You are on page 1of 9

Accelerat ing t he world's research.

Relation between facial morphology,


personality, and the functions of
facial makeup in women
Arnaud Aubert

International Journal of Cosmetic Science

Related papers Download a PDF Pack of t he best relat ed papers 

Why women use makeup: Implicat ion of psychological t rait s in makeup funct ions Recherche, …
kazar Jeff

Body dysmorphic disorder: A cognit ive behavioural model and pilot RCT, 1996
Kevin Gournay, David Veale, Windy Dryden

Beaut y surveillance: t he digit al self-monit oring cult ures of neoliberalism


Rosalind Gill
International Journal of Cosmetic Science, 2011, 33, 338–345 doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2494.2010.00632.x

Relation between facial morphology, personality and the functions


of facial make-up in women

R. Korichi*, D. Pelle-de-Queral*, G. Gazano* and A. Aubert 


*LVMH Recherche, Parfums et Cosmétiques, 45800 St Jean de Braye and  INRA UR1213, 63122 Saint-Genès-Champanelle, France

Received 18 March 2010, Accepted 22 October 2010

Keywords: facial asymmetry, make-up products, psychology, skin radiance

aussi de communiquer une certaine disposition émotionnelle (sym-


Synopsis
pathie ou antipathie) et des informations sociales (valeurs, statuts,
Our external appearance plays a key role in everyday life’s social etc.). Toutefois, toutes les parties du corps ne vont pas avoir les
interactions. Hence, taking care of our appearance allows us to mêmes effets sur la construction de l’image globale du corps. Par
adjust and protect ourselves, as well as communicate emotional exemple, l’attractivité du visage est considérée par certains auteurs
disposition (i.e. sympathy or aversion) and social information (i.e. comme l’une des composantes les plus prédictives de l’attirance
values, status). However, some discrete body parts or characteris- physique globale, et comme l’un des premiers facteurs influençant
tics appear to be more salient than others in contributing to global l’estime de soi. D’ailleurs, au cours d’une précédente étude, nous
body image. For example, authors showed that facial attractiveness avons pu montrer la forte implication de traits psychologiques dans
is one of the best predictors of overall physical attractiveness and les fonctions de maquillage (profiles psycho-comportementaux
represent one of the primary factors influencing global self-esteem. ‘Séduction’ et ‘Camouflage’, respectivement S et C). Cette nouvelle
Make-up is therefore ought to play a major influence in these étude a pour objectif de rechercher des liens possibles entre nos
parameters. Moreover, in a previous study whose subject was to deux profiles psycho-comportementaux, et certains paramètres
explain the reasons that motivate women to make-up, we showed morphologiques connus pour être impliqués dans l’attractivité d’un
a high implication of specific psychological traits in correlation visage: l’asymétrie et la qualité de la peau. Tout d’abord, les
with two make-up functions (i.e. psycho-behavioural profiles femmes du groupe C montrent une plus grande asymétrie du bas
‘Seduction’ and ‘Camouflage’; group S and group C, respectively). du visage (au niveau e la bouche). Une asymétrie qui pourrait être
The purpose of this study was to assess the possible relation reliée à une quantité plus importante d’expériences émotionnelles
between our two psycho-behavioural profiles and some morpholog- négatives. Concernant le comportement de maquillage, les résultats
ical parameters know to be involved in facial attraction (i.e. facial mettent en avant une manipulation plus intense de l’attractivité du
asymmetry and skin visual quality). First of all, our study revealed visage chez les femmes du groupe S. Une manipulation reposant
for women from the group C a greater asymmetry of the lower face sur une utilisation plus élargie des couleurs, mais aussi sur un long
(i.e. mouth area) that could be related to a possible larger amount processus de maquillage destiné à ajuster la symétrie visuelle de
of negative emotional experiences. Concerning make-up behaviour, leur visage pour renforcer leur potentiel d’attractivité. Globalement,
women from the group S more extensively manipulated their rela- nos résultats suggèrent que le maquillage est utilisé différemment,
tive facial attractiveness, by using a large range of colours, but also selon le profil psychologique des femmes, dans le but de pouvoir
through a significantly longer make-up process used to adjust their manipuler certaines caractéristiques spécifiques visuelles et morpho-
visual asymmetry and therefore increase their potential of attrac- logiques du visage impliquées dans l’attractivité.
tiveness. On the overall, our results suggest that make-up is used
differentially, according to stable psychological profiles of women,
Introduction
to manipulate specific visual/morphological facial features involved
in attractiveness. Taking care of our external appearance allow us to adjust, protect
and defend ourselves, as well as communicate feelings mood and
social values. Each of these functions can have a bearing on the
Résumé
way others perceive us, but also influence the relationship we
Notre apparence extérieure joue un rôle central dans la régulation have with ourselves. For example, certain studies showed that
des rapports et des relations sociales quotidiennes. Prendre soin de women express a higher satisfaction of their body image and their
son apparence nous permet de nous ajuster, de nous protéger, mais global appearance when they are made-up [1]. If body image can
be considered as a multidimensional phenomenon that encom-
Correspondence: Rodolphe Korichi, LVMH Recherche, Parfums & passes our internal representation of our external physical appear-
Cosmétiques, Cutaneous Biology and Objectivation Department, 185 ance, attractiveness and beauty, but also our self-image, i.e. our
avenue de Verdun, 45800 Saint Jean de Braye, France. Tel.: +33 personality and social [2–7], some discrete body parts or charac-
(0)2 38 60 31 04; fax: +33 (0)2 38 60 31 17; e-mail: rkorichi@ teristics appear to be more salient than others in contributing to
research.lvmh-pc.com global body image [8, 9]. For example, some authors showed that

