You are on page 1of 8

Living in a

Dream World
Daydreaming can help solve problems, trigger
creativity, and inspire great works of art and
science. When it becomes compulsive, however,
the consequences can be dire
By Josie Glausiusz

W
hen Rachel Stein (not her real name) was a small child,
she would pace around in a circle shaking a string for
hours at a time, mentally spinning intricate alterna-
tive plots for her favorite television shows. Usually she
was the star— the imaginary seventh child in The Bra-
dy Bunch, for example. “Around the age of eight or nine, my older
brother said, ‘You’re doing this on the front lawn, and the neighbors
are looking at you. You just can’t do it anymore,’ ” Stein recalls. So
she retreated to her bedroom, reveling in would be bad if I got caught up in a sto-
her elaborate reveries alone. As she grew ry, because then I couldn’t go back to
older, the television shows changed — sleep.” By the time she was 17, Stein was
i rst General Hospital, then The West exhausted. “I love the daydreams, but I
P H o t o i l l u S t r At i o n B Y A A r o n G o o d m A n

Wing — but her intense need to immerse just felt it was consuming my real life. I
herself in her imaginary world did not. went to parties with friends, but I just
“There were periods in my life when couldn’t wait to get home. There was
daydreaming just took over everything,” nothing else that I wanted to do as much
she recalls. “I was not in control.” She as daydreaming.”
would retreat into fantasy “any waking Convinced that she was crazy, she
moment when I could get away with it. It consulted six different therapists, none of
was the i rst thing I wanted to do when I whom could i nd anything wrong with
woke up in the morning. When I woke her. The seventh prescribed Prozac, which
up in the night to go to the bathroom, it had no effect. Eventually Stein began tak-

24 Scientific AmericAn mind m a r c h/A p r il 2 01 1

© 2011 Scientific American


w w w. S c i e nti f i c A m e r i c an .c o m/m in d Scientific AmericAn mind 25
© 2011 Scientific American
ing another antidepressant, Luvox, fantasies may be followed by feelings of ogy professor Jerome L. Singer deines
which, like Prozac, is also a selective se- dread and shame, and they may compare daydreaming as shifting attention “away
rotonin reuptake inhibitor but is usually the habit to a drug or describe an experi- from some primary physical or mental
prescribed for obsessive-compulsive dis- ence akin to drowning in honey. task toward an unfolding sequence of
order. Gradually she brought her day- The recent discovery of a network in private responses” or, more simply,
dreaming under control. Now age 37, the brain dedicated to autobiographical “watching your own mental videos.”
she is a successful lawyer, still nervously mental imagery is helping researchers un- The 86-year-old Singer, who published a
guarding her secret world. derstand the multiple purposes that day- lyrical account of his decades of research

Most people spend about 30 percent of their waking hours


spacing out, drifting off, lost in thought, woolgathering— or,
as one scientist put it, “watching your own mental videos.”
The scientiic study of people such as dreaming serves in our lives. They have on daydreams in his 1975 book, The In-
Stein is helping researchers better under- dubbed this web of neurons “the default ner World of Daydreaming (Harper &
stand the role of daydreaming in normal network,” because when we are not ab- Row), divides daydreaming styles into
consciousness — and what can happen sorbed in more focused tasks, the network two main categories: “positive-construc-
when this process becomes unhealthy. ires up. The default network appears to tive,” which includes upbeat and imagi-
For most of us, daydreaming is a virtual be essential to generating our sense of self, native thoughts, and “dysphoric,” which
world where we can rehearse the future, suggesting that daydreaming plays a cru- encompasses visions of failure or punish-
explore fearful scenarios or imagine new cial role in who we are and how we inte- ment. Most people experience both kinds
adventures without risk. It can help us de- grate the outside world into our inner lives. to a small or large degree.
vise creative solutions to problems or Cognitive psychologists are now also ex- Other scientists distinguish between
prompt us, while immersed in one task, amining how brain disease may impair mundane musings and extravagant fan-
with reminders of other important goals. our ability to meander mentally and what tasies. Michael Kane, a cognitive psy-
For others, however, the draw of an alter- the consequences are when we just spend chologist at the University of North Car-
native reality borders on addiction, chok- too much time, well, out to lunch. olina at Greensboro, considers “mind
ing off other aspects of everyday life, in- wandering” to be “any thoughts that are
cluding relationships and work. Starring Videos in the mind’s eye unrelated to one’s task at hand.” In his
as idealized versions of themselves — as Most people spend about 30 percent view, mind wandering is a broad catego-
royalty, raconteurs and saviors in a com- of their waking hours spacing out, drift- ry that may include everything from pon-
plex, ever changing cast of characters — ing off, lost in thought, woolgathering, dering ingredients for a dinner recipe to
addictive daydreamers may feel en- in a brown study or building castles in saving the planet from alien invasion.
hanced conidence and validation. Their the air. Yale University emeritus psychol- Most of the time when people fall into
mind wandering, they are thinking about
everyday concerns, such as recent en-
FAST FACTS counters and items on their to-do list.
inner World More exotic daydreams in the style of
James Thurber’s grandiose ictional fan-

