You are on page 1of 61

AN INTRODUCTION TO

MATHEMATICAL COGNITION

The last decade has seen a rapid growth in our understanding of the cognitive systems
that underlie mathematical learning and performance, and an increased recognition of
the importance of this topic. This book showcases international research on the most
important cognitive issues that affect mathematical performance across a wide age range,
from early childhood to adulthood. The book considers the foundational competencies
of nonsymbolic and symbolic number processing before discussing arithmetic, conceptual
understanding, individual differences and dyscalculia, algebra, number systems, reasoning
and higher-level mathematics such as formal proof. Drawing on diverse methodology
from behavioural experiments to brain imaging, each chapter discusses key theories and
empirical findings and introduces key tasks used by researchers. The final chapter discusses
challenges facing the future development of the field of mathematical cognition and reviews
a set of open questions that mathematical cognition researchers should address to move
the field forward. This book is ideal for undergraduate or graduate students of psychology,
education, cognitive sciences, cognitive neuroscience and other academic and clinical
audiences including mathematics educators and educational psychologists.

Camilla Gilmore is a Reader in Mathematical Cognition in the Mathematics Education


Centre at Loughborough University. Her research explores the development of numerical
skills in children and adults.

Silke M. Göbel is a Senior Lecturer in Psychology at the University of York. She teaches
courses on Numerical Cognition, Dyscalculia, Mathematics Anxiety and Neuroscience
of Numbers and Arithmetic. Her current research focuses on predictors of mathematical
development.

Matthew Inglis is a Reader in Mathematical Cognition in the Mathematics Education


Centre at Loughborough University. He is interested in understanding the processes
involved in numerical and mathematical reasoning, and how these can be promoted through
education.
International Texts in Developmental Psychology
Series editor: Peter K. Smith, Goldsmiths College, University of London, UK.

This volume is one of a rapidly developing series in International Texts in


Developmental Psychology, published by Routledge. The books in this series are
selected to be state-of-the-art, high level introductions to major topic areas in
developmental psychology. The series conceives of developmental psychology in
broad terms and covers such areas as social development, cognitive development,
developmental neuropsychology and neuroscience, language development,
learning difficulties, developmental psychopathology and applied issues. Each
volume is written by a specialist (or specialists), combining empirical data and
a synthesis of recent global research to deliver cutting-edge science in a format
accessible to students and researchers alike. The books may be used as textbooks
that match on to upper level developmental psychology modules, but many will
also have cross-disciplinary appeal.
Each volume in the series is published in hardback, paperback and eBook
formats. More information about the series is available on the official website
at: www.routledge.com/International-Texts-in-Developmental-Psychology/book-
series/DEVP, including details of all the titles published to date.

Published Titles
Child Development: Theories and Critical Perspectives, 2nd edition
By Rosalyn H. Shute and Phillip T. Slee
The Child at School: Interactions with Peers and Teachers, 2nd edition
By Peter Blatchford, Anthony D. Pellegrini and Ed Baines
Children’s Literacy Development: A Cross-Cultural Perspective on Learning to
Read and Write, 2nd edition
By Catherine McBride
Childhood Friendships and Peer Relations: Friends and Enemies, 2nd edition
By Barry Schneider
Aging and Development: Social and Emotional Perspectives, 2nd edition
By Peter Coleman and Ann O’Hanlon
An Introduction to Mathematical Cognition
By Camilla Gilmore, Silke M. Göbel, and Matthew Inglis
AN INTRODUCTION
TO MATHEMATICAL
COGNITION

Camilla Gilmore, Silke M. Göbel and


Matthew Inglis
First published 2018
by Routledge
2 Park Square, Milton Park, Abingdon, Oxon, OX14 4RN
and by Routledge
711 Third Avenue, New York, NY 10017
Routledge is an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an informa business

c 2018 Camilla Gilmore, Silke M. Göbel, and Matthew Inglis
The right of Camilla Gilmore, Silke M. Göbel, and Matthew Inglis to be identified
as authors of this work has been asserted by them in accordance with sections 77 and 78
of the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988.
All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reprinted or reproduced or utilized
in any form or by any electronic, mechanical, or other means, now known or
hereafter invented, including photocopying and recording, or in any information
storage or retrieval system, without permission in writing from the publishers.
Trademark notice: Product or corporate names may be trademarks or registered trademarks,
and are used only for identification and explanation without intent to infringe.
British Library Cataloguing-in-Publication Data
A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data
Names: Gilmore, Camilla, author. | Göbel, Silke M., author. | Inglis, Matthew, author.
Title: An introduction to mathematical cognition / Camilla Gilmore, Silke M. Göbel,
Matthew Inglis. Description: Abingdon, Oxon ; New York, NY : Routledge, 2018. |
Series: International texts in developmental psychology |
Includes bibliographical references and index.
Identifiers: LCCN 2017056370 | ISBN 9781138923942 (hbk : alk. paper) |
ISBN 9781138923959 (pbk : alk. paper) | ISBN 9781315684758 (ebk)
Subjects: LCSH: Mathematical ability in children. | Mathematics–Psychological aspects. |
Cognition in children. | Cognition.
Classification: LCC BF456.N7 G55 2018 | DDC 153.9–dc23
LC record available at https://lccn.loc.gov/2017056370
ISBN: 978-1-138-92394-2 (hbk)
ISBN: 978-1-138-92395-9 (pbk)
ISBN: 978-1-315-68475-8 (ebk)

Typeset in Bembo
by Out of House Publishing
CONTENTS

List of figures, tables and boxes x


Acknowledgements xiv

1 Introduction 1
1.1 What is mathematical cognition? 1
1.2 The development of mathematical skills 3

2 Nonsymbolic number 6
2.1 Introduction 7
2.2 Small exact system 7
2.2.1 The development of subitizing 7
2.2.2 Individual differences in subitizing ability and its
relationship with mathematics 8
2.2.3 Theoretical models of subitizing 9
2.2.4 Subitizing in groups: ‘groupitizing’ 11
2.2.5 Summary: subitizing 12
2.3 Large approximate system 12
2.3.1 The Approximate Number System (ANS) 14
2.3.2 Brain bases of ANS: numerons and genes 16
2.3.3 The ANS in infants 18
2.3.4 The development of the ANS 19
2.3.5 The ANS and arithmetic 19
2.3.6 Influence of mathematics education on the ANS 21
2.3.7 ANS training 21
2.3.8 The ANS and space 23
2.3.9 Difficulties in studying the ANS 23
2.3.10 Is there really an ANS? 25
vi Contents

2.3.11 Summary: estimation and the Approximate


Number System (ANS) 26
2.4 Summary 27

3 Symbolic number 29
3.1 Introduction 30
3.2 Number words 30
3.2.1 Number word acquisition 30
3.2.2 Theories of children’s acquisition of exact
number concepts 33
3.2.3 Number words influence place-value understanding
and exact calculation 34
3.2.4 Spontaneous Focusing on Numerosity (SFON) 36
3.2.5 Summary: number words 36
3.3 Arabic digits 37
3.3.1 Single digits 37
3.3.2 Multi-digit number processing 42
3.3.3 Summary: Arabic digits 44
3.4 The number line task 44
3.5 Transcoding: from number words to Arabic digits 45
3.5.1 Theories of number transcoding 46
3.5.2 Linguistic influences on transcoding 47
3.5.3 Summary: transcoding 47
3.6 The relationship between symbolic and nonsymbolic
numerical systems 47
3.7 The relationship between symbolic number processing
and mathematical performance 49
3.8 Summary 49

4 The development of arithmetic skills 51


4.1 Early arithmetic skills 51
4.1.1 Arithmetic in infancy? 51
4.1.2 Nonverbal arithmetic in preschoolers 53
4.2 Symbolic arithmetic 54
4.2.1 Effects of problem representation 55
4.2.2 Arithmetic word problems 56
4.3 Arithmetic strategies 59
4.3.1 Strategies to solve arithmetic problems 59
4.3.2 Strategy choice 64
4.4 Domain-general influences on arithmetic development 67
4.4.1 Working memory 68
4.4.2 Inhibition and shifting 69
4.4.3 Language skills 70
4.5 Summary 71
Contents vii

5 Understanding arithmetic concepts 73


5.1 Key concepts in arithmetic understanding 74
5.1.1 Additive composition 74
5.1.2 Commutativity and associativity 76
5.1.3 Inversion 77
5.1.4 Multiplicative reasoning 78
5.1.5 Summary 80
5.2 The relationship between conceptual and procedural
knowledge 82
5.2.1 Defining conceptual and procedural knowledge 82
5.2.2 The development of conceptual and procedural
knowledge 84
5.2.3 Individual differences in the development of
conceptual and procedural knowledge 87
5.3 Summary 90

6 Individual differences and mathematical difficulties 93


6.1 Introduction 93
6.2 Mathematical difficulties 94
6.2.1 Diagnostic criteria 95
6.2.2 Severity 97
6.2.3 Genetic risk 97
6.2.4 Difficulties of children with developmental
dyscalculia or mathematical learning disorder 98
6.2.5 Neural correlates of developmental dyscalculia 102
6.2.6 Current theories of developmental dyscalculia 104
6.2.7 Subtypes of developmental dyscalculia 107
6.2.8 Comorbidity with other developmental learning
disorders 108
6.2.9 Summary: mathematical difficulties 109
6.3 Mathematics anxiety 109
6.3.1 How is mathematics anxiety measured? 110
6.3.2 The relationship between mathematics anxiety
and mathematics achievement 112
6.3.3 Mathematics anxiety and working memory 113
6.3.4 Risk factors 115
6.3.5 Alleviating mathematics anxiety 115
6.3.6 Summary: mathematics anxiety 116
6.4 Attitudes towards mathematics 116
6.5 Summary 118

7 Number Systems 120


7.1 Going beyond the natural numbers 120
7.2 The theory of conceptual change 122
viii Contents

7.3 Zero: a special number? 123


7.4 Negative numbers 125
7.5 The transition to rational numbers 125
7.5.1 Density 126
7.5.2 Size 126
7.5.3 Operations 127
7.5.4 The developmental trajectory of the natural
number bias 127
7.6 Teaching and the natural number bias 130
7.7 Rational number arithmetic 131
7.8 Real numbers and cardinal numbers 133
7.9 Summary 136

8 Algebra and equivalence 138


8.1 Moving from arithmetic to algebra 139
8.2 The concept of a variable 140
8.3 Early algebra 143
8.4 Equivalence 144
8.4.1 The development of equivalence understanding 145
8.4.2 Influences on children’s equivalence understanding 148
8.4.3 Interventions to support equivalence
understanding 150
8.5 Summary 152

9 Mathematical argumentation and proof 154


9.1 What is a mathematical proof? 155
9.2 What difficulties do students have with proof? 157
9.2.1 Proof construction 157
9.2.2 Proof validation 158
9.2.3 Proof comprehension 159
9.3 Why do students have these difficulties? 161
9.3.1 Epistemic cognition and mathematical proof 161
9.3.2 Strategic failure 164
9.3.3 Logical problems 165
9.4 Summary 165

10 Logic, conditional reasoning and mathematics 167


10.1 Logic in proofs 167
10.2 Conditional reasoning 170
10.2.1 The Wason selection task 170
10.2.2 Evans’s conditional inference task 172
10.3 The relationship between reasoning and mathematics 177
10.4 Summary 180
Contents ix

11 Where next for mathematical cognition? 182


11.1 Mathematical cognition as an interdisciplinary field? 183
11.2 Methodological developments 185
11.2.1 The replication crisis in psychology 185
11.2.2 A replication crisis in mathematical cognition? 189
11.3 Future research directions 191

References 195
Index 238
FIGURES, TABLES AND BOXES

Figures
1.1 The number of abstracts per year identified via a search of Web of
Science with the search term ‘mathematical cognition’ OR
‘numerical cognition’. Prior to 1992 there were 6 items in total 3
2.1 Subitizing. The graph shows the average response times for
enumerating a display with 1–8 dots for primary school children
(aged 6–10 years) 8
2.2 How many dots are there in this image? 13
2.3 Schematic representations of mental representations of numbers
1–10 following (A) a linear model or (B) a logarithmic model 15
2.4 Schematic representations of the overlap of mental representations
in the linear model of (A) numbers 4 and 10 (far distance, less
overlap) and (B) numbers 8 and 10 (close distance, more overlap) 16
2.5 Schematic representations of individual differences in the acuity of
mental number representations in a linear model. Mental
representations for numbers 5,6,8,9 for a participant with a Weber
fraction of 0.125 (A) and 0.200 (B). Panel C shows the
performance patterns (percentage correct) for these two
participants depending on the ratio of the two nonsymbolic
quantities that are being compared 17
2.6 An example of an approximate arithmetic task (addition).
Participants first see dots moving into the pot (A), then another set
of dots is added to the pot (B), finally they have to decide where
the larger number of dots is, in the pot or outside the pot (C) 22
Figures, tables and boxes xi

3.1 Examples of mirrored written digits from 5- to 6-year-old children.


They were given number words and asked to write them down in
Arabic digit format from memory. The corresponding number
words were: (A) ‘seven’, (B) ‘fifteen’, (C) ‘ninety-one’, (D) ‘thirty’ 38
3.2 Two types of numerical distance effects. In the priming distance
effect (dark grey line) response times increase with numerical
distance between the prime and the target. In contrast, in the
comparison distance effect (light grey line, the classic distance
effect), response times for comparing numbers decrease with larger
numerical distance between the two numbers to be compared 39
3.3 Stimuli used to investigate the numerical Stroop or size congruity effect 40
3.4 Example of a number line task trial. Participants are asked to mark
where the number 23 appears on the line 44
3.5 Examples of multi-digit transcoding errors: (A) shows a child’s
attempt to write 1300; (B) shows 348; and (C) shows 2150 46
4.1 Models of strategy development. Left: the staircase model. Right:
the overlapping waves model, Siegler (1999) 65
5.1 Mean correct answers (out of 3) for inverse (a + b − b) and control
(a + b − c) problems presented using concrete items, as word
problems, or with Arabic digits 78
5.2 Iterative model for the development of conceptual and procedural
knowledge of fractions 87
5.3 Accuracy (standardised scores) on inversion and control problems
for children who fall into three distinct clusters 89
6.1 Different ways of defining ‘substantially below’ the average.
Percentage of children scoring in the lowest (A) 25%, (B) 10%, or
(C) 5% on a standardised mathematical achievement test 97
6.2 Response times for a dot counting task for mathematical learning
disorder (MLD: grey line) and control children (black line) 99
6.3 Neural correlates of mathematical difficulties: an overview of brain
regions where differences in brain structure, function or
connectivity have been found between individuals with
developmental dyscalculia (DD) and typical peers 103
7.1 The strength of natural number bias observed by Van Hoof,
Verschaffel, and Van Dooren (2015) on (i) all types of items
(density, size and operations), (ii) density items, (iii) size items, and
(iv) operations items. The y axis shows the odds ratio, with higher
figures representing a stronger bias and 1 representing no bias 129
8.1 Example of a variable number line used to introduce children to
early algebra 144
8.2 Typical exercises used to help children understand the equal sign in
Chinese textbooks 150
xii Figures, tables and boxes

8.3 Children’s performance on three measures of equivalence


understanding after 12 weeks of instruction with either a standard
or modified workbook 151
10.1 A diagram illustrating the material conditional ‘if p then q’ (left)
and the biconditional ‘p if and only if q’ (right) 173
11.1 A mapping of spatially relevant research in education, psychology,
neuroscience and mathematics journals. The cluster to the top
right represents education journals, the cluster to the bottom right
represents mathematics journals. The large cluster to the bottom
left represents neuroscience and psychology journals. Links
between nodes represent the extent to which the journals cite
each other 184

Tables
3.1 Number-knower levels 32
4.1 Classification of word problems 57
4.2 Summary of strategies used to solve simple addition problems 60
5.1 The relationship between procedural and conceptual group
membership 89
7.1 Example problems used by Van Hoof, Verschaffel, and Van Dooren
(2015) 128
8.1 Example responses to the question ‘David is 10 cm taller than Con.
Con is h cm tall. What can you write for David’s height?’ and the
frequency with which they were given before and after algebra
instruction 142
10.1 The truth table for the material conditional 169
10.2 The truth table for the material, biconditional and defective
interpretations of ‘if p then q’ 174
10.3 The four inferences (MP, DA, AC and MT) with and without
negated premises (Prem) and conclusions (Con) 175

Boxes
2.1 A generalised magnitude system? 26
2.2 Open questions for future research 27
3.1 No words for numbers? 35
3.2 Open questions for future research 50
4.1 Arithmetic skill or skills? 59
4.2 Neuroscience and arithmetic 66
4.3 Open questions for future research 71
5.1 Measuring conceptual and procedural knowledge 81
5.2 Open questions for future research 91
6.1 ICD 10 (World Health Organization, 2016) 95
Figures, tables and boxes xiii

6.2 DSM V (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) 96


6.3 Proposed subtypes of mathematical disorder 107
6.4 Mathematics anxiety questionnaires 111
6.5 Mathematical self-efficacy, self-concept and confidence 117
6.6 Open questions for future research 118
7.1 Is zero necessary for a place-value numeral system? 124
7.2 Are there more rational numbers or natural numbers? 134
7.3 Are there more real numbers or natural numbers? 135
7.4 Open questions for future research 136
8.1 Approaches to algebra education 140
8.2 Measuring equivalence understanding 147
8.3 Open questions for future research 152
9.1 Open questions for future research 166
10.1 Open questions for future research 180
11.1 Challenges in mathematical cognition 192
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

The authors wish to thank the following for permission to reprint copyright
material in this book:

• Figure 2.3 is adapted with permission of the authors and used under their CC
BY from Dietrich, J. F., Huber, S., & Nuerk, H. C. (2015). Methodological
aspects to be considered when measuring the Approximate Number System
(ANS) – a research review. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 295, Figure 1.1.
• Figure 2.4 is adapted with permission of the authors and used under their CC
BY from Dietrich, J. F., Huber, S., & Nuerk, H. C. (2015). Methodological
aspects to be considered when measuring the Approximate Number System
(ANS) – A research review. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 295, Figure 2.2.
• Figure 2.5 is reproduced with permission from Justin Halberda from ‘What is
a Weber Fraction?’ (www.panamath.org).
• Figure 6.3 is reprinted from Cohen Kadosh, R., & Dowker, A. (eds) The
Oxford Handbook of Numerical Cognition, p.735, Figure 40.1, Oxford: OUP
with kind permission from OUP.
• The figure in Box 6.4 on page 111 is reprinted with authors’ permission from
Wu, S., & Menon, V. (2012) Scale for Early Mathematics Anxiety (SEMA) in
Young Children. Available at: www.scsnl.stanford.edu

We are grateful to our editor Lucy Kennedy who has been extremely helpful and
efficient at every stage of this project, from developing the book proposal to getting
us to submit the final version.
We would like to thank the members of the Mathematical Cognition Group
at Loughborough University and the members of the Numerical Cognition Lab
Group at the Department of Psychology, University of York, for stimulating
conversations and challenging discussions over the years.
Acknowledgements xv

Several chapters in this book are based on a third-year undergraduate module


(Numerical Cognition) for Psychology students at the University of York. The
content of this module has been shaped and improved by several generations of
students at the Department of Psychology. Thanks to those students for their
curiosity, input and feedback, special thanks to the cohorts 2016/17 and 2017/18
for reading drafts of our textbook chapters and giving feedback.
And last but not least, thanks to our friends and family for their support, in
particular to David, Adam, Freddie and Sam Gilmore, Bill Kirkup, Helen Lake,
Jelena Mirković, and Danijela Trenkic.
1
INTRODUCTION

