You are on page 1of 15

Honor and political violence

Author(s): Elin Bjarnegård, Karen Brounéus and Erik Melander


Source: Journal of Peace Research , November 2017, Vol. 54, No. 6 (November 2017), pp.
748-761
Published by: Sage Publications, Ltd.

Stable URL: https://www.jstor.org/stable/10.2307/48590471

REFERENCES
Linked references are available on JSTOR for this article:
https://www.jstor.org/stable/10.2307/48590471?seq=1&cid=pdf-
reference#references_tab_contents
You may need to log in to JSTOR to access the linked references.

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide
range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and
facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
https://about.jstor.org/terms

Sage Publications, Ltd. is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to
Journal of Peace Research

This content downloaded from


45.77.104.39 on Tue, 19 Jul 2022 21:42:32 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Journal of Peace Research
2017, Vol. 54(6) 748–761
Honor and political violence: Micro-level ª The Author(s) 2017
Reprints and permission:
findings from a survey in Thailand sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0022343317711241
journals.sagepub.com/home/jpr

Elin Bjarnegård
Department of Government, Uppsala University
Karen Brounéus
Department of Peace and Conflict Research, Uppsala University
Erik Melander
Department of Peace and Conflict Research, Uppsala University

Abstract
Who participates in political violence? In this study, we investigate the issue at the micro-level, comparing individuals
who have used violence in political uprisings with those who have not. We develop our argument from the
observation that men are strongly overrepresented in political violence, although most men do not participate.
Literature on masculinities emphasizes the role of honor and its links to different forms of violence, such as domestic
abuse, criminal violence, and violent attitudes. Building on this literature, we discern two separate but related aspects
of honor: honor as male societal privilege and control over female sexuality, that is, patriarchal values, and honor as
ideals of masculine toughness, that is, the perceived necessity for men to be fierce and respond to affronts with violence
or threats of violence in order to preserve status. We argue that patriarchal values combined with ideals of masculine
toughness together constitute honor ideology, which contributes in turn to the explanation of who participates in
political violence. We present new and unique individual-level survey data on these issues, collected in Thailand. We
find that honor ideology strongly and robustly predicts a higher likelihood of participating in political violence
among male political activists. A number of previous studies found a macro-level relationship between gender
equality and peacefulness in a society. This study provides evidence for one micro-level mechanism linking gender
equality and political violence at the macro level. Based on these results, we conclude that honor ideology endorse-
ment is a driver of violence in political conflicts.

Keywords
gender, honor, masculinities, political violence, survey, Thailand

Although collective violence during a political struggle can Thailand, where the political struggles between the so-
be extremely destructive and have far-reaching conse- called ‘red-shirts’ and ‘yellow-shirts’ were predominantly
quences, the number of people using violence for political nonviolent, but a small number of individuals took to arms,
purposes is often surprisingly small. For example, in the causing destruction and violence that contributed to an
conflict over Northern Ireland between 1975 and 1991, escalation of the conflict and, eventually, a military coup.
the number of troops of the Provisional Irish Republican Who decides to use violence in a political struggle?
Army (IRA) was at no time higher than 500. Compared to This important question has been the subject of
the population of Ukraine at 45 million, the armed Self-
Defence Group of the Maidan protest movement is
reported to consist of only 12,000 members (UCDP Con- Corresponding author:
flict Encyclopedia). In this article we focus on the case of Elin.Bjarnegard@statsvet.uu.se

This content downloaded from


45.77.104.39 on Tue, 19 Jul 2022 21:42:32 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Bjarnegård et al. 749

surprisingly little systematic research. Most studies on that conceives of honor as male societal privilege and
the causes of collective violence within a state have used control over female sexuality, that is, patriarchal values,
aggregate units of analysis, such as country-years or and the other that focuses on ideals of masculine tough-
opposition movements. As a result, the explanations pro- ness, that is the perceived necessity for men to be fierce
vided by these studies tend to be structural in character, and respond to affronts with violence or threats of vio-
for example pointing to poverty, semi-democratic lence in order to preserve their status. We argue that
political institutions, or economic dependency on patriarchal values combined with ideals of masculine
natural-resource extraction (Blattman & Miguel, 2010; toughness together constitute honor ideology, which con-
Wimmer, 2014). While such aggregate structural expla- tributes in turn to the explanation of who fights in polit-
nations help to identify societies at risk, they have little or ical conflicts. We propose that patriarchal values lead to
no leverage when it comes to characterizing who uses othering, and that masculine toughness drives violent
violence. We thus know very little about what distin- aggression, so that honor ideology predisposes men to
guishes the small minority that engages in political participate in political violence. Previous studies have
violence from the large majority that does not. explained why variables such as right-wing authoritarian-
A small stream of research has recently begun inves- ism or machismo predispose people for extremism or
tigating the causes of violent participation by comparing sexual violence; we similarly investigate the role of honor
individuals who have used violence with those who have ideology in political violence.
not. Summarizing the existing evidence on individual-
level attributes that predict participation in various forms
of collective violence, McDoom notes that there is Political violence in Thailand
robust support for sociodemographic characteristics, in Thailand’s modern history is filled with political conflict,
particular age and gender, and ‘more contingent support mass demonstrations, and military takeovers (see e.g.
for socio-economic attributes’ (2013: 455). In other Bjarnegård, 2013). The two most recent military coups
words, we know that the relatively small numbers who took place in 2006 and 2014. In this article, we focus on
participate in collective violence are predominantly male the volatile period between these two coups. During this
young adults. Literature debating why people participate time, two groups were taking turns to demonstrate in the
in violence has mostly emphasized different aspects of streets. The groups were often referred to simply as the
grievance, selective incentives, and social networks, but ‘yellow-shirts’ and the ‘red-shirts’, but the official names
there is little consensus on what factors matter. Hum- of the main actors were the People’s Alliance for Democ-
phreys & Weinstein argue that several different causes racy (PAD) and the National United Front of Democ-
for why people engage in armed violence ‘coexist within racy Against Dictatorship (UDD), respectively. The
a single civil war’ (2008: 437). McDoom states that conflict was mainly played out in the streets of the capital
‘[b]eyond age, gender and debatably socio-economic sta- city Bangkok, but its origins lay in a division between
tus, a scholarly consensus is crystallizing that violent city and countryside. The privileged, urban ‘yellow-
perpetrators do not possess distinguishing individual shirts’ mobilized against the increasing influence of the
characteristics’ (2013: 455). What sets participants in ‘red-shirts’ who were of predominantly rural origin, so-
political violence apart from non-participants remains a called ‘urbanized villagers’ (Thabchumpon & McCargo,
puzzle, something that we address in this article. 2011). The ‘red-shirts’ were initially mobilized to oppose
We test a novel argument that relates honor ideology the coup in 2006 that had ousted the then prime min-
to participation in political violence. Our study concerns ister Thaksin Shinawatra (e.g. Dalpino, 2011). Increas-
people who make a decision to participate in violence for ingly, however, the movement went beyond being about
political purposes – not people who have been forcefully supporting Thaksin, to centering more generally on
abducted or conscripted. We develop our argument from questions of access to development, wealth, political
the observation that men are strongly overrepresented in inclusion, and influence (Sopranzetti, 2012). The vast
political violence, although most men do not participate. majority of the tens of thousands of demonstrators were
Literature on men and masculinities has long empha- peaceful, orderly, and with moderate demands (Pasuk &
sized the role of honor and its links to different forms Baker, 2012; Stent, 2012).
of violence, such as domestic abuse, criminal violence, Officially, the ‘red-shirts’ and ‘yellow-shirts’ depicted
and violent attitudes (such as approval of torture and themselves as demonstrating citizens and they advocated
racism). Building on this literature, we discern two sep- nonviolent but increasingly disruptive methods, such as
arate but related strands of research around honor: one occupying government buildings, international airports,