1>> daydreams are an inner world where we can rehearse the future and
imagine new adventures without risk. Allowing the mind to roam
freely can aid creativity— but only if we pay attention to the content of
tasist Walter Mitty— such as Mitty’s
dream of piloting an eight-engine hydro-
plane through a hurricane— are rare.
our daydreams. Humdrum concerns igured promi-
nently in one study that rigorously mea-

2>> neuroscientists have identiied the “default network”— a web of


brain regions that become active when we mentally drift away from
the task at hand into our own reveries.
sured how much time we spend mind
wandering in daily life. In a 2009 study
Kane and his colleague Jennifer McVay
asked 72 U.N.C. students to carry

3>> When daydreaming turns addictive and compulsive, it can over-


whelm normal functioning, impeding relationships and work.
PalmPilots that beeped at random inter-
vals eight times a day for a week. The
subjects then recorded their thoughts at
that moment on a questionnaire. About

26 Scientific AmericAn mind m a r c h/A p r il 2 01 1

© 2011 Scientific American


30 percent of the beeps coincided with A Key to creativity People’s minds typically wander to everyday
thoughts unrelated to the task at hand. Artists and scientists are well ac- concerns, such as conversations with co-
workers or errands they have to run. more
Mind wandering increased with stress, quainted with such playful fantasizing. elaborate fantasies—such as Walter mitty’s
boredom or sleepiness or in chaotic envi- Orhan Pamuk, the Turkish novelist who of piloting a plane—are far less common.
ronments and decreased with enjoyable won the Nobel Prize in Literature in
tasks. That may be because enjoyable ac- 2006, imagined “another world,” to be beneicial. A painful procedure in a
tivities tend to grab our attention. which he retreated as a child, where he doctor’s ofice, for example, can be made
Intense focus on our problems may was “someone else, somewhere else ... in less distressing by visualizations of
not always lead to immediate solutions. my grandmother’s sitting room, I’d pre- soothing scenes from childhood.
Instead allowing the mind to loat freely tend to be inside a submarine.” Albert Yet to enhance creativity, it is impor-
can enable us to access unconscious Einstein pictured himself running along tant to pay attention to daydreams.
ideas hovering beneath the surface — a a light wave—a reverie that led to his the- Schooler calls this “tuning out” or delib-
process that can lead to creative insight, ory of special relativity. Filmmaker Tim erate “off-task thinking.” In an as yet un-
according to psychologist Jonathan Burton daydreamed his way to Holly- published study, he and his colleague Jon-
Schooler of the University of California, wood success, spending his childhood athan Smallwood asked 122 undergrad-
Santa Barbara. holed up in his bedroom, creating posters uates at the University of British Columbia
We may not even be aware that we for an imaginary horror ilm series. to read a children’s story and press a but-
are daydreaming. We have all had the ex- Why should daydreaming aid cre- ton each time they caught themselves
periencing of “reading” a book yet ab- ativity? It may be in part because the tuning out. The researchers also periodi-
sorbing nothing— moving our eyes over waking brain is never really at rest. As
the words on a page as our attention psychologist Eric Klinger of the Univer-
(The Author)
wanders and the text turns into gibber- sity of Minnesota explains, loating in
ish. “People oftentimes don’t realize that unfocused mental space serves an evolu- JoSie GlAuSiuSz is a science jour-
they’re daydreaming while they’re day- tionary purpose: when we are engaged nalist who has written for Nature,
dreaming; they lack what I call ‘meta- with one task, mind wandering can trig- National Geographic, Discover and
awareness,’ consciousness of what is cur- ger reminders of other, concurrent goals Wired, among other publications.
rently going on in their minds,” he says. so that we do not lose sight of them. She is co-author, with photographer
Aimless rambling across the moors of Some researchers believe that increasing Volker Steger, of Buzz: The Intimate
G e t t Y i m AG e S

