You are on page 1of 13

Neuropsychologia 46 (2008) 2993–3005

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Neuropsychologia
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neuropsychologia

A case of persistent retrograde amnesia following a dissociative fugue:


Neuropsychological and neurofunctional underpinnings of loss
of autobiographical memory and self-awareness
Kristina Hennig-Fast a,∗ , Franziska Meister a , Thomas Frodl a , Anna Beraldi a , Frank Padberg a ,
Rolf R. Engel a , Maximilian Reiser b , Hans-Jürgen Möller a , Thomas Meindl b
a
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Ludwig-Maximilians-University, Nussbaumstr. 7, D-80336 Munich, Germany
b
Department for Clinical Radiology, Ludwig-Maximilians-University, Ziemssenstrasse 1, D-80336 Munich, Germany

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Autobiographical memory relies on complex interactions between episodic memory contents, associated
Received 13 September 2007 emotions and a sense of self-continuity over the course of one’s life. This paper reports a study based
Received in revised form 9 June 2008 upon the case of the patient NN who suffered from a complete loss of autobiographical memory and
Accepted 10 June 2008
awareness of identity subsequent to a dissociative fugue. Neuropsychological, behavioral, and functional
Available online 21 June 2008
neuroimaging tests converged on the conclusion that NN suffered from a selective retrograde amnesia
following an episode of dissociative fugue, during which he had lost explicit knowledge and vivid memory
Keywords:
of his personal past. NN’s loss of self-related memories was mirrored in neurobiological changes after the
Retrograde amnesia
Functional amnesia
fugue whereas his semantic memory remained intact. Although NN still claimed to suffer from a stable
Dissociative fugue loss of autobiographical, self-relevant memories 1 year after the fugue state, a proportionate improve-
Autobiographical memory ment in underlying fronto-temporal neuronal networks was evident at this point in time. In spite of this
fMRI improvement in neuronal activation, his anterograde visual memory had been decreased. It is posited
that our data provide evidence for the important role of visual processing in autobiographical memory as
well as for the efficiency of protective control mechanisms that constitute functional retrograde amnesia.
© 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction tions. According to this theory, voluntary and involuntary forms


of inhibition push unwanted memories out of awareness and
The ability to vividly re-experience autobiographical memo- reduce capacity for retrieval (see also MacLeod & Macrae, 2001).
ries is thought to be unique to humans, reflecting an advanced There is a trend towards discussing the inhibition of memory
state of consciousness that mediates awareness (Tulving, 1989). retrieval as being related to functional changes in neurobiolog-
Autobiographical memory relies on complex interactions between ical mechanisms which are often associated with alterations in
different memory systems as well as on interactions with associ- frontal, temporal and parietal brain activity (e.g. De Renzi &
ated emotions and on a sense of self-continuity across the course Lucchelli, 1993; Kritchevsky, Chang, & Squire, 2004; Nakamura et
of one’s life. It is characterized by emotional salience, indicat- al., 2002; Papagno, 1998; Sellal, Manning, Seegmuller, Scheiber, &
ing the modulating role of emotions for storage and retrieval. Schoenfelder, 2002; Starkstein, Sabe, & Dorego, 1997; Stracciari,
The loss of autobiographical memory may occur during retro- Ghidoni, Guarino, Poletti, & Pazzaglia, 1994). Evidence from studies
grade amnesia (RA) following traumatic or stressful experiences. of patients with organic amnesia suggests that visuo-spatial pro-
For example, in dissociative disorders, it can occur during or sub- cessing is crucial to autobiographical recall (e.g. Greenberg, Eacott,
sequent to a dissociative fugue (Glisky, Ryan, Reminger, & Hardt, Brechin, & Rubin, 2005). But, up until now there has been no evi-
2004; Markowitsch, Calabrese, et al., 1997a; Markowitsch, Fink, dence for the impact of visuo-spatial processing in psychogenic
Thöne, Kessler, & Heiss, 1997b). According to Kopelman (2000a,b) or functional RA. However, since memories that have the quality
RA is the result of changes in the activation of executive func- of being both threatening and painful, and vivid could endan-
ger the stability of an intact self-concept, they may be removed
from consciousness. This process of removal could help to main-
tain amnesia. It can be seen as a resource-consuming condition
∗ Corresponding author. Tel.: +49 89 5160 5565; fax: +49 89 5160 5562. requiring extensive executive and attentional control that mod-
E-mail address: kristina.fast@med.uni-muenchen.de (K. Hennig-Fast). ulates visual memory and imagery as well as related memory

0028-3932/$ – see front matter © 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.06.014
2994 K. Hennig-Fast et al. / Neuropsychologia 46 (2008) 2993–3005

qualities, e.g. the olfactory or acoustic pattern of a recollected by the emergency neurologist at the outpatient clinic of the Munich
episode. From a theoretical standpoint, gaining access to memories University Hospital and was later admitted to the psychiatric ward
includes the (re-)activation of distributed, stored memory traces, of the hospital. When first asked about memories from around the
as well as strategic operations sustaining the generation process time of fugue episode, NN reported only some vague memories of
during retrieval (Conway & Pleydell-Pearce, 2000; Moscovitch et feet walking as well as some dark silhouettes of unknown faces.
al., 2005; Rubin, 2005). Because autobiographical memories are He described those visual impressions as constricted, as though
episodic memories for contextually rich events (Tulving, 1983), he was in a tunnel looking out. One month post-onset, in the sec-
varying in temporal and spatial context, emotions, visual imagery ond examination he regained memories of several episodes and
and relation to the self, the retrieval of those memories is associ- fragments from the time of fugue. He claimed, however, to have
ated with processing in a distributed neural network (e.g. Daselaar, none of his own memories of his personal past before the DF,
Rice, Greenberg, LaBar, & Rubin, 2008; Svoboda, McKinnon, & e.g. of his childhood, family, school days, and career. He reported
Levine, 2006). Apart from the prefrontal Cortex (PFC), the network only vague feelings of familiarity towards things, e.g. his cloth-
underlying autobiographical retrieval includes the medial temporal ing. In contrast to Schacter’s finding (Schacter, Wang, Tulving, &
lobe (MTL), the visual cortex, the posterior medial parietal cor- Freedman, 1982) of some scattered happy memories in his patient,
tex and areas involved in sensory and emotional processing (for NN regained only three remote and strongly negative childhood
a review see Daselaar et al., 2008). Damasio (1989) has created a memories dating from the time before the dissociative fugue sur-
model that attempts to explain retrograde memory loss in RA in faced over the coming months. All were highly negative, vivid and
cognitive neuropsychological terms. According to this model, the fragmented episodes comparable to frozen images, e.g. of the coffin
retrieval of episodic personal memories presupposes recalling at at his grandfather’s funeral.
progressively richer levels, with the evocation of greater contextual NN’s memory for implicit skills was still intact. As was his mem-
complexity. ory for public events. Interestingly, he had no awareness of his
Besides documenting a follow-up case of RA after a dissociatve skills, e.g. with respect to using a computer, playing a guitar and
fugue (DF), the present study attempts to answer the following most remarkably writing his signature. However, when asked about
questions: How is retrograde memory loss for autobiographical public events over the last 30 years he was able to answer most
episodes and facts related to other cognitive functions? In particu- of the questions and he was fully aware of the knowledge he still
lar, how is autobiographical RA related to performance in visual had. There was some concern that NN might be feigning his amne-
memory, attention and executive function? Is there a quantita- sia for reasons unknown to us. To control for this possibility, we
tive and qualitative change in autobiographical memory over the interviewed his family and the hospital’s medical staff for possible
course of a year that can be related to a concurrent change in cog- inconsistencies in daily behavior. But neither his cognitive profile
nitive functions? Can the RA be detected as neurofunctional brain nor the observed behavior provided any evidence for feigning the
activation during functional brain imaging while performing an amnesia.
autobiographical memory task? Does brain activation differ from
the typical activation pattern known to be associated with suc- 2.1. Neurological status and brain pathology
cessful retrieval of autobiographical information (i.e. Daselaar et al.,
2008)? Are specific prefrontal activation patterns related to exec- Detailed neurological examinations revealed that NN’s neuro-
utive and attentional functions? Are temporo–parieto–occipital logical preamnestic status had been retained. Electroencephalog-
patterns related to visual imagery or vivid memory? raphy (EEG) and electrocardiogram (ECG) examinations displayed
We predicted that a person lacking autobiographical memory no pathological changes. Only an axonal sensomotoric neuropa-
would perform comparably on retrieval of real autobiographi- thy with an increased pain perception that was already diagnosed
cal and retrieval of fictitious autobiographical events. We also in 1993 was observed. All other somatosensory and motor func-
predicted that they would show better recall of semantically tions were normal. Organic (i.e. neurological, general medical) or
related public events than autobiographical events. Addition- substance-related causes of RA were excluded from considera-
ally, we hypothesized that this amnestic person would display tion.
increased activation in the brain areas relevant for controlling NN underwent conventional magnetic resonance imaging
and monitoring of memories (e.g. prefrontal cortex, anterior cin- (cMRI) and computed tomography (cCT). Cross-sectional imaging
gulated, insula). We also predicted that such a person would gave no evidence for brain abnormalities. Cerebral structures were
display a decreased activation in areas for visual information completely normal for his age. Resting 18 F-Fluor-Desoxyglukose
processing. (FDG)-positron emission tomography (PET) scans showed a
decreased tracer uptake of 8–15% (Fig. 1).

