You are on page 1of 7

Available online at www.sciencedirect.

com

ScienceDirect

Review

Emotion dysregulation in borderline personality


disorder: A fronto–limbic imbalance?
Maurizio Sicorello and Christian Schmahl

Abstract [3]. Investigating the neurobiological underpinnings of


Borderline personality disorder is most consistently charac- these developmental findings has the potential to
terized as a disorder of the experience and regulation of improve our understanding of disorder processes, (ge-
emotions. Neuropathological models have predominantly netic) risk, and plasticity. Such neurodevelopmental
explained these clinical traits with an imbalance between research programs are extremely challenging and require
prefrontal regulatory and limbic emotion generating structures. neuropathological models which must first pass the test
Here, we review the current evidential state of the of adequately explaining the emotional phenomenology
fronto–limbic imbalance hypothesis of borderline personality of BPD on a cross-sectional level.
disorder, based on task-related functional magnetic resonance
imaging research. In turn, we discuss challenges to the notion About twenty years of neurobiological research on BPD
that (1) amygdala hyperreactivity underlies emotional hyper- has been predominantly guided by the idea that
reactivity and deficits in (2) prefrontal activity or (3) emotion dysregulation can be mapped onto an imbal-
fronto–limbic connectivity underly emotion regulation deficits. ance between limbic structures, representing an
We offer several suggestions to improve consolidation and emotional response component, and prefrontal struc-
interpretation of research in this area. tures, representing a regulatory component [4]. This
idea is pervasive not only in the field of BPD, but also
Addresses depression, post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and
Department of Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy, Central
maltreatment in general, fitting the notion of emotion
Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg
University, C4, 11, 68159, Mannheim, Germany dysregulation as a transdiagnostic construct. Still, while
the frontoelimbic imbalance hypothesis has high face
Corresponding author: Sicorello, Maurizio (maurizio.sicorello@zi- validity and can be easily utilized in psychoeducation, it
mannheim.de) faces several theoretical and methodological challenges.
Here, we review the current state of the frontoelimbic
Current Opinion in Psychology 2021, 37:114–120
hypothesis of emotion dysregulation in BPD, contextu-
alized with recent insights from affective neuroscience,
This review comes from a themed issue on Personality Pathology:
Developmental Aspects
focusing on task-based functional magnet resonance
imaging (fMRI) research.
Edited by Carla Sharp, Andrew Chanen and Marialuisa Cavelti
For a complete overview see the Issue and the Editorial
The amygdala in emotional reactivity
Available online 14 December 2020
The amygdala is the central limbic target for
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2020.12.002 emotional reactivity. A recent meta-analysis of 24
2352-250X/© 2020 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. studies confirmed that individuals with BPD exhibit
larger amygdala activity in response to threat-related
Keywords imagery than both healthy and depressed samples
Borderline personality disorder, Amygdala, Affective instability, Emotion (but not in the anterior insula or anterior cingulate
regulation, Magnet resonance imaging. cortex [ACC], two other primary regions of interest)
[5]. Amygdala activity has also repeatedly been found
to be increased in BPD while viewing neutral stimuli
Borderline personality disorder (BPD) is characterized
[6]. Moreover, amygdala habituation to repeatedly
by a broad set of emotional disturbances, often sum-
presented negative stimuli is decreased in a larger
marized under the term emotion dysregulation. These
sample of patients with acute and remitted BPD [7].
disturbances include a higher emotional sensitivity (i.e.
Hence, the amygdala appears to be implicated in both
a lower threshold for emotions to occur), higher in-
emotional sensitivity and intensity as well as lowered
tensities of experienced emotions, a slower return to a
habituation. These effects were reduced by dialectical
person’s emotional baseline, and deficits in emotion
behavior therapy [8,9] and intranasal oxytocin treat-
regulation [1]. Emotion dysregulation can already be
ment [6]. Building on these findings, a recent one-
traced back to early childhood [2] and plays a central
armed preepost intervention study reported an
role in developmental etiological models of the disorder