ª 2011 The Authors


338 ICS ª 2011 Society of Cosmetic Scientists and the Société Française de Cosmétologie
Influence of make-up products R. Korichi et al.

body weight and facial attractiveness are the best predictors of correspondences between the behavioural profiles related to facial
overall physical attractiveness and represent primary factors influ- make-up (i.e. psycho-behavioural profiles ‘Seduction’ and ‘Camou-
encing global self-esteem [10]. Such an emphasis on the study of flage’; S and C, respectively) previously identified [14] and relevant
facial attractiveness is consistent with the claims that the face morphological variables known to be implied in physical attrac-
plays a central role in human social interactions (e.g. expressions tiveness (i.e. facial asymmetry and facial skin visual quality).
communicate an individual’s emotional state) and is crucial in the
judgment of one’s whole attractiveness [11–13]. Moreover, in a
Materials and methods
previous study whose subject was to explain the reasons that
motivate women to make-up, we showed a high implication of
Volunteers
specific psychological traits in correlation with different make-up
functions. On the overall, we showed that use of facial make-up Sixty-two caucasian female subjects divided into two distinct
consists in two functional categories (labelled as ‘camouflage’ and groups were used for this study. These two groups (group C and
‘seduction’), and women belonging of these classes differ signifi- group S) were established from results obtained in a previous study
cantly regarding several psychological traits, defining two distinct [14]: see (experimental groups) for details. The mean age of group
psychological profiles (high self-esteem, high assertiveness and low C subjects was 47 year ± 12 (n = 20) and 43 year ± 10 (n = 42)
anxiety, vs. low self-esteem, low assertiveness and high anxiety) for group S. All subjects were customary cosmetic users and used
[14, 15]. From this first study, one can wonder whether morpho- their own make-up products during the make-up process. Finally,
logical and facial parameters could underlie such psycho-behavio- our experiment conformed to French legislation and ethical guide-
ural structures. Indeed, most of the studies on self-image revealed lines. All subjects were informed of the experimental details and
that attractive facial appearance confers a greater variety of provided a written consent for the exploitation of data.
positive social responses, which can have a profound effect on
self-esteem and social adjustment [13, 16]. Aesthetic judgments of Experimental groups
faces (i.e. attractiveness) have been related to various adaptive The two experimental groups used in this study where determined
functions and would be, at least partly, the result of our evolu- according to a procedure previously published [14]. From self-
tionary history [17–19]. Facial attributes like high skin quality assessment questionnaires built during a qualitative survey from
(i.e. healthy) and facial symmetry (i.e. the degree to which the the interviews of women about their relation to make-up (140 sen-
two sides of the face match) signal actual health and harmonious tences proposed with a five-point Likert response scale range from
development and therefore contribute to attractiveness [20]. More- 0 to 4 (0 meaning strongly disagreeing and four strongly agree-
over, recent research has attempted to test whether facial mascu- ing), we showed that the use of facial make-up in women could be
linity, facial attractiveness or symmetry constitute ‘honest’ (i.e. defined according to two different functions. The first is rather
positively correlated) signals of biological fitness by correlating intended to decrease a negative self-perception, which we labelled
them with health outcomes or longevity [21]. In cross-cultural as ‘camouflage’ (C) (i.e. aiming at decreasing the load of their neg-
studies, it has been shown that symmetry of facial features is a atively perceived attractiveness and/or global self-image). The sec-
primary determinant of attractiveness [18, 22, 23]. And even if ond is more oriented towards the desire to please that we labelled
some authors support the idea that facial asymmetry found in as ‘seduction’ (S) (i.e. aiming at supporting and promoting a posi-
normal people does not affect attractiveness ratings (except for old tive self-image). In parallel, from well-established and validated psy-
age) [24], it has nevertheless been commonly associated with chometric self-questionnaires : the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory
health and beauty and has been proposed as a core factor for (STAI-T; designed to assess trait anxiety and instructs subjects to
interpersonal attraction [23, 25–29]. Other authors suggested that report how they ‘generally’ feel), the Coopersmith Self-Esteem Inven-
facial symmetry is a correlate of personality. For example, a signif- tory (SEI: adult version; designed to measure and evaluate one’s
icant negative association was found between facial symmetry and self opinion regarding in social, academic, family and personal
openness and agreeableness, whereas extraversion was positively areas of experience), the Rathus Assertiveness Schedule (RAS; used
associated [30, 31]. Although the relationship between aspects of to measure assertiveness level) and the Eysenck Personality Inven-
facial shape and attractiveness has been extensively studied, few tory (EPI, B form; designed to measure two pervasive, independent
studies have investigated which characteristics of the surface of dimensions of personality, neuroticism–stability and extraversion–
faces positively influence attractiveness judgments. However, introversion); we found that the subjects corresponding to each of
certain authors showed that visible skin condition is positively these functional classes are characterized by opposite psychological
associated with facial attractiveness [32]. Indeed, the skin is one profiles. Women of the group C are characterized by higher anxi-
of the most important components of an individual’s physical ety, neuroticism and introversion levels, and a lower self-esteem.
appearance, and some of these skin changes (e.g. inflammation, On the opposite, women from group S express a lower anxiety and
acne) exert more negative impact on facial attractiveness [33, higher self-esteem and extraversion levels (see Table I for
34]. For example, authors [35, 36] suggested that skin colour dis- summary). Note that aging was not a discriminating factor of our
tribution, independent of facial form and skin surface topography, different make-up functions and had no influence on the volun-
to have a major influence on the perception of female facial age teer’s psychological characteristics [14].
and judgments of attractiveness and health. Other authors [37]
found that men with symmetric faces were perceived as having
Experimental procedure
healthier facial skin than men with relatively asymmetric faces
and that facial colour and texture cues were sufficient to maintain Facial asymmetry measurement
an attractiveness–symmetry relationship. All these findings are Facial measurements were taken from digital photographs of the
consistent with the proposal that attractive physical traits are whole face without make-up. Before being photographed, subjects’
those that positively influence others’ perceptions of an individ- head position was standardized, with the chin and the two ears
ual’s health. The purpose of this study was to identify the possible being maintained by calibrated holding devices. For each digital

ª 2011 The Authors


ICS ª 2011 Society of Cosmetic Scientists and the Société Française de Cosmétologie
International Journal of Cosmetic Science, 33, 338–345 339
Influence of make-up products R. Korichi et al.

