deaths in the abstract expressionist artists of the Neurology Society; 1983 Oct 13-15; Williamsburg, VA. Ann Neurol
New York school. Am J Psychiatry 1994;151:482-8. 1983;14:362.
46. Post F. Creativity and psychopathology: a study of291 world-famous men. Br J 66. Holden C. Manic depression and creativity. Science 1986;233:725.
Psychiatry 1994;165:22-34. 67. Kinney D, Richards R. Creativity and manic depressive illness. Science
47. Post F. Verbal creativity, depression and alcoholism. Br J Psychiatry 1986;234:528.
1996;168:545-55. 68. Hare E. Creativity and mental illness. BMJ 1987;295:1587-9.
48. Andreasen N, Canter A. The creative writer: psychiatric symptoms and family 69. Kessel N. Genius and mental disorder: a history of ideas concerning their
history. ComprPsychiatry 1974;15:123-31. conjunction. In: Murray P, editor. Genius: the history of an idea. New York: Basil
49. Andreasen N, Powers P. Creativity and psychosis: an examination of conceptual Blackwell; 1989. p 196--212.
style. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1975;32:70-3. 70. Richards R, Kinney D. Correspondence. Compr Psychiatry 1989;30:272-3.
50. Andreasen N. Creativity and mental illness: prevalence rates in writers and their
71. Miller A. The untouched key. London: Virago Press; 1990.
first-degree relatives. Am J Psychiatry 1987;144:1288-92.
72. Albert R. Genius and eminence. 2nd ed. New York: Pergamon Press; 1992.
51. Ludwig A. Mental illness and creative activity in female writers. Am J Psychiatry
1994;151:1650-6. 73. Goodwin D. Alcohol as muse. Am J Psychother 1992;46:422-33.
52. Rothenberg A. Psychopathology and creative cognition. Arch Gen Psychiatry 74. Jamison K, Wyatt R. Vincent van Gogh's illness. BMJ 1992;304:577.
1983;40:937--42. 75. Kinney D. The therapist as muse: greater roles for clinicians in fostering innova-
53. Karlsson J. Academic achievement of psychotic and alcoholic patients. Hereditas tion. Am J Psychother 1992;46:434-53.
1983;99:69-72. 76. Sacks O. Tourette's syndrome and creativity. BMJ 1992;305:1515-6.
54. Akiskal H, Akiskal K. Reassessing the prevalence of bipolar disorders: clinical 77. Simkin B. Mozart's scatological disorder. BMJ 1992;305:1563-7.
significance and artistic creativity. Psychiatry and Psychobiology 78. Whybrow P. Ofthe muse and moods mundane. Am J Psychiatry 1994;151:477-9.
I988;3(Suppl):29S-36S. 79. Sackett D, Haynes B, Tugwell P. Clinical epidemiology: a basic science for
55. Richards R, Kinney D, Lunde I, Benet M, Merzel A. Creativity in manic-depres- clinical medicine. Toronto: Little, Brown and Company; 1985.
sives, cyclothymes, their normal relatives, and control subjects. J Abnorm Psychol 80. Oxman A, Guyatt G. Guidelines for reading literature reviews. Canadian Medical
1988;97:281-8. Association Journal 1988;138:697-703.
56. Funk J, Chessare J, Weaver M, Exley A. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder,
81. James W. The varieties of religious experience. New York: Collier Books; 1961.
creativity, and the effects of methylphenidate. Pediatrics 1993;91:816--9.
82. Lowry M. Under the volcano. Markham: Penguin Books Canada; 1985.
57. Roe A. Personal problems and science. In: Taylor C, Barron F, editors. Scientific
creativity: its recognition and development. New York: Wiley; 1963. p 132-8. 83. [Anonymous]. Eddie Vedder. People Magazine 1993 Dec 27:93.
58. Grant V. Great abnormals. New York: Hawthorn Books; 1968. 84. Jamison K, Gerner R, Hammen C, Padesky C. Clouds and silver linings: positive
59. Slater E. The problems of pathography. Acta Psychiatr Scand experiences associated with primary affective disorders. Am J Psychiatry
1970; (219 Suppl):2095-215S. 1980;137:198-202.
60. Andreasen N. James Joyce: a portrait of the artist as a schizoid. JAMA 85. Sontag S. Illness as metaphor and AIDS and its metaphors. Toronto: Doubleday;
1973;224:67-71. 1990.
61. Goodwin D. The muse and the martini. JAMA 1973;224:35-8. 86. Kuhn T. The structure of scientific revolutions. 2nd ed. Chicago: University of
62. Trethowan W. Music and mental disorder. In: Critchley M, Henson R, editors. Chicago Press; 1970.
Music and the brain. Illinois: Charles Thomas; 1977. p 398--432. 87. Eardley C. Artists are tougher than other people. The New York Times 1994May
63. Andreasen N. Creativity and psychiatric illness. Psychiatric Annals 1978;8:I 13-9. 6;Sect A:15.

Resume

Objectif: Evaluation critique des preuves scientijiques de I'association entre la creativite et la maladie mentale.
Methode: Des recherches menees dans MEDLINE et la litterature secondaire ont permis de reperer 29 etudes et
34 articles de synthese portant sur la creativite et la maladie mentale. Toutes les etudes ont fait l'objet d'une
evaluation critique, et on a aussi evalue les articles de synthese.
Resultats : Parmi les 29 etudes consacrees aI'evaluation des associationspossibles entre la creativite et la maladie
mentale, 15 etudes n 'ont permis de decouvrir aucune preuve du lien entre ces deux phenomenes, 9 etudes ont
donne des preuves concluantes et 5 se sont soldees par des resultats incertains. La plupart des etudes reposaient
sur des methodologies erronees et des plans (series de cas ou cas-temoins) superficiels. Aucune etude randomisee
ou prospective des cohortes n 'a ete menee. Les criteres appropries visant adeterminer I'association causale n 'ont
pas ete respectes. Dans 34 articles de synthese selectifs, malgre des preuves douteuses, de nombreux auteurs ont
pretendu qu 'il existe une association positive ou causale entre la creativite et la maladie mentale.
Conclusions: Des preuves scientijiques restreintes permettent d'associer la creativite a la maladie mentale.
Malgre cela, de nombreux auteurs ont soutenu I'existence d 'un lien entre ces deux phenomenes. Des explications
de cette contradiction font I 'objet d'un examen, et on discute des repercussions de celles-ci sur la societe et les
chercheurs.

You might also like