2. Case history 2.2. Psychiatric status

To our knowledge this is only the second follow-up study of a The patient was alcohol-dependent for about 10 years (his alco-
patient with selective functional retrograde amnesia where func- hol consumption varied over the years with a maximum of 4 l of
tional brain imaging was used to examine the change of cognitive beer per day). He underwent addiction treatment that enabled him
patterns and their underlying networks (first study: Repetto et to successfully abstain from alcohol. This took place 10 years before
al., 2007). However, this is the first follow-up study in a patient the onset of the dissociative fugue. He attempted suicide three
with preceding DF. The patient was assessed twice, once in an times: twice by cutting his wrists; and a third time by starvation
acute state and 1 year post-onset. During the 1-year interval after his ex-wife left him. During his treatment, his psychothera-
between both assessments he suffered from adjustment problems pists diagnosed an anxious-depressive and narcissistic personality
and was treated with both preventative antidepressive medication structure leading to emotional instability.
and cognitive-behavioural therapy. By the time NN was admitted to the hospital he was disori-
Patient NN was a 39-year-old right-handed male, who strayed ented with respect to time, location, and his personal identity.
through streets and lanes of Southern Germany and Austria in a Neither a loss of consciousness nor a head injury were reported
state of DF that persisted approximately 2 weeks. He was first seen or present at the time of assessment. His formal thinking was nor-
K. Hennig-Fast et al. / Neuropsychologia 46 (2008) 2993–3005 2995

Fig. 1. Decreased tracer uptake of 8–15% in the right temporo-mesial brain region of NN. Levels of significance were p < 0.001 and p < 0.05, respectively. The percent values
reflect the relatively reduced metabolism throughout the right temporo-mesial region when compared to the expected values obtained from a healthy standardization
sample. Multiple statistical comparisons were used.

mal but his mood was depressed. His affect was flattened and he 3.1. Results
complained about pain in his legs, a feeling of agitation and amph-
icrania. But neither a clinical depression nor an anxiety disorder Using the BDI-II, a 21-question multiple-choice self-report
could be diagnosed by means of the Structured Clinical Interview inventory, NN was identified as having only minimal symptoms of
for DSM-IV (SCID; Wittchen, Wunderlich, Gruschwitz, & Zaudig, depression during the acute phase, which lasted around 2 weeks
1996). An accessory amnestic syndrome (ICD-10, F 44.0; ICD-10, (raw value of 13). His symptoms were significantly reduced a
World Health Organisation, 1992) was diagnosed irrespective of few months afterwards and 1 year following onset (raw value
the prior dissociative fugue (ICD-10, F 44.1). of 5). NN’s completed the FDS, a self-report measure that asks
An organic amnestic syndrome (ICD-10, F 04) caused by pro- subjects to indicate the frequency of different dissociative expe-
longed alcohol abuse was considered but was ruled out for the riences by considering the key aspects of the dissociation construct
following reasons. Firstly, although NN was alcohol dependent (autobiographical amnesia, depersonalization, absorption, identity
for about one decade of his life, he had been alcohol-abstinent alterations). This resulted in a profile with scores (uniformly) highly
for more than 10 years since then. Additionally there was no above average and significantly higher than the mean scores of
evidence for cerebral atrophy in imaging data. Furthermore, NN patients suffering from dissociative or alcoholic disorders (NN’s
did not show any symptoms of addiction, craving or withdrawal scores: amnesia score = 38.75, absorption score = 44.44, derealisa-
after admission. If NN’s condition was a result of alcohol abuse, tion score = 48.33 and the conversion score = 24.44; for comparison:
the damage would affect also anterograde memory functions. NN, Freyberger et al., 1998).
however, was afflicted with a selective loss of autobiography and NN also completed the visual memory and imagery as well as
identity; all other memory functions remained clinically unim- related memory qualities, e.g. the TCI, which has been proposed as a
paired. Additionally, a dissociative fugue preceded the amnestic method for investigating personality using a dimensional approach
syndrome and this is not typical for alcohol-related amnestic syn- of temperament and character. It comprises four temperament
dromes. Finally, his amnesia did not occur exclusively during the dimensions (novelty seeking, harm avoidance, reward dependence,
course of a dissociative fugue and was not due to direct physiolog- persistence) and three character dimensions (self-directedness,
ical effects of a substance abuse (e.g. from drugs or medication) cooperativeness, self-transcendence). Compared to healthy con-
or a neurological or other general medical condition (e.g. head trols, NN showed diminished exploratory activity in searching for
trauma). novelty and sensations (percentile: 2), and an increased heritable
tendency to respond intensely to signals of aversive stimuli as well
as to avoid harm (percentile: 99).
3. Psychiatric psychometric assessment

In order to refine the diagnosis in accordance with the ICD- 4. General neuropsychological assessment
10 criteria, the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II, Beck, Steer,
& Brown, 1996), the Dissociative Experience Scale (DES, German Cognitive measures on intelligence (revised Wechsler Adult
version: Fragebogen für Dissoziative Symptome, FDS, Freyberger, Intelligence Scale, WAIS-R; German version: HAWIE-R, Tewes,
Spitzer, & Stieglitz, 1998) and the Temperament and Character 1991), attention (Testbatterie zur Aufmerksamkeitsprüfung, TAP,
Inventory (TCI, Cloninger, Svrakic, & Przybeck, 1993) were admin- Zimmermann & Fimm, 1993; WMS-R, German version: Härting et
istered to NN. al., 2000), executive functions (Halsted Category Test, HCT, Ger-
2996 K. Hennig-Fast et al. / Neuropsychologia 46 (2008) 2993–3005

Table 1
Results of neuropsychological assessment: a comparison of NN’s performance in first examination (T1) and 1 year later (T2)

Cognitive function Instrument T1 T2 Trend

Intelligence WAIS-R: PIQ/VIQ 106/102 109/126 ≈/⇑

Attention and executive functions Attention score (WMS-R) 109 99 ≈


Tonic alertness (TAP)a 55 76 ⇑
Phasic alertness (TAP)a 55 79 ⇑
Go/No-Go/inhibition (TAP) reaction timea 79 106 ⇑
Go/No-Go/inhibition (TAP) error scorea 79 94 ⇑
Trail making test Ab <55 97 ⇑
Trail making test Bb n.a. 80 ⇑
Digit symbol coding (WAIS-R) 79 100 ⇑
Digit span (WAIS-R) 115 118 ≈
Halstead Category Test 103 105 ≈
Picture arrangement (WAIS-R) 115 115 ≈
Comprehension (WAIS-R) 94 118 ⇑
Similarities (WAIS-R) 100 133 ⇑

Anterograde memory WMS-R (German version)


VerbM/VisMQ 111/93 119/72 ≈/⇓
Immediate memory 106 104 ≈
Delayed memory 113 109 ≈
Priming (fragmented picture test) ∅ ∅ ≈

Visual perception/construction Picture completion (WAIS-R) 88 119 ⇑


Object assembly (WAIS-R) 115 124 ≈
Block design (WAIS-R) 103 115 ≈

Note: VIQ = Verbal intelligence score, PIQ = Performance intelligence score, VerbMQ = Verbal memory quotient; VisMQ = Visual memory quotient; ≈ = unchanged, ⇑ = improved,
⇓ = decreased. ∅ = at average, n.a. = not assessable. All original standard values are transformed into IQ-scores (mean = 100; standard deviation = 15).
a
The TAP subtest on alertness measures sustained attention, the subtest on selective attention is a go/no go task, measuring behavioural inhibition.
b
Norms are from Spreen and Strauss (1998) which were extrapolated from Yeudall et al. (1987) and Tombaugh et al. (1996).

man PC-Version: Fast & Engel, 2007; Trail Making Test, TMT, Reitan, from Kopelman, Wilson and Baddeley (AMI, 1990; German ver-
1986), and anterograde memory (revised Wechsler Memory Scale, sion: Erweitertes Autobiographisches Gedächtnisinventar: Fast,
WMS-R, German version: Härting et al., 2000) were used to deter- Fujiwara, & Markowitsch, 2007a; Fast, Fujiwara, Schröder, et al.,
mine the degree to which NN’s general cognitive performance 2007b).
differs between both times of examination. All original standard
values for both assessments were transformed into IQ-scores (see
5.1. Test of semantic knowledge and memory for famous faces
Table 1).
We also tested NN’s implicit memory, using a fragmented pic-
General knowledge was assessed with the subtest Information
ture test (German version: Fragmentierter Bilder Test, Kessler,
from the WAIS-R. For evaluation of the temporal gradient of seman-
Schaaf, & Mielke, 1993). NN exhibited normal facilitation in this test
tic remote memory, a Famous Faces Test (FFT, Fast, Fujiwara, &
of visual priming in the first assessment. Thus the test was given
Markowitsch, 2007a; Fast, Fujiwara, Schröder, et al., 2007b) was
once only.
administered. In this test, subjects are asked to recall the correct
name and to provide additional detailed knowledge about a famous
4.1. Results person (e.g. nationality, profession) cued by a portrait photo. Sub-
jects are presented with portrait photos of famous popular people
Despite an average intelligence quotient (IQ) and a normal from the past decades up to 2007. If the subject fails to name the
abstract reasoning capacity at both times of assessment, NN’s atten- person in the photograph, he or she is asked to choose the correct
tion, information processing speed and executive functioning were name of the famous person from a block of four names, includ-
impaired in the first examination. At the time of the first assess- ing fictitious names. Again, they have the opportunity to search for
ment, his performance reflected an average ability to learn and store semantic information about the presented person on the basis of
new information. One year later, NN’s performance on verbal mem- the name. A total of 3 blocks of 10 portraits each were presented to
ory was above-average. In contrast, his visual memory score had NN controlling for his age at the time of assessment.
fallen by more than one standard deviation. This finding was con-
sistent with his improved superior verbal IQ at the time of retesting,
5.2. Test on self-related episodic and semantic memory
whereas his non-verbal IQ was unchanged. Additionally, 1 year after
onset, his attention, information processing speed and executive
We assessed the episodic self-relevant remote memory with an
functioning had been improved (Table 1).
autobiographical memory inventory (German version of the Auto-
biographical Memory Interview (AMI) of Kopelman, Wilson, and
5. Assessment of retrograde memory Baddeley, 1990: Erweitertes Autobiographisches Gedächtnis Inven-
tar, E-AGI, Fast, Fujiwara, & Markowitsch, 2007a; Fast, Fujiwara,
Retrograde memory was assessed with tests of semantic and Schröder, et al., 2007b). The E-AGI consists of a semi-structured
episodic remote information. Remote memory for public peo- interview dealing with facts and events in the subjects’ life during
ple was evaluated with the Famous Faces Test (Fast, Fujiwara, specific periods. Subjects are required to provide five autobi-
& Markowitsch, 2007a; Fast, Fujiwara, Schröder, & Markowitsch, ographical facts, two episodic incidents and details on one of
2007b). Remote memory for self-related, biographical informa- the two incidents for each period. Cues for generic events were
tion was tested with an autobiographical interview adapted shown throughout the entire length of the interview (e.g. falling
K. Hennig-Fast et al. / Neuropsychologia 46 (2008) 2993–3005 2997