1.1 What is mathematical cognition?


Mathematical cognition is a relatively new field of research that seeks to
understand the processes by which individuals come to understand mathematical
ideas. Mathematical cognition researchers are interested in how mathematical
understanding and performance develops from infancy to adulthood, the factors
that explain individual differences in mathematical performance and why some
individuals find mathematics so difficult. It is an interdisciplinary field, drawing on
psychology, education, and neuroscience, and cannot be reduced simply to one of
these disciplines. To fully answer questions about mathematical understanding and
processes involves an awareness of underlying brain processes, cognitive systems and
their development, and the formal and informal activities that individuals engage
in when learning mathematics. As would be expected for an interdisciplinary
field, research in mathematical cognition employs a diverse range of methodologies
including (but not limited to) experimental cognitive, neuroimaging, intervention
and individual differences research.
Part of the motivation for this area of research comes from an increasing aware-
ness that many individuals struggle with mathematics. In the UK, for example, it
is estimated that 24% of adults have numeracy levels below that needed to function
in everyday life, for example, to understand food pricing or pay household bills
(Department for Business, Innovation and Skills, 2011). This pattern is replicated
across many countries; the Organisation for Economic Co-operation and
Development’s (OECD) survey of 24 countries found that overall almost 20% of
adults have low levels of numeracy and are unable to accurately deal with two-step
calculations, or understand common decimals, percentages and fractions (OECD,
2013). These difficulties begin to arise early in schooling, for example the US
National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP) in 2015 found that only 33%
2 1 Introduction

of 10-year-olds had reached the expected level of proficiency in mathematics (Kena


et al., 2016). This is important because research has shown that poor mathematics
skills in childhood have a negative impact on later health, wealth and quality of life
(Hudson, Price, & Gross, 2009; OECD, 2013; Parsons & Bynner, 2005; Reyna,
Nelson, Han, & Dieckmann, 2009). Mathematical cognition researchers believe
that a better understanding of underlying mathematical processes can help us to
identify why so many individuals struggle with mathematics, and consequently
how to improve mathematical performance.
Having a positive impact on mathematics education and performance is one
important motivation for studying mathematical cognition, but not the only one.
Understanding the processes of mathematical performance is an ideal test case to
explore deeper psychological questions. As we will show throughout this book,
mathematical performance involves integrating formal, uniquely human, systems
of symbolic representation with more basic, intuitive (and some suggest innate)
systems for representing mathematical ideas. This pattern is repeated across many
years of mathematics education during which students must integrate formal rep-
resentations of mathematical concepts with pre-existing informal ideas developed
from everyday life. The study of mathematical cognition therefore provides a
window into deeper questions of how our cognitive systems draw on and are influ-
enced by multiple sources of information. These questions go beyond the domain
of mathematics and help us to understand the nature of our cognitive systems
themselves.
Finally, the study of mathematical cognition also raises philosophical questions
about the nature of mathematics itself. For instance, what actually is a number?
Do numbers exist as abstract objects independently of humans (as so-called
platonists would argue)? Or are they logical objects constructed within a given
axiomatic system (as formalists would suggest)? Recent work on mathematical
cognition opens up other possibilities that have not yet fully been considered
by philosophers. For instance, perhaps numbers are simply tags for Approximate
Number System representations (see Chapter 2). Considerations such as these
have led some philosophers of mathematics to pay a great deal of attention to
the mathematical cognition literature (De Cruz, 2016; Gaber & Schlimm, 2015;
Giaquinto, 2007).
The field of mathematical cognition has grown rapidly over the past decade.
For example, a citation search for the terms ‘mathematical cognition’ or ‘numerical
cognition’ in Web of Science identified a marked increase in the number of items
published since 2007 (see Figure 1.1). Developments such as a new journal, Journal
of Numerical Cognition, launched in 2015 and a new society, The Mathematical
Cognition and Learning Society, founded in 2016, demonstrate that the field has
become established and is set to grow further. As the number of academics working
in the field of mathematical cognition grows, it is likely that this is a topic that will
increasingly appear in undergraduate and postgraduate psychology, neuroscience
and education courses and therefore introduce a new generation of researchers
1.2 The development of mathematical skills 3

120

100
Number of abstracts

80

60

40

20

0
1992
1993
1994
1995
1996
1997
1998
1999
2000
2001
2002
2003
2004
2005
2006
2007
2008
2009
2010

2012
2013
2014
2015
2016
2011
Year

FIGURE 1.1 The number of abstracts per year identified via a search of Web of Science
with the search term ‘mathematical cognition’ OR ‘numerical cognition’. Prior to
1992 there were 6 items in total.

to the field. We hope that this book will help in this endeavour by providing an
accessible introduction to key theories, notable findings and ongoing debates in
the field.

1.2 The development of mathematical skills


As we describe in the following chapters, mathematical cognition research has
made substantial advances in uncovering the processes involved in understanding
mathematical ideas. To a large extent this work has focused on numeracy and
arithmetic, rather than mathematical topics more broadly, and some major theories
have been proposed about the nature of basic numerical representations. Less work
has focused on higher levels of mathematics, although this is beginning to change.
Research with infants has explored the nature of numerical representations
and questioned whether infants and young children are capable of processing
numerical information before they have learnt symbolic representations (i.e. ‘three’
or ‘3’). Prominent theories have been proposed to suggest that, even without
these symbolic representations, humans (and other animals) are able to represent
approximate nonsymbolic quantity information. Studies have explored whether
this ability is present from birth, whether it involves truly numerical processing,
and if it is related to later, formal, mathematical skills.
4 1 Introduction

The first recognisably mathematical activity that young children engage in is to


learn the count list and understand what these numbers mean. Research has shown
that this seemingly simple activity is in fact very complex and involves a protracted
period of development before number words and symbols become grounded in
meaning. The question of what gives symbols their meaning is yet to be fully
established by researchers, although this is a topic of much current interest. Once
children have learnt these number symbols they begin to use them in arithmetic
operations. Research has shown that children draw on informal or intuitive
understanding of arithmetic and attempt to integrate this with formal instruction
and symbolic representations. True competency with arithmetic does not only
involve the ability to carry out arithmetic operations, it requires knowledge of
basic number facts, conceptual understanding of the operations and how they are
related and the skill to choose appropriate strategies. Studies have shown that each
of these aspects of arithmetic are important for overall performance, and that they
all draw on basic symbolic number skills, as well as domain-general cognitive skills
such as working memory and attention.
As children progress through school they are introduced to new and increasingly
complex mathematical ideas and processes. Rational numbers require children
to move beyond the count list as a basis for number representation and prove
challenging for many. Algebra also serves as a particular stumbling block for many
children. These new topics require children to become familiar with new forms
of symbolic representation as well as important conceptual advances. Research
has begun to identify several ways in which these new topics are difficult for
children, for example where new topics directly challenge previously acquired
meanings and intuitions, as well as proposing mechanisms to help improve
children’s understanding. Finally, some individuals go on to study mathematics at
higher levels, where again new systems of representations, ways of processing and
conceptual ideas must be understood.
The following chapters of this book cover these areas in roughly develop-
mental order. Individual chapters can be read alone to focus on specific topics,
however, reading the chapters in order of development reveals key themes that
cut across these individual topics. The challenge of acquiring new symbolic
representations and grasping the meaning they encapsulate is a recurrent theme,
which is as relevant for young children learning to count as it is for advanced
students studying proof. The interplay between procedural skill (‘knowing
how-to’) and conceptual understanding (‘knowing why’) can be seen at all levels
of mathematical performance. The nature of expertise, i.e. the behaviours or
skills that differentiate those who are proficient from those who struggle, is a
relevant issue for expert mathematicians as well as children learning arithmetic.
Finally, across many topics we see that earlier understanding can interfere
with the later acquisition of ideas. As well as focusing on the patterns of
performance and development of skills that are common to most learners, the
following chapters also consider atypical development, and the reasons why a small
1.2 The development of mathematical skills 5

proportion of individuals have particular difficulty with mathematical ideas and


performance.
Throughout the book we have aimed to highlight key findings and theories
within each topic and give a flavour of the methods and approaches used to
investigate these. We also highlight some of the key unanswered questions within
each topic that require the attention of researchers in this rapidly growing field.
Nonsymbolic Number
Anobile, G. , Cicchini, G. M. , & Burr, D. C. (2016). Number as a primary perceptual attribute: a
review. Perception, 45 (1–2), 5–31.
Nieder, A. , & Dehaene, S. (2009). Representation of number in the brain. Annual Review of
Neuroscience, 32 , 185–208.
Piazza, M. (2010). Neurocognitive start-up tools for symbolic number representations. Trends in
Cognitive Sciences, 14 (12), 542–551.
Schneider, M. , Beeres, K. , Coban, L. , Merz, S. , Susan Schmidt, S. , Stricker, J. , & De Smedt,
B. (2016). Associations of non-symbolic and symbolic numerical magnitude processing with
mathematical competence: a meta-analysis. Developmental Science, 20 (3), e12372.

Symbolic Number
Holloway, I. D. , & Ansari, D. (2015). Numerical symbols: an overview of their cognitive and
neural underpinnings. In R. Cohen Kadosh & A. Dowker (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of
Numerical Cognition (pp. 531–551). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Merkley, R. , & Ansari, D. (2016). Why numerical symbols count in the development of
mathematical skills: evidence from brain and behavior. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences,
10 , 14–20.
Nuerk, H. C. , Moeller, K. , & Willmes, K. (2015). Multi-digit number processing: overview,
conceptual clarifications and language influences. In R. Cohen Kadosh & A. Dowker (eds.), The
Oxford Handbook of Numerical Cognition (pp. 106–139). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Sarnecka B. W. , Goldman, M. C. , & Slusser, E. B. (2015). How counting leads to children’s
first representations of exact, large numbers. In R. Cohen Kadosh & A. Dowker (eds.), The
Oxford Handbook of Numerical Cognition (pp. 291–309). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

The Development of Arithmetic Skills


Bull, R. , & Lee, K. (2014). Executive functioning and mathematics achievement. Child
Development Perspectives, 8 , 36–41.
Butterworth, B. (2005). The development of arithmetical abilities. Journal of Child Psychology
and Psychiatry, 46 , 3–18.
LeFevre, J.-A. , Fast, L. , Skwarchuk, S.-L. , Smith-Chant, B. L. , Bisanz, J. , Kamawar, D. , &
Penner-Wilger, M. (2010). Pathways to mathematics: longitudinal predictors of performance.
Child Development, 81 , 1753–1767.
Lemaire, P. , & Siegler, R. S. (1995). Four aspects of strategic change: contributions to
children’s learning of multiplication. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124 , 83–97.
Wynn, L. (1992). Addition and subtraction by human infants. Nature, 358 , 749–750.

Understanding Arithmetic Concepts


Canobi, K. H. , Reeve, R. A. , & Pattison, P. E. (2002). Young children’s understanding of
addition concepts. Educational Psychology, 22 , 513–532.
Crooks, N. M. , & Alibali, M. W. (2014). Defining and measuring conceptual knowledge in
mathematics. Developmental Review, 34 , 344–377.
Rittle-Johnson, B. , & Schneider, M. (2015). Developing conceptual and proedural knowledge of
mathematics. In R. Cohen Kadosh & A. Dowker (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Numerical
Cognition (pp. 732–744). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Robinson, K. M. , Dubé, A. K. , & Beatch, J.-A. (2017). Children’s understanding of additive
concepts. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 156 , 16–28.
Individual Differences and Mathematical Difficulties
Ashcraft, M. H. , & Krause, J. A. (2007). Working memory, math performance, and math
anxiety. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14 , 243–248.
Kucian, K. , Kaufmann, L. , & von Aster, M. (2015). Brain correlates of numerical disabilities. In
R. Cohen Kadosh & A. Dowker (eds.). The Oxford Handbook of Numerical Cognition (pp.
732–744). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Landerl, K. , Bevan, A. , & Butterworth, B. (2004). Developmental dyscalculia and basic
numerical capacities: a study of 8–9-year-old students. Cognition, 93 , 99–125.
Piazza, M. , Facoetti, A. , Trussardi, A. N. , Berteletti, I. , Conte, S. , Lucangeli, D. , Dehaene, S.
, & Zorzi, M. (2010). Developmental trajectory of number acuity reveals a severe impairment in
developmental dyscalculia. Cognition, 116 , 33–41.
Rubinsten, O. , & Henik, A. (2009). Developmental dyscalculia: heterogeneity might not mean
different mechanisms. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13 , 92–99.

Number Systems
Ni, Y. , & Zhou, Y.-D. (2005). Teaching and learning fraction and rational numbers: the origins
and implications of whole number bias. Educational Psychologist, 40 , 27–52.
Siegler, R. S. , Fazio, L. K. , Bailey, D. H. , & Zhou, X. (2013). Fractions: the new frontier for
theories of numerical development. Trends in Cognitive Science, 17 , 13–19.
Van Hoof, J. , Verschaffel, L. , & Van Dooren, W. (2015). Inappropriately applying natural
number properties in rational number tasks: characterizing the development of the natural
number bias through primary and secondary education. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 90
, 39–56.
Vosniadou, S. , Vamvakoussi, X. , & Skopeliti, I. (2008). The framework theory approach to
conceptual change. In S. Vosniadou (Ed.), International Handbook of Research on Conceptual
Change (pp. 3–34). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Algebra and Equivalence


Kieran, C. (1997). Mathematical concepts at the secondary school level: the learning of algebra
and functions. In P. Bryant & T. Nunes (Eds.). Learning and Teaching Mathematics: An
International Perspective. Hove: Psychology Press.
Kirshner, D. (2001). The structural algebra option revisited. In R. Sutherland , T. Rojano , A. Bell
, & R. Lins (Eds.). Perspectives on School Algebra. New York: Kluwer.
McNeil, N. M. , & Albibali, M. W. (2005). Why won’t you change your mind? Knowledge of
operational patterns hinders learning and performance on equations. Child Development, 76 ,
883–899.
McNeil, N. M. , Fyfe, E. R. , & Dunwiddie, A. E. (2015). Arithmetic practice can be modified to
promote understanding of mathematical equivalence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107 ,
423–436.

Mathematical Argumentation and Proof


Harel, G. , & Sowder, L. (1998). Students’ proof schemes: results from exploratory studies. In T.
Dick (Managing ed.), A. H. Schoenfeld , J. Kaput , & E. Dubinsky (Vol. eds.), CBMS issues in
Mathematics Education: Vol. 7. Research in Collegiate Mathematics Education III (pp.
234–282). Providence, RI: American Mathematical Society.
Healy, L. , & Hoyles, C. (2000). A study of proof conceptions in algebra. Journal for Research in
Mathematics Education, 31 , 396–428.
Stylianides, A. J. (2007). Proof and proving in school mathematics. Journal for Research in
Mathematics Education, 38 , 289–321.
Weber, K. (2008). How mathematicians determine if an argument is a valid proof. Journal for
Research in Mathematics Education, 39 , 431–459.

Logic, Conditional Reasoning and Mathematics


Attridge, N. , & Inglis, M. (2013). Advanced mathematical study and the development of
conditional reasoning skills. PLOS ONE, 8 , e69399.
Evans, J. St. B. T. , & Over, D. E. (2004). If. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Johnson-Laird, P.
N. , & Byrne, R. M. J. (2002). Conditionals: a theory of meaning, pragmatics and inference.
Psychological Review, 109 , 646–678.
Manktelow, K. (2012). Thinking and reasoning: an introduction to the psychology of reason,
judgment and decision making. Hove: Psychology Press.