Table I Summary of the main characteristics of experimental used to objectively assess visual quality of the skin. The evaluation
groups of colours was based on structural visual scales presenting a range
of four core colours of the facial skin: ‘pink red’, ‘beige’, ‘olive’ and
‘light pink’. Concerning the physical characteristics of the skin,
Group C (n = 20) Group S (n = 42) luminosity defined as the intensity of light spots on the salient
Make-up functions Camouflage Seduction areas of the face, Brightness as the synthesis of the uniformity of
Psychological profile Anxious Sociable the skin colour and of the regularity of the skin texture and Trans-
Reserved Optimistic parency as the characteristic of the skin through which we can see
Low self-esteem Emotionally stable the veins (characteristic of very fine skin). To allow these evalua-
Non-assertive Anxious and assertive tions, the subjects were sitting between two symmetrically installed
‘daylight’ lamps. The subjects carried a black cap on the head.
Adapted from Korichi et al. [14]. Additionally, a black gown was placed on their chest to hide the
neck and the colour of carried clothing. These analyses were car-
face-image, six pairs of reliably identified facial features were ried out by three trained judges, individually, without reference to
positioned [18]. Corresponding points were positioned on the inside the previous analysis. To measure the homogeneity of the skin col-
(P1 and P2) and outside (P3 and P4) corners of the eyes, cheek- our, a high-resolution polarized photograph of the whole face of
bones (widest horizontal part of the face below the eyes, P5 and each subject was captured before and after make-up to obtain
P6), widest points at the sides of the nostrils (P7 and P8), corners images without reflection, therefore allowing a reliable assessment
of the mouth (P9 and P10) and the jaw (widest horizontal part of of the skin colour. From these images, we defined five specific areas
the cheeks at the mouth, P11 and P12) (see Fig. 1). A thirteenth located on the face (dark circle, forehead, chin, cheekbone, nose)
point was placed on philtrum (i.e. the midline groove in the upper and we carried out a colorimetric analysis in the uniform L*a*b*
lip that runs from the top of the lip to the nose; P13). The vertical (CIELAB) colour space [41], used conventionally to describe all visi-
midline was calculated for each face by finding the mean midpoint ble colours. L* represents the lightness of the colour (L*, L* = 0
of six horizontal lines joining corresponding points on each hemi- yields black and L* = 100 indicates white); a* represents its posi-
face (i.e. P1 and P2, P3 and P4, P5 and P6, P7 and P8, P9 and tion between magenta and green (negative values indicate green
P10, P11 and P12) (Fig. 1). We used these horizontal lines to cal- whereas positive values indicate magenta); and b* represents its
culate horizontal asymmetry, a measure of asymmetry introduced position between yellow and blue (negative values indicate blue
by Grammer and Thornhill [18] and used in previous studies of and positive values indicate yellow). Using these colour compo-
face asymmetry [26, 29, 38, 39]. Briefly, horizontal asymmetry is nents, the individual typology angle (ITA) was calculated. This
the sum of the deviations between the midpoints of each horizontal global parameter corresponds to the skin’s melanization level (basic
line and the average midpoint calculated across these lines (i.e. the or acquired) and skin colour tone.
vertical midline used above). In a perfectly symmetrical face, all
midpoints will fall on the vertical midline. Behavioural assessment of make-up session
During the make-up session, the behaviour of women was observed
Evaluation of skin radiance and homogeneity of the skin colour and measured to assess possible differences in make-up behaviour
The skin radiance evaluation is based on a visual sensory analysis between ‘C’ and ‘S’ women. Make-up occurred in a standardized
of four descriptors: Colour, Luminosity, Brightness and Transpar- room in which subjects were left alone. A digital video camera was
ency of the skin of the face (CLBTÔ method – Spincontrol) [40], placed behind the one-way mirror used for make-up. Several
behavioural parameters were measured from a blind analysis: (i)
the total duration of the make-up session, (ii) the total duration of
make-up application, (iii) the duration of self-observation (i.e. epi-
sodes in which women watch at themselves in the mirror without
applying any cosmetics) and (iv) the duration of make-up applica-
tion for each area of the face (i.e. forehead, eyes, cheekbones,
cheeks, nose, mouth, chin, neck).

Data analysis
Analyses of variance (ANOVAs) or independent t-tests were used to
detect significant differences in facial asymmetry skin visual proper-
ties or make-up behaviours. Homoscedasticity and normality of
samples were tested before each analysis to ensure a proper use of
parametric tests (Bartlett’s and Shapiro-Wilk’s tests, respectively).
Statistical significance was set to a maximum risk a = 0.05.