This content downloaded from


45.77.104.39 on Tue, 19 Jul 2022 21:42:32 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
750 journal of PEACE RESEARCH 54(6)

and large parts of Bangkok (Chaisukkosol, 2010; Mon- voluntary. Some protesters were prepared to use vio-
tesano, 2012). However, the two groups were growing lence, although did not necessarily plan to do so. Hence,
social movements rather than coherent organizations. As it seems to have been an individual decision to bring a
such, they were loosely made up of people with similar gun or other type of weapon to a demonstration. Some
ideological convictions, but with internal differences and may have armed themselves to be prepared to defend
fractured leaderships that resulted in different sub- themselves if attacked. For instance, much of the 2010
elements advocating different strategies and methods – violence and destruction of infrastructure took place
including the question of whether or not to use violence when the military intervened against protesters.
(Chambers, 2010; Thabchumpon & McCargo, 2011). This study concentrates on the small minority of pro-
For instance, even though the ‘red-shirt’ UDD leaders testors who decided to use violence in the ‘red–yellow’
publically distanced themselves from the alleged chief conflict in Thailand, despite being part of predominantly
trainer of a hardcore armed group known as the ‘men nonviolent social movements.
in black’, ‘Seh Daeng’,1 a personality cult developed
around him and many ordinary UDD members were
inspired by him ‘as a symbol of masculinity, daring, and Previous explanations for participation in
resistance to authority’ (Thabchumpon & McCargo, political violence
2011: 998). As mentioned, a small number of studies have in recent
From time to time, violence did erupt during demon- years begun to systematically examine what sets volun-
strations, but it remains unclear which individuals used tary participants in collective violence apart from the
violence, and under whose orders. Death tolls and inju- great majority that never joins. The most recurrent
ries rose when the police stepped in against the ‘yellow- results are that young men are highly overrepresented.
shirts’ occupying the government complex in 2008, and The fact that all forms of collective violence are
in particular, when the military was called in to disperse extremely gendered is often not problematized in the
the ‘red-shirts’ who were occupying entire blocks of cen- studies reviewed here, but is rather taken as a given, so
tral Bangkok in 2010 (e.g. Montesano, 2012; Prasirtsuk, that the male sex is viewed merely as a control variable or
2010). In 2010, despite the fact that most demonstrators selection criterion.2 Yet, the estimation that less than 1%
were nonviolent, buildings and property worth billions of all warriors in history have been women (Goldstein,
of baht were destroyed and around 100 people were 2001: 10) needs to be taken into account when searching
killed, most by the military. When members of the mil- for explanations for who takes to arms.
itary launched their 2014 coup, they argued that they Beyond sex and age, different socio-economic attributes
had to restore peace and order to the country. have been found to predict violent participation, although
Over time, there were more radical and armed ele- the results differ between studies. Humphreys & Wein-
ments aligning with both movements, and these armed stein (2008) find that political alienation, poverty, and low
elements were involved in attacks that included the use education were significant predictors for voluntarily taking
of assault weapons and arson (e.g. Thabchumpon & up arms in the civil war in Sierra Leone. The authors also
McCargo, 2011). However, the ‘red-shirts’ and ‘yel- report that fighters were offered money to join, and that
low-shirts’ in Thailand cannot be described as armed they said they felt safer as part of an armed group.
rebel groups with a primary purpose of fighting state Analyzing the economic profile of perpetrators of the
forces or other groups using violence. The vast majority Rwandan genocide, Verwimp (2005) finds that both
of protesters did not use violence and were not armed. those who were economically and socially vulnerable,
Guns and weapons were not openly encouraged or cen- and therefore potentially had much to gain from partic-
trally distributed through any official channels. In addi- ipation, and the local elite who had something to defend,
tion, there is no indication that the underground militias were overrepresented among the killers (Verwimp,
used forced recruitment. Although violent methods may 2005). A study of participation in deadly riots in Nigeria
have been encouraged by certain leaders, and although shows that the number of individuals with strong
some leaders may have inspired protesters to use vio-
lence, ultimately participation in political violence was 2
Important exceptions include Carpenter (2006), who discusses
male overrepresentation in war mortality rates as gender-based
violence, and Thomas & Bond (2015), who discuss the openness
1
His real name was Khattiya Sawasdipol. He died from a sniper of violent political organizations to recruit women as key to
attack in May 2010. explaining female participation in violent armed conflict.

This content downloaded from


45.77.104.39 on Tue, 19 Jul 2022 21:42:32 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Bjarnegård et al. 751

grievances was much higher than the actual number of Explanations that involve seeking glory and respect in the
participants (Scacco, 2008). Scacco argues that grievance local community, and expressing outrage over injustices,
measures therefore are poor predictors of violent participa- are intriguing but also raise new questions. Why is it that
tion. Instead, she finds that the interaction of subjectively only some individuals seek glory and respect through vio-
experienced grievances with membership in local social lence? As researchers repeatedly point out, there is also a
networks explains who participates and who does not: peo- risk that people misrepresent their true motivations in self-
ple who felt poor in comparison to their neighbors and who serving ways when asked about why they fought (e.g.
attended local community meetings before the outbreak of Collier, 2007). In what follows, we argue that men who
riots were more likely to participate. Lower education also more strongly endorse honor ideology are more prone to
predicted participation (Scacco, 2008). political violence. We then proceed to test this hypothesis
McDoom (2013) finds that individuals were more in survey data collected among both participants and non-
prone to participate in the Rwandan genocide if they participants in political violence in Thailand.
lived in the same neighborhood as other participants,
and if other family members participated, and that these Approaching honor
results cannot be accounted for by age, gender, or socio-
economic status. McDoom thus concludes that it mat- In the most general sense, honor has been defined as ‘the
ters where an individual lives, and that mechanisms of right to be treated as having a certain worth’ and ‘a right
social influence, such as peer pressure, help to distinguish to respect’ (Stewart, 1994: 21). Also in a general sense,
who becomes a killer. most societies and cultures have a concept of honor that
Other studies focus more on in-depth understanding means something along the lines of honesty, integrity,
of the participants of political violence than on compar- and virtuous conduct. Honor also has a more sinister
ison with non-participants. For example, a study of meaning, however. The traits and behaviors that give a
recruitment to the Viet Cong guerillas in the context person honor in this latter sense concern strength and
of the Vietnam war in the early 1960s lists a number domination, and the respect that is earned entails the
of reasons for joining, including discontent and safety right to privilege and precedence (Nisbett & Cohen,
concerns, and specifically highlights that for many young 1996: 4). Two conceptions of honor that involve dom-
recruits ‘the desire to win glory, or perhaps just the ination and precedence can be discerned in the literature:
respect of their community’ was the main reason for patriarchal values and ideals of masculine toughness. We
joining (Donnell, 1967: xii). In a similar vein, Wood suggest that both must be examined in order to under-
(2001) argues that individuals who joined the FMLN stand how honor can be the driving force behind an
insurgents in El Salvador in the 1970s reaped emotional individual’s decision to participate in violence
benefits, because participation expressed their moral out-
rage at the injustices they had experienced, often in the Patriarchal values and othering
form of government violence against them or their fam- The first strand of research on honor emphasizes the role of
ilies. Fighting represented the reassertion of their per- patriarchal values in honor. Patriarchal values entail a fun-
sonal dignity, and led to a sense of pride. These damental dichotomization between ‘us’ and ‘them’,
guerillas were not expecting any material benefits; for namely between men and women, and a hierarchical power
them, fighting had a value in itself despite the obvious relationship privileging men. The othering of women
risks and costs, because of these emotional benefits. means that they are devalued, deprived of privileges, and
Considerable progress has been made in research controlled, relative to men. The two pillars of this ‘othering
about individual-level determinants of participation in logic’ correspond to what has been called the ‘gender sys-
political violence, but we still know little about what tem’. Hirdman (1988: 63) claims that the gender system
differentiates participants from non-participants.3 Expla- shows a regularity based on two different rules or principles:
natory factors like poverty, political alienation, and local
The first is the rule of distinctive separation, which can
networks would seem to pertain to more individuals than
be seen in the division of virtually all areas and levels of
the very small number who actually join out of choice.
life into male and female categories. The second rule is
that of the male norm, i.e. the way that higher value is
almost automatically accorded to things masculine.
3
With ‘participants’ we mean those who use violence in political
struggle. We operationalize ‘participation’ for the purposes of our Hudson and co-authors (Hudson & den Boer, 2012;
study in the Design and methods section below. Hudson et al., 2009; Hudson et al., 2012) review the