References
Agrillo, C. (2015). Numerical and arithmetical abilities in non-primate species. In R. Cohen
Kadosh & A. Dowker (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Numerical Cognition (pp. 214–236).
Oxford: Oxford University Press
Alcock, L. , Ansari, D. , Batchelor, S. , Bisson, M.-J. , De Smedt, B. , Gilmore, C. , Göbel, S. M. ,
Hannula Sormunen, M. , Hodgen, J. , Inglis, M. , Jones, I. , Mazzocco, M. , McNeil, N. ,
Schneider, M. , Simms, V. , & Weber, K. (2016). Challenges in mathematical cognition: a
collaboratively-derived research agenda. Journal of Numerical Cognition, 2 , 20–41
Alcock, L. , Bailey, T. , Inglis, M. , & Docherty, P. (2014). The ability to reject invalid logical
inferences predicts proof comprehension and mathematics performance. In T. Fukawa-Connolly
, G. Karakok , K. Keene , & M. Zandieh (Eds.), Proceedings of the 17th Annual Conference on
Research in Undergraduate Mathematics Education (pp. 376–383). Denver, CO: MAA
Alcock, L. , & Inglis, M. (2008). Doctoral students’ use of examples in evaluating and proving
conjectures. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 69 , 111–129
Alcock, L. , & Weber, K. (2005). Proof validation in real analysis: inferring and checking
warrants. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 24 , 125–134
Alexander, L. , & Martray, C. R. (1989). The development of an abbreviated version of the
Mathematics Anxiety Rating Scale. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and
Development, 22 (3), 143–150
Alibali, M. W. , & Sidney, P. G. (2015). Variability in the natural number bias: who, when, how
and why. Learning and Instruction, 37 , 56–61
American Psychiatric Association . (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders (DSM). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association
American Psychiatric Association . (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders. Fifth Edition. (DSM-5). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association
Andersson, U. (2008a). Mathematical competencies in children with different types of learning
difficulties. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100 , 48–66
Andersson, U. (2008b). Working memory as a predictor of written arithmetical skills in children:
the importance of central executive functions. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 78 ,
181–203
Andersson, U. (2010). Skill development in different components of arithmetic and basic
cognitive functions: findings from a 3-year longitudinal study of children with different types of
learning difficulties. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102 , 115–134
Anobile, G. , Cicchini, G. , & Burr, D. (2016). Number as a primary perceptual attribute: a
review. Perception, 45 , 5–31
Ansari, D. (2008). Effects of development and enculturation on number representation in the
brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9 , 278–291
Ansari, D. , Garcia, N. , Lucas, E. , Hamon, K. , & Dhital, B. (2005). Neural correlates of
symbolic number processing in children and adults. Neuroreport, 16 , 1769–1773
Ashcraft, M. H. (1992). Cognitive arithmetic: a review of data and theory. Cognition, 44 , 75–106
Ashcraft, M. H. , & Kirk, E. P. (2001). The relationships among working memory, math anxiety,
and performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130 , 224–237
Ashcraft, M. H. , & Krause, J. A. (2007). Working memory, math performance, and math
anxiety. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14 , 243–248
Ashcraft, M. H. , Krause, J. A. , Hopko, D. R. , Berch, D. B. , & Mazzocco, M. M. M. (2007). Is
math anxiety a mathematical learning disability? In D. B. Berch & M. M. M. Mazzocco (Eds.),
Why Is Math So Hard For Some Children? The Nature and Origins of Mathematical Learning
Difficulties and Disabilities (pp. 329–348). Baltimore, MD: Paul H Brookes Publishing Co
Ashcraft, M. H. & Ridley, K. S. (2005). Math anxiety and its cognitive consequences: a tutorial
review. In J. Campbell (Ed.), Handbook of Mathematical Cognition (pp. 315–330). New York:
Psychology Press
Ashkenazi, S. , Mark-Zigdon, N. , & Henik, A. (2009). Numerical distance effect in
developmental dyscalculia. Cognitive Development, 24 , 387–400
Ashkenazi, S. , Mark-Zigdon, N. , & Henik, A. (2013). Do subitizing deficits in developmental
dyscalculia involve pattern recognition weakness? Developmental Science, 16 , 35–46
Attridge, N. , Doritou, M. , & Inglis, M. (2015). The development of reasoning skills during
compulsory 16 to 18 mathematics education. Research in Mathematics Education, 17 , 20–37
Attridge, N. , & Inglis, M. (2013). Advanced mathematical study and the development of
conditional reasoning skills. PLoS ONE, 8 , e69399
Attridge, N. , & Inglis, M. (2014). Intelligence and negation biases on the conditional inference
task. Thinking and Reasoning, 20 , 454–471
Baddeley, A. D. & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. Psychology of Learning and Motivation,
8 , 47–89
Bailey, D. H. , Hoard, M. K. , Nugent, L. , & Geary, D. C. (2012). Competence with fractions
predicts gains in mathematics achievement. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113 ,
447–455
Bailey, D. H. , Zhou, X. , Zhang, Y. , Cui, J. , Fuchs, L. S. , Jordan, N. C. , Gersten, R. , &
Siegler, R. S. (2015). Development of fraction concepts and procedures in US and Chinese
children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 129 , 68–83
Baker, L. , & Cantwell, D. P. (1987). A prospective psychiatric follow-up of children with
speech/language disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent
Psychiatry, 26 , 546–553
Balakrishnanl,J. & Ashby, F. G. (1992). Subitizing: magical numbers or mere superstition?
Psychological Research, 54 , 80–90
Ball, D. L. & Bass, H. (2000). Making believe: the collective construction of public mathematical
knowledge in the elementary classroom. In D. Phillips (Ed.), Constructivism in Education:
Opinions and Second Opinions on Controversial Issues. Ninety-Ninth Yearbook of the National
Society for the Study of Education (pp. 193–224). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press
Barbaresi, W. J. , Katusic, S. K. , Colligan, R. C. , Weaver, A. L. , & Jacobsen, S. J. (2005).
Math learning disorder: incidence in a population-based birth cohort, 1976–82, Rochester, MN.
Ambulatory Pediatrics, 5 , 281–289
Barner, D. , Libenson, A. , Cheung, P. , & Takasaki, M. (2009). Cross-linguistic relations
between quantifiers and numerals in language acquisition: evidence from Japanese. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 103 , 421–440
Baroody, A. J. (1987). The development of counting strategies for single-digit addition. Journal
for Research in Mathematics Education, 18 , 141–157
Baroody, A. J. (1992). The development of kindergartners’ mental-addition strategies. Learning
and Individual Differences, 4 , 215–235
Baroody, A. J. (1999). The roles of estimation and the commutativity principle in the
development of third graders’ mental multiplication. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
74 , 157–193
Baroody, A. J. (2003). The development of adaptive expertise and flexibility: the integration of
conceptual and procedural knowledge. In A. J. Baroody & A. Dowker (eds.), The Development
of Arithmetic Concepts and Skills: Constructing Adaptive Expertise (pp. 1–33). Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Baroody, A. J. , Feil, Y. , & Johnson, A. R. (2007). An alternative reconceptualization of
procedural and conceptual knowledge. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 38 ,
115–131
Baroody, A. J. , & Gannon, K. E. (1984). The development of the commutativity principle and
economical addition strategies. Cognition and Instruction, 1 , 321–339
Baroody, A. J. , Ginsburg, H. P. , & Waxman, B. (1983). Children’s use of mathematical
structure. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 14 , 156–168
Baroody, A. J. , & Tiilikainen, S. H. (2003). Two perspectives on addition development. In A. J.
Baroody & A. Dowker (Eds.), The Development of Arithmetic Concepts and Skills: Constructing
Adaptive Expertise (pp. 75–125). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Baroody, A. J. , Torbeyns, J. , & Verschaffel, L. (2009). Young children’s understanding and
application of subtraction-related principles. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 11 , 2–9
Barrouillet, P. , Camos, V. , Perruchet, P. , & Seron, X. (2004). ADAPT: a developmental,
asemantic, and procedural model for transcoding from verbal to Arabic numerals. Psychological
Review, 111 , 368–394
Barrouillet, P. , & Lépine, R. (2005). Working memory and children’s use of retrieval to solve
addition problems. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 91 , 183–204
Barrouillet, P. , Thevenot, C. , & Fayol, M. (2010). Evidence for knowledge of the syntax of large
numbers in preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105 , 264–271
Barth, H. , La Mont, K. , Lipton, J. S. , Dehaene, S. , Kanwisher, N. , & Spelke, E. S. (2006).
Non-symbolic arithmetic in adults and young children. Cognition, 98 , 199–222
Batchelor, S. , Inglis, M. , & Gilmore, C. (2015). Spontaneous focusing on numerosity and the
arithmetic advantage. Learning and Instruction, 40 , 79–88
Batchelor, S. , Keeble, S. , & Gilmore, C. (2015). Magnitude representations and counting skills
in preschool children. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 17 , 116–135
Bayliss, D. M. , Jarrold, C. , Gunn, D. M. , & Baddeley, A. D. (2003). The complexities of
complex span: explaining individual differences in working memory in children and adults.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132 , 71–92
Behr, M. , Erlwanger, S. , & Nichols, E. (1980). How children view the equals sign. Mathematics
Teaching, 92 , 13–15
Beilock, S. L. , & Carr, T. H. (2005). When high-powered people fail: working memory and
‘choking under pressure’ in math. Psychological Science, 16 , 101–105
Beilock, S. L. , & DeCaro, M. S. (2007). From poor performance to success under stress:
working memory, strategy selection, and mathematical problem solving under pressure. Journal
of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33 , 983–998
Beilock, S. L. , Gunderson, E. A. , Ramirez, G. , & Levine, S. C. (2010). Female teachers’
math anxiety affects girls’ math achievement. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, 107 , 1860–1863
Beishuizen, M. (1993). Mental strategies and materials or models for addition and subtraction
up to 100 in Dutch second grades. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 24 ,
294–323
Bell, A. W. (1976). A study of pupils’ proof conceptions in mathematical situations. Educational
Studies in Mathematics, 7 , 23–40
Bender, A. , & Beller, S. (2014). Mangarevan invention of binary steps for easier calculation.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111 , 1322–1327
Benoit, L. , Lehalle, H. , & Jouen, F. (2004). Do young children acquire number words through
subitizing or counting? Cognitive Development, 19 , 291–307
Bisanz, J. , Watchorn, R. P. D. , Piatt, C. , & Sherman, J. (2009). On ‘understanding’ children’s
developing use of inversion. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 11 , 10–24
Bishop, D. V. M. (2010). Which neurodevelopmental disorders get researched and why? PLoS
ONE, 5 , e15112
Bisson, M.-J. , Gilmore, C. , Inglis, M. , & Jones, I. (2016). Measuring conceptual under-
standing using comparative judgement. International Journal of Research in Undergraduate
Mathematics Education, 1–24
Blair, C. , & Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief
understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Development, 78 ,
647–663
Blöte, A. W. , Klein, A. S. , & Beishuizen, M. (2000). Mental computation and conceptual
understanding. Learning and Instruction, 10 , 221–247
Bonato, M. , Zorzi, M. , & Umiltà, C. (2012). When time is space: evidence for a mental time
line. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36 , 2257–2273
Booth, J. L. , & Davenport, J. L. (2013). The role of problem representation and feature
knowledge in algebraic equation-solving. The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 32 , 415–423
Booth, J. L. , & Siegler, R. S. (2006). Developmental and individual differences in pure
numerical estimation. Developmental Psychology, 42 , 189–201
Brannon, E. M. , Wusthoff, C. J. , Gallistel, C. R. , & Gibbon, J. (2001). Numerical subtraction in
the pigeon: evidence for a linear subjective number scale. Psychological Science, 12 , 238–243
Briars, D. , & Siegler, R. S. (1984). A featural analysis of preschoolers’ counting knowledge.
Developmental Psychology, 20 , 607–618
Brown, J. R. (1999). Philosophy of Mathematics: An Introduction to the World of Proofs and
Pictures. New York: Routledge
Broyler, C. R. , Thorndike, E. L. , & Woodyard, E. (1927). A second study of mental discipline in
high school studies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 18 , 377–404
Bruce, C. D. , Davis, B. , Sinclair, N. , McGarvey, L. , Hallowell, D. , Drefs, M. , Francis, K. ,
Hawes, Z. , Moss, J. , Mulligan, J. , Okamoto, Y. , Whiteley, W. , & Woolcott, G. (2017).
Understanding gaps in research networks: using ‘spatial reasoning’ as a window into the
importance of networked educational research. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 95 ,
143–161
Brunyé, T. T. , Mahoney, C. R. , Giles, G. E. , Rapp, D. N. , Taylor, H. A. , & Kanarek, R. B.
(2013). Learning to relax: evaluating four brief interventions for overcoming the negative
emotions accompanying math anxiety. Learning and Individual Differences, 27 , 1–7
Bryant, P. , Christie, C. , & Rendu, A. (1999). Children’s understanding of the relation between
addition and subtraction: inversion, identity, and decomposition. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 74 , 194–212
Brysbaert, M. (1995). Arabic number reading: on the nature of the numerical scale and the
origin of phonological recoding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124 , 434–452
Brysbaert, M. (2005). Number recognition in different formats. In J. I. D. Campbell (Ed.),
Handbook of Mathematical Cognition (pp. 23–42). New York: Psychology Press
Bueti, D. , & Walsh, V. (2009). The parietal cortex and the representation of time, space,
number and other magnitudes. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B:
Biological Sciences, 364 , 1831–1840
Bugden, S. , & Ansari, D. (2015). Probing the nature of deficits in the ‘approximate number
system’ in children with persistent developmental dyscalculia. Developmental Science, 19 ,
817–833
Bugden, S. , Price, G. R. , McLean, D. A. , & Ansari, D. (2012). The role of the left intraparietal
sulcus in the relationship between symbolic number processing and children’s arithmetic
competence. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2 , 448–457
Bulf, H. , de Hevia, M. D. , & Macchi Cassia, V. (2016). Small on the left, large on the right:
numbers orient visual attention onto space in preverbal infants. Developmental Science, 19 ,
394–401
Bull, R. , Johnston, R. S. , & Roy, J. A. (1999). Exploring the roles of the visual-spatial sketch
pad and central executive in children’s arithmetical skills: views from cognition and
developmental neuropsychology. Developmental Neuropsychology, 15 , 421–442
Bull, R. , & Lee, K. (2014). Executive functioning and mathematics achievement. Child
Development Perspectives, 8 , 36–41
Bull, R. , & Scerif, G. (2001). Executive functioning as a predictor of children’s mathematics
ability: inhibition, switching, and working memory. Developmental Neuropsychology, 19 ,
273–293
Bulthé, J. , De Smedt, B. , & de Beeck, H. O. (2014). Format-dependent representations of
symbolic and non-symbolic numbers in the human cortex as revealed by multi-voxel pattern
analyses. NeuroImage, 87 , 311–322
Burr, D. C. , Turi, M. , & Anobile, G. (2010). Subitizing but not estimation of numerosity requires
attentional resources. Journal of Vision, 10 (6), 20
Busch, J. , Schmidt, C. , & Grube, D. (2015). Arithmetic fact retrieval. Zeitschrift für Psychologie,
223 , 110–119
Butterworth, B. , Reeve, R. A. , Reynolds, F. , & Lloyd, D. (2008). Numerical thought with and
without words: evidence from indigenous australian children. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences, 105 , 13179–13184
Button, K. S. , Ioannidis, J. P. A. , Mokrysz, C. , Nosek, B. A. , Flint, J. , Robinson, E. S. , &
Munafò, M. R. (2013). Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of
neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14 , 365–376
Byrd, C. E. , McNeil, N. M. , Chesney, D. L. , & Matthews, P. G. (2015). A specific
misconception of the equal sign acts as a barrier to children’s learning of early algebra.
Learning and Individual Differences, 38 , 61–67
Cai, J. , Wang, N. , Moyer, J. C. , Wang, C. , & Nie, B. (2011). Longitudinal investigation of the
curricular effect: an analysis of student learning outcomes from the lieCal project in the United
States. International Journal of Educational Research, 50 , 117–136
Camos, V. (2008). Low working memory capacity impedes both efficiency and learning of
number transcoding in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 99 , 37–57
Campbell, J. I. D. (1987). Network interference and mental multiplication. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13 , 109–123
Campbell, J. I. D. (1995). Mechanisms of simple addition and multiplication: a modified network-
interference theory and simulation. Mathematical Cognition, 1 , 121–164
Campbell, J. I. D. (1999). Division by multiplication. Memory & Cognition, 27 , 791–802
Campbell, J. I. D. , & Graham, D. J. (1985). Mental multiplication skill: structure, process, and
acquisition. Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie, 39 , 338–366
Canobi, K. H. (2004). Individual differences in children’s addition and subtraction knowledge.
Cognitive Development, 19 , 81–93
Canobi, K. H. (2005). Children’s profiles of addition and subtraction understanding. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 92 , 220–246
Canobi, K. H. (2009). Concept–procedure interactions in children’s addition and subtraction.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102 , 131–149
Canobi, K. H. , Reeve, R. A. , & Pattison, P. E. (1998). The role of conceptual understanding in
children’s addition problem solving. Developmental Psychology, 34 , 882–891
Canobi, K. H. , Reeve, R. A. , & Pattison, P. E. (2002). Young children’s understanding of
addition concepts. Educational Psychology, 22 , 513–532
Canobi, K. H. , Reeve, R. A. , & Pattison, P. E. (2003). Patterns of knowledge in children’s
addition. Developmental Psychology, 39 , 521–534
Cantlon, J. F. , Brannon, E. M. , Carter, E. J. , & Pelphrey, K. A. (2006). Functional imaging of
numerical processing in adults and 4-year-old children. PLoS Biol, 4, e125
Cantlon, J. F. , Libertus, M. E. , Pinel, P. , Dehaene, S. , Brannon, E. M. , & Pelphrey, K. A.
(2009). The neural development of an abstract concept of number. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 21 , 2217–2229
Capano, L. , Minden, D. , Chen, S. X. , Schachar, R. J. , & Ickowicz, A. (2008). Mathematical
learning disorder in school-age children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. The
Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 53 , 392–399
Carey, D. A. (1991). Number sentences: linking addition and subtraction word problems and
symbols. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 22 , 266–280
Carey, E. , Hill, F. , Devine, A. , & Szűucs, D. (2015). The chicken or the egg? The direction of
the relationship between mathematics anxiety and mathematics performance. Frontiers in
Psychology, 6 , 1987
Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual Change in Childhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Carpenter, T. P. , Ansell, E. , Franke, M. L. , Fennema, E. , & Weisbeck, L. (1993). Models of
problem solving: a study of kindergarten children’s problem-solving processes. Journal for
Research in Mathematics Education, 24 , 428–441
Carpenter, T. P. , Franke, M. L. , Jacobs, V. R. , Fennema, E. , & Empson, S. B. (1998). A
longitudinal study of invention and understanding in children’s multidigit addition and
subtraction. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 29 , 3–20
Carpenter, T. P. , Franke, M. L. , & Levi, L. (2003). Thinking Mathematically: Integrating
Arithmetic and Algebra in Elementary School. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann
Carpenter, T. P. , & Levi, L. (2000). Developing Conceptions of Algebraic Reasoning in the
Primary Grades. (Research Report No. NCISLA-RR-00-2). Wisconsin University, Madison:
National Center for Improving Student Learning and Achievement in Mathematics and Science
Camos, V. (2008). Low working memory capacity impedes both efficiency and learning of
number transcoding in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 99 , 37–57
Campbell, J. I. D. (1987). Network interference and mental multiplication. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13 , 109–123
Campbell, J. I. D. (1995). Mechanisms of simple addition and multiplication: a modified network-
interference theory and simulation. Mathematical Cognition, 1 , 121–164
Campbell, J. I. D. (1999). Division by multiplication. Memory & Cognition, 27 , 791–802
Campbell, J. I. D. , & Graham, D. J. (1985). Mental multiplication skill: structure, process, and
acquisition. Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie, 39 , 338–366
Canobi, K. H. (2004). Individual differences in children’s addition and subtraction knowledge.
Cognitive Development, 19 , 81–93
Canobi, K. H. (2005). Children’s profiles of addition and subtraction understanding. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 92 , 220–246
Canobi, K. H. (2009). Concept–procedure interactions in children’s addition and subtraction.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102 , 131–149
Canobi, K. H. , Reeve, R. A. , & Pattison, P. E. (1998). The role of conceptual understanding in
children’s addition problem solving. Developmental Psychology, 34 , 882–891
Canobi, K. H. , Reeve, R. A. , & Pattison, P. E. (2002). Young children’s understanding of
addition concepts. Educational Psychology, 22 , 513–532
Canobi, K. H. , Reeve, R. A. , & Pattison, P. E. (2003). Patterns of knowledge in children’s
addition. Developmental Psychology, 39 , 521–534
Cantlon, J. F. , Brannon, E. M. , Carter, E. J. , & Pelphrey, K. A. (2006). Functional imaging of
numerical processing in adults and 4-year-old children. PLoS Biol, 4, e125
Cantlon, J. F. , Libertus, M. E. , Pinel, P. , Dehaene, S. , Brannon, E. M. , & Pelphrey, K. A.