Results

Facial asymmetry
No significant differences were found between our two groups of
Figure 1 Location of pairs of facial landmarks used to calculate volunteers concerning the global facial asymmetry (F19,41 = 1.37,
facial asymmetry on each hemiface. P = 0.55; mean ± SEM: 1.82 ± 1.11 and 1.65 ± 0.95, respec-

ª 2011 The Authors


ICS ª 2011 Society of Cosmetic Scientists and the Société Française de Cosmétologie
340 International Journal of Cosmetic Science, 33, 338–345
Influence of make-up products R. Korichi et al.

tively, for subjects from group C and S). However, if the global significant difference was observed between groups for colours and
asymmetry score was not significantly different between our two physical characteristics of the skin (see Table II for full details of
groups, the asymmetry of several facial zones was significantly dif- statistics). Concerning the evolution of colorimetric descriptors
ferent between ‘C’ and ‘S’ subjects. Indeed, distances between P1 (CLBT), results showed that make-up produced a significant effect
and P2 and between P3 and P4 (i.e. eyes area) were significantly on the skin radiance, whatever the group of volunteers, by decreas-
lower in group C than in group S (p1p2: F19,41 = 0.02, P < 0.01; ing of the complexion’s ‘olive’ skin cast and ‘transparency’, and by
p3p4: F19,41 = 0.22, P < 0.01). On the contrary, asymmetry increasing of the ‘beige’ colour, ‘Luminosity’ and ‘Brightness’ (see
between P9 and P10 (i.e. mouth area) was significantly higher in Table III for full details of statistics).
group C than in group S (F19,41 = 6.35, P < 0.01). These results Concerning the homogeneity of the skin colour, as summarized
suggest that subjects from group C would have a more symmetrical in Table IV, we observed that women of our panel did not use
upper face than those of group S, whereas it would be the opposite make-up colours in the same way according to the group they
for the lower part of the face. As shown in Fig. 2, upper face asym- belong to. Indeed, subjects of group C displayed only a significant
metry (i.e. eyes and cheekbones) tended to be greater in group S increase in the b* parameters for each zones of the face after make-
(t = 1.858; df = 60; P = 0.068), whereas a significantly higher up (Forehead: F1,19 = 13.21, P < 0.01; Dark circles: F1,19 = 6.5, P
asymmetry was found in group C for lower face (i.e. mouth and = 0.02; Cheekbone: F1,19 = 15.4, P < 0.01; Nose: F1,19 = 19.06,
jaws; t = 2.391; df = 60; P = 0.020). P < 0.01; Chin: F1,19 = 10.48, P < 0.01). On the contrary, in
women of group S, the colour variations were significant for each
zone of the face and almost all coordinates of the CIE L*a*b* space
Skin radiance and colour
and the individual typology angle parameter (ITA°): Forehead: L*:
ANOVAs revealed a comparable homogeneity of skin radiance in F1,41 = 5.91, P = 0.02, b*: F1,41 = 34.36, P < 0.01; Dark circles:
our two groups of volunteers, before and after make-up. Indeed, no L*: F1,41 = 12.52, P < 0.01, a*: F1,41 = 14.42, P < 0.01, b*:
F1,41 = 29.57, P < 0.01, ITA°: F1,41 = 9.87, P < 0.01; Cheek-
bones: b*: F1,41 = 64.07, P < 0.01; Nose: L*: F1,41 = 18.13,
P < 0.01, a*: F1,41 = 9.34, P < 0.01, b*: F1,41 = 49.32, P < 0.01,
ITA°: F1,41 = 8.89, P < 0.01; Chin: L*: F1,41 = 20.7, P < 0.01, a*:
F1,41 = 12.31, P < 0.01, b*: F1,41 = 50.09, P < 0.01, ITA°:
F1,41 = 11.65, P < 0.01.

Make-up behaviour
Total duration of make-up, duration of self-observation and dura-
tion of make-up for forehead, eyes, cheekbones, cheeks, chin and
neck were not significantly different between C and S groups
(P > 0.22). However, as shown in Fig. 3, subjects from group S
displayed significantly longer application of make-up on the mouth
and lips area compared to group C (t = 2.098; df = 59;
Figure 2 Mean ± SEM upper (eyes and cheek bones) and lower P < 0.0412). Moreover, make-up duration of the mouth was
(jaws and mouth) facial asymmetry. Black columns represent data highly correlated with mouth asymmetry (p9p10) in subjects of the
for subjects from group C (camouflage) and grey columns represent group S (r = 0.938; P < 0.0001), although there was no statistical
relation between mouth asymmetry and make-up in subjects of
data for subjects from group S (seduction). +P < 0.10; *P < 0.05.
group C (r = )0.161; P > 0.31).

Table II Analysis of the homogeneity of the skin radiance between groups of volunteers

Before make-up After make-up

Group C Group S Group C Group S


(n = 20) (n = 42) (n = 20) (n = 42)

Mean ±SD Mean ±SD Significance Mean ±SD Mean ±SD Significance

Skin colours Pink red 11.8 6.6 12.4 5.4 NS 10.5 5.8 10.8 6.2 NS
Olive 10.5 5.0 9.5 3.5 NS 6.7 3.8 6.3 3.3 NS
Beige 33.2 13.2 31.9 4.0 NS 38.9 13.3 37.2 5.5 NS
Light pink 19.8 3.0 20.5 3.0 NS 19.9 3.3 20.7 2.9 NS
Physical characteristics Luminosity 3.2 0.4 3.2 0.5 NS 3.6 0.5 3.5 0.7 NS
Brightness 3.1 0.7 2.9 0.6 NS 3.7 0.7 3.7 0.8 NS
Transparency 1.6 0.8 1.4 0.6 NS 1.3 0.7 1.1 0.5 NS

ª 2011 The Authors


ICS ª 2011 Society of Cosmetic Scientists and the Société Française de Cosmétologie
International Journal of Cosmetic Science, 33, 338–345 341
Influence of make-up products R. Korichi et al.