biographical episodes and retrograde public episodes. As well as


this we used fictitious scenarios and episodes similar to the real
used retrograde events. This paradigm was developed based on the
experimental procedure of Piefke, Weiss, Zilles, Markowitsch, and
Fink (2003) who has provided evidence for the sensitivity of this
method to generate activation in neuronal networks related to auto-
biographical retrieval in healthy participants (see also Daselaar et
al., 2008). Hence, we implemented an experimental design that
included two memory conditions for retrieving real public (RP)
and autobiographical (RA) memories ( = two memory conditions) as
well as two imagination conditions for visualizing fictitious unex-
perienced public (FP) and autobiographical (FA) scenarios ( = two
fictitious memory conditions).
Fig. 2. Performance scores obtained by NN on the famous faces test in the first and For NN, individual stimuli were acquired by interviewing his
second assessment (time 1 and time 2). wife who collected different episodes of his life. Some of these were
drawn from her memory and some were collected with the help of
in love/first kiss, school enrolment, birth of child) in order to aid other relatives. For the healthy male controls the individual stim-
retrieval. Periods of 0–5 years of age, 6–10 years, 11–18 years, 19–34 uli were collected by interviewing each participant 4 weeks prior
years and the last 5 years prior to the onset of amnesia were tested scanning. None of them were informed about the later memory test
in NN. prior scanning. Sixty different stimulus sentences were constructed
for all autobiographical episodes. In order to differentiate between
vivid remembering and pure imagination 60 stimulus sentences
5.3. Results
constructed from fictitious, never experienced autobiographical
events expressed in the style of real autobiographical stimuli were
In the first measurement, NN exhibited no impairment in access
generated. Additionally, 60 real public events distributed over the
to general knowledge on the subtest Information of the WAIS-R. His
last four decades were collected and transformed into 60 stimulus
ability to recall names of famous people and facts about those peo-
sentences as was done with the autobiographical cues. Again 60
ple was above average. His performance in the semantic knowledge
similar fictitious public events were generated for comparison of
test and his memory for names and facts about public people were
recall and imagination. Hence, a total of 60 short sentences, com-
equally strong in the second examination, 1 year later (Fig. 2).
parable in length and word order, were generated for each of the
At initial testing, his memory for autobiographical episodes
four memory conditions: (a) real autobiographical condition (RA);
and facts was severely impaired. NN’s low scores for retrograde
(b) fictitious autobiographical condition (FA); (c) real public con-
autobiographical memory were relatively unaltered over the 12-
dition (RP); and (d) fictitious public condition (FP). This material
month period. The only improvement consisted of his being able
(240 non-identical individual stimulus sentences in total) was used
to remember a few fragmented episodes from childhood and some
during the fMRI experiment to trigger the associated autobiograph-
episodes from the time of his dissociative fugue (Fig. 3).
ical and public memories. There was no repetition of the individual
stimulus sentences, either within or across the experimental condi-
6. Functional imaging study tions. This was done in order to avoid potential habituation effects.
Prior to scanning, NN and the control subjects were familiarized
6.1. fMRI paradigm with the experimental set-up and the tasks.
Presentation and timing of stimuli was accomplished using
NN underwent an fMRI measurement twice, during the acute Presentation® software (Version 0.80, built on 13.02.2004). Inside
state and 1 year after onset. His patterns of brain activation the MR scanner, NN viewed the stimuli over a head-coil compatible
were compared to neuronal activation in a group of eight healthy mirror system (300 cm screen to mirror, 15 cm mirror to subject’s
men (mean age = 36, range of age = 30–50, mean years of educa- eyes). Stimuli were projected on a translucent screen in white capi-
tion = 11.6) who underwent a single fMRI measurement. In the tals on black background by a commercially available video beamer
fMRI procedure we used a memory paradigm on retrograde auto- (INTouch, resolution of 1024 × 768 pixel) protected by a Faraday
cage.
A factorial-blocked design was applied to evoke the neural
responses associated a) with retrieval of different types of mem-
ory content (autobiographical, public) and b) with real retrieval
compared to imagination (real, fictitious). Six individual trials of
one of the four experimental conditions (e.g. real autobiograph-
ical memory) were blocked together. Each trial consisted of the
stimulus sentence that should trigger autobiographical episodes
(presented for 5 s) 5 s and an interstimulus interval (ISI) of 100 ms.
Thus, each block of trials lasted 30.6 s. The complete experimental
paradigm consisted of two repeated blocks of one memory/imagery
condition. The order of blocked conditions (RA, FA, RP, FP) was coun-
terbalanced across five runs (run1: EI RA FA RP FP EI FP RP FA RA;
run2: EI FA RA FP RP EI RP RA FA FP; run3: EI FA FP RP RA EI RP
FP RA FA; run4: EI RA RP FP FA EI FA RP FP RA; run5: EI RP FA FP
RA EI FP RA RP FA.) The experimental instructions (EI) lasted 30.6 s
Fig. 3. Performance scores obtained by NN on the autobiographical memory test in and were repeated at the beginning of each new run and after four
the first and second assessment (time 1 and time 2). blocks within each run in order to use it as baseline for statistical
2998 K. Hennig-Fast et al. / Neuropsychologia 46 (2008) 2993–3005

Fig. 4. The experimental task consists of four conditions presented in a block design. The blocks were separated by a break. The order of the four conditions was counterbalanced
across all runs. The design is exemplified by the RA and RP condition.

comparison. Activation blocks were separated from each other by size, 1.05 mm × 1.05 mm, field of view, 270 mm, matrix, 224 × 256,
fixation crosses each lasting 3 s (see Fig. 4). Instructions were as numbers of slices, 128.
follows: ‘Please remember all presented events specified in the
displayed sentences as vividly as possible.’
6.3. Image and statistical analysis

6.2. fMRI data acquisition Imaging data were analyzed with BrainVoyager QX (Goebel,
2006). The first three scans were excluded from data analysis to
Functional MRI images were collected with a 1.5 Tesla minimize T1 effects. The functional images were 3D motion and
scanner (Magnetom Vision, Siemens Erlangen, Germany) using slice scan-time corrected using sinc-interpolation. The anatomi-
T2*-weighted BOLD (blood oxygen level dependent) sensitive cal data were normalized into a standard Talairach template for
echo planar imaging (EPI) sequences (echo time, 60 ms, rep- each healthy participant and NN separately. Finally the functional
etition time, 3600 ms, flip angle = 90◦ , slice thickness = 4 mm, data were co-registered with the individual 3D anatomical data of
inter-slice gap = 0.25 mm, field of view, 240 mm, in plane resolu- each participant. Differences between the experimental conditions
tion, 3.75 × 3.75 mm, matrix = 64 × 64). Twenty-eight transversal were assessed voxel by voxel over the conditions using random
slices (slice thickness 4 mm) from the cerebellum to the cortex were effects analysis which is based on a general linear model. The visu-
acquired parallel to the anterior commissure–posterior commis- alized fMRI results are due to BOLD contrasts which were analyzed
sure (AC–PC) in interleaved order. using the subtraction method between the different experimen-
For visualization of the functional data, high resolution anatomi- tal conditions (e.g. RA minus FA). In this voxel-based approach,
cal images (3D MPRAGE, magnetization-prepared rapid acquisition contrast maps were computed for the predictors RA minus FA,
gradient echo) were recorded following the functional measure- RA minus. RP, RP minus FP, and vice versa. Standard stereotac-
ments with following parameters: echo time, 4.4 ms, repetition tic coordinates of pixels showing local maximum activation were
time, 11.4 ms, flip angle = 8◦ , effective slice thickness, 1.25 mm, pixel determined within areas of significant relative changes in neural