(2009 ). The neural development of an abstract concept of number. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 21 , 2217–2229
Capano, L. , Minden, D. , Chen, S. X. , Schachar, R. J. , & Ickowicz, A. (2008). Mathematical
learning disorder in school-age children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. The
Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 53 , 392–399
Carey, D. A. (1991). Number sentences: linking addition and subtraction word problems and
symbols. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 22 , 266–280
Carey, E. , Hill, F. , Devine, A. , & Szűcs, D. (2015 ). The chicken or the egg? The direction of
the relationship between mathematics anxiety and mathematics performance. Frontiers in
Psychology, 6 , 1987
Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual Change in Childhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Carpenter, T. P. , Ansell, E. , Franke, M. L. , Fennema, E. , & Weisbeck, L. (1993). Models of
problem solving: a study of kindergarten children’s problem-solving processes. Journal for
Research in Mathematics Education, 24 , 428–441
Carpenter, T. P. , Franke, M. L. , Jacobs, V. R. , Fennema, E. , & Empson, S. B. (1998). A
longitudinal study of invention and understanding in children’s multidigit addition and
subtraction. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 29 , 3–20
Carpenter, T. P. , Franke, M. L. , & Levi, L. (2003). Thinking Mathematically: Integrating
Arithmetic and Algebra in Elementary School. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann
Carpenter, T. P. , & Levi, L. (2000). Developing Conceptions of Algebraic Reasoning in the
Primary Grades. (Research Report No. NCISLA-RR-00-2). Wisconsin University, Madison:
National Center for Improving Student Learning and Achievement in Mathematics and Science
Carpenter, T. P. , & Moser, J. M. (1984). The acquisition of addition and subtraction concepts in
grades one through three. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 15 , 179–202
Carpentieri, J. D. , Lister, J. , & Frumkin, L. (2009). Adult Numeracy: A Review of Research.
London: National Research and Development Centre for Literacy and Numeracy, Institute of
Education, University of London
Carraher, D. W. , Schliemann, A. D. , Brizuela, B. M. , & Earnest, D. (2006). Arithmetic and
algebra in early mathematics education. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 37 ,
87–115
Carraher, T. N. , Carraher, D. W. , & Schliemann, A. D. (1985). Mathematics in the streets and
in schools. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 3 , 21–29
Castronovo, J. , & Göbel, S. M. (2012 ). Impact of high mathematics education on the number
sense. PLoS ONE, 7 , e33832
Chambers, C. D. (2013). Registered reports: a new publishing initiative at cortex. Cortex, 49 ,
609–610
Chambers, C. D. (2017). The Seven Deadly Sins of Psychology: A Manifesto for Reforming the
Culture of Scientific Practice. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press
Chan, B. M. , & Ho, C. S. (2010). The cognitive profile of Chinese children with mathematics
difficulties. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107 , 260–279
Chan, W. W. L. , Au, T. K. , & Tang, J. (2011). Exploring the developmental changes in
automatic two-digit number processing. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109 ,
263–274
Chandler, C. C. , & Kamii, C. (2009). Giving change when payment is made with a dime: the
difficulty of tens and ones. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 40 , 97–118
Chazan, D. (1993). High school geometry students’ justification for their views of empirical
evidence and mathematical proof. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 24 , 359–387
Chen, Q. , & Li, J. (2014). Association between individual differences in non-symbolic number
acuity and math performance: a meta-analysis. Acta Psychologica, 148 , 163–172
Chi, M. T. , & Klahr, D. (1975). Span and rate of apprehension in children and adults. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 19 , 434–439
Chinn, C. , & Brewer, W. F. (1993). The role of anomalous data in knowledge acquisition: a
theoretical framework and implications for science instruction. Review of Educational Research,
63 , 1–49
Choo, H. , & Franconeri, S. (2014). Enumeration of small collections violates Weber’s law.
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21 , 93–99
Christley, R. (2010). Power and error: increased risk of false positive results in underpowered
studies. The Open Epidemiology Journal, 3 , 16–19
Christou, K. P. , & Vosniadou, S. (2012). What kinds of numbers do students assign to literal
symbols? Aspects of the transition from arithmetic to algebra. Mathematical Thinking and
Learning, 14 , 1–27
Clark, C. A. , Pritchard, V. E. , & Woodward, L. J. (2010). Preschool executive functioning
abilities predict early mathematics achievement. Developmental Psychology, 46 , 1176–1191
Clarke, D. M. , & Roche, A. (2009). Students’ fraction comparison strategies as a window into
robust understanding and possible pointers for instruction. Educational Studies in Mathematics,
72 , 127–138
Clayton, S. , Gilmore, C. , & Inglis, M. (2015). Dot comparison stimuli are not all alike: the effect
of different visual controls on ans measurement. Acta Psychologica, 161 , 177–184
Cockcroft, W. H. (1982). Mathematics counts: Report of the Committee of Inquiry into the
Teaching of Mathematics in Schools under the Chairmanship of Dr WH Cockcroft. London: HM
Stationery Office
Coe, R. , & Ruthven, K. (1994). Proof practices and constructs of advanced mathematical
students. British Educational Research Journal, 20 , 41–53
Cohen Kadosh, R. , Bahrami, B. , Walsh, V. , Butterworth, B. , Popescu, T. , & Price, C. J.
(2011). Specialization in the human brain: the case of numbers. Frontiers in Human
Neuroscience, 5, 62
Cohen Kadosh, R. , Cohen Kadosh, K. , Kaas, A. , Henik, A. , & Goebel, R. (2007). Notation-
dependent and-independent representations of numbers in the parietal lobes. Neuron, 53 ,
307–314
Cohen Kadosh, R. , Dowker, A. , Heine, A. , Kaufmann, L. , & Kucian, K. (2013). Interventions
for improving numerical abilities: present and future. Trends in Neuroscience and Education, 2 ,
85–93
Condry, K. F. , & Spelke, E. S. (2008). The development of language and abstract concepts: the
case of natural number. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137 , 22–38
Cosmides, L. (1989). The law of social exchange: has natural selection shaped how humans
reason? Studies with the Wason selection task. Cognition, 31 , 187–276
Cosmides, L. , & Tooby, J. (1992). Cognitive adaptations for social exchange. In J. Barkow , L.
Cosmides , & J. Tooby (Eds.), The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation
of Culture. Oxford: Oxford University Press
Cowan, R. (2015). Education. In R. Cohen Kadosh & A. Dowker (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook
of Numerical Cognition (pp. 1021–1035). Oxford: Oxford University Press
Cowan, R. , & Powell, D. (2014). The contributions of domain-general and numerical factors to
third-grade arithmetic skills and mathematical learning disability. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 106 , 214–229
Cowan, R. , & Renton, M. (1996). Do they know what they are doing? Children’s use of
economical addition strategies and knowledge of commutativity. Educational Psychology, 16 ,
407–420
Cragg, L. , & Gilmore, C. (2014). Skills underlying mathematics: the role of executive function in
the development of mathematics proficiency. Trends in Neuroscience and Education, 3 , 63–68
Cragg, L. , Keeble, S. , Richardson, S. , Roome, H. E. , & Gilmore, C. (2017). Direct and indirect
influences of executive functions on mathematics achievement. Cognition, 162 , 12–26
Crollen, V. , Castronovo, J. , & Seron, X. (2015). Under- and over-estimation. Experimental
Psychology, 58 , 39–49
Crooks, N. M. , & Alibali, M. W. (2014). Defining and measuring conceptual knowledge in
mathematics. Developmental Review, 34 , 344–377
Cutini, S. , Scatturin, P. , Moro, S. B. , & Zorzi, M. (2014). Are the neural correlates of subitizing
and estimation dissociable? An fNIRS investigation. Neuroimage, 85 , 391–399
Dauben, J. W. (1990). Georg Cantor: His Mathematics and Philosophy of the Infinite. Princeton,
NJ: Princeton University Press
David, C. V. (2012). Working memory deficits in math learning difficulties: a meta-analysis.
International Journal of Developmental Disabilities, 58 , 67–84
Davidson, K. , Eng, K. , & Barner, D. (2012). Does learning to count involve a semantic
induction? Cognition, 123 , 162–173
Davies, B. , Xu Rattanasone, N. , & Demuth, K. (2015). Revisiting 2;0-year-olds’ understanding
of plural morphology. In Paper presented at the Fortieth Annual Boston University Conference
on Language Development, Boston, MA, USA.
De Bock, D. , Van Dooren, W. , Janssens, D. , & Verschaffel, L. (2007). The Illusion of Linearity:
From Analysis to Improvement. New York: Springer Science & Business Media
De Bock, D. , Verschaffel, L. , & Janssens, D. (1998). The predominance of the linear model in
secondary school students’ solutions of word problems involving length and area of similar
plane figures. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 35 , 65–83
De Bock, D. , Verschaffel, L. , Janssens, D. , Van Dooren, W. , & Claes, K. (2003). Do realistic
contexts and graphical representations always have a beneficial impact on students’
performance? Negative evidence from a study on modelling non-linear geometry problems.
Learning and Instruction, 13 , 441–463
De Cruz, H. (2016). Numerical cognition and mathematical realism. Philosophers’ Imprint, 16 ,
1–13
DeFranco, T. C. (1996). A perspective on mathematical problem solving expertise based on the
performance of male Ph.D. mathematicians. CBMS Issues in Mathematics Education, 6 ,
195–213
Dehaene, S. (1997). The Number Sense. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Dehaene, S. (2001). Précis of the number sense. Mind & Language, 16 , 16–36
Dehaene, S. (2007). Symbols and quantities in parietal cortex: elements of a mathematical
theory of number representation and manipulation. In P. Haggard , Y. Rossetti , & M. Kawato
(Eds.), Sensori-Motor Foundations of Higher Cognition: Attention & Performance XXII. (pp.
527–574). Oxford: Oxford University Press
Dehaene, S. , Bossini, S. , & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number
magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122 , 371–396
Dehaene, S. , & Brannon, E. M. (2011). Space, Time and Number in the Brain: Searching for
the Foundations of Mathematical Thought. San Diego, CA: Academic Press
Dehaene, S. , & Changeux, J.- P. (1993). Development of elementary numerical abilities: a
neuronal model. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5 , 390–407
Dehaene, S. , & Cohen, L. (1994). Dissociable mechanisms of subitizing and counting:
neuropsychological evidence from simultanagnosic patients. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20 , 958–975
Dehaene, S. , & Cohen, L. (1995). Towards an anatomical and functional model of number
processing. Mathematical Cognition, 1 , 83–120
Dehaene, S. , & Cohen, L. (2007). Cultural recycling of cortical maps. Neuron, 56, 384–398.
Dehaene, S. , Dupoux, E. , & Mehler, J. (1990). Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and
symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human
Perception and Performance, 16 , 626–641
Dehaene, S. , Naccache, L. , Le Clec’H, G. , Koechlin, E. , Mueller, M. , Dehaene-Lambertz, G.
, van de Moortele, P.-F. , & Le Bihan, D. (1998). Imaging unconscious semantic priming.
Nature, 395 , 597–600
Demeyere, N. , Rotshtein, P. , & Humphreys, G. W. (2012). The neuroanatomy of visual
enumeration: differentiating necessary neural correlates for subitizing versus counting in a
neuropsychological voxel-based morphometry study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24 ,
948–964
Department for Business, Innovation and Skills . (2011). 2011 Skills for Life Survey: Headline
Findings. London: Department for Business, Innovation and Skills
Department for Education and Skills . (2003). The Skills for Life Survey. A National Needs and
Impact Survey of Literacy, Numeracy and ICT Skills. London: Department for Education and
Skills
De Rammelaere, S. , Stuyven, E. , & Vandierendonck, A. (2001). Verifying simple arithmetic
sums and products: are the phonological loop and the central executive involved? Memory &
Cognition, 29 , 267–273
De Smedt, B. , Ansari, D. , Grabner, R. H. , Hannula-Sormunen, M. M. , Schneider, M. , &
Verschaffel, L. (2011). Cognitive neuroscience meets mathematics education: it takes two to
tango. Educational Research Review, 6 , 232–237
De Smedt, B. , & Gilmore, C. (2011). Defective number module or impaired access? Numerical
magnitude processing in first graders with mathematical difficulties. Journal of Experimental
Child Psychology, 108 , 278–292
De Smedt, B. , Noël, M.-P. , Gilmore, C. , & Ansari, D. (2013). How do symbolic and non-
symbolic numerical magnitude processing skills relate to individual differences in children’s
mathematical skills? A review of evidence from brain and behavior. Trends in Neuroscience and
Education, 2 , 48–55
Devine, A. , Fawcett, K. , Szűcs, D. , & Dowker, A. (2012). Gender differences in mathematics
anxiety and the relation to mathematics performance while controlling for test anxiety.
Behavioral and Brain Functions, 8, 33
De Visscher, A. , & Noël, M.- P. (2014). Arithmetic facts storage deficit: the hypersensitivity-to-
interference in memory hypothesis. Developmental Science, 17 , 434–442
DeWind, N. K. , & Brannon, E. M. (2012). Malleability of the approximate number system:
effects of feedback and training. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 68
DeWind, N. K. , & Brannon, E. M. (2016). Significant inter-test reliability across approximate
number system assessments. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 310
Diester, I. , & Nieder, A. (2007). Semantic associations between signs and numerical categories
in the prefrontal cortex. PLoS Biol, 5, e294
Dietrich, J. F. , Huber, S. , & Nuerk, H.-C. (2015). Methodological aspects to be considered
when measuring the approximate number system (ANS) – a research review. Frontiers in
Psychology, 6, 295
Dirks, E. , Spyer, G. , van Lieshout, E. C. D. M. , & de Sonneville, L. (2008). Prevalence of
combined reading and arithmetic disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41 , 460–473
Donlan, C. , Cowan, R. , Newton, E. J. , & Lloyd, D. (2007). The role of language in
mathematical development: evidence from children with specific language impairments.
Cognition, 103 , 23–33
Dowker, A. (2005). Individual Differences in Arithmetic: Implications for Psychology,
Neuroscience and Education. Hove: Psychology Press
Dowker, A. (2008). Individual differences in numerical abilities in preschoolers. Develop-mental
Science, 11 , 650–654
Dowker, A. (2009). Use of derived fact strategies by children with mathematical difficulties.
Cognitive Development, 24 , 401–410
Dowker, A. (2013). Young children’s use of derived fact strategies for addition and subtraction.
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 924
Dowker, A. , & Nuerk, H.-C. (2016). Linguistic influences on mathematics. Frontiers in
Psychology, 7, 1035
Droit-Volet, S. , Clément, A. , & Fayol, M. (2003). Time and number discrimination in a bisection
task with a sequence of stimuli: a developmental approach. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 84 , 63–76
Dubé, A. K. (2014). Adolescents’ understanding of inversion and associativity. Learning and
Individual Differences, 36 , 49–59
Dubé, A. K. , & Robinson, K. M. (2010). Accounting for individual variability in inversion shortcut
use. Learning and Individual Differences, 20 , 687–693
Dubinsky, E. , Weller, K. , McDonald, M. A. , & Brown, A. (2005a). Some historical issues and
paradoxes regarding the concept of infinity: an APOS-based analysis: Part 1. Educational
Studies in Mathematics, 58 , 335–359
Dubinsky, E. , Weller, K. , McDonald, M. A. , & Brown, A. (2005b). Some historical issues and
paradoxes regarding the concept of infinity: an APOS analysis: Part 2. Educational Studies in
Mathematics, 60 , 253–266
Duncan, G. J. , Dowsett, C. J. , Claessens, A. , Magnuson, K. , Huston, A. C. , Klebanov, P. ,
Pagani, L. S. , Feinstein, L. , Engel, M. , Brooks-Gunn, J. , Sexton, H. , Duckworth, K. , & Japel,
C. (2007). School readiness and later achievement. Developmental Psychology, 43 ,
1428–1446
Durand, M. , Hulme, C. , Larkin, R. , & Snowling, M. J. (2005). The cognitive foundations of
reading and arithmetic skills in 7- to 10-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 91
, 113–136
Duval, R. (2007). Cognitive functioning and the understanding of mathematical processes of
proof. In P. Boero (Ed.), Theorems in School: From History, Epistemology and Cognition to
Classroom Practice (pp. 25–41). Rotterdam: Sense
Egeth, H. E. , Leonard, C. J. , & Palomares, M. (2008). The role of attention in subitizing: is the
magical number 1? Visual Cognition, 16 , 463–473
Egner, T. (2008). Multiple conflict-driven control mechanisms in the human brain. Trends in
Cognitive Sciences, 12 , 374–380
Ehlert, A. , Schroeders, U. , & Fritz-Stratmann, A. (2012). Criticism of the discrepancy criterion
in the diagnosis of dyslexia and dyscalculia. Lernen und Lernstörungen, 1 , 169–184
Emerson, R. W. , & Cantlon, J. F. (2012). Early math achievement and functional connectivity in
the fronto-parietal network. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2 , S139–S151
Emerson, R. W. , & Cantlon, J. F. (2015). Continuity and change in children’s longitudinal neural
responses to numbers. Developmental Science, 18 , 314–326
Espy, K. A. , McDiarmid, M. M. , Cwik, M. F. , Stalets, M. M. , Hamby, A. , & Senn, T. E. (2004).
The contribution of executive functions to emergent mathematic skills in preschool children.
Developmental Neuropsychology, 26 , 465–486
Evans, J. St. B. T. (1977). Linguistic factors in reasoning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 29 , 297–306
Evans, J. St. B. T. , & Ball, L. J. (2010). Do people reason on the selection task? A new look at
the data of Ball et al.. (2003). Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63 , 434–441
Evans, J. St. B. T. , Clibbens, J. , & Rood, B. (1995). Bias in conditional inference: implications
for mental models and mental logic. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48A ,
644–670
Evans, J. St. B. T. , Clibbens, J. , & Rood, B. (1996). The role of implicit and explicit negation in
conditional reasoning bias. Journal of Memory and Language, 35 , 392–409
Evans, J. St. B. T. , & Handley, S. J. (1999). The role of negation in conditional inference.
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52A , 739–769
Evans, J. St. B. T. , Handley, S. J. , Neilens, H. , & Over, D. E. (2007). Thinking about
conditionals: a study of individual differences. Memory & Cognition, 35 , 1772–1784
Evans, J. St. B. T. , & Lynch, J. S. (1973). Matching bias in the selection task. British Journal of
Psychology, 64 , 391–397
Evans, J. St. B. T. , Newstead, S. E. , & Byrne, R. M. J. (1993). Human Reasoning: The
Psychology of Deduction. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum
Evans, J. St. B. T. , & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Dual-process theories of higher cognition:
advancing the debate. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8 , 223–241
Fazio, L. K. , Bailey, D. H. , Thompson, C. A. , & Siegler, R. S. (2014). Relations of different
types of numerical magnitude representations to each other and to mathematics achievement.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 123 , 53–72
Feigenson, L. (2008). Parallel non-verbal enumeration is constrained by a set-based limit.
Cognition, 107 , 1–18
Feigenson, L. , & Carey, S. (2005). On the limits of infants’ quantification of small object arrays.
Cognition, 97 , 295–313
Feigenson, L. , Carey, S. , & Hauser, M. (2002). The representations underlying infants’ choice
of more: object files versus analog magnitudes. Psychological Science, 13 , 150–156
Feigenson, L. , Carey, S. , & Spelke, E. S. (2002). Infants’ discrimination of number vs.
continuous extent. Cognitive Psychology, 44 , 33–66
Feigenson, L. , Dehaene, S. , & Spelke, E. S. (2004). Core systems of number. Trends in
Cognitive Sciences, 8 , 307–314
Feyerabend, P. (1993). Against Method. London: Verso
Fischbein, E. (1982). Intuition and proof. For the Learning of Mathematics, 3 (2), 9–18.
Fischbein, E. , Deri, M. , & Marino, M. (1985). The role of implicit models in solving problems in
multiplication and division. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 16 , 3–17
Fischer, J.-P. , & Koch, A.- M. (2016). Mirror writing in typically developing children: a first
longitudinal study. Cognitive Development, 38 , 114–124
Fischer, M. H. (2003). Cognitive representation of negative numbers. Psychological Science, 14
, 278–282
Fischer, M. H. , & Rottmann, J. (2005). Do negative numbers have a place on the mental
number line. Psychology Science, 47 , 22–32
Fitch, J. G. (1883). Lectures on Teaching. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Fosnot, C. T. , & Dolk, M. L. A. M. (2001). Young Mathematicians at Work. Portsmouth, NH:
Heinemann
Foster, J. E. (1947). A number system without a zero-symbol. Mathematics Magazine, 21 ,
39–41
Franco, A. , Malhotra, N. , & Simonovits, G. (2014). Publication bias in the social sciences:
unlocking the file drawer. Science, 345 , 1502–1505
Friso-van den Bos, I. , van der Ven, S. H. , Kroesbergen, E. H. , & van Luit, J. E. H. (2013).
Working memory and mathematics in primary school children: a meta-analysis. Educational
Research Review, 10 , 29–44
Frith, C. D. , & Frith, U. (1972). The solitaire illusion: an illusion of numerosity. Perception &
Psychophysics, 11 , 409–410
Frosch, C. , & Simms, V. (2015). Understanding the role of reasoning ability in mathematical
achievement. In G. Airenti , B. G. Bara , & G. Sandini (Eds.), Euroasianpacific Joint Conference
on Cognitive Science (pp. 633–638). Torino, Italy
Frydman, O. , & Bryant, P. (1988). Sharing and the understanding of number equivalence by
young children. Cognitive Development, 3 , 323–339
Fuchs, L. S. , Compton, D. L. , Fuchs, D. , Paulsen, K. , Bryant, J. D. , & Hamlett, C. L. (2005).
The prevention, identification, and cognitive determinants of math difficulty. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 97 , 493–513
Fuchs, L. S. , Fuchs, D. , Compton, D. L. , Powell, S. R. , Seethaler, P. M. , Capizzi, A. M. ,