This content downloaded from


45.77.104.39 on Tue, 19 Jul 2022 21:42:32 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
752 journal of PEACE RESEARCH 54(6)

study of othering related to patriarchal values, and link the strongest predictor for sexual violence against women
such othering to proneness to violence. Psychological was a combination of adhering to a masculine ideology
studies show that human beings notice three basic dif- that included the acceptance of aggression against
ferences almost from infancy: age, gender, and race. women and holding negative, hostile beliefs regarding
Furthermore, the first adults that most children observe women. Santana, Raj & Decker (2006) found that men
regularly interacting are their parents. The way in which who reported more ‘traditional masculine gender role
men and women relate to each other therefore becomes ideologies’ had engaged in sexual risk behaviors and inti-
the fundamental template for differentiating between mate partner violence (IPV) perpetration to a significantly
groups of people, and to the extent that the father dom- higher degree than others. In a recent study on male
inates and controls the mother, this model teaches the perpetration of IPV conducted in the Asia-Pacific region
child that domination by one group over another is involving over 10,000 men in six countries, Jewkes et al.
appropriate and normal. (2013) found that the most important explanatory factors
Psychological research also shows that children who for IPV were norms and practices relating to gender and
grow up in violent homes have a greater tendency to relationships. Using data from Africa, Velitchkova (2015)
become violent themselves (Alexander, Morre & Alex- shows that individuals who support patriarchal norms
ander, 1991; Ehrensaft et al., 2003; see also Velitchkova, have a stronger tendency to participate in political vio-
2015). Hudson et al. hold that ‘the first “other” is always lence. These studies follow a long line of scholarship
woman, and if one can make peace with the first other pointing to the significance of sex role stereotyping (Burt,
without resorting to coercion, one will have a template in 1980), norms of hypermasculinity (Mosher & Sirkin,
place to know how to do so with other “others’” (Hud- 1984), and hostile masculinity (Malamuth et al., 1991)
son & den Boer, 2012: 317). In other words, when for explaining men’s violence against women.
patriarchal dominance by men over women is reduced,
the type of othering that breeds intolerance and violence
is weakened, and more companionate marriages instead Ideals of masculine toughness
serve as templates for respectful and equal interaction The second strand of research looks at honor from the
with others (Hudson & den Boer, 2012; Hudson point of view of ideals of masculine toughness. According
et al., 2009; Hudson et al., 2012). to this perspective, honor depends on violence or threats
The process of othering and dehumanizing has been of violence as a means to protect a man’s reputation and
referred to as a sociopsychological process that is a pre- maintain his societal status. Indeed, Nisbett & Cohen
requisite for being able to consider killing another human (1996: 4) claim that the distinguishing feature of cul-
being (Potts & Hayden, 2008: 50). In the context of war, tures where honor is strong is that men ‘are prepared to
such othering has been argued to lead to the perception fight or even to kill to defend their reputations as honor-
that the perpetration of violence – including killing – is able men’. Another key aspect is the importance placed
the right thing to do (Staub, 1989). This psychological on insults and displays of disrespect. If a man’s social
process implies a strong differentiation between oneself standing and power is built on his preparedness for vio-
and the enemy, to the extent that enemies are devalued lence, not reacting to an insult can be interpreted as a
and excluded from the moral realm (Staub, 2006). In line lack of strength and an incapacity to protect himself and
with the literature reviewed above we argue that the other- what belongs to him – including the female members of
ing and devaluation of group differences inherent in patri- his family (Nisbett & Cohen, 1996). The extent to
archal values provide a template for justifying violence which insults or affronts to a man’s female family mem-
against collectives such as political opponents. bers are considered threats to the man’s own honor is, in
A host of studies have found that patriarchal values are turn, related to patriarchal values. In other words, trying
related to violent outcomes, both at the societal level and to live up to the ideals of masculine toughness requires
at the individual level. Cohrs et al. (2007) argue that men, in particular, to use violence.
egalitarian values in society are linked to more positive In many societies, ‘honor’ has been used as justification
orientations toward human rights; conversely, Feather & for interpersonal violence. Ethnographer David Mandel-
McKee (2012) demonstrate a link between the value baum, studying honor in North India, describes this con-
systems underlying right-wing authoritarianism and cept as ‘a word often heard in men’s talk, particularly
social dominance orientation on the one hand, and pre- when the talk is about conflict, rivalry and struggle. It
judice against women on the other. In a meta-analytic crops up as a kind of final explanation for motivation,
review, Murnen, Wright & Kaluzny (2002) found that whether for acts of aggression or beneficence’

This content downloaded from


45.77.104.39 on Tue, 19 Jul 2022 21:42:32 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Bjarnegård et al. 753