our imaginings may allow us to stumble the amount of imaginative daydreaming Bond between Humans and Insects
on ideas and associations that we may we do or replaying variants of the mil- (chronicle, 2004).
never ind if we strive to seek them. lions of events we store in our brains can

w w w. S c i e nti f i c A m e r i c an .c o m/m in d Scientific AmericAn mind 27


© 2011 Scientific American
od, two researchers at the University of
Lancaster in England, conducted a re-
cent meta-analysis of studies of these
brief reveries. They found that people
who engaged in a mildly demanding
task, such as reading, during a break
from, say, a visual assignment, such as
the hat-rack problem— in which partici-
pants have to construct a sturdy hat rack
using two boards and a clamp — did bet-
ter on that problem than those who did
nothing at all. They also scored higher
than those engaged in a highly demand-
ing task— such as mentally rotating
shapes — during the interval. Allowing
our minds to ramble during a moderately
challenging task, it seems, enables us to
access ideas not easily available to our
conscious minds or to combine these in-
sights in original ways. Our ability to do
so is now known to depend on the nor-
mal functioning of a dedicated day-
dreaming network deep in our brain.

the mental matrix of fantasy


Like Facebook for the brain, the de-
fault network is a bustling web of mem-
ories and streaming movies, starring
ourselves. “When we daydream, we’re
at the center of the universe,” says neu-
rologist Marcus Raichle of Washington
University in St. Louis, who first de-
scribed the network in 2001. It consists
of three main regions: the medial pre-
frontal cortex, the posterior cingulate
cortex and the parietal cortex. The me-
dial prefrontal cortex helps us imagine
ourselves and the thoughts and feelings
of others; the posterior cingulate cortex
draws personal memories from the
if you are faced with a dificult decision, try not to think about the problem for a while.
instead do something else while letting your mind ramble. You may get a lash of insight
brain; and the parietal cortex has major
from your subconscious that will lead you in the right direction. connections with the hippocampus,
which stores episodic memories — what
cally interrupted the students as they longer and more creative list. “You need we ate for breakfast, say— but not imper-
were reading and asked them if they were to have the mind-wandering process,” sonal facts, such as the capital of Kyr-
“zoning out” or drifting off without be- Schooler explains, “but you also need to gyzstan. “The default mode network is
ing aware of it. “What we ind is that the have meta-awareness to say, ‘That’s a cre- critical to the establishment of a sense of
W i l l dAt e n e a g e f o t o s t o c k

people who regularly catch themselves— ative idea that popped into my mind.’” self,” Raichle says.
who notice when they’re doing it— seem The mind’s freedom to wander dur- It was not until 2007, however, that
to be the most creative,” Schooler says. ing a period of deliberate tuning out cognitive psychologist Malia Fox Mason,
They score higher on a standard test of could also explain the lash of insight now at Columbia University, discovered
creativity, in which they are asked to de- that may pop into a person’s head when that the default network—which lights up
scribe all the uses of a common object he or she takes a break from an unsolved when people switch from an attention-
such as a brick; high scorers compile a problem. Ut Na Sio and Thomas Ormer- demanding activity to drifting reveries