Schatschneider, C. , & Fletcher, J. M. (2006). The cognitive correlates of third-grade skill in
arithmetic, algorithmic computation, and arithmetic word problems. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 98 , 29–43
Fuchs, L. S. , Geary, D. C. , Compton, D. L. , Fuchs, D. , Hamlett, C. L. , Seethaler, P. M. ,
Bryant, J. D. , & Schatschneider, C. (2010). Do different types of school mathematics
development depend on different constellations of numerical versus general cognitive abilities?
Developmental Psychology, 46 , 1731–1746
Fuchs, L. S. , Schumacher, R. F. , Long, J. , Namkung, J. , Hamlett, C. L. , Cirino, P. T. ,
Jordan, N. C. , Siegler, R. S. , Gersten, R. , & Changas, P. (2013). Improving at-risk learners’
understanding of fractions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105 , 683–700
Fuhs, M. W. , & McNeil, N. M. (2013). ANS acuity and mathematics ability in preschoolers from
low-income homes: contributions of inhibitory control. Developmental Science, 16 , 136–148
Fuson, K. C. (1988). Children’s Counting and Concepts of Number. New York: Springer Verlag
Fuson, K. C. (1990). Conceptual structures for multiunit numbers: implications for learning and
teaching multidigit addition, subtraction, and place value. Cognition and Instruction, 7 , 343–403
Fuson, K. C. (1992). Research on whole number addition and subtraction. In D. A. Grouws
(Ed.), Handbook of Research on Mathematics Teaching and Learning: A Project of the National
Council of Teachers of Mathematics (pp. 243–275). New York: Macmillan
Gaber, D. , & Schlimm, D. (2015). Basic mathematical cognition. Wiley Interdisciplinary
Reviews: Cognitive Science, 6 , 355–369
Gabriel, F. , Coché, F. , Szűcs, D. , Carette, V. , Rey, B. , & Content, A. (2012). Developing
children’s understanding of fractions: an intervention study. mind brain educ. Mind, Brain and
Education, 6 , 137–146
Gabriel, F. , Coché, F. , Szűcs, D. , Carette, V. , Rey, B. , & Content, A. (2013). A componential
view of children’s difficulties in learning fractions. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 715
Gallistel, C. R. , & Gelman, R. (1991). Subitizing: the preverbal counting process. In W. Kessen
, A. Ortony , & F. Craik (Eds.), Memories, Thoughts and Emotions: Essays in Honor of George
Mandler (pp. 65–81). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates
Gallistel, C. R. , & Gelman, R. (1992). Preverbal and verbal counting and computation.
Cognition, 44 , 43–74
Gallistel, C. R. , & Gelman, R. (2000). Non-verbal numerical cognition: from reals to integers.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4 , 59–65
Geary, D. C. (1990). A componential analysis of an early learning deficit in mathematics.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 49 , 363–383
Geary, D. C. (1993). Mathematical disabilities: cognitive, neuropsychological, and genetic
components. Psychological Bulletin, 114 , 345–362
Geary, D. C. (2004). Mathematics and learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 37 ,
4–15
Geary, D. C. (2015). The classification and cognitive characteristics of mathematical disabilities
in children. In R. Cohen Kadosh & A. Dowker (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Numerical
Cognition (pp. 767–786). Oxford: Oxford University Press
Geary, D. C. , Bow-Thomas, C. C. , & Yao, Y. (1992). Counting knowledge and skill in cognitive
addition: a comparison of normal and mathematically disabled children. Journal of Experimental
Child Psychology, 54 , 372–391
Geary, D. C. , Brown, S. C. , & Samaranayake, V. (1991). Cognitive addition: a short
longitudinal study of strategy choice and speed-of-processing differences in normal and
mathematically disabled children. Developmental Psychology, 27 , 787–797
Geary, D. C. , Frensch, P. A. , & Wiley, J. G. (1993). Simple and complex mental subtraction:
strategy choice and speed-of-processing differences in younger and older adults. Psychology
and Aging, 8 , 242–256
Geary, D. C. , Hamson, C. O. , & Hoard, M. K. (2000). Numerical and arithmetical cognition: a
longitudinal study of process and concept deficits in children with learning disability. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 77 , 236–263
Geary, D. C. , & Hoard, M. K. (2001). Numerical and arithmetical deficits in learning-disabled
children: relation to dyscalculia and dyslexia. Aphasiology, 15 , 635–647
Geary, D. C. , & Hoard, M. K. (2005). Learning disabilities in arithmetic and mathematics. In J. I.
D. Campbell (Ed.), Handbook of Mathematical Cognition (pp. 253–267). New York: Psychology
Press
Geary, D. C. , Hoard, M. K. , Byrd-Craven, J. , & DeSoto, M. C. (2004). Strategy choices in
simple and complex addition: contributions of working memory and counting knowledge for
children with mathematical disability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 88 , 121–151
Geary, D. C. , Hoard, M. K. , Byrd-Craven, J. , Nugent, L. , & Numtee, C. (2007). Cognitive
mechanisms underlying achievement deficits in children with mathematical learning disability.
Child Development, 78 , 1343–1359
Geary, D. C. , Hoard, M. K. , & Hamson, C. O. (1999). Numerical and arithmetical cognition:
patterns of functions and deficits in children at risk for a mathematical disability. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 74 , 213–239
Geary, D. C. , Hoard, M. K. , Nugent, L. , & Bailey, D. H. (2012). Mathematical cognition deficits
in children with learning disabilities and persistent low achievement: a five-year prospective
study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104 , 206–223
Geary, D. C. , Hoard, M. K. , Nugent, L. , & Byrd-Craven, J. (2008). Development of number line
representations in children with mathematical learning disability. Developmental
Neuropsychology, 33 , 277–299
Gebuis, T. , Cohen Kadosh, R. , de Haan, E. , & Henik, A. (2009). Automatic quantity
processing in 5-year olds and adults. Cognitive Processing, 10 , 133–142
Gebuis, T. , Cohen Kadosh, R. , & Gevers, W. (2016). Sensory-integration system rather than
approximate number system underlies numerosity processing: a critical review. Acta
Psychologica, 171 , 17–35
Gebuis, T. , & Reynvoet, B. (2012). The role of visual information in numerosity estimation. PloS
ONE, 7, e37426
Gebuis, T. , & Van Der Smagt, M. J. (2011). False approximations of the approximate number
system? PLoS ONE, 6, e25405
Gelman, R. , & Butterworth, B. (2005). Number and language: how are they related? Trends in
Cognitive Sciences, 9 , 6–10
Gelman, R. , & Gallistel, C. R. (1978). The Children’s Understanding of Number. Cambridge,
MA: Harvard University Press
Gelman, R. , & Williams, E. M. (1998). Enabling constraints for cognitive development and
learning: domain specificity and epigenesis. In D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of
Child Psychology, Vol. 2, Cognition, Perception and Language, 5th Edition. New York: John
Wiley & Sons Inc
Giaquinto, M. (2007). Visual thinking in mathematics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Gilbert, D. T. , King, G. , Pettigrew, S. , & Wilson, T. D. (2016). Comment on ‘estimating the
reproducibility of psychological science’. Science, 351, 1037–1037.
Gillies, D. (1992). Revolutions in Mathematics. Oxford: Oxford University Press
Gilmore, C. , Attridge, N. , Clayton, S. , Cragg, L. , Johnson, S. , Marlow, N. , Simms, V. , &
Inglis, M. (2013). Individual differences in inhibitory control, not non-verbal number acuity,
correlate with mathematics achievement. PloS ONE, 8, e67374
Gilmore, C. , Attridge, N. , De Smedt, B. , & Inglis, M. (2014). Measuring the approximate
number system in children: exploring the relationships among different tasks. Learning and
Individual Differences, 29 , 50–58
Gilmore, C. , Attridge, N. , & Inglis, M. (2011). Measuring the approximate number system. The
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64 , 2099–2109
Gilmore, C. , & Bryant, P. (2006). Individual differences in children’s understanding of inversion
and arithmetical skill. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76 , 309–331
Gilmore, C. , & Papadatou-Pastou, M. (2009). Patterns of individual differences in conceptual
understanding and arithmetical skill: a meta-analysis. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 11 ,
25–40
Ginsburg, H. P. (1978). Children’s Arithmetic. Oxford: D. Van Nostrand
Girelli, L. , Lucangeli, D. , & Butterworth, B. (2000). The development of automaticity in
accessing number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 76 , 104–122
Göbel, S. M. , Johansen-Berg, H. , Behrens, T. , & Rushworth, M. F. (2004). Response-
selection-related parietal activation during number comparison. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 16 , 1536–1551
Göbel, S. M. , Shaki, S. , & Fischer, M. H. (2011). Cultural effects on the mental number line.
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42 , 543–565
Göbel, S. M. , Watson, S. E. , Lervåg, A. , & Hulme, C. (2014). Children’s arithmetic
development: it is number knowledge, not the approximate number sense, that counts.
Psychological Science, 25 , 789–798
Gordon, P. (2004). Numerical cognition without words: evidence from Amazonia. Science, 306 ,
496–499
Greer, B. (1992). Multiplication and division as models of situations. In D. A. Grouws (Ed.),
Handbook of Research on Mathematics Teaching and Learning: A Project of the National
Council of Teachers of Mathematics (pp. 276–295). New York: Macmillan
Gresham, G. (2007). A study of mathematics anxiety in preservice teachers. Early Childhood
Education Journal, 35 , 181–188
Griffiths, B. J. , & Peitz, E. A. (2016). A comparison of syllogistic reasoning skills among
American undergraduates. In G. Kaiser & R. vom Hofe (Eds.), Proceedings of the 13th
International Congress on Mathematical Education. Hamburg, Germany
Griggs, R. A. , & Cox, J. R. (1982). The elusive thematix-materials effect in Wason’s selection
task. British Journal of Psychology, 73 , 407–420
Grill-Spector, K. , Henson, R. , & Martin, A. (2006). Repetition and the brain: neural models of
stimulus-specific effects. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10 , 14–23
Groen, G. , & Resnick, L. B. (1977). Can preschool children invent addition algorithms? Journal
of Educational Psychology, 69 , 645–652
Gross-Tsur, V. , Manor, O. , & Shalev, R. S. (1996). Developmental dyscalculia: prevalence and
demographic features. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 38 , 25–33
Grotheer, M. , Ambrus, G. G. , & Kovács, G. (2016). Causal evidence of the involvement of the
number form area in the visual detection of numbers and letters. Neuroimage, 132 , 314–319
Grotheer, M. , Herrmann, K.-H. , & Kovács, G. (2016). Neuroimaging evidence of a bilateral
representation for visually presented numbers. The Journal of Neuroscience, 36 , 88–97
Guillaume, M. , & Gevers, W. (2016). Assessing the approximate number system: no relation
between numerical comparison and estimation tasks. Psychological Research, 80 , 248–258
Gunderson, E. A. , & Levine, S. C. (2011). Some types of parent number talk count more than
others: relations between parents’ input and children’s cardinal-number knowledge.
Developmental Science, 14 , 1021–1032
Gunderson, E. A. , Ramirez, G. , Beilock, S. L. , & Levine, S. C. (2012). The relation between
spatial skill and early number knowledge: the role of the linear number line. Developmental
Psychology, 48 , 1229–1241
Hafner, J. , & Mancosu, P. (2005). The varieties of mathematical explanation. In P. Mancosu ,
K. F. Jørgensen , & S. A. Pedersen (Eds.), Visualization, Explanation and Reasoning Styles in
Mathematics (pp. 215–250). Berlin: Springer
Halberda, J. , & Feigenson, L. (2008). Developmental change in the acuity of the ‘number
sense’: The approximate number system in 3-, 4-, 5-, and 6-year-olds and adults.
Developmental Psychology, 44 , 1457–1465
Halberda, J. , Ly, R. , Wilmer, J. B. , Naiman, D. Q. , & Germine, L. (2012). Number sense
across the lifespan as revealed by a massive internet-based sample. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences, 109 , 11116–11120
Halberda, J. , Mazzocco, M. M. M. , & Feigenson, L. (2008). Individual differences in non-verbal
number acuity correlate with maths achievement. Nature, 455 , 665–668
Halberda, J. , & Odic, D. (2015). The precision and internal confidence of our approximate
thoughts. In D. C. Geary , D. B. Berch , & K. Mann Koepke (Eds.), Evolutionary Origins and
Early Development of Number Processing (pp. 305–333). London: Academic Press
Handley, S. J. , Capon, A. , Beveridge, M. , Dennis, I. , & Evans, J. St. B. T. (2004). Working
memory, inhibitory control, and the development of children’s reasoning. Thinking and
Reasoning, 10 , 175–195
Hanich, L. B. , Jordan, N. C. , Kaplan, D. , & Dick, J. (2001). Performance across different areas
of mathematical cognition in children with learning difficulties. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 93 , 615–626
Hanna, G. (1991). Mathematical proof. In D. O. Tall (Ed.), Advanced Mathematical Thinking (pp.
54–61). Dordrecht: Kluwer
Hannula, M. M. (2005). Spontaneous Focusing on Numerosity in the Development of Early
Mathematical Skills (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Annales Universitatis Turkuensis B 282
Hannula, M. M. , & Lehtinen, E. (2001). Spontaneous tendency to focus on numerosities in the
development of cardinality. In M. Panhuizen-Van Heuvel (Ed.), Proceedings of 25th Conference
of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (Vol. 3 , pp. 113–120).
Amersfoort: Drukkerij Wilco
Hannula, M. M. , & Lehtinen, E. (2005). Spontaneous focusing on numerosity and mathematical
skills of young children. Learning and Instruction, 15 , 237–256
Hannula-Sormunen, M. M. (2015). Spontaneous focusing on numerosity and its relation to
counting and arithmetic. In R. Cohen Kadosh & A. Dowker (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of
Numerical Cognition (pp. 275–290). Oxford: Oxford University Press
Harel, G. (2001). The development of mathematical induction as a proof scheme: a model for
DNR-based instruction. In S. Campbell & R. Zazkis (Eds.), Learning and Teaching Number
Theory (pp. 185–212). Westport, CT: Ablex Publishing Corp
Harel, G. (2007). Students’ proof schemes revisited: historical and epistemological
considerations. In P. Boero (Ed.), Theorems in School: From History, Epistemology and
Cognition to Classroom Practice (pp. 65–78). Rotterdam: Sense
Harel, G. , & Sowder, L. (1998). Students’ proof schemes: results from exploratory studies. In A.
H. Schoenfeld , J. Kaput , & E. Dubinsky (Eds.), Research in Collegiate Mathematics III (pp.
234–282). Providence, RI: American Mathematical Society
Harel, G. , & Sowder, L. (2005). Advanced mathematical-thinking at any age: its nature and
development. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 7 , 27–50
Harnad, S. (1990). The symbol grounding problem. Physica D: Nonlinear Phenomena, 42 ,
335–346
Harvey, B. , Klein, B. , Petridou, N. , & Dumoulin, S. (2013). Topographic representation of
numerosity in the human parietal cortex. Science, 341 , 1123–1126
Healy, L. , & Hoyles, C. (2000). A study of proof conceptions in algebra. Journal for Research in
Mathematics Education, 31 , 396–428
Hecht, S. A. , Torgesen, J. K. , Wagner, R. K. , & Rashotte, C. A. (2001). The relations between
phonological processing abilities and emerging individual differences in mathematical
computation skills: a longitudinal study from second to fifth grades. Journal of Experimental
Child Psychology, 79 , 192–227
Hembree, R. (1990). The nature, effects, and relief of mathematics anxiety. Journal for
Research in Mathematics Education, 21 , 33–46
Hembree, R. (1992). Experiments and relational studies in problem solving: a meta-analysis.
Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 23 , 242–273
Hendel, D. D. , & Davis, S. O. (1978). Effectiveness of an intervention strategy for reducing
mathematics anxiety. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 25 , 429–434
Hendrickson, G. , & Schroeder, W. H. (1941). Transfer of training in learning to hit a submerged
target. Journal of Educational Psychology, 32 , 205–213
Henik, A. , & Tzelgov, J. (1982). Is three greater than five: the relation between physical and
semantic size in comparison tasks. Memory & Cognition, 10 , 389–395
Herscovics, N. , & Linchevski, L. (1994). A cognitive gap between arithmetic and algebra.
Educational Studies in Mathematics, 27 , 59–78
Hersh, R. (1993). Proving is convincing and explaining. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 24
, 389–399
Hiebert, J. (1986). Conceptual Knowledge and Procedural Knowledge: The Case of
Mathematics. Hilsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Hiebert, J. , & Carpenter, T. P. (1992). Learning and teaching with understanding. In D. A.
Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of Research on Mathematics Teaching and Learning: A Project of the
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (pp. 65–97). New York: Macmillan
Hiebert, J. , & Lefevre, P. (1986). Conceptual and procedural knowledge in mathematics: an
introductory analysis. In J. Hiebert (Ed.), Conceptual and Procedural Knowledge: The Case of
Mathematics (pp. 1–27). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Hiebert, J. , & Wearne, D. (1996). Instruction, understanding, and skill in multidigit addition and
subtraction. Cognition and Instruction, 14 , 251–283
Hilbert, T. S. , Renkl, A. , Kessler, S. , & Reiss, K. (2008). Learning to prove in geometry:
learning from heuristic examples and how it can be supported. Learning and Instruction, 18 ,
54–65
Hitch, G. J. , & McAuley, E. (1991). Working memory in children with specific arithmetical
learning difficulties. British Journal of Psychology, 82 , 375–386
Hodds, M. , Alcock, L. , & Inglis, M. (2014). Self-explanation training improves proof
comprehension. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 45 , 62–101
Hodgen, J. , Coe, R. , Brown, M. , & Küchemann, D. (2014). Improving students’ understanding
of algebra and multiplicative reasoning: did the ICCAMS intervention work? In In Proceedings of
8th British Congress of Mathematics Education. Nottingham
Hodgen, J. , Küchemann, D. , Brown, M. , & Coe, R. (2009). Children’s understandings of
algebra 30 years on. Research in Mathematics Education, 11 , 193–194
Hodgen, J. , Pepper, D. , Sturman, L. , & Ruddock, G. (2010). Is the UK an Outlier? An
International Comparison of Upper Secondary Mathematics Education. London: Nuffield
Foundation
Hollingsworth, W. H. , Simmons, J. P. , Coates, T. R. , & Cross, H. A. (1991). Perceived
numerosity as a function of array number, speed of array development, and density of array
items. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 29 , 448–450
Holloway, I. D. , & Ansari, D. (2009). Mapping numerical magnitudes onto symbols: the
numerical distance effect and individual differences in children’s mathematics achievement.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103 , 17–29
Holloway, I. D. , & Ansari, D. (2010). Developmental specialization in the right intraparietal
sulcus for the abstract representation of numerical magnitude. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 22 , 2627–2637
Holloway, I. D. , & Ansari, D. (2015). Numerical symbols: an overview of their cognitive and
neural underpinnings. In R. Cohen Kadosh & A. Dowker (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of
Numerical Cognition (pp. 531–551). Oxford: Oxford University Press
Holloway, I. D. , Battista, C. , Vogel, S. E. , & Ansari, D. (2013). Semantic and perceptual
processing of number symbols: evidence from a cross-linguistic fMRI adaptation study. Journal
of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25 , 388–400
Holloway, I. D. , Price, G. R. , & Ansari, D. (2010). Common and segregated neural pathways
for the processing of symbolic and nonsymbolic numerical magnitude: an fMRI study.
Neuroimage, 49 , 1006–1017
Hopko, D. R. , Mahadevan, R. , Bare, R. L. , & Hunt, M. K. (2003). The Abbreviated Math
Anxiety Scale (AMAS): construction, validity, and reliability. Assessment, 10 , 178–182
Hubber, P. J. , Gilmore, C. , & Cragg, L. (2014). The roles of the central executive and
visuospatial storage in mental arithmetic: a comparison across strategies. The Quarterly Journal
of Experimental Psychology, 67 , 936–954
Huber, S. , Sury, D. , Moeller, K. , Rubinsten, O. , & Nuerk, H.- C. (2015). A general number-to-
space mapping deficit in developmental dyscalculia. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 43
, 32–42
Hudson, C. , Price, D. , & Gross, J. (2009). The Long-Term Costs of Numeracy Difficulties.
London: Every Child a Chance Trust
Huizinga, M. , Dolan, C. V. , & van der Molen, M. W. (2006). Age-related change in executive
function: developmental trends and a latent variable analysis. Neuropsychologia, 44 ,
2017–2036
Huntley-Fenner, G. , & Cannon, E. (2000). Preschoolers’ magnitude comparisons are mediated
by a preverbal analog mechanism. Psychological Science, 11 , 147–152
Huttenlocher, J. , Jordan, N. C. , & Levine, S. C. (1994). A mental model for early arithmetic.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123 , 284–296
Hyde, D. C. , Khanum, S. , & Spelke, E. S. (2014). Brief non-symbolic, approximate number
practice enhances subsequent exact symbolic arithmetic in children. Cognition, 131 , 92–107
Iannone, P. , & Inglis, M. (2010). Self-efficacy and mathematical proof: are undergraduates
good at assessing their own proof production ability? In Proceedings of the 13th Conference for
Research in Undergraduate Mathematics Education. Raleigh, NC
Ifrah, G. (1985). From One to Zero: A Universal History of Numbers (L. Bair, Trans.). New York:
Viking Penguin
Imbo, I. , Vanden Bulcke, C. , De Brauwer, J. , & Fias, W. (2014). Sixty-four or four-and-sixty?
The influence of language and working memory on children’s number transcoding. Frontiers in
Psychology, 5, 313
Imbo, I. , & Vandierendonck, A. (2007a). The development of strategy use in elementary school
children: working memory and individual differences. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
96 , 284–309
Imbo, I. , & Vandierendonck, A. (2007b). The role of phonological and executive working
memory resources in simple arithmetic strategies. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology,
19 , 910–933
Imbo, I. , Vandierendonck, A. , & Vergauwe, E. (2007). The role of working memory in carrying
and borrowing. Psychological Research, 71 , 467–483
Indow, T. , & Ida, M. (1977). Scaling of dot numerosity. Perception & Psychophysics, 22 ,
265–276
Inglis, M. , & Alcock, L. (2012). Expert and novice approaches to reading mathematical proofs.
Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 43 , 358–390