Fig. 5. Significant clusters of activation in response to the contrasts of real autobiographical memory condition against fictitious autobiographical imagination condition: (a)
of NN in the first measurement: RA > FA: BA 25/32; (b) of NN in the second measurement: RA > FA: BA 11/13; BA 35/38/20/21 and (c) of healthy controls: RA > FA: BA 37/38,
BA 46/47, BA 18/20 (threshold: uncorrected p < 0.01).
K. Hennig-Fast et al. / Neuropsychologia 46 (2008) 2993–3005 2999

Table 2
NN’s brain regions activated in response to contrasts of different experimental conditions during the first fMRI-measurement (p < 0.01, uncorrected for multiple comparisons,
extent threshold 50 voxels, balanced)

Region Talairach co-ordinates (in mm)

BA Left Right

X, Y, Z z-value X, Y, Z z-value

Real autobiographical > fictitious autobiographical


Posterior cingulate 30 −9, −55, 10 3.46
Subcallosal gyrus 25 −6, 23, −14 3.76
Real autobiographical > real public
Anterior cingulate 32 −9, 44, −5 3.81
Posterior cingulate 30 −9, −55, 10 3.94
Precentral gyrus 6 30, 5, 31 3.48
Real public > fictitious public
Mesial frontal gyrus 10 18, 47, −5 3.39

Note: Coordinates of voxels are presented in Talairach and Tournoux space (z-value refers to peak of activated cluster); BA = Brodmann area.

activity associated with the demands of the different experimental between the real autobiographical memory and fictitious auto-
conditions. These local maxima were anatomically localized by ref- biographical memory showed that the left sub-callosal and the
erence to a standard stereotactic atlas (Talairach & Tournoux, 1988) left cingulate were more active during the condition of autobi-
(Fig. 5). ographical memory than when attempting to retrieve fictitious
events. Relative to the public memory task (RA minus RP), para-
6.4. Experimental fMRI results: first measurement during the metric effects of self-relevance of remembering were revealed in
acute state only left cingulate regions and the superior frontal gyrus. Activa-
tion was found in the mesial frontal gyrus for the contrast of real
The Brain Voyager analysis conducted on the whole brain did public memory (RP) minus fictitious public memory (FP). Results
not find activation in our a priori regions of interest during auto- of the conjunction analysis performed on the reverse comparison
biographical retrieval during the acute state. Direct comparison (FA > RA; RP > RA, FP > RP) yielded no activation, using the same

Table 3
Brain regions activated in response to contrasts of different experimental conditions during the second measurement after 1 year (p < 0.01, uncorrected for multiple
comparisons, extent threshold 50 voxels, balanced)

Region Talairach co-ordinates (in mm)

BA Left Right

X, Y, Z z-value X, Y, Z z-value

Real autobiographical > fictitious autobiographical


Mesial frontal gyrus 11 36, 38, −8 4.53
Insula 13 −42, −19, −8 4.82 21, −4, 7 4.52
Anterior cingulate 24 9, 23, 1 4.68
Parahippocampal gyrus 35 −27, −25, −26 5.07
Superior temporal gyrus 38 −42, 26, −23 4.88
Superior temporal gyrus 22 −51, −25, −2 5.37
Inferior temporal gyrus 20 −52, −7, −38 5.44
Fusiform gyrus 20 −42, −28, −17 5.03

Fictitious autobiographical > real autobiographical


Superior frontal gyrus 9 −12, 44, 50 3.96
Superior frontal gyrus 6 18, 18, 64 4.32
Mesial frontal gyrus 8 27, 41, 43 4.46
Superior temporal gyrus 38 −33, 8, −32 5.21
Real autobiographical > real public
Insula 13 −45, 5, 1 3.39 27, 25, 13 3.74
Middle frontal gyrus 11 −21, 50, −8 3.74 36, 44, −11 3.57
Inferior frontal gyrus 47 39, 26, 4 3.75
Extra nuclear/sub-lobar gyrus 47 30, 17, 1 3.77
Precentral gyrus 6 −64, −7, 31 3.77
Cingulate gyrus 24 −9, 7, 28 4.02
Real public > real autobiographical
Inferior frontal cortex 47 −21,11, −17 4.21
Fusiform gyrus 20 42, −22, −11 3.36
Inferior temporal gyrus 21 60, −7, −17 3.44
Middle temporal gyrus 21 60, −19, −5 3.48
Subgyral (temporal lobe) 21 42, −16, −14 3.41
Mesial frontal gyrus 25 12, 31, −14 4.55
Anterior Cingulate 32 12, 32, −14 3.70
Real public > fictitious public
Mesial temporal gyrus 21 51, 8, −32 3.79

Note: Coordinates of voxels are presented in Talairach and Tournoux space (z-value refers to peak of activated cluster); BA = Brodmann area.
3000 K. Hennig-Fast et al. / Neuropsychologia 46 (2008) 2993–3005

thresholds (p < 0.01, uncorrected for multiple comparisons, cluster- poral activation was found when fictitious public memory (FP) was
level k > 50 voxels; see Table 2). subtracted from public memory (RP).

6.5. Experimental fMRI results: second measurement 1 year after 6.6. Comparison of the activation pattern during acute state and
onset 1 year after onset

The comparison between the real autobiographical memory NN’s responsive patterns during the first measurement were
(RA) and the fictitious memory (FA) condition revealed increased restricted in terms of activations within anterior frontal brain
activation in an extensive mainly left-hemispherically distributed regions, whereas the activated networks during the follow-up mea-
network including the left parahippocampus and frontal and surement were more differentiated (Tables 2 and 3). One year after
temporal lobe (Table 3). This contrast yielded activations in the onset NN showed additional clusters of activation in regions that
following temporal regions: left inferior and superior temporal are known to be crucial for autobiographical memory: the parahip-
gyrus and the left fusiform gyrus. Furthermore, this comparison pocampus and the temporal lobe. However, he did not display
yielded evidence of activation in the left superior frontal gyrus and this additional temporal activation when autobiographical mem-
the right cingulate gyrus. In addition, the bilateral insulae showed ory was contrasted with public memory (RA minus RP). The reverse
increased activity associated with the autobiographical condition contrast revealed a temporal activation in the right hemisphere in
in NN (Table 3). The retrieval of self-related memories contrasted response to public events.
against public memories was associated with activation in a bilat- The statistical comparison of time of follow-up and time of
eral anterior network within frontal brain regions: right sub-lobar, onset, however, resulted in clusters of increased activation in the
right superior and inferior frontal gyrus, bilateral mesial frontal temporal, limbic and frontal lobes in response to autobiograph-
gyrus, the left cingulate and precentral gyrus, and left insula. The ical stimuli. Elevated activation was found within a network of
reverse contrasts resulted in activations within separable cerebral the following temporo–limbic regions: the medial, superior, infe-
networks: FA > RA was associated with frontal activation, RP > RA rior, fusiform temporal gyrus and the right parahippocampal gyrus.
involved only temporal regions, see Table 3). A single mesial tem- Additionally, the left inferior frontal gyrus was responsive to autobi-

Table 4
Brain regions activated in response to contrasts of the two times of measurement (follow up minus first measurement, and vice versa) for the main experimental conditions

Region Talairach co-ordinates (in mm)

BA Left Right

X, Y, Z z-value X, Y, Z z-value

Real autobiographical at follow up > first assessment


Inferior frontal lobe 47 −43, 22, 6 4.03
Middle temporal gyrus 21/22 −64, −40, 0 3.74 49, −40, −2 3.91
21 −61, −30, −1 3.27
Inferior temporal gyrus 20 −58, −39, −15 3.16
Fusiform gyrus 37 −51, −58, −17 4.12
20 −39, −31, −14 3.70
Parahippocampal gyrus 36 41, −33, −11 3.30
Real autobiographical at first assessment > follow up
Middle frontal Cortex 10 −29, 47, 8 4.37 39, 45, −2 4.31
Inferior frontal Cortex 47 −18, 23, −16 3.88 31, 31, −15, 3.43
Anterior Cingulate 32 −17, 23, 17 4.23
Insula 13 42, −16, 24 3.44
Parahippocampal gyrus 37 −9, −21, −11 3.90
Superior temporal lobe 39 41, −56, 24 3.38
38 36, 14, −28 5.11
Cuneus 30 −9, −58, 10 4.97
Real public at follow up > first assessment
Subgyral temporal gyrus 20 39, −19, −16 3.71
Middle temporal gyrus 21 57, −37, −2 3.65
Superior temporal gyrus 22 48, −22, −5 4.78
Fusiform gyrus 37 −49, −36, −10 3.36
Cuenus 18 −15, −100, −2 3.69
Real public at first assessment > follow up
Superior and middle frontal gyrus 6 −10, 33, 55 3.64 25, −1, 60 3.75
Middle frontal gyrus 46 −47, 38, 17 4.18
9 40, 11, 26 4.39
Paracentral gyrus/ 6 −9, −22, 52 4.16
Precentral gyrus 6 51, −1, 22 3.98
Middle frontal gyrus 11 36, 38, −8 4.53
Insula 13 42, −13, 23 3.99
Cingulate gyrus 32 −14, 20, 39 4.51
Posterior cingulated 31/29 −9, −65, 17 3.88 3, −43, 10 4.62
Hippocampus 32, −16, −14 3.91
Parahippocampal region 30 7, −38, 5 4.11
Inferior parietal lobule 40 47, −28, 29
Fusiform gyrus 19 36, −69, −7 4.50
Cuneus 19 2, −94, 24 3.38
Lingual Gyrus 17 18, −85, 2 3.56