Table III Evolution of the skin radiance dur-


Comparison before and after make-up ing the make-up process (before and after
make-up; within groups)
Group Group
C (n = 20) S (n = 42)

Mean ±SD Significance Mean ±SD Significance

Skin colours Pink red )1.3 5.0 NS )1.6 5.6 LS (P = 0.07)


Olive 3.8 2.9 S (P < 0.01) )3.2 2.4 S (P < 0.01)
Beige 5.7 7.0 S (P < 0.01) 5.3 4.7 S (P < 0.01)
Light pink 0.2 0.2 NS 0.2 2.7 NS
Physical Luminosity 0.5 0.3 S (P < 0.01) 0.3 0.6 S (P < 0.01)
characteristics Brightness 0.6 0.6 S (P < 0.01) 0.7 0.5 S (P < 0.01)
Transparency )0.2 0.3 S (P < 0.01) )0.3 0.3 S (P < 0.01)

Table IV Evolution of the homogeneity of


Comparison before and after make-up the skin colour during the make-up process
(before and after make-up; within groups)
Group Group
C (n = 20) S (n = 42)

CIELAB
Facial zones parameters Mean ±SD Significance Mean ±SD Significance

Forehead L* 0.7 2.7 NS 0.8 1.9 S (P =0.02)


a* 0 0.8 NS )0.1 0.6 NS
b* 0.6 0.7 S (P < 0.01) 0.6 0.6 S (P < 0.01)
ITA° )0.1 3.9 NS 0 2.8 NS
Dark circles L* 0.7 3.0 NS 1.2 2.1 S (P < 0.01)
a* )0.3 1.0 NS )0.4 0.7 S (P < 0.01)
b* 0.3 0.6 S (P = 0.02) 0.3 0.5 S (P < 0.01)
ITA° 1.7 9.9 NS 3.3 6.8 S (P < 0.01)
Cheekbone L* 0.1 2.9 NS 0.7 2.3 LS (P = 0.06)
a* 0.3 1.1 NS 0.1 0.9 NS
b* 0.7 0.7 S (P < 0.01) 0.7 0.6 S (P < 0.01)
ITA° )0.2 10.0 NS 0.9 6.9 NS
Nose L* 1.7 2.9 S (P = 0.01) 1.7 2.6 S (P < 0.01)
a* )0.4 1.1 NS )0.5 1.1 S (P < 0.01)
b* 0.7 0.7 S (P < 0.01) 0.6 0.6 S (P < 0.01)
ITA° 4.4 9.8 LS (P = 0.06) 4.2 9.0 S (P < 0.01)
Chin L* 1.4 3.5 NS 1.6 2.2 S (P < 0.01)
a* )0.4 1.3 NS )0.6 0.9 S (P < 0.01)
b* 0.6 0.7 S (P < 0.01) 0.6 0.5 S (P < 0.01)
ITA° 3.6 11.9 NS 4.2 8.1 S (P < 0.01)

associated with symmetry. Other authors have also investigated


Discussion
associations between facial fluctuating asymmetry and various
Our first result showed that the two groups of volunteers, defined personality factors. They reported a general tendency for facial fluc-
by two major functional use of make-up (Camouflage and Seduc- tuating asymmetry to be correlated with emotional and psychologi-
tion) and specific emotional and psychological profiles (as summa- cal health [42]. However, our first finding supports the idea that
rized in Table I), are not discriminated by global facial asymmetry. the affective and behavioural differences about the use of make-up
This result contrasts with those reported by Kink and al. [30] who are not sustained by morphological differences, such as facial asym-
found a significant negative association between facial symmetry, metry considered as a transcultural parameter of physical attrac-
openness and agreeableness, whereas extraversion was positively tiveness [19, 43, 44], but rather by psychobiological factors such

ª 2011 The Authors


ICS ª 2011 Society of Cosmetic Scientists and the Société Française de Cosmétologie
342 International Journal of Cosmetic Science, 33, 338–345
Influence of make-up products R. Korichi et al.