Current Opinion in Psychology 2021, 37:114–120 www.sciencedirect.com


Fronto–limbic imbalance in BPD Sicorello and Schmahl 115

improvement of BPD symptoms after amygdala discrete emotion and extents to nonemotional but
neurofeedback training [10]. relevant experimental events [12]. Furthermore, deep
brain stimulation of the human amygdala does not
Although further replications of the intervention systematically lead to negatively toned emotional ex-
studies above are necessary, the confirmation of periences [13]. Rather, affect and emotions are
amygdala aberrations in BPD during emotion-related empirically best represented by complex activation
tasks by meta-analyses [5] and by a study with larger patterns which span multiple distributed subsystems
sample size [7] is an important achievement for the across the brain [12]. These findings support the long-
field. Still, although amygdala aberrations are standing argument that the amygdala’s main role is a
commonly thought to indicate emotion dysregulation, functionally related combination of relevance detec-
this interpretation needs to be put into context. To tion, associative learning, and response facilitation.
infer psychological functions, a biomarker must be both The view of the amygdala as part of a defensive survival
sensitive and specific to the function of interest circuit ignores its involvement in positive and nonaf-
(reverse inference problem, Box 1). While the amyg- fective states, which is more easily accommodated by
dala does respond sensitively to emotional stimuli, its viewing the amygdala as a co-regulator of allostatic
involvement is not specific to negative affect (i.e. physiological responses more generally [14,15].
similarly involved in positive affect [11]) or any Notably, one well-powered study suggests that amyg-
dala activity does not increase monotonically with
higher negative affective responses, plateauing after
small increases in negative affect and not distinguish-
ing between very high and very low negative affect
Box 1. The reverse inference problem in neuroimaging
research above chance. In contrast, a multivariate pattern
reached over 90% accuracy [16].

Most neuroimaging studies measure brain activity (A) within Even if the amygdala does not allow strong reverse
experimental tasks designed to involve a certain mental inference to negative emotional states in the labo-
process of interest (M). Here, in essence, studies start from ratory due to nonspecificity and non-monotonicity, it
the psychological end to learn something about the brain. might still contribute to psychopathological models
This forward inference is formalized as the probability of
observing brain activity A given mental process M [p(A|M)]
by capturing general differential relevance of affec-
[47]. Usually, however, clinical researchers are interested in tive stimuli such as faces or scenes, the most
using the brain to learn something about a psychological common stimuli in fMRI research. Nevertheless, it is
construct. For example, observing a heightened amygdala unclear whether this is informative about the core
response becomes practically relevant if it indicates a clinical concept of emotion dysregulation. Group comparisons
abnormality (e.g. emotional hyperreactivity), which can be
targeted with psychotropic medication, neurofeedback, or
between BPD patients and healthy controls are
brain stimulation. This reverse inference corresponds to the severely limited, as these groups might differ on
probability of p(M|A), which can be derived using Bayes rule: many dimensions. From a trait perspective, there
pðAjMÞ pðM Þ appears to be no association between amygdala
pðM jAÞ ¼ reactivity and neuroticism [17], which maps on the
pðAÞ
For this probability to be high, ensuring correct inference, the
negative affectivity trait characteristic for BPD [18].
targeted neural activity must be both sensitive and specific to Similarly, there was no relationship between amyg-
the construct of interest. This can be seen in the formula dala reactivity to negative stimuli and affective
above: The unconditional probability to observe activity reactivity in daily life [19].
pattern A is in the denominator of the right-hand side of the
equation. Hence, a larger general probability to observe
activity in the region of interest p(A) is associated with less Another possible explanation for amygdala hyperreac-
certainty to infer a specific mental state p(M|A). To give a tivity in BPD might be a higher relevance of experi-
practical example, while the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex mental stimuli due to past negative experiences. The
(dACC) is involved in cognitive control, it is also involved in previously mentioned meta-analysis found that amyg-
pain and affect. In fact, the dACC is among the regions with
the most significant results across research topics [48].
dala hyperreactivity is present in both BPD and
Therefore, activity in the dACC cannot clearly indicate the PTSD [5], which overlap not only in their phenom-
occurrence of a specific affective mental event. enology, but also the presence of such experiences.
In contrast, machine learning can be used to discover brain Amygdala hyperreactivity is also present in adults with
patterns which predict concepts such as negative affect with adverse childhood experiences [20,21], even in the
high accuracy, even differentiating it from similar but distinct
concepts such as the subjective response to physical pain
absence of psychopathology [22]. In further support,
[16]. These patterns can be reused and shared across labs, amygdala habituation to negative stimuli was related
facilitating the translation from basic to clinical neuroscience. to adverse childhood experiences, but not to BPD
symptom severity or clinical state in a well-powered
study [7].