Note: Coordinates of voxels are presented in Talairach and Tournoux space (z-value refers to peak of activated cluster); BA = Brodmann area.
K. Hennig-Fast et al. / Neuropsychologia 46 (2008) 2993–3005 3001

ographical stimuli. The converse comparison resulted in extended a response of anterior frontal brain regions after the 1 year delay,
activation within a network of frontal, temporal, limbic and occip- while the healthy subjects’ response for this contrast involved
ital brain regions (Table 4). NN activated bilateral orbitofrontal and temporo–occipital regions (for comparison: Tables 3 and 5). In gen-
dorsolateral prefrontal regions, the right posterior cingulate, the eral NN’s activation pattern did not show the posterior occipital
insula, the left parahippocampal gyrus, the right superior temporal activations displayed in the healthy sample.
gyrus and the left cuneus. The reverse contrasts of FA minus RA, RP minus RA and FP minus
When comparing the neural response to real public stimuli in RP yielded no pattern of activation in the healthy control sample.
the follow-up examination with time of onset, an extensive acti-
vation was evident in the bilateral superior, right mesial and left
7. Discussion
fusiform temporal lobe as well as in the left cuneus (see Table 4).
The converse contrast demonstrated extensive activation, in the
Similarly to previous cases of RA and dissociatve amnesia (e.g.
hippocampal as well as parahippocampal, parietal and occipital
Campodonico & Rediess, 1996; Markowitsch, Calabrese, et al.,
regions. The anterior network included the left precentral gyrus,
1997a; Markowitsch, Fink, et al., 1997b; Kopelman, Christensen,
right medial frontal gyrus and right paracentral lobe, the bilat-
Puffett, & Stanhope, 1994; Kritchevsky et al., 2004; Schacter et al.,
eral dorsolateral prefrontal, the right ventromedial cortex, the right
1982) the patient NN displayed an all-inclusive loss of biographical
insula, the left cingulate gyrus and the bilateral posterior cingu-
memory and identity but intact semantic knowledge about facts
lates. The posterior network consisted of the right inferior parietal
and time-related public events. Although no brain abnormality
lobule and occipital regions, right fusiform gyrus, right cuneus and
was detected by conventional electrophysiological investigations
right lingual gyrus. The temporo–limbic activations were found in
and high-resolution structural neuroimaging, NN exhibited a slight
the hippocampus, the right parahippocampal region and the right
temporal hypometabolism in resting state FDG-PET, pointing to
medial temporal lobe.
possible diffuse disturbances of brain functions underlying the RA
in his case. Concurrent to the possible neurofunctional origin of his
6.7. NN’s activation pattern in comparison with activation amnesia the most obvious psychological indication was the pre-
patterns of healthy controls vious fugue state, but beyond this, his biography was marked by
diverse aversive experiences of clinical relevance (see case report).
Relative to the brain activation of healthy male controls, NN’s The findings in the formal clinical and neuropsychological assess-
responsive activation was limited to anterior frontal brain regions ment and the functional imaging data supported our argument,
in all memory conditions during the first measurement (compare which will be outlined below.
Tables 2 and 5). During the second measurement NN’s network of A central finding is that during his acute state the
activation was still different to those of the control subjects, even if temporo–occipital part of the network known to be related
it was more distributed than during the first measurement. In con- to autobiographical memory was not activated. Instead, autobi-
trast to the fictitious autobiographical condition NN’s responsive ographical stimuli were associated with activation of the dorsal
neural networks recorded during the real autobiographical condi- anterior and posterior cingulate gyrus, the precentral and sub-
tion included the bilateral insulae and the parahippocampal gyrus, callosal gyrus as well as of the medial frontal lobe. The cingulate
while other brain regions overlapped with those of healthy men. gyrus functions as an integral part of the limbic system, which
When RA was contrasted against the RP condition NN still displayed is involved in emotion formation and processing, learning, and

Table 5
Brain regions activated in response to autobiographical and public memory in healthy male controls (n = 8, mean age = 36, range of age: 30–50, mean years of education: 11.6)

Region Talairach co-ordinates (in mm)

BA Left Right
X, Y, Z z-value X, Y, Z z-value

Real autobiographical > fictitious autobiographical


Inferior frontal gyrus 46 −39, 32, 8 5.57
Inferior frontal gyrus 47 −24, 23, −9 4.14
Anterior cingulate 32 −12, −55, 10 4.41
Inferior temporal gyrus 37 −60, −58, −8 5.33
Superior temporal gyrus 38 −39, 20, −20 4.96
Mesial temporal gyrus 21 51, 5, −26 4.12
Mesial temporal gyrus 21 48, −22, −11 4.55
Fusiform gyrus 20 −51, −22, −21 5.05
Fusiform gyrus 20 −39, −22, −21 4.36
Fusiform gyrus 20 39, −19, −23 4.90
Lingual gyrus 18 −12, −55, 10 4.53
Inferior occipital gyrus 17 −27, −97, −8 5.75
Inferior occipital gyrus 17 21, −97, −8 4.79
Real autobiographical > real public
cingulate 23 −9, −55, 16 3.76
Superior temporal gyrus 38 −39, 20, −20 4.04
Mesial temporal gyrus 21 −57, −19, −8 4.26 48, −22, −1 4.03
Inferior occipital gyrus 18 −31, −88, −17 4.12 36, −85, −11 3.95
Mesial occipital gyrus 18 −46, −76, −14 4.26
Lingual gyrus 18 15, −88, −17 5.51
Real public > fictitious public
Anterior cingulate 32 0, 41, −8, 3.60
Fusiform gyrus 18 18, .88, −14 3.60
Inferior occipital gyrus 18 −27, −85, −18 3.99 36, −88, −8 3.68

Note: Coordinates of voxels are presented in Talairach and Tournoux space (z-value refers to peak of activated cluster); BA = Brodmann area.
3002 K. Hennig-Fast et al. / Neuropsychologia 46 (2008) 2993–3005

memory. The executive control function needed to suppress inap- found during the public condition, the lack of activation during
propriate unconscious priming is known to involve the anterior autobiographical retrieval could not indicate a general deficit in
cingulate gyrus which is bounded by the agranular frontal BA6 perceptual experience. Nevertheless, because successful retrieval
and the frontopolar BA10. Dorsomedial frontal/anterior cingulate depends on simultaneous neural activity in multiple brain regions
cortex and posterior cingulate cortex/precuneus are also known this lack of occipital response could reflect a failure of integration
to be active during self-reflection (Johnson et al., 2006). Thus, this of spatial, temporal, visual, emotional and self-related features of
finding indicates that NN activated self-reflective and monitoring one experienced episode during the attempted retrieval (Damasio,
processes in response to autobiographical stimuli at time of onset. 1989). The lack of neural activation when public memory is con-
On the other hand this finding is probably the result of a failure in trasted with autobiographical memory in healthy controls may be
vivid remembering (for comparison: Daselaar et al., 2008). When due to an overlap of both memory tasks, since both conditions
contrasting the real public condition to the real autobiographical require the retrieval of information which is bound to space and
condition we found only medial frontal activation (BA10). This time.
finding can be explained by either a general loss of brain activation In line with most prior studies on autobiographical memory,
at time of first assessment, probably caused either by the prior we found a preponderance of left-hemispherical activation in NN
state of dissociative fugue (e.g. a form of deficiency syndrome as during autobiographical memory retrieval in both examinations
a result of walking for about 2 weeks), or by the theoretical and (Gusnard et al., 2001; Svoboda et al., 2006). Nevertheless, in the
neurophysiological overlap of both retrieval conditions. Public second examination the responding network was more bilaterally
events and autobiographical events share some features since distributed (in accordance with Levine et al., 2004; Markowitsch,
both are related to time and space. Semantic memory and general Vandekerckhove, Lanfermann, & Russ 2003).
autobiographical knowledge seem to permeate much of our auto- A second central finding in this study relates to NN’s general
biographical recollections. Moreover, specific incidents contain cognitive performance. Attentional and executive functions that
numerous personal or general semantic representations related were initially impaired during the acute phase improved over
to the recollected environment (e.g. people or broader public time. While self-related retrograde memory remained impaired
events, etc.), that, in combination with other factors (e.g. personal and anterograde visual memory deteriorated further. A few previ-
relevance or emotional relevance), allow us to form an episodic ous studies on RA patients that investigated these functions in detail
autobiographical memory. found attentional and executive deficits underlying the amnes-
Thus, the retrieval of both forms of events should involve to some tic syndrome (Campodonico & Rediess, 1996; Glisky et al., 2004;
extent the same neural network. In functional RA it could be that Stracciari et al., 1994). Others found visual deficits influencing auto-
this overlapping neural network is not activated, not only because biographical retrieval (Greenberg & Rubin, 2003).
of organic but also because of psychogenic reasons, e.g. to prevent Here, in accordance with previous cases, e.g. Kritchevsky et
a triggering of covered memories and to hold up the inhibitory al. (2004), our patient’s anterograde memory was only slightly
state. The substantial overlap in neural activation in response to impaired during the first assessment but – in contrast to these
episodic and general semantic tasks (e.g. in the lateral temporal cor- prior cases – at the time of retesting it was found be progressively
tex, as well as the extended semantic memory network including selectively impaired for visual stimuli. NN’s selective impairment in
the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, temporoparietal junction, ante- visual episodic anterograde memory resembles the one described
rior cingulate and cerebellum; for review: Martin, 2001) may reflect in MS, a patient with extensive damage in the occipital and tempo-
both substantial semantic representation within the AMs retrieved ral lobes (Greenberg et al., 2005). Greenberg et al. (2005) assumed
and the contribution of domain-general processes (e.g. executive that the destruction of visual cortices disrupts the spread of acti-
functions) that serve diverse cognitive and memory functions (see vation that autobiographical memory retrieval requires. The term
Cabeza & Nyberg, 2000). of “visual-memory deficit amnesia (VDMS)” established by Rubin
In the similar vein, recent studies of self-referential process- and Greenberg (1998) refers to the assumption that the disrup-
ing have reported activation in medial frontal regions (BA9, 10) tion in recruitment of visual brain regions has the effect that other
when self-referential or internally orientated processing is manip- sensory percepts and knowledge are no longer elicited.
ulated (e.g. Gusnard, Akbudak, Shulman, & Raichle 2001). When we If this is correct, a visual memory deficit could account for defi-
assume that NN had to decide whether this stimulus is self-related cient autobiographical memory in NN as well. We suggest that
the activation of BA10 in NN at time of first assessment might reflect visual imagery plays a central role in the recall of autobiograph-
such self-referential processing during condition RP. ical memories and that anterograde visual memory impairment
One year later temporo–limbic brain regions were responsive may be crucial for the perpetuation of RA (e.g. Greenberg &
to autobiographical stimuli. Additionally, we found bilateral acti- Rubin, 2003). Considering three aspects of NN’s amnestic pro-
vation of the insulae, a structure that is involved in the perception file, (a) the increasing deficit in visual anterograde memory over
of pain and somatic regulation of emotions. When calculating the time, (b) the irreversibility and selectivity of RA, and (c) the
statistical comparison of neural response at the time of onset only proportionate retrieval-related increase of brain activity over
and the time of follow-up for each separate experimental con- time (with a lack of enhancement within the visual cortices), we
dition, we found an unexpected temporo–limbic activation in think that an impairment in visual processing may be crucial for
response to autobiographical stimuli at time of first assessment. the long-term persistence of NN’s loss of autobiographical mem-
This occurred in spite of the already reported frontal brain acti- ory.
vations. This puzzling result indicates a form of temporo–limbic Behavioral studies have shown that the best predictor of the
neural responsiveness at the time of onset. Nevertheless, when degree of experienced reliving is the visual imagery (Brewer, 1995;
compared either to brain activation related to autobiographical Rubin, 2006; Rubin, Schrauf, & Greenberg, 2003) and the visuo-
stimuli at time of follow-up or responsive brain areas to pub- spatial processing (Gilboa, Winocur, Grady, Hevenor, & Moscovitch,
lic stimuli at time of onset or to brain activation of the healthy 2004; Greenberg et al., 2005; Teng & Squire, 1999). On the one
controls, this temporo–limbic activation is limited. In general, we hand, long-term visual representations may enable the retrieval
found only fusiform activation (occipito–temporal area 37/inferior of memories through the imagination of detailed visual features
temporal area 20) but any occipital brain activation in NN dur- present at the time of encoding. On the other hand, the visual
ing the autobiographical condition. Since occipital activation was representations themselves may re-activate non-visual percepts,
K. Hennig-Fast et al. / Neuropsychologia 46 (2008) 2993–3005 3003