between facial asymmetry and emotional expression remains to be


further examined.
This naturally leads us to broach the concept of skin quality (and
uniformity of skin colour), which is, as we know, one of the trans-
cultural criteria defining a beautiful face [20, 53]), and closely
linked with the ‘psychology of the appearance’. The colorimetric
analyses that we have carried out show that, contrary to popular
belief, skin quality is not an explanatory factor for our two psycho-
functional profiles. Our results show that women of our panel
display the same natural skin features (hence, the same skin-related
attractiveness features) and that data concerning facial colorimetry
and radiance revealed same significant effects of make-up, whatever
the group (C and S). Decrease in the ‘olive’ colour and increase in
the ‘beige’ colour can be interpreted as a ‘good looking’ effect [40,
54], through a lesser greenish-olive complexion. The decrease in
Figure 3 Mean ± SEM duration of make-up of the mouth and lips ‘Transparency’ expresses a masking effect of the skin by the make-
up and confirms the application of cosmetic products on the face.
area (sec). Black columns represent data for subjects from group C
Moreover, the increase in ‘Luminosity’ and ‘Brightness’ after make-
(camouflage) and grey columns represent data for subjects from
up confirms that cosmetics are useful tools to improve skin apparent
group S (seduction). *P < 0.05. quality (i.e. radiance and homogeneity) and are potent factors for
facial attractiveness [20]. This fact nevertheless opposes the conclu-
as the personality and temperament. In fact, published documents sions reached in numerous studies, which, despite certain controver-
investigating the role of cosmetics in social perception have shown sial issues, show that the skin quality perceived would be associated
that, globally, make-up is associated with a positive evaluation of with facial attractiveness [55]. Other studies also suggest that the
the subject by her social environment, both in terms of social homogeneity of skin colour, regardless of the shape of the face and
image (i.e. evaluation of the subject’s social qualities) and physical skin relief, affects judgments in terms of physical attraction [56].
attractiveness [45, 46]. This accentuation could not be entirely These observations therefore lead us to believe that some women in
assumed from the structure of the subjects’ personality, especially our study (camouflage group and seduction group) would have
the more introvert of subjects for whom this accentuating approach other reasons to immediately conceal skin defects by applying make-
would cause anxiety. Thus, on the one hand, make-up tends to up. Furthermore, colorimetric data specific to certain parts of the
serve as an essential tool to offset an image of poor self-esteem [47] face show that make-up strategies are preferably based on colours
and, on the other hand, physical satisfaction appears to be posi- that harmonize, to varying degree, with the female personality. On
tively linked to the importance of make-up [48]. Make-up would, the one hand, women belonging to the ‘camouflage’ profile (group
therefore, play a more expressive function and, as such, would be C) seek to limit their attractiveness and positive impressions by
more relevant for women with greater social confidence, regardless restricting the use of colours on the face. Indeed, skin colour
of whether the women are physically attractive or deem themselves expressed by each coordinates of the colour space CIELAB (a colori-
to be. Make-up would therefore take on either a protective role metric space of reference normalized by the international commis-
under the gaze of onlookers, acting as a form of camouflage (group sion of the lighting CIE 1976 L*a*b* and allowing to obtain a colour
C), or would heighten the powers of seduction (women in group S) representation closes to the eye vision) clearly showed that female
[49]. subjects of group C used significantly less colours to enhance their
Concerning morphological variables, further analyses of our data facial appearance than those of group S. Colour changes, which
revealed significant differences in upper vs. lower facial asymmetry have been clearly visible on the colorimetric b* axis (see Table IV),
(i.e. eyes and mouth) between our two groups. Indeed, group C are known to indicate how healthy a face looks [57]. Moreover, the
subjects have significantly greater lower facial asymmetry than ‘S’ absence of any variation in the ITA° parameter, which we know is
subjects, while expressing marginally lower upper asymmetry. Such connected to the level of melanization in the skin and the skin col-
differences could be related to fluctuating asymmetry (i.e. random our tone [58], strengthens our conclusions with regard to the lim-
differences between two sides, as opposed to the global asymmetry) ited use of make-up colours in group C women. These results tend to
that have been argued to develop throughout the lifespan of the confirm the fact that the inferences produced in reaction to make up
individual and would represent a sign of the phenotype being sub- depend on the quality of the skin and the amount of make-up
jected to some levels of stress [50]. Interestingly, some studies applied [59]. In fact, make-up loses its aesthetic value as soon as it is
reveal that lower face was associated with emotions and more applied in excess and therefore becomes perceptibly dissociated from
specifically with valence-related asymmetries [51]. As lower face the face to which it is applied. Conversely, the enhancing effect of
asymmetry is greater in women from group C, it could be hypothe- make-up would be more pronounced in attractive women or women
sized a link between their facial asymmetry pattern and a possible who consider themselves to be. These findings are corroborated in
larger amount of negative emotional experiences [50], especially as the colorimetric measurements recorded in women belonging to the
negative emotions implies more salient facial features [52]. Interest- ‘seduction’ profile (group S) in cases where their appearance and
ingly, mouth asymmetry and make-up duration was highly corre- attractiveness have been substantially manipulated by an extended
lated in subject from group S, but not in group C. As mouth and colour range as confirmed by changes in all of the colorimetric
lips have been related to secondary sexual signals [33], it could be parameters.
therefore hypothesized that make-up would be used as a tool to On the overall, these results further reinforce the previously
adjust visual asymmetry in women from group S, and therefore defined psychological profiles of our subjects as women of group C
increase for potential attractiveness. However, the exact relation would rather wish to remain ‘transparent’ and ‘natural’ to the

ª 2011 The Authors


ICS ª 2011 Society of Cosmetic Scientists and the Société Française de Cosmétologie
International Journal of Cosmetic Science, 33, 338–345 343
Influence of make-up products R. Korichi et al.

others, probably to remain unobtrusive but limiting their attrac- suggest that make-up is used differentially, according to stable
tiveness and positive impression. In our previous work, we showed psychological profiles of women, to manipulate specific visual/
that make-up application can be considered as a daily routine to morphological facial features involved in attractiveness. Finally, as
decrease negative affects and/or increase positive affects related to our results have been obtained from a single geographical location
self-image and one’s relation to his social environment. Basically, (i.e. France), one should be cautious in transposing these results to
our main results show that our two groups of women (i.e. S and women from other socio-cultural backgrounds.
C) did not differ in global asymmetry and skin radiance before
make-up. However, the relative control of specific facial features
Acknowledgements
(i.e. asymmetry of discrete facial zones implicated in sexual behav-
iours and skin colour) by make-up is differentially expressed We are grateful to all volunteers for their patience during our
according to the psycho-behavioural profiles previously highlighted experiments. The authors thank Dr F. Vial and C. Delmas from
[14]. Even if these findings have been established from a limited Spincontrol for productive discussions and active participation in
population (in term of sample size and social status), our results this work.