www.sciencedirect.com Current Opinion in Psychology 2021, 37:114–120


116 Personality Pathology: Developmental Aspects

The prefrontal cortex in emotion regulation effects in the same gross anatomical brain region, even if
deficits the significant voxels are separated by considerable
While the BPD meta-analysis described earlier revealed margins [30]. The dACC peak voxel for emotion regu-
both hyperactive and hypoactive clusters in the pre- lation deficits in BPD are roughly between 2 and 4 cm
frontal cortex (PFC) during emotion processing in BPD apart, which is above the median of most replication
[5], only few fMRI studies have probed neural deficits studies and especially problematic for this particular
during explicit emotion regulation. Most studies brain region: The dACC is unspecifically involved in a
employed cognitive reappraisal strategies such as hy- broad set of processes, including pain, affect, and
pothetical distancing, where participants imagine cognitive control. These different processes are likely
negative scenes as not being real [23]. The dorsal ACC enacted by different adjacent neuron populations,
(dACC) is the prefrontal brain region most consistently making spatial distinctions essential [31].
associated with emotion regulation deficits in BPD and
has been argued to underlie cognitive control abilities. On an epistemological level, interpreting group dif-
Three studies found that the dACC was deactivated, ferences in any prefrontal area as aberrations in
instead of activated, during reappraisal in individuals emotion regulation is a very broad hypothesis with low
with BPD [24e27]. As for the amygdala, these findings falsifiability, as the PFC makes up more than 20% of
were also apparent in healthy but trauma-exposed con- the gray matter volume in humans. Meta-analyses on
trols [25]. Lower ACC recruitment during distancing the neural basis of emotion regulation are becoming
was also associated with a continuous measure of increasingly refined, offering a more constrained hy-
emotion regulation difficulties in BPD [28]. In addition, pothesis space. Powers and LaBar [23] published a
recruitment of the dACC during distancing was meta-analysis of 22 studies on brain regions involved
increased after dialectical behavior therapy in a clinical in distancing together with a neurocognitive model,
trial [29]. mapping psychological aspects on neural structures,
which can be used to more strongly constrain hy-
Although these results might seem consistent at the potheses and interpretations. Figure 1 displays the
first glance, they lack evidence of spatial overlap. The regions they identified, together with prefrontal re-
inference from brain measures to psychological states gions which exhibited reduced activity during emotion
not only requires psychological but also a degree of regulation in BPD. None of these single-study regions
spatial specificity. In neuroimaging, replication success overlapped with the meta-analytical network or each
is predominantly attested when studies find significant other, granting no evidence for prefrontal emotion

Figure 1

Prefrontal regions with lower activity during emotion regulation in BPD. BPD, borderline personality disorder.

Current Opinion in Psychology 2021, 37:114–120 www.sciencedirect.com


Fronto–limbic imbalance in BPD Sicorello and Schmahl 117

regulation deficits in BPD when employing constraints trait emotion regulation in BPD instead of comparing
based on prior neurobiological knowledge (but see the a BPD group to a clinical control group.
figure caption for details and limitations). Importantly,
low statistical power likely plays a major role in these On a psychological level, hypothetical distancing rep-
inconsistent findings, increasing spatial uncertainty resents one of the major emotion regulation strategies
and potentially obscuring smaller but relevant over- used in fMRI research, but a recent review questions its
lapping effects [32]. For between-person hypotheses, practical relevance [23]. While it might be effective in
low testeretest reliability of classic fMRI measures the laboratory to imagine the content of aversive pic-
further exacerbates this problem [33] (but refer tures as not being real (which in fact is often staged), it
Kragel et al. [34]). is therapeutically questionable whether this strategy
also makes sense outside the lab. As a further sign of
This figure depicts prefrontal regions involved in the limited ecological validity, none of the BPD emotion
emotion regulation strategy distancing in a meta- regulation studies found group differences in self-
analysis [23] and prefrontal regions which exhibited reported emotion regulation success during the task
reduced recruitment in BPD during emotion regula- and most did not report group differences in amygdala
tion in single studies. The meta-analytic map was attenuation during regulation.
retrieved from neurovault (https://identifiers.org/
neurovault.collection:8386). Regions of interest from Brain connectivity and fronto–limbic
single studies were created as spheres around reported inhibition
peak voxels while maintaining the same cluster The frontoelimbic imbalance hypothesis is guided by a
volume. Importantly, the original clusters might be causal assumption. Especially the dorsolateral PFC
nonspherical, impeding the inference to overlap be- (dlPFC) is argued to (indirectly) exert inhibitory control
tween regions. This is especially relevant for the over presumably emotion generating regions like the
clusters from van Zutphen et al. [26] and the amygdala during emotion regulation [23,35]. Connec-
distancing meta-analysis, which are relatively close but tivity analyses can provide information on the commu-
nonoverlapping in the figure above. While the clusters nication between regions beyond task-related activation
appear to be approximately spherical in van Zutphen [36] and PFCeamygdala connectivity during emotion
et al. [26], there is still a degree of uncertainty regulation has been argued to underly emotion regula-
concerning overlap. Note, the meta-analysis and single tion success and related traits [37]. Unfortunately, to
studies reported further significant differences in non- our knowledge, none of the around 30 published con-
PFC regions, which are not shown here. Moreover, nectivity studies on BPD-targeted individual differ-
Silvers et al. [28] investigated neural correlates of ences in connectivity during an explicit cognitive

Table 1

Avenues toward consolidation and interpretation.