(Mandelbaum, 1988: 20). Several other studies also find women’s sexuality is to be controlled and defended by
that attitudes that are linked to this understanding of men (McGregor, 2003; Nagel, 2005).
honor are important factors in the study of violence. Ideals of masculine toughness endow individuals
Barnes, Brown & Osterman (2012) found that ideals of steeped in patriarchal values with an additional impetus
masculine toughness among men in the USA were linked to to violence. Thus, we propose that patriarchal values lead
more militant attitudes toward terrorism, such as support- to othering, and that masculine toughness drives violent
ing the use of torture during interrogation. aggression, so that the combination of patriarchal values
Similarly, the link between honor and violence is key and masculine toughness, which we refer to as honor
in the literature on the ‘code of the street’ (Anderson, ideology, predisposes men to participate in political vio-
1999). Anderson found that in low-status groups in dis- lence. We will investigate this proposition with the help
advantaged communities, self-respect and honor were of survey data on attitudes regarding gender and actual
dependent on cultivating a tough reputation, whereby any participation in political violence in Thailand. As
insult must be avenged with violence. Brezina et al. (2004) described above, Thailand has seen several episodes of
conducted a longitudinal study with 900 adolescent men political violence in recent years. While both the ‘red-
and found that youths who (at time 1) held street-code shirt’ movement and the ‘yellow-shirt’ movement
beliefs, that is, beliefs that violence is an appropriate and described themselves as peaceful, some radical elements
legitimate response to disrespect and insults to honor, used violence. Thailand thus represents a case where
were involved in more violence than others (at time 2, there have been ample opportunities for participating
measured one year later), including deadly violence. in political violence, and where provocations and calls
to stand up for a particular cause have been fairly com-
mon. Structural and organizational characteristics inher-
ent in the political conflict provided a context in which
Honor ideology as patriarchal values and ideals
violence was more of an option, but individual activists
of masculine toughness have nevertheless had a choice. Thailand is also a country
We conceive of honor ideology as the combination of in which gender roles have shifted quite remarkably in
patriarchal values and ideals of masculine toughness. Honor some areas but not in others. Women are highly visible
ideology is present in some way or other in all contempo- in the public sphere and are participating in the work-
rary countries and cultures, although individuals vary in force to a large extent, but they have not reached high
the degree to which they endorse it. We argue that the levels of representation in the political sphere (Bjar-
othering associated with patriarchal values predisposes negård, 2013). Thailand thus provides an interesting
men to violence, and that it becomes particularly danger- case for our analysis as we can expect to find variation
ous when it is combined with the proneness to violence both in participation in political violence and in attitudes
that is associated with idealizing masculine toughness. We concerning honor ideology. Focusing on Thailand also
also think that these two components tend to correlate to addresses the identified lack of studies on honor and
some extent, although most previous research has focused violence in East Asia (Vandello & Cohen, 2008).
on either one or the other. We will therefore test whether
honor ideology in this sense, that is, the combination of
Design and methods4
patriarchal values and ideals of masculine toughness, pre-
dicts participation in political violence in Thailand. This study builds on the Survey on Gender, Politics and
We are inspired by Mahalingam, who states that the Violence in Thailand (more information on the survey
reputation and standing of men in cultures of honor design can be found at [http://www.pcr.uu.se/research/
hinge to a large extent on two things: men demonstrating gender-and-conflict/survey-on-gender–politics-and-vio
aggressiveness in the face of insult; and the chastity and lence-in-thailand/]), that was conducted in collaboration
loyalty of their female family members (Mahalingam, with King Prajadhipok’s Institute (KPI) in Bangkok,
2007). There is also a feminist literature investigating Thailand. Data were collected between November
the role of honor in nationalism that has, at least 2012 and February 2013; in total 1,200 questionnaires
implicitly, linked patriarchal values and ideals of mascu-
line toughness to the concept of honor. In nationalist 4
In the following, a short description of the main characteristics of
discourses, the nation is often likened to a family; as the design and methods will be provided. A more detailed description
such, men and women are assigned different roles. As of the design and methods of the survey study can be found in
wives and mothers, women are bearers of honor, and Appendix A of the Online appendix.

This content downloaded from


45.77.104.39 on Tue, 19 Jul 2022 21:42:32 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
754 journal of PEACE RESEARCH 54(6)

were completed. We assumed that participation in polit- Dependent variable: Participation in Political Violence
ical violence (our dependent variable) will generally be (PPV)
very rare in Thailand, as in most societies. We therefore The dependent variable is ‘participation in political
collected two sets of data: first, a cluster survey of 200 violence’, and it is measured by the following three
respondents who are politically active as either ‘red- questions:
shirts’ or ‘yellow-shirts’, and second, a nationally repre-
sentative sample of 1,000 respondents. The reason for 1. Have you ever carried a weapon (e.g. a gun,
drawing special samples with politically active and pos- bomb, knife, or club) during a protest for polit-
sibly radical ‘red-shirt’ and ‘yellow-shirt’ members was to ical purposes?
try to obtain larger variation in the rare phenomenon of 2. Have you ever used a weapon during a protest for
participation in political violence. The activist sample is political purposes?
the main sample of interest for this study. We will how- 3. Have you ever caused damage (to a building, car,
ever use the nationally representative sample to construct or infrastructure, or assaulted others) during a
our measures of honor in Thailand. protest for political purposes?
The two hundred political activist interviewees – 100 Responding ‘yes’ to any of these three questions yields
‘red-shirts’ and 100 ‘yellow-shirts’ – were chosen by pur- a code 1 on the dichotomous dependent variable PPV
posive sampling, with 20 interviewees per district in ten because it denotes that an activist has either carried out
districts that were considered to be either ‘red’ or ‘yellow’ acts of violence or at least considered violence as an
strongholds. The representative survey of the national option before going to a demonstration.
population was conducted by selecting 1,000 interviewees In an alternative test, we used a more restrictive
using multi-stage random sampling: (1) regional sampling, dependent variable. This alternative dependent variable
(2) district sampling, (3) subdistrict sampling, and (4) does not include causing material damage as part of
household sampling. There are 76 provinces in Thailand, PPV; in this test, only bringing a weapon to or using
and 37 were surveyed. More details about both the pur- a weapon in a political protest counts as PPV. In yet
posive sampling of ‘red-shirt’ and ‘yellow-shirt’ activists, as another alternative test we used an even more restrictive
well as about the nationally representative survey of the dependent variable. This alternative dependent variable
Thai population, can be found in Online Appendix A. takes the value 1 if the respondent admits to actually
Most of the survey questionnaire was conducted in having used a weapon during a protest for political
face-to-face interviews; however, one section containing purposes, and 0 otherwise.
sensitive questions on personal experiences of violence
and the personal use of violence was self-administered on
paper. This has been recommended in earlier studies that Explanatory variables
have demonstrated that self-administration of sensitive In the theory section, we argued that honor ideology
questions tends to reduce interviewer effects such as should be associated with PPV, and that honor ideology
social desirability bias and thereby increase the probabil- consists of the combination of an attitude that embraces
ity of getting honest answers even to sensitive questions ideals of patriarchal values and masculine toughness. The
(see Jewkes et al., 2013 and more information in Online implication of our theoretical argument is that the appro-
Appendix A). According to our argument, honor ideol- priate way of testing for the effect of honor ideology is to
ogy predisposes men, but not women, to participate in test for the effects on PPV of the interaction of patriar-
political violence. Consequently, we only use the male chal values and ideals of masculine toughness.
part of the sample in the analyses. There are 113 men Based on previous research, we developed survey
among the 200 activists, and 572 men in the nationally questions to capture our proposed construct of honor
representative sample.5 ideology. In doing so, we take on Vandello &
Cohen’s call for developing better ‘individual-level
measures of the endorsement of honor beliefs and
5
Both our theory and our analysis focus on explaining male violence. values’ (2008: 662).6
Although the overwhelming majority of warriors in the history of
warfare have indeed been male, we do acknowledge that women
6
become warriors too. Out of the 200 activists, eight women and 25 Our study also addresses two additional lacunae articulated by these
men participated in political violence. The ideologies and motives of authors: the need to bring the research on honor and violence to East
women participating in political violence, and possible links to honor Asia, as well as to groups outside the traditional college student
ideology, are in our view an important topic for future research. sample (Vandello & Cohen, 2008).