self-relevant (knowledge based) concepts as well as emotional and emotions should be at hand even before a situation is fully com-
visceral states related to certain past episodes (Gardini, De Beni, prehended or a memory is fully retrieved. However, other studies
Cornoldi, Bromiley, & Venneri, 2005; Gardini, Coronoldi, De Beni, & suggest a modulating frontal brain system (dorsal and ventral
Venneri, 2006; Intons-Peterson & McDaniel, 1991, see for “knowl- aspects of the mesial PFC) that differentially regulates neuroen-
edge based” images). If this is true, a failure in visual imagery or docrine and autonomic hormonal outputs in immediate response to
visual retrieval may also distort the sense of reliving for NN. The emotional stress (e.g. Radley, Arias, & Sawchenko, 2006). Recently
impaired activation in the visual cortex could impede the activa- theories, posit that the insular cortex is thought to be addition-
tion in non-visual cortices even if the non-visual cortices are intact. ally important for the detection of aversive somatic reactions
In NN, for example, the recollection of the funeral of NN’s grandfa- with a predictive value for prospective behavior (Paulus, Rogalsky,
ther might involve generating different flexible visual scenarios and Simmons, Feinstein, & Stein, 2003). Moreover, the representation of
populating it with other non-visual percepts that refer to the over- interoceptive, precognitive aspects of self-knowledge and of multi-
all scenario. NN, however, recollected a fragmented frozen image. modal aspects of visual memory may be reflected in amydgdaloid
In comparison to VDMA his fragmented memories did not involve and insular activity during retrieval (Craig, 2002, 2004). The insula’s
sounds, smells, tastes or tactile sensations. role in processing affective visual and somatic stimuli may also be
If a neurofunctional interaction of memory and imagination meaningful for the interpretation of NN’s unsuccessful attempts at
exists (Sakai & Miyashita, 1994), it can be concluded that a primary vivid remembering.
defect in imagery impairs the ease of recall of episodic memories Considering three different aspects of NN’s amnestic profile, (a)
and the specificity of autobiographical memory (e.g. Gardini et al., the selectivity of RA for self-related memories, (b) his elevated
2006; Guralnik, Schmeidler, & Simeon, 2000, see also Damasio, scores for dissociation and harm avoidance and, (c) the bilateral
1989). This after-effect may lead to a greater sense that the world, activation of the insulae and the predominant frontal activation
the people and parts of the self are strange and unfamiliar for NN, a when contrasting autobiographical memory against public mem-
phenomenon that is well known in dissociative disorder and post- ory during the second measurement, we think that preserving the
traumatic stress disorder. integrity of internal body states and the elimination of “risky feel-
However, compared to prior cases of organic RA our case did ings, memories and imaginations” may play a further role in the
not meet the full criteria of either VMDA or a RA caused by persistence of NN’s loss of autobiographical memory.
the damage of the MTL and the hippocampus. Consistent with As already proposed by Schacter et al. (1982) and Kopelman
patients with VMDA, our patient suffered from only mild (dur- (2000a,b) a self control entity that differentiates between
ing second measurement moderate) selective anterograde amnesia self-relevant, self-insignificant, self-consistent as well as self-
for visual stimuli. Moreover, NN did not show sparing of child- threatening memories is necessary to understand these phenom-
hood memories (in the first examination). NN’s ungraded RA for ena.
self-related memories is reminiscent of cases that probably com- Schacter et al. (1982) already suggested that the scattered
prise a parieto–occipital brain damage (e.g. Kapur et al., 1996). episodic memories (“islands”) in his patient PN can be charac-
However, in contrast to several VDMA cases, his retrograde mem- terized by a positive affective valence. Thus, he proposed that
ory for autobiographical facts was also severely impaired. At the functionally, RA may be primarily organized along affective, rather
same time, his retrograde memory for episodes and facts that than temporal dimensions. Taking this perspective, the posited
were not related to his person was almost normal. Two phe- self-control entity may become operative as a protective mecha-
nomenological properties play a key role in autobiographical nism to avoid harm by fending off aversive memories. However,
memory, distinguishing it from other forms of memory retrieval. unlike Schacter’s patient, NN had a spontaneous selective access
The first is the extent to which a memory is recollected, e.g. to three aversive (negative and arousing) mnestic “islands” from
reliving and reexperiencing as opposed to a feeling of familiarity. childhood. If the task of remembering an autobiographical event
According to Tulving (2002) vivid recollection is a defining fea- requires the decision in favor of a “risky” response, because of
ture of episodic memory: expressed in terms of traveling back in the possibility of retrieving harmful memories, NN could have
time and in detailed spatio-temporal detail information. The abil- attempted to keep these “risky” memories away from conscious-
ity to mentally travel back in time during reexperience of past ness. Instead he failed in some moments for reasons that are
events is thought to depend on frontal lobe structures. Hence, outlined below. However, following Stein, Simmons, Feinstein,
intact prefrontal functioning is seen to be associated with suc- and Paulus (2007) we can assume that NN’s general mental pro-
cessful autobiographical recollection (Moscovitch & Melo, 1997; file is characterized by a reduced emotional resilience and an
Konishi, Wheeler, Donaldson, & Buckner, 2000; Wheeler & Buckner, increased physiological reactivity to aversive stimuli. His resilience
2004), while impaired executive control and prefrontal pathology could have activated the self-regulative processes as posited in
are known to be associated with dysfunctional autobiographical other cases of functional RA (e.g. in PN). It is likely however,
retrieval (e.g. Bright et al., 2006; Svoboda et al., 2006). Furthermore, that NN was not successful in modulating the emergence of
activity in posterior cortices, especially visual areas (e.g. Kosslyn, memories by means of attentional control. For his biography, we
Ganis, & Thompson, 2001) are assumed to be involved in vivid conclude, that NN developed a life long sensitivity to interper-
recollection which is associated with a high degree of subjective sonal conflicts. According to Dorahy (2006) this resilience towards
reliving. stressful conflicts depends on an alteration in mental organiza-
A second phenomenological property that is important in the tion that underlies a dissociative psychopathology. This alteration
retrieval of personal memories is emotional intensity (for review affects particularly selective attention processing that serves as
Talarico, LaBar, & Rubin, 2004). Several neuroimaging studies have a threat monitoring system. If this is the case, then the cogni-
shown that the functional interactions between the amygdala tive inhibition that results from a stressful experience could be
and hippocampus during encoding predict subsequent memory weakened. Suppressed memories could resurface in these condi-
retrieval (e.g. Dolcos, LaBar, & Cabeza, 2004). Less is known about tions. This weakened cognitive inhibitory control may explain NN’s
the role of emotions during retrieval. Some neuroimaging stud- spontaneous recollection of aversive memories. Autonomously
ies provided support for the perspective that basic emotions act erupting somatic responses to autobiographical stimuli could have
as an early warning system, preparing the (human) organism for led to an inefficient prefrontal control which was replaced by
fast, adaptive actions in probably life-threatening situations. Thus, a somatic, affect-related control system (that is reflected in the
3004 K. Hennig-Fast et al. / Neuropsychologia 46 (2008) 2993–3005