Recommendation Advantage Paper


suggestions
(References)

1. Use maps from meta-analyses on basic A smaller search space can increase [19]
processes to spatially statistical power and facilitate interpretability.
constrain hypotheses
2. Publish (un-)thresholded group level maps Can be used as regions of interest to [30]
of single experiments conduct spatially precise replications
and improve meta-analytic aggregation
3. Use psychometric principles to develop paradigms which Reduces error variance, which increases effect sizes and [49,50]
capture individual differences statistical power for
between-person effects
4. Use established (or develop novel) machine learning- Facilitates reverse inference. Increases statistical power [51,52]
based brain activity/connectivity patterns which are sen- through a smaller number of tests and higher reliability
sitive and specific to the construct of interest

5. Consider hyperalignment as an additional preprocessing Reduces noise of intrinsic spatial between-person [53]
step variability in brain function (potentially for
the price of lower between-study comparability
in some instances)

www.sciencedirect.com Current Opinion in Psychology 2021, 37:114–120


118 Personality Pathology: Developmental Aspects

emotion regulation paradigm to provide a strong test of and necessary for the scientific progress, especially
this hypothesis. concerning the brain basis of complex traits. There is,
however, much than can be done. In Table 1, we high-
A considerable number of studies looked at connectivity light several suggestions how research on emotion
during rest, with some reporting decreased medial dysregulation in BPD might be refined and consoli-
PFCeamygdala connectivity [38e40], whereas others dated. Notably, most of these suggestions are applicable
did not find this effect [41,42]. Still, a basis to infer even to existing data sets. Above all, building neuro-
deficits in specific clinical phenomena is needed, as the pathological models from basic affective neuroscience
role of the medial PFC in emotion regulation has been and emotion dysregulation as a dimensional trans-
argued to be less about regulation and more about the diagnostic will be vital.
representation and transmission of information on af-
fective states [35,43]. Especially, the relationship be- Credit author statement
tween resting state connectivity and trait emotion Maurizio Sicorello: Conceptualization, Writing - Orig-
regulation deficits is still unclear [44]. inal Draft, Visualization; Christian Schmahl: Writing -
Review & Editing, Supervision.
As for neural activity, these inconsistencies might be
explained by the very low reliability of resting state con-
nectivity [45], which can be substantially alleviated by Conflict of interest statement
engaging participants in a task [46]. Nevertheless, even Nothing declared.
when broad frontoelimbic connectivity aberrations are
identified during emotional tasks, interpretation has to be References
Papers of particular interest, published within the period of review,
guided by prior knowledge on neural correlates of emotion have been highlighted as:
regulation states and traits. Hence, as for prefrontal ac-
* of special interest
tivity, the hypotheses that frontoelimbic inhibition un- * * of outstanding interest
derlies emotion regulation deficits has to be constrained
spatially (i.e. where in the PFC), directionally (i.e. 1. Ebner-Priemer UW, Houben M, Santangelo P, Kleindienst N,
Tuerlinckx F, Oravecz Z, Verleysen G, Van Deun K, Bohus M,
negative or positive, higher or lower), and psychologically Kuppens P: Unraveling affective dysregulation in borderline