Inglis, M. , & Attridge, N. (2016). Does mathematical study develop logical thinking? Testing the
Theory of Formal Discipline. London: World Scientific
Inglis, M. , Attridge, N. , Batchelor, S. , & Gilmore, C. (2011). Non-verbal number acuity
correlates with symbolic mathematics achievement: but only in children. Psychonomic Bulletin &
Review, 18 , 1222–1229
Inglis, M. , Batchelor, S. , Gilmore, C. , & Watson, D. G. (2017). Is the ANS linked to
mathematics performance? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 40, e174
Inglis, M. , & Gilmore, C. (2013). Sampling from the mental number line: how are approximate
number system representations formed? Cognition, 129 , 63–69
Inglis, M. , & Gilmore, C. (2014). Indexing the Approximate Number System. Acta Psychologica,
145 , 147–155
Inglis, M. , & Mejía-Ramos, J. P. (2009). The effect of authority on the persuasiveness of
mathematical arguments. Cognition and Instruction, 27 , 25–50
Inglis, M. , Mejía-Ramos, J. P. , & Simpson, A. (2007). Modelling mathematical argu-mentation:
the importance of qualification. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 66 , 3–21
Inglis, M. , Mejía-Ramos, J. P. , Weber, K. , & Alcock, L. (2013). On mathematicians’ different
standards when evaluating elementary proofs. Topics in Cognitive Science, 5 , 270–282
Inglis, M. , & Simpson, A. (2004). Mathematicians and the selection task. In M. Johnsen Høines
& A. B. Fuglestad (Eds.), Proceedings of the 28th International Conference on the Psychology
of Mathematics Education (Vol. 3 , pp. 89–96). Bergen, Norway: IGPME
Inhelder, B. , & Piaget, J. (1958). The Growth of Logical Thinking from Childhood to
Adolescence: An Essay on the Construction of Formal Operational Structures. New York: Basic
Books
Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2005). Why most published research findings are false. PLoS Med, 2, e124
Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2008). Why most discovered true associations are inflated. Epidemiology, 19
, 640–648
Iuculano, T. , Tang, J. , Hall, C. W. , & Butterworth, B. (2008). Core information processing
deficits in developmental dyscalculia and low numeracy. Developmental Science, 11 , 669–680
Izard, V. , Dehaene-Lambertz, G. , & Dehaene, S. (2008). Distinct cerebral pathways for object
identity and number in human infants. PLoS Biol, 6, e11
Izard, V. , Sann, C. , Spelke, E. S. , & Streri, A. (2009). Newborn infants perceive abstract
numbers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106 , 10382–10385
Jensen, E. , Reese, E. , & Reese, T. W. (1950). The subitizing and counting of visually
presented fields of dots. The Journal of Psychology, 30 , 363–392
Jevons, W. S. (1871). The power of numerical discrimination. Nature, 3(67), 281–282.
John, L. K. , Loewenstein, G. , & Prelec, D. (2012). Measuring the prevalence of questionable
research practices with incentives for truth telling. Psychological Science, 23, 524–532.
Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental Models: Towards a Cognitive Science of Language,
Inference,
and Consciousness (No. 6 ). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Johnson-Laird, P. N. , & Byrne, R. M. J. (2002). Conditionals: a theory of meaning, pragmatics
and inference. Psychological Review, 109 , 646–678
Jones, I. , Inglis, M. , Gilmore, C. , & Dowens, M. (2012). Substitution and sameness: two
components of a relational conception of the equals sign. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 113 , 166–176
Jones, I. , Inglis, M. , Gilmore, C. , & Evans, R. (2013). Teaching the substitutive conception of
the equals sign. Research in Mathematics Education, 15 , 34–49
Jordan, J.-A. , Mulhern, G. , & Wylie, J. (2009). Individual differences in trajectories of
arithmetical development in typically achieving 5- to 7-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 103 , 455–468
Jordan, N. C. , Hanich, L. B. , & Kaplan, D. (2003a). Arithmetic fact mastery in young children: a
longitudinal investigation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 85 , 103–119
Jordan, N. C. , Hanich, L. B. , & Kaplan, D. (2003b). A longitudinal study of mathematical
competencies in children with specific mathematics difficulties versus children with comorbid
mathematics and reading difficulties. Child Development, 74 , 834–850
Jordan, N. C. , Kaplan, D. , Locuniak, M. N. , & Ramineni, C. (2007). Predicting first-grade math
achievement from developmental number sense trajectories. Learning Disabilities Research &
Practice, 22 , 36–46
Jordan, N. C. , Kaplan, D. , Nabors Oláh, L. , & Locuniak, M. N. (2006). Number sense growth
in kindergarten: a longitudinal investigation of children at risk for mathematics difficulties. Child
Development, 77 , 153–175
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, Fast and Slow. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.
Kahneman, D., Treisman, A., & Gibbs, B. J. (1992). The reviewing of object files: object-specific
integration of information. Cognitive Psychology, 24 , 175–219
Kallai, A. Y. , & Tzelgov, J. (2012). The place-value of a digit in multi-digit numbers is processed
automatically. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38 ,
1221–1233
Kamann, M. P. , & Wong, B. Y. (1993). Inducing adaptive coping self-statements in children with
learning disabilities through self-instruction training. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 26 ,
630–638
Kamii, C. (1986). Place value: an explanation of its difficulty and educational implications for the
primary grades. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 1 , 75–86
Kaput, J. J. (2000). Transforming Algebra from an Engine of Inequity to an Engine of
Mathematical Power by ‘Algebrafying’ the K-12 curriculum. (Tech. Rep.). Washington, DC: US
Department of Education
Kaufman, E. L. , Lord, M. W. , Reese, T. W. , & Volkmann, J. (1949). The discrimination of
visual number. The American Journal of Psychology, 62 , 498–525
Kaufmann, L. , Koppelstaetter, F. , Siedentopf, C. , Haala, I. , Haberlandt, E. , Zimmerhackl, L.-
B. , Felber, S. , & Ischebeck, A. (2006). Neural correlates of the number–size interference task
in children. Neuroreport, 17, 587
Kaufmann, L. , Mazzocco, M. M. M. , Dowker, A. , von Aster, M. G. , Göbel, S. M. , Grabner, R.
H. , Henik, A. , Jordan, N. C. , Karmiloff-Smith, A. D. , Kucian, K. , Rubinsten, O. , Szűcs, D. ,
Shalev, R. , & Nuerk, H.-C. (2013). Dyscalculia from a developmental and differential
perspective. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 516
Kaufmann, L. , Pixner, S. , & Göbel, S. M. (2011). Finger usage and arithmetic in adults with
math difficulties: evidence from a case report. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 254
Kaufmann, L. , Vogel, S. E. , Starke, M. , Kremser, C. , Schocke, M. , & Wood, G. (2009).
Developmental dyscalculia: compensatory mechanisms in left intraparietal regions in response
to nonsymbolic magnitudes. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 5, 35
Kaufmann, L. , Wood, G. , Rubinsten, O. , & Henik, A. (2011). Meta-analyses of developmental
fMRI studies investigating typical and atypical trajectories of number processing and calculation.
Developmental Neuropsychology, 36 , 763–787
Kena, G. , Hussar, W. , McFarland, J. , de Brey, C. , Musu-Gillette, L. , Wang, X. , Zhng, J. ,
Rathbun, A. , Wilkinson-Flicker, S. , Diliberti, M. , Barmer, A. , Bullock Mann, F. , & Dunlop
Velez, E. (2016). The Condition of Education 2016. National Center for Education Statistics
2016–144. Washington, DC: US Department of Education
Kieran, C. (1997). Mathematical concepts at the secondary school level: the learning of algebra
and functions. In P. Bryant & T. Nunes (Eds.), Learning and Teaching Mathematics: An
International Perspective. Hove: Psychology Press
Kieran, C. (2004). Algebraic thinking in the early grades: what is it. The Mathematics Educator,
8 , 139–151
Kieran, C. (2006). Research on the learning and teaching of algebra. In A. Gutiérrez & P. Boero
(Eds.), Handbook of Research on the Psychology of Mathematics Education: Past, Present and
Future (pp. 11–49). Rotterdam: Sense
Kieran, C. (2007). Learning and teaching of algebra at the middle school through college level:
building meaning for symbols and their manipulation. In F. K. Lester Jr (Ed.), Second Handbook
of Research on Mathematics Teaching and Learning (p. 707). Charlotte, NC: Information Age
Publishing
Kirshner, D. (2001). The structural algebra option revisited. In R. Sutherland , T. Rojano , A. Bell
, & R. Lins (Eds.), Perspectives on School Algebra (pp. 83–98). New York: Kluwer
Klahr, D. , & Wallace, J. (1973). The role of quantification operators in the development of
conservation of quantity. Cognitive Psychology, 4 , 301–327
Klauer, K. C. , Stahl, C. , & Erdfelder, E. (2007). The abstract selection task: new data and an
almost comprehensive model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and
Cognition, 33 , 680–703
Klein, J. S. , & Bisanz, J. (2000). Preschoolers doing arithmetic: the concepts are willing but the
working memory is weak. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de
psychologie expérimentale, 54 , 105–116
Knudsen, B. , Fischer, M. H. , & Aschersleben, G. (2015). Development of spatial preferences
for counting and picture naming. Psychological Research, 79 , 939–949
Knuth, E. J. (2002). Secondary school mathematics teachers conceptions of proof. Journal for
Research in Mathematics Education, 33 , 379–405
Knuth, E. J. , Choppin, J. , & Bieda, K. (2009). Middle school students’ productions of
mathematical justification. In M. Blanton , D. Stylianou , & E. J. Knuth (Eds.), Teaching and
Learning Proof across the Grades: A K-16 Perspective (pp. 153–212). New York: Routledge
Knuth, E. J. , Alibali, M. W. , McNeil, N. M. , Weinberg, A. , & Stephens, A. C. (2005). Middle
school students’ understanding of core algebraic concepts: equivalence & variable. Zentralblatt
für Didaktik der Mathematik, 37 , 68–76
Knuth, E. J. , Stephens, A. C. , McNeil, N. M. , & Alibali, M. W. (2006). Does understanding the
equal sign matter? Evidence from solving equations. Journal for Research in Mathematics
Education, 37 , 297–312
Koechlin, E. (1997). Numerical transformations in five-month-old human infants.
Mathematical Cognition, 3, 89–104
Kohn, J. , Wyschkon, A. , & Esser, G. (2013). Psychische Auffälligkeiten bei umschriebe-nen
Entwicklungsstörungen: gibt es unterschiede zwischen Lese-rechtschreib- und
Rechenstörungen? Lernen und Lernstörungen, 2 , 7–20
Koontz, K. L. , & Berch, D. B. (1996). Identifying simple numerical stimuli: Processing
inefficiencies exhibited by arithmetic learning disabled children. Mathematical Cognition, 2 ,
1–24
Kovas, Y. , Harlaar, N. , Petrill, S. A. , & Plomin, R. (2005). ‘Generalist genes’ and mathematics
in 7-year-old twins. Intelligence, 33(5), 473–489
Kovas, Y. , Haworth, C. M. A. , Harlaar, N. , Petrill, S. A. , Dale, P. S. , & Plomin, R. (2007).
Overlap and specificity of genetic and environmental influences on mathematics and reading
disability in 10-year-old twins. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48 , 914–922
Krajewski, K. , & Schneider, W. (2009). Exploring the impact of phonological awareness,
visual–spatial working memory, and preschool quantity–number competencies on mathematics
achievement in elementary school: findings from a 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 103 , 516–531
Krinzinger, H. , Kaufmann, L. , Dowker, A. , Thomas, G. , Graf, M. , Nuerk, H.-C. , & Willmes, K.
(2007). Deutschsprachige Version des Fragebogens für Rechenangst (fra) für 6- bis 9-jährige
Kinder. Zeitschrift fur Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie und Psychotherapie, 35 , 341–351
Krinzinger, H. , Kaufmann, L. , & Willmes, K. (2009). Math anxiety and math ability in early
primary school years. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 27 , 206–225
Kroesbergen, E. H. , van Luit, J. E. H. , van Lieshout, E. C. D. M. , van Loosbroek, E. , & van de
Rijt, B. (2009). Individual differences in early numeracy: the role of executive functions and
subitizing. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 27 , 226–236
Küchemann, D. (1978). Children’s understanding of numerical variables. Mathematics in
School, 7 , 23–26
Kucian, K. , Ashkenazi, S. S. , Hänggi, J. , Rotzer, S. , Jäncke, L. , Martin, E. , & von Aster, M.
G. (2014). Developmental dyscalculia: a dysconnection syndrome? Brain Structure and
Function, 219 , 1721–1733
Kucian, K. , Grond, U. , Rotzer, S. , Henzi, B. , Schönmann, C. , Plangger, F. , Gälli, M. , Martin,
E. , & von Aster, M. G. (2011). Mental number line training in children with developmental
dyscalculia. NeuroImage, 57 , 782–795
Kucian, K. , Kaufmann, L. , & von Aster, M. G. (2015). Brain correlates of numerical disabilities.
In R. Cohen Kadosh & A. Dowker (Ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Numerical Cognition (pp.
732–744). Oxford: Oxford University Press
Kuhn, J.-T. , Ise, E. , Raddatz, J. , Schwenk, C. , & Dobel, C. (2016). Basic numerical
processing, calculation, and working memory in children with dyscalculia and/or ADHD
symptoms. Zeitschrift für Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie und Psychotherapie, 44 , 365–375
Kuhn, T. (1962). The Structure of Scientific Revolutions. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago
Press
Kulpa, Z. (2009). Main problems of diagrammatic reasoning. Part I: the generalization problem.
Foundations of Science, 14 , 75–96
Lakatos, I. (1976). A renaissance of empiricism in the recent philosophy of mathematics. British
Journal of the Philosophy of Science, 27 , 201–223
Lakatos, I. (1980). The Methodology of Scientific Research Programmes: Volume 1:
Philosophical Papers (Vol. 1 ). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Landerl, K. (2013). Development of numerical processing in children with typical and dyscalculic
arithmetic skills: a longitudinal study. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 459
Landerl, K. , Bevan, A. , & Butterworth, B. (2004). Developmental dyscalculia and basic
numerical capacities: a study of 8–9-year-old students. Cognition, 93 , 99–125
Landerl, K. , Fussenegger, B. , Moll, K. , & Willburger, E. (2009). Dyslexia and dyscalculia: two
learning disorders with different cognitive profiles. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
103 , 309–324
Landerl, K. , & Kölle, C. (2009). Typical and atypical development of basic numerical skills in
elementary school. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103 , 546–565
Landerl, K. , & Moll, K. (2010). Comorbidity of learning disorders: prevalence and familial
transmission. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51 , 287–294
Landerl, K. , & Wimmer, H. (2008). Development of word reading fluency and spelling in a
consistent orthography: an 8-year follow-up. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100 , 150–161
Langford, P. E. (1981). A longitudinal study of children’s understanding of logical laws in
arithmetic and Boolean algebra. Educational Psychology, 1 , 119–139
Lave, J. (1988). Cognition in practice: Mind, Mathematics and Culture in Everyday Life.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Le Corre, M. , & Carey, S. (2007). One, two, three, four, nothing more: an investigation of the
conceptual sources of the verbal counting principles. Cognition, 105 , 395–438
Le Corre, M. , Li, P. , Huang, B. H. , Jia, G. , & Carey, S. (2016). Numerical morphology
supports early number word learning: evidence from a comparison of young Mandarin and
English learners. Cognitive Psychology, 88 , 162–186
Le Corre, M. , Van de Walle, G. , Brannon, E. M. , & Carey, S. (2006). Re-visiting the
competence/performance debate in the acquisition of the counting principles. Cognitive
Psychology, 52 , 130–169
Lee, J. (2009). Universals and specifics of math self-concept, math self-efficacy, and math
anxiety across 41 PISA 2003 participating countries. Learning and Individual Differences, 19 ,
355–365
Lee, J. , & Stankov, L. (2013). Higher-order structure of noncognitive constructs and prediction
of PISA 2003 mathematics achievement. Learning and Individual Differences, 26 , 119–130
Lee, K. , Ng, S. F. , Pe, M. L. , Ang, S. Y. , Hasshim, M. N. A. M. , & Bull, R. (2012). The
cognitive underpinnings of emerging mathematical skills: executive functioning, patterns,
numeracy, and arithmetic. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 82 , 82–99
LeFevre, J.-A. , Fast, L. , Skwarchuk, S.-L. , Smith-Chant, B. L. , Bisanz, J. , Kamawar, D. , &
Penner-Wilger, M. (2010). Pathways to mathematics: longitudinal predictors of performance.
Child Development, 81 , 1753–1767
Leibovich, T. , & Henik, A. (2013). Magnitude processing in non-symbolic stimuli. Frontiers in
Psychology, 4, 375
Leibovich, T. , Katzin, N. , Harel, M. , & Henik, A. (2017). From ‘sense of number’ to ‘sense of
magnitude’: the role of continuous magnitudes in numerical cognition. Behavioral and Brain
Sciences, 40, e164
Lemaire, P. , & Arnaud, L. (2008). Young and older adults’ strategies in complex arithmetic. The
American Journal of Psychology, 121 , 1–16
Lemaire, P. , & Lecacheur, M. (2001). Older and younger adults’ strategy use and execution in
currency conversion tasks: insights from French franc to euro and euro to French franc
conversions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 7 , 195–206
Lemaire, P. , & Siegler, R. S. (1995). Four aspects of strategic change: contributions to
children’s learning of multiplication. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124 , 83–97
Leslie, A. M. , Gelman, R. , & Gallistel, C. R. (2008). The generative basis of natural number
concepts. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12 , 213–218
Levine, S. C. , Jordan, N. C. , & Huttenlocher, J. (1992). Development of calculation abilities in
young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 53 , 72–103
Levine, S. C. , Suriyakham, L. W. , Rowe, M. L. , Huttenlocher, J. , & Gunderson, E. A. (2010).
What counts in the development of young children’s number knowledge? Developmental
Psychology, 46 , 1309–1319
Lew, K. , Fukawa-Connelly, T. , Mejía-Ramos, J. P. , & Weber, K. (2016). Lectures in advanced
mathematics: why students might not understand what the professor is trying to convey. Journal
for Research in Mathematics Education, 47 , 162–198
Lewis, C. , Hitch, G. J. , & Walker, P. (1994). The prevalence of specific arithmetic difficulties
and specific reading difficulties in 9- to 10-year-old boys and girls. Journal of Child Psychology
and Psychiatry, 35 , 283–292
Li, X. , Ding, M. , Capraro, M. M. , & Capraro, R. M. (2008). Sources of differences in children’s
understandings of mathematical equality: comparative analysis of teacher guides and student
texts in China and the United States. Cognition and Instruction, 26 , 195–217
Li, Y. , & Geary, D. C. (2013). Developmental gains in visuospatial memory predict gains in
mathematics achievement. PLoS ONE, 8, e70160
Libertus, M. E. , & Brannon, E. M. (2010). Stable individual differences in number discrimination
in infancy. Developmental Science, 13 , 900–906
Libertus, M. E. , Feigenson, L. , & Halberda, J. (2011). Preschool acuity of the approximate
number system correlates with school math ability. Developmental Science, 14 , 1292–1300
Libertus, M. E. , Feigenson, L. , & Halberda, J. (2013a). Is approximate number precision a
stable predictor of math ability? Learning and Individual Differences, 25 , 126–133
Libertus, M. E. , Feigenson, L. , & Halberda, J. (2013b). Numerical approximation abilities
correlate with and predict informal but not formal mathematics abilities. Journal of Experimental
Child Psychology, 116 , 829–838
Lindskog, M. , Winman, A. , Juslin, P. , & Poom, L. (2013). Measuring acuity of the approximate
number system reliably and validly: the evaluation of an adaptive test procedure. Frontiers in
Psychology, 4, 510
Lipton, J. S. , & Spelke, E. S. (2003). Origins of number sense: large-number discrimination in
human infants. Psychological Science, 14 , 396–401
Lobato, J. (2006). Alternative perspectives on the transfer of learning: history, issues, and
challenges for future research. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 15 , 431–449
Locke, J. (1751). The Works of John Locke Esq in Three Volumes. London: Desmaizeaux.
Logan, G. D., & Zbrodoff, N. J. (2003). Subitizing and similarity: toward a pattern-matching
theory of enumeration. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10 , 676–682
Lopes-Silva, J. B. , Moura, R. , Júlio-Costa, A. , Haase, V. G. , & Wood, G. (2014). Phonemic
awareness as a pathway to number transcoding. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 13
Lortie-Forgues, H. , Tian, J. , & Siegler, R. S. (2015). Why is learning fraction and decimal
arithmetic so difficult? Developmental Review, 38 , 201–221
Lourenco, S. F. , & Longo, M. R. (2010). General magnitude representation in human infants.
Psychological Science, 21 , 873–881
Lubin, A. , Vidal, J. , Lanoë, C. , Houdé, O. , & Borst, G. (2013). Inhibitory control is needed for
the resolution of arithmetic word problems: a developmental negative priming study. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 105 , 701–708
Luo, Y. L. , Kovas, Y. , Haworth, C. M. , & Plomin, R. (2011). The etiology of mathematical self-
evaluation and mathematics achievement: understanding the relationship using a cross-lagged
twin study from ages 9 to 12. Learning and Individual Differences, 21 , 710–718
Luwel, K. , Onghena, P. , Torbeyns, J. , Schillemans, V. , & Verschaffel, L. (2009). Strengths
and weaknesses of the choice/no-choice method in research on strategy use. European
Psychologist, 14 , 351–362
Lyons, I. M. , Ansari, D. , & Beilock, S. L. (2012). Symbolic estrangement: evidence against a
strong association between numerical symbols and the quantities they represent. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: General, 141 , 635–641
Lyons, I. M. , Ansari, D. , & Beilock, S. L. (2015). Qualitatively different coding of symbolic and
nonsymbolic numbers in the human brain. Human Brain Mapping, 36 , 475–488
Lyons, I. M. , & Beilock, S. L. (2012a). Mathematics anxiety: separating the math from the
anxiety. Cerebral Cortex, 22 , 2102–2110
Lyons, I. M. , & Beilock, S. L. (2012b). When math hurts: math anxiety predicts pain network
activation in anticipation of doing math. PloS ONE, 7, e48076
Ma, X. (1999). A meta-analysis of the relationship between anxiety toward mathematics and
achievement in mathematics. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 30 , 520–540
Ma, X. , & Kishor, N. (1997). Attitude toward self, social factors, and achievement in