This content downloaded from


45.77.104.39 on Tue, 19 Jul 2022 21:42:32 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Bjarnegård et al. 755

Table I. Results of exploratory factor analysis of Patriarchal Values


Item Range Factor loading Scoring coefficients
Overall, men are better political leaders than women. Strongly Disagree/Disagree/ 0.56 0.18
Agree/Strongly Agree
Overall, men are able to administrate a business better than Strongly Disagree/Disagree/ 0.64 0.27
women. Agree/Strongly Agree
University education is more important to men than to Strongly Disagree/Disagree/ 0.54 0.17
women. Agree/Strongly Agree
The husband should make the final decision on all matters. Strongly Disagree/Disagree/ 0.50 0.15
Agree/Strongly Agree
A woman should bear violence to keep her family together. Ten-step scale: 1 (Disagree) – 0.40 0.11
10 (Agree)
It is appropriate to beat a child for his/her good upbringing. Ten-step scale: 1 (Disagree) – 0.44 0.12
10 (Agree)
A good wife should obey her husband despite disagreeing Ten-step scale: 1 (Disagree) – 0.59 0.21
with him. 10 (Agree)
A woman should remain a virgin until her marriage. Ten-step scale: 1 (Disagree) – 0.46 0.16
10 (Agree)
It is a man’s duty to protect his family’s dignity by watching Ten-step scale: 1 (Disagree) – 0.39 0.13
over his woman’s chastity and ethics. 10 (Agree)

To capture the first element of honor ideology, we scores. Despite referring to different spheres in life, this
constructed the measure of Patriarchal values as an index, analysis demonstrates that these items taken together rep-
composed of a combination of nine different questions. resent a single dimension that has to do with the view of
The questions are developed from previous theory and the appropriate roles of men and women in a society. We
surveys (Vandello & Cohen, 2003; Figueredo et al., will hereafter refer to this index as Patriarchal values.
2001; Neff, 2001) and the index has been constructed Ideals of masculine toughness (henceforth shortened to
using factor analysis. In line with theory, we argue that masculine toughness) is captured by two statements
patriarchal values consist of the belief in male privilege derived from previous theory (Nisbett & Cohen, 1996;
and dominance in society at large as well as in the family, Brezina et al., 2004; Barnes et al., 2012) and by adding
and that they extend to control over female sexuality. We the answers to these two survey items. The first item is a
thus expect that a set of questionnaire items tapping into dichotomous indicator that reflects whether or not the
these attitudes can be combined to measure patriarchal respondent agrees with the statement ‘A man shouldn’t
values with more precision than any item used by itself. show emotions and weakness’. The second item is likewise
We submitted nine items that we, based on theory, a dichotomous indicator that reflects whether or not the
believe tap into patriarchal values to an exploratory factor respondent agrees with the statement ‘It is fair for a man
analysis using principal axis factoring as the extraction to assault anyone who has spread a rumor that he is a
method (Table I). coward’. Taken together, these two statements capture the
These nine items measure the view of appropriate roles main components of masculine toughness as described in
of men and women in different spheres of life, including the literature: that a man’s reputation and social status is
in the family, education, working-life, and politics. One upheld by emotional detachment and by displaying signs
dominant factor emerged, accounting for 82% of the total of strength (for example through using threats of vio-
variance, with an eigenvalue of 2.34. No other factor had lence), not least in response to insults and signs of disre-
an eigenvalue greater than 1, which indicates that a single spect (Nisbett & Cohen, 1996). The resulting variable
dimension captured the bulk of the variation in these Masculine toughness thus ranges from 0 to 2 depending
items. All items load positively and strongly or moderately on whether the respondent agreed with neither of the two
strongly on the first factor, and the alpha internal relia- items, with one item, or both statements.
bility value for the resulting index is an acceptable 0.67. Based on our reading of the relevant literatures, we
We therefore use these items combined as our index of argue that Patriarchal values and Masculine toughness are
patriarchal values. The last column of Table I gives the separate but related elements of honor ideology. The
regression coefficients used to estimate the individual correlation between Patriarchal values and Masculine

This content downloaded from


45.77.104.39 on Tue, 19 Jul 2022 21:42:32 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
756 journal of PEACE RESEARCH 54(6)

Table II. The effect of honor ideology on participation in political violence


Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4
Dependent variable Standard Standard Alternative DV1 Alternative DV2
Patriarchal values –0.74 0.35 –0.68 –1.03
(0.45) (0.28) (0.47) (0.63)

Masculine toughness 1.53 1.56 1.55


(0.56)** (0.59)** (0.783)*

Patriarchal values  0.78 0.87 0.99


Masculine toughness (0.42) (0.45) (0.48)*

Constant –2.59 –0.94 –2.73 –4.02


(0.70)** (0.29)** (0.73)** (1.11)**
N 83 87 83 83
* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01

toughness is positive as expected, but not very strong In Model 1 we examine the effect of honor ideology
(0.23). Moreover, if the variable measuring Masculine by including the two component variables Patriarchal
toughness is added to the factor analysis of the items that values and Masculine toughness together with the interac-
constitute Patriarchal values, it loads only weakly on the tion term Patriarchal values  Masculine toughness.
first factor (0.27). Thus, we consider it to be warranted Model 2 tests for an unconditional effect of Patriarchal
to treat Patriarchal values and Masculine toughness as sep- values, but no significant effect is found for the uncondi-
arate but related elements of honor ideology. Next, we tional effect. Model 3 uses the first alternative, more
will test whether these two elements indeed interact as restrictive dependent variable (which does not include
we expect in explaining PPV. causing material damage). Model 4 uses the most restric-
We construct the interaction term Patriarchal val- tive dependent variable (which only includes admitting
ues  Masculine toughness by multiplying the index to actual use of a weapon during political protests).
Patriarchal values with the indicator for Masculine Interpreting the coefficients for the components that
toughness. When testing for the effect of honor ideol- together capture an interaction is not straightforward
ogy, we will thus include both the component vari- when dealing with regressions with a binary dependent
ables Patriarchal values and Masculine toughness as variable. The sign, magnitude, and significance of the
well as the interaction term Patriarchal values  individual coefficients that together capture the interac-
Masculine toughness in our baseline regression models, tion effect (Patriarchal values, Masculine toughness, and
as is standard practice. Patriarchal values  Masculine toughness) tell us nothing
In addition, we used a number of control variables: age, in and of themselves about the existence and statistical
education, income, importance of politics, importance of significance of substantially meaningful interaction effects
religion, marital status, identification as a Muslim, iden- in terms of the relevant probabilities (i.e. the likelihood of
tification as a ‘red’ sympathizer, identification as a ‘yellow’ PPV). As pointed out by Ai and Norton, the interaction
sympathizer, identification as a military veteran, identifi- effect can be non-zero even if the coefficient of the inter-
cation as Malayu ethnicity, and employment status. action term (Patriarchal values  Masculine toughness) is
0; the statistical significance of the interaction cannot be
tested with a simple t-test on the interaction term; and
Results the sign of the interaction term is not necessarily the same
The purpose of our analysis is to determine if honor as the sign of the interaction effect (Ai & Norton, 2003).
ideology played a part in the individual decisions to par- Using the software Clarify (Tomz, Wittenberg & King,
ticipate in or prepare for political violence among ‘red- 2003; King, Tomz & Wittenberg, 2000), we calculated
shirt’ and ‘yellow-shirt’ activists in Thailand. Table II pre- the change in the predicted risk of PPV for different
sents multivariate logit regressions testing whether honor combinations of variable values in Model 1.
ideology is associated with a higher likelihood of PPV Table III reports the change in the predicted likeli-
among male activists. hood of PPV for three different scenarios, all three of

This content downloaded from


45.77.104.39 on Tue, 19 Jul 2022 21:42:32 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Bjarnegård et al. 757