28 Scientific AmericAn mind m a r c h/A p r il 2 01 1

© 2011 Scientific American


with no speciic goal— becomes more ac- fault network activity shot up. Periodi- coming from. People with schizophrenia
tive when people engage in a monotonous cally the investigators also interrupted daydream normally most of the time,
verbal task, when they are more likely to the subjects and asked them if they had but when they are ill, “they often com-
mind wander. In an experiment, partici- zoned out. Again, activity in the default plain that someone is reading their mind
pants were shown a string of four letters network was higher in the seconds be- or that someone is putting thoughts in
such as R H V X for one second, which fore the moment they were caught in the their head,” Williamson says.
was then replaced by an arrow pointing act. Notably, activity was strongest On the other hand, those who rumi-
either left or right, to indicate whether the when people were unaware that they nate obsessively— rehashing past events,
sequence should be read forwards or had lost their focus. “The more complex repetitively analyzing their causes and
backwards. When one of the characters your mind-wandering episode is, the consequences, or worrying about all the
in the string appeared, subjects were more of your mind it’s going to con- ways things could go wrong in the fu-
asked to indicate its position (irst, sec- sume,” Smallwood says. ture — are well aware that their thoughts
ond, third or last, depending on the di- Defects in the default network may are their own, but they have intense dif-

“What we ind is that the people who regularly catch


themselves [daydreaming]—who notice when they’re doing
it— seem to be the most creative,” one psychologist says.
rection of the arrow). The more the par- also impair our ability to daydream. A iculty turning them off. Yale psycholo-
ticipants practiced on each of the four range of disorders — including schizo- gist Susan Nolen-Hoeksema does not
original letter strings, the better they per- phrenia and depression — have been believe that rumination is a form of day-
formed. They were then given a novel linked to malfunctions in the default dreaming, which she deines as “imagin-
task, consisting of letter sequences they network in recent years. In a 2007 study ing situations in the future that are large-
had not seen before. Activity in the de- neuroscientist Peter Williamson of the ly positive in tone.” Nevertheless, she
fault network went down during the nov- University of Western Ontario found has found that in obsessive ruminators,
el version of the test. Subjects who day- that people with schizophrenia have def- who are at greater risk of depression, the
dreamed more in everyday life — as deter- icits in the medial prefrontal cortex, same default network circuitry turns on
mined by a questionnaire — also showed which is associated with self-relection. that is activated when we daydream.
greater activity in the default network In patients experiencing hallucinations, These ruminators— who may repeat-
during the monotonous original task. the medial prefrontal cortex dropped edly scrutinize faux pas, family issues or
Mason did not directly measure out of the network altogether. Although lovers’ betrayals — have trouble switch-
mind wandering during the scans, how- the patients were thinking, they could ing off the default network when asked
ever, so she could not determine exactly not be sure where the thoughts were to focus mentally on a neutral image,
when subjects were “on task” and when
they were daydreaming. In 2009 Small-
wood, Schooler and Kalina Christoff of
the University of British Columbia pub-
lished the first study to directly link
mind wandering with increased activity
in the default network. The researchers
scanned the brains of 15 U.B.C. students
while they performed a simple task in
which they were shown random num-
bers from zero to nine. Each was asked
to push a button when he or she saw any
number except three. In the seconds be-
fore making an error— a key sign that an
individual’s attention had drifted — de-
AG e f o t o S t o c K

daydreaming can serve as a useful distrac-


tion. for example, conjuring up soothing
scenes from the past could make a visit
to the doctor more bearable.

w w w. S c i e nti f i c A m e r i c an .c o m/m in d Scientific AmericAn mind 29