bilateral activation of the insulae during the second measure- References


ment).
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory-
Our findings converge to suggest a fronto-subcortial modulat-
II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
ing system and participation of the visual cortices in this persistent Brewer, W. F. (1995). What is recollective memory? In D. C. Rubin (Ed.), Remember-
RA after a dissociative fugue. This is not completely consistent to ing our past: Studies in autobiographical memory. (pp. 19–66). Cambridge, MA:
prior cases and models of isolated retrograde amnesia (e.g. Levine Cambridge University Press.
Bright, P., Buckman, J., Fradera, A., Yoshimasu, H., Colchester, A. C., & Kopelman, M.
et al., 1998; Kopelman, 2000a,b), which posited a right frontal D. (2006). Retrograde amnesia in patients with hippocampal, medial temporal,
dysfunction (and fronto-temporal disconnection) underlying the temporal lobe, or frontal pathology. Learning & Memory, 13(5), 545–557.
loss of self-related (autonoetic) awareness. The findings are rather Cabeza, R., & Nyberg, L. (2000). Imaging cognition II: An empirical review of 275 PET
and fMRI studies. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 1–47.
in accordance with a recent hypothesis on the role of visual and Campodonico, J. R., & Rediess, S. (1996). Dissociation of implicit and explicit knowl-
emotional properties in autobiographical retrieval (Daselaar et al., edge in a case of psychogenic retrograde amnesia. Journal of the International
2008). Neuropsychological Society: JINS, 2(2), 146–158.
Cloninger, C. R., Svrakic, D. M., & Przybeck, T. R. (1993). A psychobiological model of
However, the interpretation of our findings is limited by all temperament and character. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 975–990.
restrictions of single case reports. When discussing several issues Conway, M. A., & Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000). The construction of autobiographical
raised by our case study, it should be noted that we are well aware memories in the self-memory system. Psychological Review, 107(2), 261–288.
Craig, A. D. (2002). How do you feel? Interoception: The sense of the physiological
that data deriving from a single subject require interpretative cau- condition of the body. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(8), 655–666.
tion. Thus we emphasize the usefulness of this study in generating Craig, A. D. (2004). Human feelings: Why are some more aware than others? Trends
hypotheses for further studies of the characteristics of functional in Cognitive Sciences, 8(6), 239–241.
Damasio, A. (1989). Time-locked multiregional retroactivation: A systems-level pro-
RA. One further limitation is the fact that we could not prevent a
posal for the neural substrates of recall and recognition. Cognition, 33, 25–62.
re-encoding between both assessments, i.e. through viewing pho- Daselaar, S. M., Rice, H. J., Greenberg, D. L., LaBar, K. S., & Rubin, D. C. (2008). The spa-
tos or visiting places. This could limit the explanatory power of our tiotemporal dynamics of autobiographical memory: Neural correlates of recall,
speculations about the role of defensive mechanisms. emotional intensity and reliving. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 217–229.
De Renzi, E., & Lucchelli, F. (1993). Dense retrograde amnesia, intact learning capabil-
ity and abnormal forgetting rate: A consolidation deficit? Cortex, 29(3), 449–466.
Dolcos, F., LaBar, K. S., & Cabeza, R. (2004). Interaction between the amygdala and
8. Conclusion the medial temporal lobe memory system predicts better memory for emotional
events. Neuron, 42, 855–863.
The present case provides some insights into new aspects of Dorahy, M. J. (2006). The dissociative processing style: A cognitive organization acti-
vated by perceived or actual threat in clinical dissociators. Journal of Trauma
the phenomenology of autobiographical memory in selective RA.
Dissociation, 7(4), 29–53.
Attentional and executive impairments are already recognized as Erdelyi, M. H. (2006). The unified theory of repression. Behavioral and Brain Sciences,
properties of acute RA. Our findings highlight the additional role of 29, 499–551.
visual and emotional properties of autobiographical memory in the Fast, K., & Engel, R. R. (2007). Halstead Category Test—Deutsche Version (HCT-D). Göt-
tingen: Hogrefe.
maintenance of RA. We provided evidence for an extensive neural Fast, K., Fujiwara, E., & Markowitsch, H.-J. (2007). Famous Faces Test (FFT). Göttingen:
hypoactivity within the known network producing autobiographi- Hogrefe.
cal memory retrieval. Our findings can be explained by a part-time Fast, K., Fujiwara, E., Schröder, J., & Markowitsch, H.-J. (2007). Erweitertes Autobi-
ographisches Gedächtnisinterview (E-AGI). Frankfurt: Harcourt.
dysfunction of the PFC and a simultaneous predetermined or Freyberger, H., Spitzer, C., & Stieglitz, R. (1998). Fragebogen zu dissoziativen Symp-
learned hyperreactivity of anxiety-related brain structures. This tomen FDS (Aufl. 1). Bern: Hans Huber.
dysregulation within the system of affective and behavioral regula- Gardini, S., De Beni, R., Cornoldi, C., Bromiley, A., & Venneri, A. (2005). Different neu-
ronal pathways support the generation of general and specific mental images.
tion may be caused by disruptions within an underlying distributed Neuroimage, 27(3), 544–552.
cortico–limbic network. However, even though NN’s brain activ- Gardini, S., Cornoldi, C., De Beni, R., & Venneri, A. (2006). Left mediotemporal
ity had returned to a relatively normal level of functioning, we structures mediate the retrieval of episodic autobiographical mental images.
Neuroimage, 30(2), 645–655.
found lingering impairments in anterograde visual memory and Gilboa, A., Winocur, G., Grady, C. L., Hevenor, S. J., & Moscovitch, M. (2004). Remem-
persistent autographical retrograde amnesia. Therefore, we posit a bering our past: Functional neuroanatomy of recollection of recent and very
protective defence mechanism caused by neuronal inhibition that remote personal events. Cerebral Cortex, 14, 1214–1225.
Glisky, E. L., Ryan, L., Reminger, S., & Hardt, O. (2004). A case of psychogenic fugue:
served to prevent an overflow of intensive aversive emotions. Fur-
I understand, aber ich verstehe nichts. Neuropsychologia, 42, 1132–1147.
ther, we posit an impairment of visual information processing that Goebel, R. (2006). Brain Voyager QX (version 2.2) (Computer software and manual).
was the result of disruptions within the temporo–occipital neu- The Netherlands: Maastricht Brain Innovation B.V.
ronal network. Both inhibitory impairments could have reduced Greenberg, D. L., Eacott, M. J., Brechin, D., & Rubin, D. C. (2005). Visual mem-
ory loss and autobiographical amnesia: A case study. Neuropsychologia, 43(10),
the triggering of multi-modal memory traces of visual and other 1493–1502.
percepts, and hence, impeded a reactivation of vivid, aversive mem- Greenberg, D. L., & Rubin, D. C. (2003). The neuropsychology of autobiographical
ories. Nonetheless, the persistence of NN’s amnesia in spite of his memory. Cortex, 39(4–5), 687–728.
Guralnik, O., Schmeidler, J., & Simeon, D. (2000). Feeling unreal: Cognitive processes
comparatively strong neural recovery is puzzling and invites mul- in depersonalization. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157(1), 103–109.
tiple possible interpretations. Further longitudinal functional brain Gusnard, D. A., Akbudak, E., Shulman, G. L., & Raichle, M. E. (2001). Medial prefrontal
imaging studies in patients with dissociative or functional RA are cortex and self-referential mental activity: Relation to a default mode of brain
function. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 98(7), 4259–4264.
needed to help us to understand the interaction between the psy- Härting, C., Markowitsch, H.-J., Neufeld, H., Calabrese, P., Deisinger, K., & Kessler, J.
chological and neural patterns in persistent RA. (2000). Wechsler Gedächtnis Test—Revidierte Fassung (WMS-R). Bern: Huber.
Intons-Peterson, M., & McDaniel, M. (1991). Symmetries and asymmetries between
imagery and perception. In C. Cornoldi & M. A. McDaniel (Eds.), Imagery and
Cognition (pp. 47–76). New York: Springer.
Acknowledgments Johnson, M. K., Raye, C. L., Mitchell, K. J., Touryan, S. R., Greene, E. J., & Nolen-
Hoeksema, S. (2006). Dissociating medial frontal and posterior cingulate activity
We are grateful to Calum Anderson, Sandra Dittmann and James during self-reflection. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1(1), 56–64.
Kapur, S., Scholey, K., Moore, E., Barker, S., Brice, J., & Thompson, S. (1996). Long-term
Moran for proofreading this paper. Further, we would like to thank retention-deficits in two cases of disproportionate retrograde amnesia. Journal
NN for his patience and interest, his relatives for their support. This of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, 416–434.
study has been approved by the medical ethic committee of the Kessler, J., Schaaf, A., & Mielke, R. (1993). Fragmentierter Bildertest (FBT). Göttingen:
Hogrefe.
LMU and has therefore been performed in accordance with the Konishi, S., Wheeler, M. E., Donaldson, D. I., & Buckner, R. L. (2000). Neural correlates
ethical standards laid down in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki. of episodic retrieval success. Neuroimage, 12, 276–286.
K. Hennig-Fast et al. / Neuropsychologia 46 (2008) 2993–3005 3005