References 12. Berry, D.S. and Finch-Wero, J.L. Accuracy ness: a monozygotic co-twin comparison. J.
in face perception: a view from ecological Pers. Soc. Psychol. 76, 151–158 (1999).
1. Cash, T.F., Dawson, K., Davis, P., Bowen, M. psychology. J. Pers. 61, 497–520 (1993). 24. Kowner, R. Facial asymmetry and attrac-
and Galumbeck, C. The effects of cosmetics 13. Berry, D.S. and Landry, J.C. Facial maturity tiveness judgment in developmental perspec-
use on the physical attractiveness and body and daily social interaction. J. Pers. Soc. Psy- tive. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.
image of college women. J. Soc. Psychol. chol. 72, 570–580 (1997). 22, 662–675 (1996).
129, 349–356 (1989). 14. Korichi, R., Pelle-de-Queral, D., Gazano, G. 25. Rhodes, G., Zebrowitz, L.A., Clark, A.,
2. Schilder, P. The Image and Appearance of the and Aubert, A. Why women use make-up? Kalick, S.M., Hightower, A. and McKay, R.
Human Body. Kegan, Paul, Trench, Trubner Implication of psychological traits in Do facial averageness and symmetry signal
and Co, London (1935). makeup functions. J. Cosmet. Sci. 59, 127– health? Evol. Hum. Behav. 22, 31–46
3. Coon, D. Essentials of Psychology, 6th edn. 137 (2008). (2001).
West Publishing Company, Los Angeles 15. Korichi, R., Pelle-de-Queral, D., Gazano, G. 26. Penton-Voak, I.S., Jones, B.C., Little, A.C.,
(1994). and Aubert, A. Psychological approach of Baker, S.E., Tiddeman, B.P., Burt, D.M. and
4. Thompson, J.K., Heinberg, L.J., Altabe, M. the hedonic process implicated in the Perrett, D.I. Symmetry, sexual dimorphism
and Tantleff-Dunn, S. Exacting Beauty: The- make-up of human face and relation with in facial proportions, and male sexual
ory, Assessment, and Treatment of Body Image morphometric parameters. In: 24th IFSCC attractiveness. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 268,
Disturbance. American Psychological Associ- congress, Osaka, Japan (2006). 1617–1623 (2001).
ation, Washington DC (1999). 16. Howells, D.J. and Shaw, W.C. The validity 27. Zaidel, D.W., Aarde, S.M. and Baig, K. The
5. Hoyt, W. and Kogan, L. Satisfaction with and reliability of ratings of dental and facial appearance of symmetry, beauty, and health
body image and peer relationships for males attractiveness for epidemiologic use. Am. J. in human face. Brain Cogn., 57, 261–263
and females in a college environment. Sex Orthod. 88, 402–408 (1985). (2005).
Roles: J. Res. 45, 199–215 (2001). 17. Thornhill, R. and Gangestad, S.W. Human 28. Little, A.C., Burt, D.M., Penton-Voak, I.S.
6. Cash, T.F. and Fleming, E.C. Body image facial beauty: averageness, symmetry, and and Perrett, D.I. Self-perceived attractive-
and social relations. In: Body Image: A parasite resistance. Hum. Nat. 4, 237–269 ness influences human preferences for sex-
Handbook of Theory, Research, and Clinical (1993). ual dimorphism and symmetry in male
Practice (Cash, T.F. and Pruzinsky, T., eds), 18. Grammar, K. and Thornhill, R. Human faces. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 268, 39–44
pp. 277–286. Guilford Press, New York (Homo sapiens) facial attractiveness and (2001).
(2002). sexual selection: the role of symmetry and 29. Rhodes, G. and Zebrowitz, L.A. Facial Attrac-
7. Cash, T.F. and Pruzinsky, T.P. Body Images: averageness. J. Comp. Psychol. 108, 233– tiveness: Evolutionary, Cognitive and Social
Development, Deviance, and Change. Guilford 242 (1994). Perspectives. Ablex Publishers, Westport, CT
Press, New York (1990). 19. Buss, D.M. Evolutionary Psychology, 3rd edn. (2002).
8. Cash, T.F. Body-image affect: gestalt versus Pearson, Allyn and Bacon, Boston (2008). 30. Fink, B., Neave, N., Manning, J.T. and
summing the parts. Percept. Mot. Skills 69, 20. Fink, B., Grammer, K. and Thornhill, R. Grammar, K. Facial symmetry and the ‘big-
17–18 (1989). Human (Homo sapiens) facial attractiveness five’ personality factors. Pers. Indiv. Differ.
9. Franzoi, S.L. and Herzog, M.E. Judging phys- in relation to skin texture and color. J. 39(3), 523–529 (2005).
ical attractiveness: what body aspects do we Comp. Psychol. 115, 92–99 (2001). 31. Noor, F. and Evans, D.C. The effect of facial
use? Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 13, 19–33 21. Kalick, S.M., Zebrowitz, L.A., Langlois, H. symmetry on perceptions of personality and
(1987). and Johnson, R.M. Does human facial attractiveness. J. Res. Pers. 37, 339–347
10. Swami, V., Furnham, A., Georgiades, C. and attractiveness honestly advertise health? (2003).
Pang, L. Evaluating self and partner physi- Longitudinal data on an evolutionary ques- 32. Jones, B.C., Little, A.C., Feinberg, D.R.,
cal attractiveness. Body Image 4, 97–101 tion. Psychol. Sci. 9, 8–13 (1998). Penton-Voak, I.S., Tiddeman, B.P. and Per-
(2007). 22. Little, A.C. and Jones, B.C. Evidence against rett, D.I. The relationship between shape
11. Langlois, J.H., Kalakanis, L., Rubenstein, perceptual bias views for symmetry prefer- symmetry and perceived skin condition in
A.J., Larson, A., Hallam, M. and Smoot, M. ences in human faces. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B male facial attractiveness. Evol. Hum. Behav.
Maxims or myths of beauty? A meta-ana- 270, 1759–1763 (2003). 25, 24–30 (2004).
lytic and theoretical review. Psychol. Bull. 23. Mealey, L., Bridgstock, N. and Townsend, G. 33. Etcoff, N. Survival of the Prettiest: the Science
126, 390–423 (2000). Symmetry and perceived facial attractive- of Beauty. Anchor Books, New York (1999).