(i.e. which dimensional construct) to be informative. personality disorder: a theoretical model and empirical evi-
dence. J Abnorm Psychol 2015, 124:186–198, https://doi.org/
10.1037/abn0000021.
2. Stepp SD, Scott LN, Jones NP, Whalen DJ, Hipwell AE: Negative
Conclusions and future directions emotional reactivity as a marker of vulnerability in the
development of borderline personality disorder symptoms.
Frontoelimbic imbalance is a central hypothesis to Dev Psychopathol 2016, 28:213–224, https://doi.org/10.1017/
explain emotion dysregulation, a core transdiagnostic S0954579415000395.
dimension of psychopathology. Here, we argue that 3. Crowell SE, Beauchaine TP, Linehan MM: A biosocial devel-
amygdala hyperactivity in BPD is a credible and rela- opmental model of borderline personality: elaborating and
extending Linehan's theory. Psychol Bull 2009, 135:495–510,
tively consistent research finding, but currently lacks https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015616.
the empirical basis to confidently infer emotional hy-
4. Brendel GR, Stern E, Silbersweig DA: Defining the neuro-
perreactivity on a psychological level. Rather, the hy- circuitry of borderline personality disorder: functional
perreactivity of this valence-unspecific brain region neuroimaging approaches. Dev Psychopathol 2005, 17:
1197–1206, https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457940505056X.
might also be parsimoniously explained by a heightened
psychological relevance of the experimental stimuli, 5. Schulze L, Schulze A, Renneberg B, Schmahl C, Niedtfeld I:
** Neural correlates of affective disturbances: a comparative
which relate to past adverse experiences. This high- meta-analysis of negative affect processing in borderline
lights the need for dimensional approaches, as BPD personality disorder, major depressive disorder, and post-
traumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatr Cogn Neurosci
patients can differ from healthy controls in many ways. Neuroimag 2019, 4:220–232, https://doi.org/10.1016/
Establishing prefrontal deficits, in turn, is hindered by j.bpsc.2018.11.004.
Meta-analysis on neural differences and commonalities between BPD,
the lack of spatial specificity concerning hypotheses and PTSD, and depression during emotion processing.
findings. Taken together, there is currently only weak
6. Lischke A, Herpertz SC, Berger C, Domes G, Gamer M: Diver-
evidence that a frontoelimbic imbalance underlies gent effects of oxytocin on (para-)limbic reactivity to
emotion dysregulation in BPD. emotional and neutral scenes in females with and without
borderline personality disorder. Soc Cognit Affect Neurosci
2017, 12:1783–1792, https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsx107.
Importantly, the issues we pointed out do not fully
7. Bilek E, Itz ML, Stößel G, Ma R, Berhe O, Clement L, Zang Z,
disagree with the idea of a frontoelimbic imbalance. * Robnik L, Plichta MM, Neukel C, Schmahl C, Kirsch P, Meyer-
They do, however, point toward crucial challenges for Lindenberg A, Tost H: Deficient amygdala habituation to
the establishment and interpretation of neuropatho- threatening stimuli in borderline personality disorder relates
to adverse childhood experiences. Biol Psychiatr 2019, 86:
logical models. These include limited testeretest reli- 930–938, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.06.008.
ability [33,45], spatial imprecision [30], and ambiguous Well-powered study on acute and remitted BPD patients finding that
deficits in amygdala habituation are related to adverse childhood ex-
interpretations [47]. Such challenges are both inherent periences, but not psychopathology.