Table III. Changes in predicted probability of participation in political violence when Masculine toughness changes from 0 to 1
Scenario [1] Scenario [2] Scenario [3]
Low Patriarchal values ¼–1.97 Medium Patriarchal values ¼–0.57 High Patriarchal values ¼ 0.62
(10th percentile) (59th percentile) (90th percentile)
Not significant 11% to 26% 6% to 27%

which show the effect of a change in Masculine toughness These gender ideologies influence the propensity to use
from 0 to 1 (41% of the male activists had Masculine violence more than the political ideologies they fight
toughness ¼ 0 whereas 47% scored 1 on this variable and over. Neither being a ‘yellow-shirt’ nor being a ‘red-shirt’
12% had the highest value 2). The three scenarios differ has any statistically significant effect on participation in
in the value of the conditioning variable Patriarchal political violence in any of the models (as shown in
values, and scenario [1] looks at low Patriarchal values. Online Appendix B, Tables B3, B7, B11).
Note that the effects in scenario [1] are not statistically As pointed out above, we analyze the data on acti-
significant, and that this finding agrees with our expec- vists; given how rare participation in political violence is
tations. In other words, Masculine toughness has no sta- among ordinary people it follows that it would have
tistically significant effect when Patriarchal values are been very difficult to obtain statistically significant
low. It is the combination of the two components of results had we instead used the nationally representative
honor that creates the predisposition to participate in sample. Indeed, only eight men (out of 533) in the
political violence. The effect of the one-unit increase nationally representative sample reported having parti-
from 0 to 1 in Masculine toughness is significant from cipated in political violence, and hence running multi-
levels of Patriarchal values equal to or greater than –.57, ple logistic regression using this sample is not
which among the activists is the 59th percentile of this meaningful because of the very small cell counts. Seven
variable. Scenario [2] shows the effect of a change in out of the eight agreed with one of the two items used
Masculine toughness from 0 to 1 when Patriarchal values to capture Masculine toughness, whereas the eighth par-
are somewhat higher. The statistically significant effect at ticipating non-activist did not answer the question
the 59th percentile is strong in that the predicted prob- about men showing emotions and weakness. Further-
ability of PPV is more than twice as high (26% compared more, in the national sample the average value of Patri-
to 11%) for a male activist with Masculine toughness 1 archal values among those who had participated in
compared to an activist with Masculine toughness 0. Strik- political violence was more than three times the average
ingly, scenario [3] shows a very strong effect when Patri- value of those who had not (0.55 versus 0.16). These
archal values are high, at the 90th percentile: a change in numbers together suggest that honor ideology may
Masculine toughness from 0 to 1 now corresponds to a operate in a similar way among non-activists, although
more than four-fold increase in the risk of PPV, from this cannot be tested with these data. The unsurprising
6% to 27%. fact that participation in political violence is much rarer
These numbers show that Patriarchal values has a among ordinary Thais than among the activists also
strong conditioning effect on the relationship between suggests that other violent triggers exist among activists.
Masculine toughness and PPV. Moreover, the effect of Potentially, encouragement to use violence from within
Masculine toughness is very strong in substantial terms the movement, or inspiration from idealized radical
when combined with higher levels of Patriarchal values. front-figures, may trigger violence among those that
On the other hand, there is no statistically significant subscribe to honor ideology and who already see vio-
relationship between Masculine toughness and PPV lence as a justifiable way of solving political conflicts.
among the men with low Patriarchal values. These results The importance of such triggers for honor ideology to
are in line with our arguments about the function of translate into violent political actions is an important
honor ideology with regard to participation in political avenue for future research.
violence. In other words, ‘red-shirt’ and ‘yellow-shirt’ A number of control variables were added, one by
male activists with strongly patriarchal values in combi- one, to each of Models 1–3.7 The controls failed to
nation with strong ideals of masculine toughness were far
more likely to participate in political violence than were
7
activists who did not agree with these values and ideals. See Tables B1 to B12 in Online Appendix B.

This content downloaded from


45.77.104.39 on Tue, 19 Jul 2022 21:42:32 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
758 journal of PEACE RESEARCH 54(6)

achieve significance, whereas the results for honor ideol- they do. In particular, male activists who more strongly
ogy remained similar throughout. We must also ask: is endorse honor ideology are more likely to have partici-
it possible that embracing honor ideology is a conse- pated in political violence than other male activists. We
quence of being exposed to violence or threats of vio- argue that honor ideology consists of patriarchal values
lence, so that the direction of causality is from (i.e. male societal privilege and control over female sexu-
participation in violence to honor ideology? A coherent ality) combined with ideals of masculine toughness (i.e.
argument can be made that people become more war- the perceived necessity for men to display fierceness in
like in their attitudes when their environment and order to preserve their status).
experiences suggest that there is a need for manly war- Thailand is a country that has seen recent political
riors. We explored this possibility in several different violence in predominantly nonviolent ‘red-’ and ‘yel-
ways, and conclude that there is no indication that our low-shirt’ demonstrations. More than 100 fatalities were
results are due to reverse causality. incurred and enormous material damage inflicted in this
First, we tested whether honor ideology is systemati- political conflict. We collected and analyzed survey data
cally related to having served in the armed forces. All in order to examine the effect of honor ideology on
armed forces socialize recruits into a warrior role and try participation in political violence in Thailand. Our find-
to convince them of the necessity and virtue of military ings were that honor ideology strongly and robustly pre-
service. One would thus expect that men who have dicted a higher likelihood of participation among male
served in the armed forces would espouse honor ideology political activists in Thailand. Based on these results, we
more strongly than other men, if the causal direction is conclude that honor ideology endorsement is a driver of
the opposite from what we hypothesize. In Thailand, violence in political conflicts.
conscription among men is enforced through lottery. We should mention a few caveats. The first is that
As a result, a large proportion of the men in Thailand while participation in political violence is rare, it is also
have served in the armed forces (18% in our survey). likely to be underreported. As in all survey work, receiv-
However, in numerous tests, we find no indication that ing honest, truthful answers to questions – particularly
honor ideology is stronger among men who have served sensitive questions – is a challenge. Biases such as
in the military compared to other men.8 acquiescence (‘yah-saying’) and social-desirability rating
Furthermore, our survey includes questions on (the tendency to answer in a way that gives others a
whether the respondent has been a victim of violence favorable view of oneself) are always part of survey
or threats of violence, personally or directed against one’s responses. In order to minimize such effects, sensitive
family. If the causal direction goes from violence to questions were asked in a self-administered part of this
honor ideology, we would expect to see that people who survey. This should also minimize any potential incen-
have experienced violence or threats of violence have tive of honor endorsers to exaggerate their participation
higher honor ideology; however, numerous tests failed in violence to appear more manly, since not even the
to show any such relationship.9 enumerator will know the respondent’s answers.10
It should also be noted that the statistically signifi-
Conclusion cant and robust effects that we found pertain to male
political activists. By design, our sample of political
In this study, we set out to investigate to what extent activists differs from the complementary nationally rep-
honor ideology predicts participation in political vio- resentative sample. Whereas participation in political
lence. Previous research has established that the level of violence is extremely rare among ordinary Thais
gender equality within a country correlates to the peace- (1.5% of the men reported that they had participated),
fulness of the state (Caprioli, 2005; Melander, 2005; a substantial share, namely 23%, of the male political
Hudson et al., 2009; Gleditsch et al., 2011; Reiter, activists had participated in political violence. Previous
2014). This study provides unique micro-level evidence research has shown that misogynist ideals increase vio-
that links misogynist attitudes with self-reported partic- lence in families; here we demonstrate that this is also
ipation in political violence among political activists. Do true for political activists: future research should inves-
gender-unequal norms increase the likelihood of partici- tigate to what extent this is also the case for political
pating in political conflict? Our findings suggest that yes,

8 10
See Online Appendix B, Tables B16 to B18. More details on the self-administration procedure can be found in
9
See Online Appendix B, Tables B16 to B18. Online Appendix A.