ª 2011 The Authors


ICS ª 2011 Society of Cosmetic Scientists and the Société Française de Cosmétologie
344 International Journal of Cosmetic Science, 33, 338–345
Influence of make-up products R. Korichi et al.

34. Fink, B. and Neave, N. The biology of facial 43. Zebrowitz, L.A. Reading Faces: Window to the skin color distribution. J Cosm. Dermatol, 7,
beauty. Int. J. Cosmet. Sci. 27, 317–325 Soul? Wesview Press, Boulder (1997). 155–161 (2008).
(2005). 44. Thornhill, R. and Gangestad, S.W. Facial 54. Perin, F., Saetun, K., Pungpod, P., Pram-On,
35. Fink, B., Grammer, K. and Matts, P. Visible attractiveness. Trends Cogn. Sci. 3, 452–460 M., Perin, N. and Aroonrat, N. A new
skin color distribution plays a role in the (1999). method for the in vivo visual evaluation of the
perception of age, attractiveness, and health 45. Graham, J.A. and Jouhar, A.J. The effects of radiance of the Asian skin complexion. Confer-
in female faces? Evol. Hum. Behav. 27(6), cosmetics on person perception. Int. J. Cos- ence of the Asian Societies of Cosmetics
433–442 (2006). met. Sci., 3, 199–210 (1981). Scientists, pp. 7–9, Singapore (2007).
36. Grammer, K. and Fink, B. Women’s Skin 46. Workman, J.E. and Johnson, K.K.P. The role 55. Jones, B.C., Little, A.C., Feinberg, D.R., Pen-
Tone Influences Perception of Beauty, Health, of cosmetics in impression formation. Cloth. ton-Voak, I.S., Tiddeman, B.P. and Perrett,
Age: Sociobiologists’ Find. Human Behavior Text. Res. J., 10, 63–67 (1991). D.I. The relationship between shape symme-
and Evolution Society annual meeting, 47. Zaltman, G. and Wallendorf, M. Consumer try and perceived skin condition in male
Philadelphia (2006). Behavior. John Wiley and Sons, New York facial attractiveness. Evol. Hum. Behav., 2,
37. Jones, B.C., Little, A.C., Burt, D.M. and Per- (1984). 24–30 (2004).
rett, D.I. When facial attractiveness is only 48. Theberge, L. and Kernaleguen, A. Importance 56. Fink, B., Grammer, K. and Matts, P. Visible
skin deep. Perception 33, 569–576 (2004). of cosmetics related to aspects of the self. Per- skin color distribution plays a role in the
38. Hume, D.K. and Montgomerie, R. Facial cept. Mot. Skills, 48, 827–830 (1979). perception of age, attractiveness, and health
attractiveness signals different aspects of 49. Bloch, P.H. and Richins, M.L. You look in female faces? Evol. Hum. Behav., 27,
‘‘quality’’ in women and men. Evol. Hum. mahvelous: the pursuit of beauty and the 433–442 (2006).
Behav. 22, 93–112 (2001). marketing concept. Psychol. Market., 9, 3– 57. Stephen, I.D., Law Smith, M.J., Stirrat, M.R.
39. Scheib, J.E., Gangestad, S.W. and Thornhill, 15 (1992). and Perrett, D.I. Facial skin colouration
R. Facial attractiveness, symmetry and cues 50. Edler, R.J. Background considerations to affects perceived health of human faces. Int.
of good genes. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 266, facial aesthetics. J. Ortho. 28, 159–168 J. Primat., 30, 845–857 (2009).
1913–1917 (1999). (2001). 58. Camel, E., Arnaud-Boissel, L., Schnebert,
40. Musnier, C., Piquemal, P., Beau, P. and Pittet, 51. Richardson, C.K., Bowers, D., Bauer, R.M., S., Neveu, M., Tan, S.K. and Guillot, J.P.
J.C. Visual evaluation in vivo of complexion Heilman, K.M. and Leonard, C.M. Digitizing Does Asian skin induce significant changes
radiance using C.L.B.T sensory methodology. the moving face during dynamic displays of in sun protection factor determination
Skin Res. Tech. 10, 50–56 (2004). emotion. Neuropsychologia 38, 1028–1039 compared to Caucasian skin: one of the
41. McLaren, K. The development of the CIE (2000). first in vivo correlations. IFSCC Mag, 5,
1976 (L*a*b*) uniform colour-space and col- 52. Ekman, P. Basic Emotions. In: Handbook of 31–34 (2002).
our-difference formula. J. Soc. Dy. Col. 92, Cognition and Emotion (Dalgleish, T. and 59. Richetin, J., Croizet, J.C. and Huguet, P.
338–341 (1976). Power, T., eds), pp. 45–60. John Wiley and Facial make-up elicits positive attitudes as
42. Shackelford, T.K. and Larsen, R.J. Facial Sons Ltd, Sussex (1999). the implicit level: evidence from the implicit
asymmetry as an indicator of psychological, 53. Fink, B., Matts, P.J., Klingenberg, H., association test. Curr. Res. Soc. Psychol., 9,
emotional, and physiological distress. J. Pers. Kuntze, S., Weege, B. and Grammer, K. 145–165 (2004).
Soc. Psychol., 72, 456–466 (1997). Visual attention to variation in female facial

ª 2011 The Authors


ICS ª 2011 Society of Cosmetic Scientists and the Société Française de Cosmétologie
International Journal of Cosmetic Science, 33, 338–345 345

You might also like