Current Opinion in Psychology 2021, 37:114–120 www.sciencedirect.com


Fronto–limbic imbalance in BPD Sicorello and Schmahl 119

8. Goodman M, Carpenter D, Tang CY, Goldstein KE, Avedon J, Psychopathol 2018, 30:1475–1485, https://doi.org/10.1017/
Fernandez N, Mascitelli KA, Blair NJ, New AS, Triebwasser J, S0954579417001717.
Siever LJ, Hazlett EA: Dialectical behavior therapy alters
emotion regulation and amygdala activity in patients with 22. Dannlowski U, Stuhrmann A, Beutelmann V, Zwanzger P,
borderline personality disorder. J Psychiatr Res 2014, 57: Lenzen T, Grotegerd D, Domschke K, Hohoff C, Ohrmann P,
108–116, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.06.020. Bauer J, Lindner C, Postert C, Konrad C, Arolt V, Heindel W,
Suslow T, Kugel H: Limbic scars: long-term consequences of
9. Winter D, Niedtfeld I, Schmitt R, Bohus M, Schmahl C, childhood maltreatment revealed by functional and structural
Herpertz SC: Neural correlates of distraction in borderline magnetic resonance imaging. Biol Psychiatr 2012, 71:286–293,
personality disorder before and after dialectical behavior https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.10.021.
therapy. Eur Arch Psychiatr Clin Neurosci 2017, 267:51–62,
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-016-0689-2. 23. Powers JP, LaBar KS: Regulating emotion through distancing:
* * a taxonomy, neurocognitive model, and supporting meta-
10. Zaehringer J, Ende G, Santangelo P, Kleindienst N, Ruf M, analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2019, 96:155–173, https://
Bertsch K, Bohus M, Schmahl C, Paret C: Improved emotion doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.04.023.
regulation after neurofeedback: a single-arm trial in patients Comprehensive psychological and neurobiological account of the
with borderline personality disorder. NeuroImage Clin 2019, emotion regulation strategy distancing, providing a strong fundament
24:102032, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2019.102032. for further research
11. Lindquist KA, Satpute AB, Wager TD, Weber J, Barrett LF: The 24. Koenigsberg HW, Fan J, Ochsner KN, Liu X, Guise KG,
brain basis of positive and negative affect: evidence from a Pizzarello S, Dorantes C, Guerreri S, Tecuta L, Goodman M,
meta-analysis of the human neuroimaging literature. Cerebr New A, Siever LJ: Neural correlates of the use of psycholog-
Cortex 2016, 26:1910–1922, https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/ ical distancing to regulate responses to negative social
bhv001. Cues: a study of patients with borderline personality disor-
der. Biol Psychiatr 2009, 66:854–863, https://doi.org/10.1016/
12. Kragel PA, LaBar KS: Decoding the nature of emotion in the j.biopsych.2009.06.010.
brain. Trends Cognit Sci 2016, 20:444–455, https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.tics.2016.03.011. 25. Lang S, Kotchoubey B, Frick C, Spitzer C, Grabe HJ, Barnow S:
Cognitive reappraisal in trauma-exposed women with
13. Inman CS, Bijanki KR, Bass DI, Gross RE, Hamann S, Willie JT: borderline personality disorder. Neuroimage 2012, 59:
* Human amygdala stimulation effects on emotion physiology 1727–1734, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.061.
and emotional experience. Neuropsychologia 2018:106722,
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.03.019. 26. van Zutphen L, Siep N, Jacob GA, Domes G, Sprenger A,
Clear association between amygdala deep brain stimulation and * Willenborg B, Goebel R, Arntz A: Always on guard: emotion
physiological arousal, but not self-reported affect or emotion regulation in women with borderline personality disorder
compared to nonpatient controls and patients with cluster-C
14. Kleckner IR, Zhang J, Touroutoglou A, Chanes L, Xia C, personality disorder. J Psychiatry Neurosci 2018, 43:37–47,
Simmons WK, Quigley KS, Dickerson BC, Feldman Barrett L: https://doi.org/10.1503/jpn.170008.
Evidence for a large-scale brain system supporting allostasis Largest fMRI study employing the emotion regulation task in BPD.
and interoception in humans. Nat Hum Behav 2017, 1, https:// Based their emotion regulation instruction on schema therapy.
doi.org/10.1038/s41562-017-0069.
27. Schulze L, Domes G, Krüger A, Berger C, Fleischer M, Prehn K,
15. Taschereau-Dumouchel V, Kawato M, Lau H: Multivoxel pattern Schmahl C, Grossmann A, Hauenstein K, Herpertz SC: Neuronal