© 2011 Scientific American
such as a truckload of watermelons. When daydreaming “I learned to socialize internally with ic-
They may spend hours going over some Becomes a drug tional characters I get along with,” she
past incident, asking themselves how it “I’m like an alcoholic with an un- says. She could engage them in intellec-
could have happened and why they did limited supply of booze everywhere I tual debate, whereas “socializing with
not react differently and end up feeling go,” says Cordellia Amethyste Rose. A outside people frustrates me. They all
overwhelmed instead of searching for 30-year-old computer science student in want to talk about the silliest things.”
solutions. Experimental studies have Oregon, she started an online forum Rose says that she has no friends, but
shown that positive distraction— for ex- called Wild Minds (http://wildminds. on Wild Minds she has found her peers.
ample, exercise and social activities — ning.com) for people who simply cannot Many people posting to the site express
can help ruminators reappraise their sit- stop daydreaming. Since childhood, relief that they have found others like
uation, as can techniques for cultivating Rose has conjured up countless imagi- themselves, emerging from a cocoon of
mindfulness that teach individuals to nary characters in ever changing plots. loneliness and shame to share their ex-
pay precise attention to activities such as “They’ve grown right along with me, periences: misdiagnoses, lack of under-
breathing or walking, rather than to had children; some have died,” she says. standing from families and therapists,
thoughts. Yet people who daydream ex- The deeper she delved into her virtual and rituals like the one described by a
cessively may have the same problems ig- world, though, the more distressed she quiet girl who spends “endless hours”
noring their thoughts once they get go- became. “I couldn’t pay attention for swaying in a rocking chair listening to
ing. Indeed, extreme daydreamers ind more than a split second. I would look at music, daydreaming her life away. “It’s
their private world so dificult to escape a book and zone out after every word.” like a drug, poisoning and destroying
that they describe it as an addiction — Even so, she found her invented compan- your life,” says one anonymous fanta-
one as enslaving as heroin. ions more compelling than anyone real. sist, who admits to bingeing for days on
a story line. “It’s even worse, because an
addict can put a drug down and walk
away. You can’t put down your mind
and walk away from it.”
Yet few of the members of the Wild
Minds community would abandon their
mental creations, even if they could. One
hardworking nurse revels in imagined
adventures starring a ictional medieval
Queen Eleanor of Scotland, a skilled
horsewoman with four concurrent hus-
bands, who practices a made-up religion
and is “a genius in both state and battle-
craft ... trained in martial arts and is al-
ways inventing marvelous things.” Like
Thurber’s ictional fantasist, Queen El-
eanor’s creator spends a lot of time men-
tally rescuing disaster victims from burn-
ing buildings or “abseiling over cliffs, be-
ing winched in and out of helicopters
with casualties.”
She has also documented her prepos-
terous plots for independent biopsycho-
d r e A m P i c t u r e S/ B l e n d i m AG e S/c o r B i S

logical researcher Cynthia Schupak, a


woman with a single-minded mission to
understand compulsive daydreamers,
who treated Rachel Stein and described
her ordeal in a journal article published

A pleasant reverie about a successful


acting career might motivate you to work
hard for a desired outcome, but it could
be detrimental if you become oblivious to
pickpockets or vehicular trafic.

30 Scientific AmericAn mind m a r c h/A p r il 2 01 1


© 2011 Scientific American
in 2009. Schupak is convinced that com- inescapable realities. When their en- your daydreams. To distinguish between
pulsive daydreaming is a unique disorder, hanced ability to conjure up vivid imag- beneicial and pathological imaginings,
characterized by an inability to control it ery is under control and does not inter- he adds, “Ask yourself if this is some-
and the deep distress over the condition. fere with social or academic success, “the thing useful, helpful, valuable, pleasant,
“Everyday escapist fantasy is ine and phenomenon should probably be classi- or am I just rehashing the same old per-
dandy, but this syndrome is different,” ied as a talent rather than a disorder,” he severative thoughts over and over again?”
she says. Schupak has enrolled 85 sub- says. Attitude may also be important. And if daydreaming feels out of control,