This content downloaded from


45.77.104.39 on Tue, 19 Jul 2022 21:42:32 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Bjarnegård et al. 759

violence among ordinary people. Relatedly, the ques- References


tion of how and why a person becomes a political acti- Ai, Chungron & Edward C Norton (2003) Interaction terms
vist in the first place also deserves more study. in logit and probit models. Economics Letters 80(1):
Furthermore, we argue in this article that honor ideol- 123–129.
ogy pivots around two elements: patriarchal values and Alexander, Pamela C; Sharon Morre & Elmore R Alexander
ideals of masculine toughness. While these factors seem to III (1991) What is transmitted in the intergenerational
be fundamental and universal elements of honor ideol- transmission of violence? Journal of Marriage and Family
ogy, they are found to a different extent and take differ- 53(3): 657–667.
ent forms around the world. Therefore, the measures Anderson, Elijah (1999) Code of the Street. New York: WW
Norton.
developed in this article are likely to be culture-
Barnes, Collin D; Ryan P Brown & Lindsey L Osterman
specific, and if applied to other contexts, will require care
(2012) Don’t tread on me: Masculine honor ideology in
in adapting wording and meaning. the US and militant responses to terrorism. Personality and
We also acknowledge that we do not know the scope Social Psychology Bulletin 38(8): 1018–1029.
conditions for the explanation that participation in polit- Bjarnegård, Elin (2013) Gender, Informal Institutions and
ical violence is driven in part by honor ideology. Does Political Recruitment: Explaining Male Dominance in
this explanation only apply to the kind of violent pro- Parliamentary Representation, Gender and Politics. Hound-
testing and street clashes that characterize the recent mills: Palgrave Macmillan.
turmoil in Thailand, or is the explanation applicable also Bjarnegård, Elin & Erik Melander (2014) Thailand’s missing
to other forms of political violence such as guerilla war- democrats: Reds, yellows and the silent majority. Foreign
fare and massacres? This is an important issue for further Affairs 22 May (http://www.foreignaffairs.com/articles/
study. We argue that it is likely that honor ideology will 141454/elin-bjarnegard-and-erik-melander/
thailands-missing-democrats).
also turn out to be a driver of other forms of political
Blattman, Christopher & Edward Miguel (2010) Civil War.
violence as all forms of deadly political violence exhibit
Journal of Economic Literature 48(1): 3–57.
the particular pattern that an overwhelming majority of Brezina, Timothy; Robert Agnew, Francis T Cullen & John
those who fight are men while most men never take up Paul Wright (2004) The code of the street: A quantitative
arms even under conditions of political conflict. assessment of Elijah Anderson’s subculture of violence the-
Future research should also explore the multifaceted sis and its contribution to youth violence research. Youth
concept of honor ideology further. We need to concep- Violence and Juvenile Justice 2(4): 303–328.
tually and empirically refine this concept in order to deter- Burt, Martha R (1980) Cultural myths and supports for
mine exactly what it entails. We also need to complement rape. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 38(2):
studies of one country, such as this one, with comparisons 217–230.
of other contexts. Much remains to be done, but the Caprioli, Mary (2005) Primed for violence: The role of gender
micro-level findings presented in this article provide inequality in predicting internal conflict. International
Studies Quarterly 49(2): 161–178.
important and unique insights into what causes male
Carpenter, R Charli (2006) Recognizing gender-based
political activists to use violence for political purposes.
violence against civilian men and boys in conflict situations.
Security Dialogue 37(1): 83–103.
Replication data Chaisukkosol, Chanchai (2010) The internet & nonviolent
The dataset, codebook, and do-files for the empirical struggle: The anti-government movement in Thailand
analysis in this article, as well as the Online appendix, 2005–2006. Social Alternatives 29(3): 49–55.
can be found at http://www.prio.org/jpr/datasets. Chambers, Paul (2010) Thailand on the brink: Resurgent
military, eroded democracy. Asian Survey 50(5):
835–858.
Acknowledgments Cohrs, J Christopher; Jürgen Maes, Barbara Moschner &
We thank the Editor and reviewers for insightful Sven Kielmann (2007) Determinants of human rights atti-
comments. tudes and behavior: A comparison and integration of psy-
chological perspectives. Political Psychology 28(4):
441–469.
Funding Collier, Paul (2007) The Bottom Billion. Oxford: Oxford Uni-
This work was supported by Riksbankens Jubileumsfond versity Press.
(grant number M10-0100:1), The East Asian Peace Dalpino, Catharin (2011) Thailand in 2010: Rupture and
Program. attempts at reconciliation. Asian Survey 51(1): 155–162.

This content downloaded from


45.77.104.39 on Tue, 19 Jul 2022 21:42:32 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
760 journal of PEACE RESEARCH 54(6)

Donnell, John C (1967) Viet Cong recruitment: Why and interpretation and presentation. American Journal of Polit-
how men join. DTIC Document (http://www.dtic.mil/cgi- ical Science 44(2): 347–361.
bin/GetTRDoc?AD¼ADA032187). Mahalingam, Ramaswami (2007) Beliefs about chastity,
Ehrensaft, Miriam K; Patricia Cohen, Jocelyn Brown, Smailes machismo, and caste identity: A cultural psychology of
Elizabeth, Chen Henian & Jeffrey G Johnson (2003) gender. Sex Roles 56(3): 239–249.
Intergenerational transmission of partner violence: A Malamuth, Neil M; Robert J Sockloskie, Mary P Koss & JS
20-year prospective study. Journal of Consulting and Clin- Tanaka (1991) Characteristics of aggressors against women:
ical Psychology 71(4): 741–753. Testing a model using a national sample of college students.
Feather, Norman T & Ian R McKee (2012) Values, Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 59(5): 670–681.
right-wing authoritarianism, social dominance orientation, Mandelbaum, David G (1988) Women’s Seclusion and Men’s
and ambivalent attitudes toward women. Journal of Applied Honor: Sex Roles in North India, Bangladesh, and Pakistan.
Social Psychology 42(10): 2479–2504. Tucson, AZ: University of Arizona Press.
Figueredo, Aurelio José; Victor Corral-Verdugo, Martha McDoom, Omar Shahabudin (2013) Who killed in Rwanda’s
Frı́as-Armenta, Karen J Bachar, Janine White, Prentiss L genocide? Micro-space, social influence and individual par-
McNeill, Beth R Kirsner & Irasema Del Pilarcastell-Ruiz ticipation in intergroup violence. Journal of Peace Research
(2001) Blood, solidarity, status, and honor: The sexual 50(4): 453–467.
balance of power and spousal abuse in Sonora, Mexico. McGregor, Robert (2003) The popular press and the creation
Evolution and Human Behavior 22(5): 295–328. of military masculinities in Georgian Britain. In: Paul R
Gleditsch, Kristian Skrede; Julian Wucherpfennig, Simon Higate (ed.) Military Masculinities. Westport, CT: Praeger,
Hug & Karina Garnes Reigstad (2011) Polygyny or mis- 143–156.
ogyny? Reexamining the ‘First Law of Intergroup Conflict’. Melander, Erik (2005) Gender equality and intrastate armed
Journal of Politics 73(1): 265–270. conflict. International Studies Quarterly 49(4): 695–714.
Goldstein, Joshua S (2001) War and Gender: How Gender Montesano, Michael J; Pavin Chachavalpongpun & Aekapol
Shapes the War System and Vice Versa. Cambridge: Cam- Chongvilaivan (2012) Bangkok May 2010: Perspectives on a
bridge University Press. Divided Thailand. Singapore: Institute of Southeast Asian
Hirdman, Yvonne (1988) Genussystemet – Reflexioner kring Studies.
kvinnors sociala underordning [Gender system: Reflexions Mosher, Donald L & Mark Sirkin (1984) Measuring a macho
on women’s social subordination]. Kvinnovetenskaplig tids- personality constellation. Journal of Research in Personality
skrift 3: 49–63. 18(2): 150–163.
Hudson, Valerie M & Andrea M den Boer (2012) A feminist Murnen, Sarah K; Carrie Wright & Gretchen Kaluzny (2002)
evolutionary analysis of the relationship between violence If ‘boys will be boys,’ then girls will be victims? A
against and inequitable treatment of women, and conflict meta-analytic review of the research that relates masculine
within and between human collectives, including nation- ideology to sexual aggression. Sex Roles 46(11): 359–375.
states. In: Todd K Shackelford & Viviana A Weekes-Shack- Nagel, Joane (2005) Nation. In: Michael S Kimmel, Jeff
elford (eds) The Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Hearn & RW Connell (eds) Handbook of Studies on
Perspectives on Violence, Homicide, and War. Oxford: Men and Masculinities. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage,
Oxford University Press, 301–323. 397–413.
Hudson, Valerie M; Bonnie Ballif-Spanvill, Mary Caprioli & Neff, James A (2001) A confirmatory factor analysis of a
Chad F Emmett (2012) Sex and World Peace. New York: measure of ‘Machismo’ among Anglo, African American
Columbia University Press. and Mexican American male drinkers. Hispanic Journal of
Hudson, Valerie M; Mary Caprioli, Bonnie Ballif-Spanvill, Behavioral Sciences 23(2): 171–188.
McDermott Rose & Chad F Emmett (2009) The heart Nisbett, Richard E & Dov Cohen (1996) Culture of Honor:
of the matter: The security of women and the security of The Psychology of Violence in the South: New Directions in
states. International Security 33(3): 7–45. Social Psychology. Boulder, CO: Westview.
Humphreys, Macartan & Jeremy M Weinstein (2008) Who Phongpaichit, Pasuk & Chris Baker (2012) Thailand in
fights? The determinants of participation in civil war. trouble: Revolt of the downtrodden or conflict among
American Journal of Political Science 52(2): 436–455. elites? In: Michael J Montesano, Pavin Chachavalpongpun
Jewkes, Rachel; Emma Fulu, Tim Roselli & Garcia-Moreno & Aekapol Chongvilaivan (eds) Bangkok May 2010: Per-
Claudia (2013) Prevalence of and factors associated with spectives on a Divided Thailand. Singapore: Institute of
male perpetration of intimate partner violence: Findings Southeast Asian Studies, 214–229.
from the UN multi-country cross-sectional study on men Potts, Malcolm & Thomas Hayden (2008) Sex and War: How
and violence in Asia and the Pacific. The Lancet Global Biology Explains Warfare and Terrorism and Offers a Path to
Health 1(4): e187–e207. a Safer World. Dallas, TX: BenBella.
King, Gary; Michael Tomz & Jason Wittenberg (2000) Prasirtsuk, Kitti (2010) Thailand in 2009: Colored by turbu-
Making the most of statistical analyses: Improving lence. Asian Survey 50(1): 203–210.