* * analysis reveals dissociations between subjective fear and correlates of cognitive reappraisal in borderline patients with
its physiological correlates. Mol Psychiatr 2019, https://doi.org/ affective instability. Biol Psychiatr 2011, 69:564–573, https://
10.1038/s41380-019-0520-3. doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.10.025.
Amygdala activation patterns are highly predictive of physiological
arousal in response to threatening pictures, but contain little informa- 28. Silvers JA, Hubbard AD, Biggs E, Shu J, Fertuck E, Chaudhury S,
tion on subjective fear ratings Grunebaum MF, Weber J, Kober H, Chesin M, Brodsky BS,
Koenigsberg H, Ochsner KN, Stanley B: Affective lability and
16. Chang LJ, Gianaros PJ, Manuck SB, Krishnan A, Wager TD: difficulties with regulation are differentially associated with
A sensitive and specific neural signature for picture-induced amygdala and prefrontal response in women with Borderline
negative affect. PLoS Biol 2015, 13:1–28, https://doi.org/ Personality Disorder. Psychiatry Res - Neuroimaging 2016, 254:
10.1371/journal.pbio.1002180. 74–82, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2016.06.009.
17. Servaas MN, van der Velde J, Costafreda SG, Horton P, Ormel J, 29. Schmitt R, Winter D, Niedtfeld I, Herpertz SC, Schmahl C: Effects
Riese H, Aleman A: Neuroticism and the brain: a quantitative of Psychotherapy on neuronal correlates of reappraisal in
meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies investigating emotion female patients with borderline personality disorder. Biol
processing. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2013, 37:1518–1529, Psychiatr Cogn Neurosci Neuroimag 2016, 1:548–557, https://
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.05.005. doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2016.07.003.
18. Gore WL, Widiger TA: Negative emotionality across diagnostic 30. Hong YW, Yoo Y, Han J, Wager TD, Woo CW: False-positive
* * models: RDoC, DSM-5 section III, and FFM. Personal. Disord * * neuroimaging: Undisclosed flexibility in testing spatial hy-
Theory Res Treat 2018, 9:155–164, https://doi.org/10.1037/ potheses allows presenting anything as a replicated finding.
per0000273. Neuroimage 2019, 195:384–395, https://doi.org/10.1016/
Striking overlap between negative emotionality traits of different major j.neuroimage.2019.03.070.
clinical and personality models Points towards crucial shortcomings concerning replication practices in
neuromaging studies and suggests best practices
19. Bastiaansen JA, Bennik EC, Marsman JBC, Ormel J, Aleman A,
* Oldehinkel AJ: Prefrontal cortex activation during a cognitive 31. Kragel PA, Kano M, Van Oudenhove L, Ly HG, Dupont P,
reappraisal task is associated with real-life negative affect Rubio A, Delon-Martin C, Bonaz BL, Manuck SB, Gianaros PJ,
reactivity. PloS One 2018, 13:1–17, https://doi.org/10.1371/ Ceko M, Reynolds Losin EA, Woo C-W, Nichols TE, Wager TD:
journal.pone.0202888. Generalizable representations of pain, cognitive control, and
Combining an fMRI emotion regulation task with affective functioning in negative emotion in medial frontal cortex. Nat Neurosci 2018,
daily life, based on spatially constrained a priori regions 21:283–289, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-017-0051-7.
20. Hein TC, Monk CS: Research Review: neural response to 32. Bossier H, Roels SP, Seurinck R, Banaschewski T, Barker GJ,
threat in children, adolescents, and adults after child Bokde ALW, Quinlan EB, Desrivières S, Flor H, Grigis A,
maltreatment - a quantitative meta-analysis. J Child Psychol Garavan H, Gowland P, Heinz A, Ittermann B, Martinot JL,
Psychiatr 2017, 58:222–230, https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12651. Artiges E, Nees F, Orfanos DP, Poustka L, Fröhner Dipl-
Psych JH, Smolka MN, Walter H, Whelan R, Schumann G,
21. Heany SJ, Groenewold NA, Uhlmann A, Dalvie S, Stein DJ,
Moerkerke B: The empirical replicability of task-based fMRI as
Brooks SJ: The neural correlates of Childhood Trauma
a function of sample size. Neuroimage 2020, 212:1–12, https://
Questionnaire scores in adults: a meta-analysis and review
doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116601.
of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Dev