“I’m like an alcoholic with an unlimited supply of booze


wherever I go,” says Cordellia Amethyste Rose, who started
an online forum for people who cannot stop daydreaming.
jects — garnered mostly from Web post- Singer, who grew up during the Great then even if it is pleasant it is probably
ings — for an in-depth study of the syn- Depression and had no formal musical not useful or valuable.
drome. Respondents to her question- training, he says, entertained himself Whether or not mind wandering
naires devote between 12 and 90 percent through childhood and adolescence with causes distress often depends on the con-
of their waking hours to daydreaming— the imaginary achievements of “Singer text, Kane observes. “We argue that it’s
often while pacing, twirling or waving a the Composer,” an alter ego who wrote not inherently good or bad; it all depends
string. Nearly all believe that everyday a complete repertoire of classical music, on what the goals of the person are at the
activities pale by comparison with their including operas and an uninished Sev- time.” It may be perfectly reasonable for
vivid inner worlds, and some often drift enth Symphony. He does not consider his a scientist to mentally check out in the
in and out of their alternative reality in inner adventures harmful but rather sees midst of a repetitive experiment. And a
the midst of conversation. Typically they them as a boredom-banishing sport— novelist who can pour her reveries onto
report that their daydreams made them one that likely helped to propel him into paper and publish them is clearly putting
feel comforted or conident, “because it’s his profession. them to good use.
me, just magniied,” as one subject put it. “Happily, a lot of what we do in life
Nevertheless, 93 percent say they feel an- is Your mind Wandering doesn’t require that much concentra-
guished over the amount of time they out of control? tion,” Kane says. “But there are going to
spend fantasizing, admitting that their How do you know when you have be some contexts in which it is costly.
habit has prevented them from forming tipped over from useful and creative day- Does the cost to your activity, to your
relationships, studying or holding down dreaming into the netherworld of com- reputation, to your performance, over-
any but the dullest jobs. pulsive fantasizing? First, notice whether whelm the beneit that you may be get-
Schupak believes the syndrome could you are deriving any useful insights from ting from those thoughts? You can imag-
be a psychiatric illness, but is it? Singer, your fantasies. “The proof is in the pud- ine situations where it is so costly that
for example, believes it is nothing new: ding,” Schooler says. “Creative individ- there’s no thought you could be having
he says that he encountered many similar uals — artists, scientists, and so on — of- that’s worth it,” he says, pausing to con-
cases in his years of research and prac- tentimes report ideas that have occurred sider the possibilities. “You’ve crossed
tice. Yet some evidence suggests that to them during daydreams.” Second, it is the line,” he concludes, “if you walk into
maladaptive daydreaming could be a dis- important to take stock of the content of trafic and get killed.” M
tinctive disorder. Eight years ago clinical
psychologist Eli Somer of the University (Further Reading)
of Haifa in Israel recounted cases of six
◆ The Secret Life of Walter Mitty. James thurber in My World and Welcome to It.
people consumed by fantasy lives packed Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, 1937.
with sadism and bloodshed. All had suf- ◆ The Inner World of Daydreaming. Jerome l. Singer. Harper and row, 1975.
fered some form of childhood trauma. ◆ Mind-Play: The Creative Uses of Fantasy: Using Mind Imagery to Relax, Overcome
One had been sexually molested by her Fears and Bad Habits, Cope with Pain, Improve Your Decision-Making and Plan-
ning, Perfect Your Skill at Sports, and Enhance Your Sex Life. Jerome l. Singer
grandfather. Another described his fa- and ellen Switzer. Prentice-Hall, 1980.
ther as a brutal man who humiliated and ◆ The Daydreamer. reprint edition. ian mcewan. Anchor, 2000.
physically abused family members. ◆ Maladaptive Daydreaming: A Qualitative Inquiry. eli Somer in Journal of Contem-
Somer believes that this mental activ- porary Psychotherapy, Vol. 32, nos. 2–3; fall 2002.
◆ Rethinking Rumination. Susan nolen-Hoeksema, Blair e. Wisco and Sonja lyubomir-
ity emerged as a coping mechanism to sky in Perspectives on Psychological Science, Vol. 3, no. 5, pages 400–424; 2008.
help his patients deal with intolerable or

w w w. S c i e nti f i c A m e r i c an .c o m/m in d Scientific AmericAn mind 31


© 2011 Scientific American

You might also like