Kopelman, M. D. (2000a). Focal retrograde amnesia and the attribution of causal- Repetto, C., Manenti, R., Sansone, V., Cotelli, M., Perani, D., Garibotto, V., Zanetti, O.,
ity: An exceptionally critical review. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17, 585– Meola, G., & Miniussi, C. (2007). Persistent autobiographical amnesia: A case
621. report. Behavioural Neurology, 18(1), 13–17.
Kopelman, M. D. (2000b). Fear can interrupt the continuum of memory. Journal of Rubin, D. C. (2005). A basic-systems approach to autobiographical memory. Current
Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 69(4), 431–432. Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 79–83.
Kopelman, M., Wilson, B., & Baddeley, A. (1990). The autobiographical Memory Inter- Rubin, D. C. (2006). The basic-systems model of episodic memory. Perspectives on
view. Bury St. Edmunds: Thames Valley Test Company. Psychological Science, 1, 277–311.
Kopelman, M. D., Christensen, H., Puffett, A., & Stanhope, N. (1994). The great escape: Rubin, D. C., & Greenberg, D. L. (1998). Visual memory-deficit amnesia: A distinct
A neuropsychological study of psychogenic amnesia. Neuropsychologia, 32(6), amnesic presentation and etiology. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 95(9), 5413–5416.
675–691. Rubin, D. C., Schrauf, R. W., & Greenberg, D. L. (2003). Belief and recollection of
Kosslyn, S. M., Ganis, G., & Thompson, W. L. (2001). Neural foundations of imagery. autobiographical memories. Memory & Cognition, 31(6), 887–901.
Nature Review: Neuroscience, 2, 635–642. Sakai, K., & Miyashita, Y. (1994). Neuronal tuning to learned complex forms in vision.
Kritchevsky, M., Chang, J., & Squire, L. R. (2004). Functional amnesia: Clinical descrip- Neuroreport, 5(7), 829–832.
tion and neuropsychological profile of 10 cases. Learning & Memory, 11(2), Schacter, D. L., Wang, P. L., Tulving, E., & Freedman, M. (1982). Functional retrograde
213–226. amnesia: A quantitative case study. Neuropsychologia, 20(5), 523–532.
Levine, B., Black, S. E., Cabeza, R., Sinden, M., Mcintosh, A. R., Toth, J. P., Tulving, E., & Sellal, F., Manning, L., Seegmuller, C., Scheiber, C., & Schoenfelder, F. (2002). Pure
Stuss, D. T. (1998). Episodic memory and the self in a case of isolated retrograde retrograde amnesia following a mild head trauma: A neuropsychological and
amnesia. Brain, 121, 1951–1973. metabolic study. Cortex, 38(4), 499–509.
Levine, B., Turner, G. R., Tisserand, D., Hevenor, S. J., Graham, S. J., & McIntosh, Spreen, O., & Strauss, E. (1998). A Compendium of neuropsychological tests: Adminis-
A. R. (2004). The functional neuroanatomy of episodic and semantic autobio- tration, norms, and commentary (2nd ed.). NY: Oxford University Press.
graphical remembering: A prospective functional MRI study. Journal of Cognitive Starkstein, S. E., Sabe, L., & Dorego, M. F. (1997). Severe retrograde amnesia after a
Neuroscience, 16, 1633–1646. mild closed head injury. Neurocase, 3, 105–109.
MacLeod, M. D., & Macrae, C. N. (2001). Gone but not forgotten: The transient nature Stein, M. B., Simmons, A. N., Feinstein, J. S., & Paulus, M. P. (2007). Increased amyg-
of retrieval-induced forgetting. Psychological Science, 12, 148–152. dala and insula activation during emotion processing in anxiety-prone subjects.
Markowitsch, H. J., Calabrese, P., Fink, G. R., Durwen, H. F., Kessler, J., Härting, C., American Journal of Psychiatry, 164(2), 318–327.
König, M., Mirzaian, E. B., Heiss, W.-D., Heuser, L., & Gehlen, W. (1997). Impaired Stracciari, A., Ghidoni, E., Guarino, M., Poletti, M., & Pazzaglia, P. (1994). Post-
episodic memory retrieval in a case of probable psychogenic amnesia. Psychiatry traumatic retrograde amnesia with selective impairment of autobiographic
Research: Neuroimaging Section, 74, 119–126. memory. Cortex, 30, 459–468.
Markowitsch, H. J., Fink, G. R., Thöne, A. I. M., Kessler, J., & Heiss, W.-D. (1997). Svoboda, E., McKinnon, M. C., & Levine, B. (2006). The functional neuroanatomy
Persistent psychogenic amnesia with a PET-proven organic basis. Cognitive Neu- of autobiographical memory: A meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia, 44(12),
ropsychiatry, 2, 135–158. 2189–2208.
Markowitsch, H. J., Vandekerckhove, M. M., Lanfermann, H., & Russ, M. O. (2003). Talairach, J., & Tournoux, P. (1988). Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain.
Engagement of lateral and medial prefrontal areas in the ecphory of sad and New York: Thieme Medical.
happy autobiographical memories. Cortex, 39(4–5), 643–665. Talarico, J. M., LaBar, K. S., & Rubin, D. C. (2004). Emotional intensity predics autobi-
Martin, A. (2001). Functional neuroimaging of semantic memory. In R. Cabeza & A. ographical memory experience. Memory & Cognition, 32, 1118–1132.
Kingstone (Eds.), Handbook of functional neuroimaging of cognition (pp. 153–186). Teng, E., & Squire, L. R. (1999). Memory for places learned long ago is intact after
London, England: The MIT Press. hippocampal damage. Nature, 400(6745), 675–677.
Moscovitch, M., Rosenbaum, R. S., Gilboa, A., Addis, D. R., Westmacott, R., Grady, C., Tewes, U. (1991). Hamburg-Wechsler-Intelligenztest für Erwachsene Revision.
McAndrews, M. P., Levine, B., Black, S., & Winocur, G. (2005). Functional neu- Bern/Stuttgart/Toronto: Verlag Hans Huber.
roanatomy of remote episodic, semantic and spatial memory: A unified account Tombaugh, T. N., Rees, L., & McIntyre, N. (1996). Normative data for the Trail Making
based on multiple trace theory. Journal of Anatomy, 207, 35–66. Test. Personal communication. In O. Spreen & E. Strauss (Eds.), A Compendium of
Moscovitch, M., & Melo, B. (1997). Strategic retrieval and the frontal lobes: Evidence Neuropsychological Tests. Oxford, N.Y.: Oxford University Press, p. 540.
from confabulation and amnesia. Neuropsychologia, 35, 1017–1034. Tulving, E. (1983). Elements of episodic memory. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Nakamura, H., Kunori, Y., Mori, K., Nakaaki, S., Yoshida, S., & Hamanaka, T. (2002). Tulving, E. (1989). Remembering and knowing the past. American Scientist, 77,
Two cases of functional focal retrograde amnesia with impairment of object use. 361–367.
Cortex, 38, 613–622. Tulving, E. (2002). Episodic memory: From mind to brain. Annual Review of Psychol-
Papagno, C. (1998). Transient retrograde amnesia associated with impaired naming ogy, 53, 1–25.
of living categories. Cortex, 34(1), 111–121. Wheeler, M. E., & Buckner, R. L. (2004). Functional anatomic correlates of remem-
Paulus, M. P., Rogalsky, C., Simmons, A., Feinstein, J. S., & Stein, M. B. (2003). Increased bering and knowing. Neuroimage, 21, 1249–1337.
activation in the right insula during risk taking decision making is related to Wittchen, H.-U., Wunderlich, U., Gruschwitz, S., & Zaudig, M. (1996). Strukturiertes
harm avoidance and neuroticism. NeuroImage, 19, 1439–1448. Klinisches Interview für DSM-IV (SKID). Göttingen: Beltz-Test.
Piefke, M., Weiss, P. H., Zilles, K., Markowitsch, H. J., & Fink, G. R. (2003). Differential World Health Organisation. (1992). The ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioural
remoteness and emotional tone modulate the neural correlates of autobiograph- disorders, clinical descritions and diagnostic guidelines. Genève: World Health
ical memory. Brain, 126, 650–668. Organization.
Radley, J. J., Arias, C. M., & Sawchenko, P. E. (2006). Regional differentiation of the Yeudall, L. T., Reddon, J. R., Gill, D. M., & Stefanyk, W. O. (1987). Normative data for
medial prefrontal cortex in regulating adaptive responses to acute emotional the Halstead-Reitan neuropsychiological tests stratified by age and sex. Journal
stress. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(50), 12967–12976. of Clinical Psychology, 43, 346–367.
Reitan, R. M. (1986). Trail-making test manual for administration and scoring. Tucson, Zimmermann, P., & Fimm, B. (1993). Testbatterie zur Aufmerksamkeitsprüfung (TAP).
AZ: Reitan Neuropsychology Laboratory. München: Psytest.

You might also like