www.sciencedirect.com Current Opinion in Psychology 2021, 37:114–120


120 Personality Pathology: Developmental Aspects

33. Elliott ML, Knodt AR, Ireland D, Morris ML, Poulton R, 2014, 44:2889–2901, https://doi.org/10.1017/
* Ramrakha S, Sison ML, Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Hariri AR: What is S0033291714000324.
the test-retest reliability of common task-functional MRI
measures? New empirical evidence and a meta-analysis. 43. Buhle JT, Silvers JA, Wager TD, Lopez R, Onyemekwu C,
Psychol Sci 2020, 31:792–806, https://doi.org/10.1177/ Kober H, Webe J, Ochsner KN: Cognitive reappraisal of
0956797620916786. emotion: a meta-analysis of human neuroimaging studies,
Influential paper revealing low test-retest reliability of single voxels and Cereb. Cortex 2014, 24:2981–2990, https://doi.org/10.1093/
regions of interest in task-based fMRI, including the amygdala. But cercor/bht154.
note the rebuttal below.
44. Dörfel D, Gärtner A, Scheffel C: Resting state cortico-limbic
34. Kragel P, Han X, Kraynak T, Gianaros P, Wager T: fMRI can be * * functional connectivity and dispositional use of emotion
highly reliable, but it depends on what you measure (v2). regulation strategies : a replication and extension study (v4).
PsyArXiv 2020, https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/9eaxk. PsyArXiv 2020, https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/tc58u.
Preregistered replication study finding no relationship between resting
35. Kohn N, Eickhoff SB, Scheller M, Laird AR, Fox PT, Habel U: state frontolimbic connectivity and emotion regulation success on a
Neural network of cognitive emotion regulation - an ALE neural, behavioral, or self-report level
meta-analysis and MACM analysis. Neuroimage 2014, 87:
345–355, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.001. 45. Noble S, Scheinost D, Constable RT: A decade of test-retest
* reliability of functional connectivity: a systematic review and
36. Gerchen MF, Kirsch P: Combining task-related activation and meta-analysis. Neuroimage 2019, 203:116157, https://doi.org/
connectivity analysis of fMRI data reveals complex modula- 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116157.
tion of brain networks. Hum Brain Mapp 2017, 38:5726–5739, Reviews low test-retest reliability of functional connectivity measures
https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23762.
46. Greene AS, Gao S, Scheinost D, Constable RT: Task-induced
37. Paschke LM, Dörfel D, Steimke R, Trempler I, Magrabi A, brain state manipulation improves prediction of individual
Ludwig VU, Schubert T, Stelzel C, Walter H: Individual differ- traits. Nat Commun 2018, 9, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-018-
ences in self-reported self-control predict successful 04920-3.
emotion regulation. Soc Cognit Affect Neurosci 2016, 11:
1193–1204, https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw036. 47. Poldrack RA: Inferring mental states from neuroimaging data:
from reverse inference to large-scale decoding. Neuron 2011,
38. Baczkowski BM, van Zutphen L, Siep N, Jacob GA, Domes G, 72:692–697, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2011.11.001.
Maier S, Sprenger A, Senft A, Willenborg B, Tüscher O, Arntz A,
van de Ven V: Deficient amygdala–prefrontal intrinsic con- 48. Yarkoni T, Poldrack RA, Nichols TE, Van Essen DC, Wager TD:
nectivity after effortful emotion regulation in borderline per- Large-scale automated synthesis of human functional
sonality disorder. Eur Arch Psychiatr Clin Neurosci 2017, 267: neuroimaging data. Nat Methods 2011, 8:665–670, https://
551–565, https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-016-0760-z. doi.org/10.1038/nmeth.1635.

39. Balducci T, González-Olvera JJ, Angeles-Valdez D, Espinoza- 49. McDermott TJ, Kirlic N, Aupperle RL: Roadmap for optimizing
Luna I, Garza-Villarreal EA: Borderline personality disorder the clinical utility of emotional stress paradigms in human
with cocaine dependence: impulsivity, emotional dysregula- neuroimaging research. Neurobiol Stress 2018, 8:134–146,
tion and amygdala functional connectivity. Front Psychiatr https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ynstr.2018.05.001.
2018, 9:1–12, https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00328.
50. Enkavi AZ, Poldrack RA: Implications of the lacking relation-
40. Reich DB, Belleau EL, Temes CM, Gonenc A, Pizzagalli DA, ship between cognitive task and self report measures for
Gruber SA: Amygdala resting state connectivity differences psychiatry. Biol Psychiatr Cogn Neurosci Neuroimag 2020,
between bipolar II and borderline personality disorders. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2020.06.010.
Neuropsychobiology 2019, 78:229–237, https://doi.org/10.1159/
51. Woo CW, Chang LJ, Lindquist MA, Wager TD: Building better
000502440.
biomarkers: brain models in translational neuroimaging. Nat
41. Salvador R, Vega D, Pascual JC, Marco J, Canales- Neurosci 2017, 20:365–377, https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4478.
Rodríguez EJ, Aguilar S, Anguera M, Soto A, Ribas J, Soler J,
52. Kragel PA, Koban L, Barrett LF, Wager TD: Representation,
Maristany T, Rodríguez-Fornells A, Pomarol-Clotet E: * * pattern information, and brain signatures: from neurons to
Converging medial frontal resting state and diffusion-based
neuroimaging. Neuron 2018, 99:257–273, https://doi.org/
Abnormalities in borderline personality disorder. Biol
10.1016/j.neuron.2018.06.009.
Psychiatr 2016, 79:107–116, https://doi.org/10.1016/
Excellent overview of the philosophy and methods of pattern-based
j.biopsych.2014.08.026.
fMRI analyses
42. Krause-Utz A, Veer IM, Rombouts SARB, Bohus M, Schmahl C,
53. Haxby JV, Guntupalli JS, Nastase SA, Feilong M: Hyperalign-
Elzinga BM: Amygdala and anterior cingulate resting-state
ment: Modeling shared information encoded in idiosyncratic
functional connectivity in borderline personality disorder
cortical topographies. Elife 2020, 9:1–26, https://doi.org/
patients with a history of interpersonal trauma. Psychol Med
10.7554/eLife.56601.

Current Opinion in Psychology 2021, 37:114–120 www.sciencedirect.com

You might also like