mathematics: a meta-analytic review. Educational Psychology Review, 9 , 89–120
Ma, X. , & Xu, J. (2004). The causal ordering of mathematics anxiety and mathematics
achievement: a longitudinal panel analysis. Journal of Adolescence, 27 , 165–179
MacGregor, M. , & Stacey, K. (1997). Students’ understanding of algebraic notation: 11–15.
Educational Studies in Mathematics, 33 , 1–19
Maehler, C. , & Schuchardt, K. (2011). Working memory in children with learning disabilities:
rethinking the criterion of discrepancy. International Journal of Disability, Development and
Education, 58 , 5–17
Maloney, E. A. , Ansari, D. , & Fugelsang, J. A. (2011). The effect of mathematics anxiety on the
processing of numerical magnitude. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64 ,
10–16
Maloney, E. A. , Risko, E. F. , Preston, F. , Ansari, D. , & Fugelsang, J. (2010). Challenging the
reliability and validity of cognitive measures: the case of the numerical distance effect. Acta
Psychologica, 134 , 154–161
Mandler, G. , & Shebo, B. J. (1982). Subitizing: an analysis of its component processes. Journal
of Experimental Psychology: General, 111 , 1–22
Manktelow, K. I. (2012). Thinking and Reasoning: An Introduction to the Psychology of Reason,
Judgment and Decision Making. Hove: Psychology Press
Markowitz, E. M. , Willemsen, G. , Trumbetta, S. L. , van Beijsterveldt, T. C. , & Boomsma, D. I.
(2005). The etiology of mathematical and reading (dis)ability covariation in a sample of Dutch
twins. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 8 , 585–593
Marle, K. , Chu, F. W. , Li, Y. , & Geary, D. C. (2014). Acuity of the approximate number system
and preschoolers’ quantitative development. Developmental Science, 17 , 492–505
Martin, W. G. , & Harel, G. (1989). Proof frames of preservice elementary teachers. Journal for
Research in Mathematics Education, 20 , 41–51
Martins-Mourão, A. , & Cowan, R. (1998). The emergence of additive composition of number.
Educational Psychology, 18 , 377–389
Mason, J. (1996). Expressing generality and roots of algebra. In N. Bednarz , C. Kieran , & L.
Lee (Eds.), Approaches to Algebra: Perspectives for Research and Teaching (pp. 65–86).
Dordrecht: Kluwer
Mason, J. , Burton, L. , & Stacey, K. (1982). Thinking Mathematically. London: Addison-Wesley
Matejko, A. A. , & Ansari, D. (2016). Trajectories of symbolic and nonsymbolic magnitude
processing in the first year of formal schooling. PloS ONE, 11, e0149863
Mattarella-Micke, A. , Mateo, J. , Kozak, M. N. , Foster, K. , & Beilock, S. L. (2011). Choke or
thrive? The relation between salivary cortisol and math performance depends on individual
differences in working memory and math-anxiety. Emotion, 11 , 1000–1005
Mayes, S. D. , & Calhoun, S. L. (2006). Frequency of reading, math, and writing disabilities in
children with clinical disorders. Learning and Individual Differences, 16 , 145–157
Mazzocco, M. M. M. (2015). The contributions of syndrome research to the study of mld. In R.
Cohen Kadosh & A. Dowker (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Numerical Cognition (pp.
678–695). Oxford: Oxford University Press
Mazzocco, M. M. M. , Feigenson, L. , & Halberda, J. (2011). Impaired acuity of the approximate
number system underlies mathematical learning disability (dyscalculia). Child Development, 82 ,
1224–1237
Mazzocco, M. M. M. , Murphy, M. M. , Brown, E. C. , Rinne, L. , & Herold, K. H. (2012).
Persistent consequences of atypical early number concepts. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 486
McCloskey, M. (1992). Cognitive mechanisms in numerical processing: evidence from acquired
dyscalculia. Cognition, 44 , 107–157
McCrink, K. , & Wynn, K. (2004). Large-number addition and subtraction by 9-month-old infants.
Psychological Science, 15 , 776–781
McLean, J. F. , & Hitch, G. J. (1999). Working memory impairments in children with specific
arithmetic learning difficulties. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 74 , 240–260
McNeil, N. M. (2008). Limitations to teaching children 2 + 2 = 4: typical arithmetic problems can
hinder learning of mathematical equivalence. Child Development, 79 , 1524–1537
McNeil, N. M. , & Alibali, M. W. (2005). Why won’t you change your mind? Knowledge of
operational patterns hinders learning and performance on equations. Child Development, 76 ,
883–899
McNeil, N. M. , Fyfe, E. R. , & Dunwiddie, A. E. (2015). Arithmetic practice can be modified to
promote understanding of mathematical equivalence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107 ,
423–436
Mejía-Ramos, J. P. , Fuller, E. , Weber, K. , Rhoads, K. , & Samkoff, A. (2012). An assessment
model for proof comprehension in undergraduate mathematics. Educational Studies in
Mathematics, 79 , 3–18
Mejía-Ramos, J. P. , & Inglis, M. (2009). Argumentative and proving activities in mathematics
education research. In F.-L. Lin , F. J. Hsieh , G. Hanna , & M. de Villiers (Eds.), Proceedings of
the ICMI Study Conference: Proof and Proving in Mathematics Education (Vol. 2 , pp. 88–93).
Taipei, Taiwan: Department of Mathematics, National Taiwan Normal University
Mejía-Ramos, J. P. , & Weber, K. (2014). Why and how mathematicians read proofs: further
evidence from a survey study. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 85 , 161–173
Melby-Lervåg, M. , & Hulme, C. (2013). Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic
review. Developmental Psychology, 49 , 270–291
Menninger, K. (1969). Number Words and Number Symbols, (P. Broneer, Trans.). Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press
Menon, V. (2010). Developmental cognitive neuroscience of arithmetic: implications for learning
and education. Zentralblatt für Didaktik der Mathematik, 42 , 515–525
Menon, V. (2016). Working memory in children’s math learning and its disruption in dyscalculia.
Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 10 , 125–132
Merkley, R. , & Ansari, D. (2016). Why numerical symbols count in the development of
mathematical skills: evidence from brain and behavior. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences,
10 , 14–20
Merkley, R. , Matejko, A. A. , & Ansari, D. (2017). Strong causal claims require strong evidence:
a commentary on Wang and colleagues. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 153 ,
163–167
Merkley, R. , Thompson, J. , & Scerif, G. (2015). Of huge mice and tiny elephants: exploring the
relationship between inhibitory processes and preschool math skills. Frontiers in Psychology, 6,
1903
Merritt, D. J. , & Brannon, E. M. (2013). Nothing to it: precursors to a zero concept in
preschoolers. Behavioural Processes, 93 , 91–97
Meyerhoff, H. S. , Moeller, K. , Debus, K. , & Nuerk, H.- C. (2012). Multi-digit number processing
beyond the two-digit number range: a combination of sequential and parallel processes. Acta
Psychologica, 140 , 81–90
Miller, K. F. , Perlmutter, M. , & Keating, D. (1984). Cognitive arithmetic: comparison of
operations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10 , 46–60
Miller, K. F. , & Paredes, D. R. (1990). Starting to add worse: effects of learning to multiply on
children’s addition. Cognition, 37 , 213–242
Minturn, A. L. , & Reese, T. W. (1951). The effect of differential reinforcement on the
discrimination of visual number. The Journal of Psychology, 31 , 201–231
Mitchell, D. (1962). An Introduction to Logic. London: Hutchinson
Miura, I. T. , Kim, C. C. , Chang, C.-M. , & Okamoto, Y. (1988). Effects of language char-
acteristics on children’s cognitive representation of number: cross-national comparisons. Child
Development, 59 , 1445–1450
Mix, K. S. , Prather, R. W. , Smith, L. B. , & Stockton, J. D. (2014). Young children’s
interpretation of multidigit number names: from emerging competence to mastery. Child
Development, 85 , 1306–1319
Moeller, K. , Pixner, S. , Kaufmann, L. , & Nuerk, H.- C. (2009). Children’s early mental number
line: logarithmic or decomposed linear? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103 ,
503–515
Moeller, K. , Pixner, S. , Zuber, J. , Kaufmann, L. , & Nuerk, H.- C. (2011). Early place-value
understanding as a precursor for later arithmetic performance: a longitudinal study on numerical
development. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32 , 1837–1851
Moeller, K. , Shaki, S. , Göbel, S. M. , & Nuerk, H.- C. (2015). Language influences number
processing: a quadrilingual study. Cognition, 136 , 150–155
Moll, K. , Göbel, S. M. , & Snowling, M. J. (2015). Basic number processing in children with
specific learning disorders: comorbidity of reading and mathematics disorders. Child
Neuropsychology, 21 , 399–417
Moll, K. , Kunze, S. , Neuhoff, N. , Bruder, J. , & Schulte-Körne, G. (2014). Specific learning
disorder: prevalence and gender differences. PLoS ONE, 9, e103537
Monette, S. , Bigras, M. , & Guay, M.- C. (2011). The role of the executive functions in school
achievement at the end of Grade 1. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109 , 158–173
Moore, R. (1994). Making the transition to formal proof. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 27
, 249–266
Morsanyi, K. , & Szűcs, D. (2014). The link between mathematics and logical reasoning:
implications for research and education. In S. Chinn (Ed.), The Routledge Handbook of
Dyscalculia and Mathematical Learning Difficulties (pp. 101–114). Oxford: Routledge
Moser Opitz, E. (2007). Rechenschwäche, Dyskalkulie: theoretische Klärungen und empirische
Studien an betroffenen Schülerinnen und Schülern (Vol. 31 ). Zurich: Haupt Verlag AG
Moss, J. (2005). Pipes, tubes and beakers: new approaches to teaching the rational-number
system. In M. S. Donovan & J. D. Bransford (Eds.), How Students Learn: Mathematics in the
Classroom (pp. 121–162). Washington, DC: National Academic Press
Moura, R. , Lopes-Silva, J. B. , Vieira, L. R. , Paiva, G. M. , de Almeida Prado, A. C. , Wood, G.
, & Haase, V. G. (2015). From ‘five’ to 5 for 5 minutes: Arabic number transcoding as a short,
specific, and sensitive screening tool for mathematics learning difficulties. Archives of Clinical
Neuropsychology, 30 , 88–98
Moura, R. , Wood, G. , Pinheiro-Chagas, P. , Lonnemann, J. , Krinzinger, H. , Willmes, K. , &
Haase, V. G. (2013). Transcoding abilities in typical and atypical mathematics achievers: the
role of working memory and procedural and lexical competencies. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 116 , 707–727
Moyer, R. S. , & Landauer, T. K. (1967). Time required for judgements of numerical inequality.
Nature, 215 , 1519–1520
Muldoon, K. , Simms, V. , Towse, J. , Burns, V. , & Yue, G. (2011). Cross-cultural comparisons
of 5-year-olds’ estimating and mathematical ability. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42 ,
669–681
Mulligan, J. (1992). Children’s solutions to multiplication and division word problems: a
longitudinal study. Mathematics Education Research Journal, 4 , 24–41
Mullis, I. V. S. , Marting, M. O. , & Foy, P. (2008). TIMSS 2007 International Mathematics
Report: Findings from IEA’s Trends in International Mathematics and Science Study at the
Fourth and Eighth Grades. Chestnut Hill, MA: International Association for the Evaluation of
Educational Achievement (IEA)
Munafò, M. R. , Nosek, B. A. , Bishop, D. V. M. , Button, K. S. , Chambers, C. D. , du Sert, N. P.
, Simonsohn, U. , Wagenmakers, E.-J. , Ware, J. J. , & Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2017). A manifesto
for reproducible science. Nature Human Behaviour, 1, 0021
Mundy, E. , & Gilmore, C. (2009). Children’s mapping between symbolic and nonsymbolic
representations of number. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103 , 490–502
Mussolin, C. , De Volder, A. , Grandin, C. , Schlögel, X. , Nassogne, M.-C. , & Noël, M.- P.
(2010). Neural correlates of symbolic number comparison in developmental dyscalculia. Journal
of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22 , 860–874
Mussolin, C. , Mejias, S. , & Noël, M.- P. (2010). Symbolic and nonsymbolic number comparison
in children with and without dyscalculia. Cognition, 115 , 10–25
Mussolin, C. , Nys, J. , Content, A. , & Leybaert, J. (2014). Symbolic number abilities predict
later approximate number system acuity in preschool children. PloS ONE, 9, e91839
Nairne, J. S. , & Healy, A. F. (1983). Counting backwards produces systematic errors. Journal
of Experimental Psychology: General, 112 , 37–40
National Mathematics Advisory Panel . (2008). Foundations for Success: The Final Report of
the National Mathematics Advisory Panel. US Department of Education
National Science Foundation . (2015). Women, Minorities and Persons with Disabilities in
Science and Engineering. Arlington, VA: National Center for Science and Engineering Statistics
Negen, J. , & Sarnecka, B. W. (2012). Number-concept acquisition and general vocabulary
development. Child Development, 83(6), 2019–2027
Ni, Y. , & Zhou, Y.- D. (2005). Teaching and learning fraction and rational numbers: the origins
and implications of whole number bias. Educational Psychologist, 40 , 27–52
Nieder, A. , & Miller, E. K. (2004). A parieto-frontal network for visual numerical information in
the monkey. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,
101(19), 7457–7462
Nisbett, R. E. (2009). Can reasoning be taught? In E. Callan , T. Grotzer , J. Kagan , R. E.
Nisbett , D. N. Perkins , & L. S. Shulman (Eds.), Education and a Civic Society: Teaching
Evidence-based Decision Making. Cambridge, MA: American Academy of Arts & Sciences
Noël, M.-P. , & Rousselle, L. (2011). Developmental changes in the profiles of dyscalculia: an
explanation based on a double exact-and-approximate number representation model. Frontiers
in Human Neuroscience, 5, 165
Norris, J. E. , McGeown, W. J. , Guerrini, C. , & Castronovo, J. (2015). Aging and the number
sense: preserved basic non-symbolic numerical processing and enhanced basic symbolic
processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(999)
Nosek, B. A. , Alter, G. , Banks, G. C. , Borsboom, D. , Bowman, S. D. , Breckler, S. J. , . . .
Yarkoni, T. (2015). Promoting an open research culture. Science, 348(6242), 1422–1425.
Notebaert, K. , Nelis, S. , & Reynvoet, B. (2011). The magnitude representation of small and
large symbolic numbers in the left and right hemisphere: an event-related fMRI study. Journal of
Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(3), 622–630
Novick, M. R. (1966). The axioms and principal results of classical test theory. Journal of
Mathematical Psychology, 3(1), 1–18
Nuerk, H.-C. , Moeller, K. , Klein, E. , Willmes, K. , & Fischer, M. H. (2011). Extending the
mental number line. Zeitschrift Für Psychologie, 219 , 3–22
Nuerk, H.-C. , Moeller, K. , & Willmes, K. (2015). Multi-digit number processing: overview,
conceptual clarifications and language influences. In R. Cohen Kadosh & A. Dowker (Eds.), The
Oxford Handbook of Numerical Cognition (pp. 106–139). Oxford: Oxford University Press
Nuerk, H.-C. , Weger, U. , & Willmes, K. (2001). Decade breaks in the mental number line?
Putting the tens and units back in different bins. Cognition, 82 , B25–B33
Nuerk, H.-C. , & Willmes, K. (2005). On the magnitude representations of two-digit numbers.
Psychology Science, 47 , 52–72
Nunes, T. , & Bryant, P. (1996). Children Doing Mathematics. Oxford: Blackwell
Nys, J. , Ventura, P. , Fernandes, T. , Querido, L. , Leybaert, J. , & Content, A. (2013). Does
math education modify the approximate number system? A comparison of schooled and
unschooled adults. Trends in Neuroscience and Education, 2 , 13–22
Oaksford, M. , & Chater, N. (1994). A rational analysis of the selection task as optimal data
selection. Psychological Review, 101 , 608–631
Oaksford, M. , & Chater, N. (1995). Information gain explains relevance which explains the
selection task. Cognition, 57 , 97–108
Oaksford, M. , Chater, N. , & Larkin, J. (2000). Probabilities and polarity biases in conditional
inference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 26 , 883–899
Obersteiner, A. , Van Dooren, W. , Van Hoof, J. , & Verschaffel, L. (2013). The natural num-ber
bias and magnitude representation in fraction comparison by expert mathematicians. Learning
and Instruction, 28 , 64–72
OCR . (2003). Examiner’s report for MEI Structured Mathematics Advanced Level, Decision and
Discrete Mathematics 2. Online article [accessed 03/08/2005]:
www.meidistance.co.uk/npapers/d203ju.pdf
OECD . (2004). Learning for Tomorrow’s World: First Results from Pisa 2003. Paris, France:
Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development
OECD . (2013). OECD Skills Outlook 2013: First Results from the Survey of Adult Skills. Paris,
France: Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development
Olivers, C. N. , & Watson, D. G. (2008). Subitizing requires attention. Visual Cognition, 16 ,
439–462
Open Science Collaboration . (2015). Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science.
Science, 349, aac4716
Owens, M. , Stevenson, J. , Hadwin, J. A. , & Norgate, R. (2014). When does anxiety help or
hinder cognitive test performance? The role of working memory capacity. British Journal of
Psychology, 105 , 92–101
Park, D. , Ramirez, G. , & Beilock, S. L. (2014). The role of expressive writing in math anxiety.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 20 , 103–111
Park, J. , & Brannon, E. M. (2013). Training the Approximate Number System improves math
proficiency. Psychological Science, 24 , 2013–2019
Park, J. , & Brannon, E. M. (2014). Improving arithmetic performance with number sense
training: an investigation of underlying mechanism. Cognition, 133 , 188–200
Park, J.-H. , & Nunes, T. (2001). The development of the concept of multiplication. Cognitive
Development, 16 , 763–773
Parkman, J. M. (1971). Temporal aspects of digit and letter inequality judgments. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 91 , 191–205
Parnes, M. , Berger, A. , & Tzelgov, J. (2012). Brain representations of negative numbers.
Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie
expérimentale, 66 , 251–258
Parsons, S. , & Bynner, J. (2005). Does Numeracy Matter More? London: National Research
and Development Centre for Adult Literacy and Numeracy
Pashler, H. , & Wagenmakers, E.- J. (2012). Editors’ introduction to the special section on
replicability in psychological science: a crisis of confidence? Perspectives on Psychological
Science, 7 , 528–530
Passolunghi, M. C. , Caviola, S. , De Agostini, R. , Perin, C. , & Mammarella, I. C. (2016).
Mathematics anxiety, working memory, and mathematics performance in secondary-school
children. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 42
Passolunghi, M. C. , & Siegel, L. S. (2001). Short-term memory, working memory, and inhibitory
control in children with difficulties in arithmetic problem solving. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 80 , 44–57
Passolunghi, M. C. , & Siegel, L. S. (2004). Working memory and access to numerical
information in children with disability in mathematics. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
88 , 348–367
Peng, P. , Namkung, J. , Barnes, M. , & Sun, C. (2016). A meta-analysis of mathematics and
working memory: moderating effects of working memory domain, type of mathematics skill, and
sample characteristics. Journal of Educational Psychology, 108 , 455–473
Penner-Wilger, M. , Fast, L. , LeFevre, J.-A. , Smith-Chant, B. L. , Skwarchuk, S.-L. , Kamawar,
D. , & Bisanz, J. (2009). Subitizing, finger gnosis, and the representation of number. In
Proceedings of the 31st Annual Cognitive Science Society (pp. 520–525). Austin, TX: Cognitive
Science Society
Peterson, D. (2016). The baby factory: difficult research objects, disciplinary standards, and the
production of statistical significance. Socius, 2, 2378023115625071
Phillipson, S. , & Phillipson, S. N. (2007). Academic expectations, belief of ability, and
involvement by parents as predictors of child achievement: a cross-cultural comparison.
Educational Psychology, 27 , 329–348
PIAAC . (2012). OECD Skills Surveys. Data downloaded through http://piaacdataexplorer.
oecd.org/ide/idepiaac/
Piaget, J. (1952). The Child’s Conception of Number ( C. Gattegno & F. M. Hodgson , Trans.).
London: Routledge
Piantadosi, S. T. , Jara-Ettinger, J. , & Gibson, E. (2014). Children’s learning of number words in
an indigenous farming-foraging group. Developmental Science, 17 , 553–563
Piantadosi, S. T. , Tenenbaum, J. B. , & Goodman, N. D. (2012). Bootstrapping in a language of
thought: a formal model of numerical concept learning. Cognition, 123 , 199–217
Piazza, M. (2010). Neurocognitive start-up tools for symbolic number representations. Trends in
Cognitive Sciences, 14 , 542–551
Piazza, M. , Facoetti, A. , Trussardi, A. N. , Berteletti, I. , Conte, S. , Lucangeli, D. , Dehaene, S.
, & Zorzi, M. (2010). Developmental trajectory of number acuity reveals a severe impairment in
developmental dyscalculia. Cognition, 116 , 33–41
Piazza, M. , Fumarola, A. , Chinello, A. , & Melcher, D. (2011). Subitizing reflects visuo-spatial
object individuation capacity. Cognition, 121 , 147–153
Piazza, M. , Giacomini, E. , Le Bihan, D. , & Dehaene, S. (2003). Single-trial classification of
parallel pre-attentive and serial attentive processes using functional magnetic resonance
imaging. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 270 , 1237–1245
Piazza, M. , Izard, V. , Pinel, P. , Le Bihan, D. , & Dehaene, S. (2004). Tuning curves for
approximate numerosity in the human intraparietal sulcus. Neuron, 44 , 547–555
Piazza, M. , Mechelli, A. , Butterworth, B. , & Price, C. J. (2002). Are subitizing and counting
implemented as separate or functionally overlapping processes? Neuroimage, 15 , 435–446
Piazza, M. , Pica, P. , Izard, V. , Spelke, E. S. , & Dehaene, S. (2013). Education enhances the
acuity of the nonverbal approximate number system. Psychological Science, 24 , 1037–1043
Piazza, M. , Pinel, P. , Le Bihan, D. , & Dehaene, S. (2007). A magnitude code common to
numerosities and number symbols in human intraparietal cortex. Neuron, 53 , 293–305
Pica, P. , Lemer, C. , Izard, V. , & Dehaene, S. (2004). Exact and approximate arithmetic in an
Amazonian indigene group. Science, 306 , 499–503
Pinel, P. , Dehaene, S. , Riviere, D. , & LeBihan, D. (2001). Modulation of parietal activation by
semantic distance in a number comparison task. Neuroimage, 14 , 1013–1026
Pinhas, M. , & Tzelgov, J. (2012). Expanding on the mental number line: zero is perceived as
the ‘smallest’. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38 ,
1187–1205
Pinto, M. , & Tall, D. O. (2002). Building formal mathematics on visual imagery: a case study
and a theory. For the Learning of Mathematics, 22 , 2–10