This content downloaded from


45.77.104.39 on Tue, 19 Jul 2022 21:42:32 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Bjarnegård et al. 761

Reiter, Dan (2014) The positivist study of gender and inter- domestic violence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
national relations. Journal of Conflict Resolution 59(7): ogy 84(5): 997–1010.
1301–1326. Vandello, Joseph A & Dov Cohen (2008) Culture, gender,
Santana, M Christina; Anita Raj, Michele R Decker, Ana and men’s intimate partner violence. Social and Personality
La Marche & Jay G Silverman (2006) Masculine gender Psychology Compass 2(2): 652–667.
roles associated with increased sexual risk and intimate Velitchkova, Ana (2015) World culture, uncoupling,
partner violence perpetration among young adult men. institutional logics, and recoupling: Practices and
Journal of Urban Health: Bulletin of the New York Acad- self-identification as institutional microfoundations of
emy of Medicine 83(4): 575–585. political violence. Sociological Forum 30(3): 698–720.
Scacco, Alexandra (2008) Who Riots? Explaining Individual Verwimp, Philip (2005) An economic profile of peasant per-
Participation in Ethnic Violence. New York: Columbia Uni- petrators of genocide: Micro-level evidence from Rwanda.
versity Press. Journal of Development Economics 77(2): 297–323.
Sopranzetti, Claudio (2012) Burning red desires: Isan migrants Wimmer, Andreas (2014) War. Annual Review of Sociology 40:
and the politics of desire in contemporary Thailand. South 173–197.
East Asia Research 20(3): 361–379. Wood, Elisabeth Jean (2001) The emotional benefits of insur-
Staub, Ervin (1989) The Roots of Evil: The Origins of Genocide gency in El Salvador. In: Jeff Goodwin, James M Jasper &
and Other Group Violence. Cambridge: Cambridge Univer- Francesca Polletta (eds) Passionate Politics: Emotions and
sity Press. Social Movements. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago
Staub, Ervin (2006) Reconciliation after genocide, mass kill- Press, 267–281.
ing, or intractable conflict: Understanding the roots of vio-
lence, psychological recovery, and steps toward a general ELIN BJARNEGÅRD, b. 1976, PhD in Political Science
theory. Political Psychology 27(6): 867–894. (Uppsala University, 2010); Associate Professor at the
Stent, James (2012) Thoughts on Thailand’s turmoil. In: Department of Government, Uppsala University; articles
Michael J Montesano, Pavin Chachavalpongpun & Aeka- published in journals such as Comparative Politics, Government
pol Chongvilaivan (eds) Bangkok May 2010: Perspectives on & Opposition and International Interactions; author of Gender,
a Divided Thailand. Singapore: Institute of Southeast Asian Informal Institutions and Political Recruitment: Explaining
Studies, 15–41. Male Dominance in Parliamentary Representation (Palgrave
Stewart, Frank Henderson (1994) Honor. Chicago, IL: Uni- Macmillan, 2013).
versity of Chicago Press.
Thabchumpon, Naruemon & Duncan McCargo (2011) KAREN BROUNEUS, b. 1973, PhD in Peace and Conflict
Urbanized villagers in the 2010 Thai red-shirt protests. Research (Uppsala University, 2008); Associate Professor at
Asian Survey 51(6): 993–1018. the Department of Peace and Conflict Research, Uppsala
Thomas, Jakana L & Kanisha D Bond (2015) Women’s par- University; previous works published in journals such as
ticipation in violent political organizations. American Polit- Journal of Conflict Resolution, Security Dialogue, Journal of
ical Science Review 109(3): 488–506. Peace Research, and Signs: Journal of Women in Culture and
Tomz, Michael; Jason Wittenberg & Gary King (2003) Clar- Society.
ify: Software for interpreting and presenting statistical
results. Version 2.1. Journal of Statistical Software 8(1). ERIK MELANDER, b. 1969, PhD in Peace and Conflict
Uppsala Conflict Data Program (2015) UCDP Conflict Ency- Research (Uppsala University, 1999); Professor at the
clopedia. Uppsala: Department of Peace and Conflict Department of Peace and Conflict Research, Uppsala
Research, Uppsala University. (http://www.pcr.uu.se/ University; articles published in journals such as Journal of
research/ucdp/database/, accessed 22 April 2015). Conflict Resolution and Journal of Gender Studies; co-authored
Vandello, Joseph A & Dov Cohen (2003) Male honor and book The Peace Continuum (with Christian Davenport &
female fidelity: Implicit cultural scripts that perpetuate Patrick Regan) forthcoming with Oxford University Press.

This content downloaded from


45.77.104.39 on Tue, 19 Jul 2022 21:42:32 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms

You might also like