Plaisier, M. A. , Tiest, W. M. B. , & Kappers, A. M. L. (2009). One, two, three, many: subitizing in
active touch. Acta Psychologica, 131 , 163–170
Plaisier, M. A. , Tiest, W. M. B. , & Kappers, A. M. L. (2010). Grabbing subitizing with both
hands: bimanual number processing. Experimental Brain Research, 202 , 507–512
Plato . (375BC/2003). The Republic ( D. Lee , Ed.). London: Penguin
Pletzer, B. , Kronbichler, M. , Nuerk, H.-C. , & Kerschbaum, H. H. (2015). Mathematics anxiety
reduces default mode network deactivation in response to numerical tasks. Frontiers in Human
Neuroscience, 9, 202
Plomin, R. , & Kovas, Y. (2005). Generalist genes and learning disabilities. Psychological
Bulletin, 131 , 592–617
Pollard, P. , & Evans, J. St. B. T. (1980). The influence of logic on conditional reasoning
performance. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32 , 605–624
Pollmann, T. (2003). Some principles involved in the acquisition of number words. Language
Acquisition: A Journal of Developmental Linguistics, 11 , 1–31
Power, R. J. D. , & Dal Martello, M. F. (1990). The dictation of Italian numerals. Language and
Cognitive Processes, 5 , 237–254
Prather, R. W. , & Alibali, M. W. (2009). The development of arithmetic principle knowledge:
how do we know what learners know? Developmental Review, 29 , 221–248
Price, G. R. , Holloway, I. D. , Räsänen, P. , Vesterinen, M. , & Ansari, D. (2007). Impaired
parietal magnitude processing in developmental dyscalculia. Current Biology, 17 ,
R1042–R1043
Price, G. R. , Palmer, D. , Battista, C. , & Ansari, D. (2012). Nonsymbolic numerical magnitude
comparison: reliability and validity of different task variants and outcome measures, and their
relationship to arithmetic achievement in adults. Acta Psychologica, 140 , 50–57
Purpura, D. J. , Baroody, A. J. , & Lonigan, C. J. (2013). The transition from informal to formal
mathematical knowledge: mediation by numeral knowledge. Journal of Educational Psychology,
105 , 453–464
Purpura, D. J. , & Ganley, C. M. (2014). Working memory and language: skill-specific or
domain-general relations to mathematics? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 122 ,
104–121
Purpura, D. J. , Hume, L. E. , Sims, D. M. , & Lonigan, C. J. (2011). Early literacy and early
numeracy: the value of including early literacy skills in the prediction of numeracy development.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110 , 647–658
Purpura, D. J. , & Reid, E. E. (2016). Mathematics and language: individual and group
differences in mathematical language skills in young children. Early Childhood Research
Quarterly, 36 , 259–268
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1989). The role of location indexes in spatial perception: a sketch of the FINST
spatial-index model. Cognition, 32 , 65–97
Raghubar, K. , Cirino, P. T. , Barnes, M. , Ewing-Cobbs, L. , Fletcher, J. M. , & Fuchs, L. S.
(2009). Errors in multi-digit arithmetic and behavioral inattention in children with math difficulties.
Journal of Learning Disabilities, 42 , 356–371
Railo, H. , Koivisto, M. , Revonsuo, A. , & Hannula, M. M. (2008). The role of attention in
subitizing. Cognition, 107 , 82–104
Ramani, G. B. , & Siegler, R. S. (2008). Promoting broad and stable improvements in low-
income children’s numerical knowledge through playing number board games. Child
Development, 79 , 375–394
Ramirez, G. , Gunderson, E. A. , Levine, S. C. , & Beilock, S. L. (2013). Math anxiety, working
memory, and math achievement in early elementary school. Journal of Cognition and
Development, 14 , 187–202
Ramirez-Cardenas, A. , Moskaleva, M. , & Nieder, A. (2016). Neuronal representation of
numerosity zero in the primate parieto-frontal number network. Current Biology, 26 , 1285–1294
Rasmussen, C. , & Bisanz, J. (2005). Representation and working memory in early arithmetic.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 91 , 137–157
Rasmussen, C. , Ho, E. , & Bisanz, J. (2003). Use of the mathematical principle of inversion in
young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 85 , 89–102
Rav, Y. (1999). Why do we prove theorems? Philosophia Mathematica, 7 , 5–41
Recio, A. , & Godino, J. (2001). Institutional and personal meanings of mathematical proof.
Educational Studies in Mathematics, 48 , 83–99
Resnick, L. B. (1983). A developmental theory of number understanding. In H. P. Ginsburg
(Ed.), The Development of Mathematical Thinking (pp. 109–151). New York: Academic Press
Resnik, L. B. , Nesher, P. , Leonard, F. , Magone, M. , Omanson, S. , & Peled, I. (1989).
Conceptual bases of arithmetic errors: the case of decimal fractions. Journal for Research in
Mathematics Education, 20 , 8–27
Resnik, M. D. , & Kushner, D. (1987). Explanation, independence and realism in mathematics.
British Journal of the Philosophy of Science, 38 , 141–158
Revkin, S. K. , Piazza, M. , Izard, V. , Cohen, L. , & Dehaene, S. (2008). Does subitizing reflect
numerical estimation? Psychological Science, 19 , 607–614
Reyna, V. F. , Nelson, W. L. , Han, P. K. , & Dieckmann, N. F. (2009). How numeracy influences
risk comprehension and medical decision making. Psychological Bulletin, 135 , 943–973
Reynvoet, B. , De Smedt, B. , & Van den Bussche, E. (2009). Children’s representation of
symbolic magnitude: the development of the priming distance effect. Journal of Experimental
Child Psychology, 103 , 480–489
Reynvoet, B. , & Sasanguie, D. (2016). The symbol grounding problem revisited: a thorough
evaluation of the ans mapping account and the proposal of an alternative account based on
symbol–symbol associations. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1581
Richardson, F. C. , & Suinn, R. M. (1972). The Mathematics Anxiety Rating Scale: psychometric
data. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 19 , 551–554
Rickard, T. C. , & Bourne Jr, L. E. (1996). Some tests of an identical elements model of basic
arithmetic skills. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22 ,
1281–1295
Riggs, K. J. , Ferrand, L. , Lancelin, D. , Fryziel, L. , Dumur, G. , & Simpson, A. (2006).
Subitizing in tactile perception. Psychological Science, 17 , 271–272
Riley, M. S. , Greeno, J. G. , & Heller, J. I. (1984). Development of children’s problem-solving
ability in arithmetic. In H. P. Ginsburg (Ed.), The Development of Mathematical Thinking. (pp.
153–196). New York: Academic Press
Rittle-Johnson, B. , & Alibali, M. W. (1999). Conceptual and procedural knowledge of
mathematics: does one lead to the other? Journal of Educational Psychology, 91 , 175–189
Rittle-Johnson, B. , Matthews, P. G. , Taylor, R. S. , & McEldoon, K. L. (2011). Assessing
knowledge of mathematical equivalence: a construct-modeling approach. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 103 , 85–104
Rittle-Johnson, B. , & Siegler, R. S. (1998). The relation between conceptual and procedural
knowledge in learning mathematics: a review. In C. Donlan (Ed.), The Development of
Mathematical Skills (pp. 75–110). Hove: Psychology Press
Rittle-Johnson, B. , Siegler, R. S. , & Alibali, M. W. (2001). Developing conceptual
understanding and procedural skill in mathematics: an iterative process. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 93 , 346–362
Rivera, S. M. , Reiss, A. , Eckert, M. A. , & Menon, V. (2005). Developmental changes in mental
arithmetic: evidence for increased functional specialization in the left inferior parietal cortex.
Cerebral Cortex, 15 , 1779–1790
Robinson, K. M. (2001). The validity of verbal reports in children’s subtraction. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 93 , 211–222
Robinson, K. M. , & Dubé, A. K. (2009a). A microgenetic study of the multiplication and division
inversion concept. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de
psychologie expérimentale, 63 , 193–200
Robinson, K. M. , & Dubé, A. K. (2009b). The multiplication and division inversion concept:
relationships between procedural, conceptual, and factual knowledge. Cognitive Development,
24 , 310–321
Robinson, K. M. , & Dubé, A. K. (2013a). Children’s additive concepts: promoting understanding
and the role of inhibition. Learning and Individual Differences, 23 , 101–107
Robinson, K. M. , & Dubé, A. K. (2013b). Children’s additive concepts: promoting understanding
and the role of inhibition. Learning and Individual Differences, 23 , 101–107
Robinson, K. M. , Dubé, A. K. , & Beatch, J.- A. (2017). Children’s understanding of additive
concepts. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 156 , 16–28
Robinson, K. M. , & Ninowski, J. E. (2003). Adults’ understanding of inversion concepts: How
does performance on addition and subtraction inversion problems compare to performance on
multiplication and division inversion problems? Canadian Journal of Experimental
Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 57 , 321–330
Robinson, K. M. , Ninowski, J. E. , & Gray, M. L. (2006). Children’s understanding of the
arithmetic concepts of inversion and associativity. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 94
, 349–362
Roitman, J. D. , Brannon, E. M. , & Platt, M. L. (2007). Monotonic coding of numerosity in
macaque lateral intraparietal area. PLoS Biol, 5, e208
Rosenberg-Lee, M. , Ashkenazi, S. , Chen, T. , Young, C. B. , Geary, D. C. , & Menon, V.
(2015). Brain hyper-connectivity and operation-specific deficits during arithmetic problem
solving in children with developmental dyscalculia. Developmental Science, 18 , 351–372
Ross, J. (2003). Visual discrimination of number without counting. Perception, 32 , 867–870
Ross, J. , & Burr, D. C. (2010). Vision senses number directly. Journal of Vision, 10, 10
Rotzer, S. , Kucian, K. , Martin, E. , von Aster, M. G. , Klaver, P. , & Loenneker, T. (2008).
Optimized voxel-based morphometry in children with developmental dyscalculia. Neuroimage,
39 , 417–422
Rousselle, L. , & Noël, M.- P. (2007). Basic numerical skills in children with mathematics
learning disabilities: a comparison of symbolic vs non-symbolic number magnitude processing.
Cognition, 102 , 361–395
Rubinsten, O. , Bialik, N. , & Solar, Y. (2012). Exploring the relationship between math anxiety
and gender through implicit measurement. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 279
Rubinsten, O. , & Henik, A. (2005). Automatic activation of internal magnitudes: a study of
developmental dyscalculia. Neuropsychology, 19 , 641–648
Rubinsten, O. , & Henik, A. (2006). Double dissociation of functions in developmental dyslexia
and dyscalculia. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98 , 854–867
Rubinsten, O. , & Tannock, R. (2010). Mathematics anxiety in children with developmental
dyscalculia. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 6, 46
Rugani, R. , Vallortigara, G. , Priftis, K. , & Regolin, L. (2015). Number-space mapping in the
newborn chick resembles humans’ mental number line. Science, 347 , 534–536
Rykhlevskaia, E. , Uddin, L. Q. , Kondos, L. , & Menon, V. (2009). Neuroanatomical correlates
of developmental dyscalculia: combined evidence from morphometry and tractography.
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 3, 51
Sagi, D. , & Julesz, B. (1984). Detection versus discrimination of visual orientation. Perception,
13 , 619–628
Sarnecka, B. W. (2015). Learning to represent exact numbers. Synthese, 1–18. Published
online
Sarnecka, B. W. , & Gelman, S. A. (2004). Six does not just mean a lot: preschoolers see
number words as specific. Cognition, 92 , 329–352
Sarnecka, B. W. , Kamenskaya, V. G. , Yamana, Y. , Ogura, T. , & Yudovina, Y. B. (2007).
From grammatical number to exact numbers: early meanings of ‘one’, ‘two’, and ‘three’ in
English, Russian, and Japanese. Cognitive Psychology, 55 , 136–168
Sarnecka, B. W. , & Lee, M. D. (2009). Levels of number knowledge during early childhood.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103 , 325–337
Sathian, K. , Simon, T. J. , Peterson, S. , Patel, G. A. , Hoffman, J. M. , & Grafton, S. T. (1999).
Neural evidence linking visual object enumeration and attention. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 11 , 36–51
Schleifer, P. , & Landerl, K. (2011). Subitizing and counting in typical and atypical development.
Developmental Science, 14 , 280–291
Schlimm, D. (2012). A decimal place-value system without zero. (Unpublished manuscript.)
Schlimm, D. , & Neth, H. (2008). Modeling ancient and modern arithmetic practices: addition
and multiplication with arabic and roman numerals. In Proceedings of the 30th Annual
Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 2097–2102). Washington, DC
Schneider, M. , Beeres, K. , Coban, L. , Merz, S. , Schmidt, S. , Stricker, J. , & De Smedt, B.
(2017). Associations of non-symbolic and symbolic numerical magnitude processing with
mathematical competence: a meta-analysis. Developmental science, 20, e12372
Schoenfeld, A. H. (1985). Mathematical problem solving. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Schoenfeld, A. H. (1992). Learning to think mathematically: problem solving, metacognition and
sense making in mathematics. In D. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of Research on Mathematics
Teaching and Learning: A Project of the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (pp.
334–370). New York: Macmillan
Schoenfeld, A. H. (2004). The math wars. Educational Policy, 18 , 253–286
Schroyens, W. , Schaeken, W. , & d’Ydewalle, G. (2001). The processing of negations in
conditional reasoning: a meta-analytical case study in mental model and/or mental logic theory.
Thinking and Reasoning, 7 , 121–172
Schubring, G. (2005). Conflicts between Generalization, Rigor, and Intuition: Number Concepts
Underlying the Development of Analysis in 17th–19th Century France and Germany. New York,
NY: Springer
Schumacher, R. F. , & Fuchs, L. S. (2012). Does understanding relational terminology mediate
effects of intervention on compare word problems? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
111 , 607–628
Scottish Survey of Achievement . (2009). 2008 Scottish Survey of Achievement: Mathematics
and Core Skills. Retrieved from www.gov.scot/Publications/2009/04/02133043/7
Segal, J. (1999). Learning about mathematical proof: conviction and validity. Journal of
Mathematical Behavior, 18 , 191–210
Selden, A. , & Selden, J. (2003). Validations of proofs considered as texts: can undergraduates
tell whether an argument proves a theorem? Journal for Research in Mathematics Education,
34, 4–36
Selden, J. , & Selden, A. (1995). Unpacking the logic of mathematical statements. Educational
Studies in Mathematics, 29 , 123–151
Senk, S. L. (1989). Van Hiele levels and achievement in writing geometry proofs. Journal for
Research in Mathematics Education, 20 , 309–321
Seo, K.-H. , & Ginsburg, H. P. (2003). ‘You’ve got to carefully read the math sentence. . . ‘:
classroom context and children’s interpretations of the equals sign. In A. J. Baroody & A.
Dowker (Eds.), The Development of Arithmetic Concepts and Skills: Constructing Adaptive
Expertise (pp. 161–187). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Seron, X. , & Fayol, M. (1994). Number transcoding in children: a functional analysis. British
Journal of Developmental Psychology, 12 , 281–300
Shaki, S. , & Fischer, M. H. (2008). Reading space into numbers: a cross-linguistic comparison
of the SNARC effect. Cognition, 108 , 590–599
Shaki, S. , Fischer, M. H. , & Petrusic, W. M. (2009). Reading habits for both words and
numbers contribute to the SNARC effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16 , 328–331
Shalev, R. S. , Auerbach, J. , Manor, O. , & Gross-Tsur, V. (2000). Developmental dyscalculia:
prevalence and prognosis. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 9 , S58–S64
Shalev, R. S. , Manor, O. , Auerbach, J. , & Gross-Tsur, V. (1998). Persistence of
developmental dyscalculia: what counts? Results from a 3-year prospective follow-up study.
The Journal of Pediatrics, 133 , 358–362
Shalev, R. S. , Manor, O. , & Gross-Tsur, V. (2005). Developmental dyscalculia: a prospective
six-year follow-up. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 47 , 121–125
Share, D. L. , Moffitt, T. E. , & Silva, P. A. (1988). Factors associated with arithmetic-and-
reading disability and specific arithmetic disability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 21 , 313–320
Shum, J. , Hermes, D. , Foster, B. L. , Dastjerdi, M. , Rangarajan, V. , Winawer, J. , Miller, K. J. ,
& Parvizi, J. (2013). A brain area for visual numerals. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33 ,
6709–6715
Siegler, R. S. (1987). The perils of averaging data over strategies: an example from children’s
addition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 116 , 250–264
Siegler, R. S. (1988). Strategy choice procedures and the development of multiplication skill.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117 , 258–275
Siegler, R. S. (1999). Strategic development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences</it>, 3, 430–435.
Siegler, R. S., & Booth, J. L. (2004). Development of numerical estimation in young children.
Child Development, 75 , 428–444
Siegler, R. S. , & Crowley, K. (1991). The microgenetic method: a direct means for studying
cognitive development. American Psychologist, 46 , 606–620
Siegler, R. S. , Duncan, G. J. , Davis-Kean, P. E. , Duckworth, K. , Claessens, A. , Engel, M. ,
Susperreguy, M. I. , & Chen, M. (2012). Early predictors of high school mathematics
achievement. Psychological Science, 23 , 691–697
Siegler, R. S. , & Jenkins, E. A. (1989). How Children Discover New Strategies. New York:
Psychology Press
Siegler, R. S. , & Lemaire, P. (1997). Older and younger adults’ strategy choices in
multiplication: testing predictions of ASCM using the choice/no-choice method. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: General, 126 , 71–92
Siegler, R. S. , & Lortie-Forgues, H. (2015). Conceptual knowledge of fraction arithmetic.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 107 , 909–918
Siegler, R. S. , & Mu, Y. (2008). Chinese children excel on novel mathematics problems even
before elementary school. Psychological Science, 19 , 759–763
Siegler, R. S. , & Pyke, A. A. (2013). Developmental and individual differences in understanding
of fractions. Developmental Psychology, 49 , 1994–2004
Siegler, R. S. , & Ramani, G. B. (2008). Playing linear numerical board games promotes low-
income children’s numerical development. Developmental Science, 11 , 655–661
Siegler, R. S. , & Ramani, G. B. (2009). Playing linear number board games—but not circular
ones—improves low-income preschoolers’ numerical understanding. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 101 , 545–560
Siegler, R. S. , & Stern, E. (1998). Conscious and unconscious strategy discoveries: a
microgenetic analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127 , 377–397
Sikora, D. M. , Haley, P. , Edwards, J. , & Butler, R. W. (2002). Tower of London test
performance in children with poor arithmetic skills. Developmental Neuropsychology, 21 ,
243–254
Simmons, J. P. (n.d.). Mturk vs. the lab: Either way we need big samples. Retrieved
11/07/2017, from http://datacolada.org/18
Simmons, J. P. , Nelson, L. D. , & Simonsohn, U. (2011). False-positive psychology:
undisclosed flexibility in data collection and analysis allows presenting anything as significant.
Psychological Science, 22 , 1359–1366
Simmons, J. P. , Nelson, L. D. , & Simonsohn, U. (2012). A 21-word solution. SSRN:
http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2160588
Simms, V. , Clayton, S. , Cragg, L. , Gilmore, C. , & Johnson, S. (2016). Explaining the
relationship between number line estimation and mathematical achievement: the role of
visuomotor integration and visuospatial skills. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 145 ,
22–33
Simon, T. J. (1997). Reconceptualizing the origins of number knowledge: A ‘non-numerical’
account. Cognitive Development, 12 , 349–372
Simon, T. J. , Hespos, S. J. , & Rochat, P. (1995). Do infants understand simple arithmetic? A
replication of Wynn (1992). Cognitive Development, 10 , 253–269
Simonsohn, U. , Nelson, L. D. , & Simmons, J. P. (2014a). P-curve: a key to the file-drawer.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143 , 534–547
Simonsohn, U. , Nelson, L. D. , & Simmons, J. P. (2014b). P-curve and effect size: correcting
for publication bias using only significant results. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9 ,
666–681
Simpson, A. (1995). Focusing on student attitudes to proof. Teaching and Learning
Undergraduate Mathematics Newsletter, 3 , 1–4
Singh, S. (1997). Fermat’s Last Theorem. London: Fourth Estate
Skemp, R. R. (1976). Relational understanding and instrumental understanding. Mathemat-ics
Teaching, 77 , 20–26
Skwarchuk, S.-L. , Sowinski, C. , & LeFevre, J.- A. (2014). Formal and informal home learning
activities in relation to children’s early numeracy and literacy skills: the development of a home
numeracy model. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 121 , 63–84
Slaughter, V. , Kamppi, D. , & Paynter, J. (2006). Toddler subtraction with large sets: further
evidence for an analog-magnitude representation of number. Developmental Science, 9 , 33–39
Slavin, R. E. , & Lake, C. (2008). Effective programs in elementary mathematics: a best-
evidence synthesis. Review of Educational Research, 78 , 427–515
Smets, K. , Gebuis, T. , Defever, E. , & Reynvoet, B. (2014). Concurrent validity of approximate
number sense tasks in adults and children. Acta Psychologica, 150 , 120–128
Smets, K. , Sasanguie, D. , Szűcs, D. , & Reynvoet, B. (2015). The effect of different methods to
construct non-symbolic stimuli in numerosity estimation and comparison. Journal of Cognitive
Psychology, 27 , 310–325
Smith, A. (2004). Making Mathematics Count: The Report of Professor Adrian Smith’s Inquiry
into Post-14 Mathematics Education. London: The Stationery Office
Snowling, M. J. (2012). Editorial: seeking a new characterisation of learning disorders. Journal
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53 , 1–2
Sophian, C. , & McCorgray, P. (1994). Part-whole knowledge and early arithmetic problem
solving. Cognition and Instruction, 12 , 3–33
Soto-Calvo, E. , Simmons, F. R. , Willis, C. , & Adams, A.- M. (2015). Identifying the cognitive
predictors of early counting and calculation skills: evidence from a longitudinal study. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 140 , 16–37
Sowell, E. J. (1989). Effects of manipulative materials in mathematics instruction. Journal for
Research in Mathematics Education, 20 , 498–505
Spelke, E. S. , & Kinzler, K. D. (2007). Core knowledge. Developmental Science, 10 , 89–96.
Sperber, D. , Cara, F. , & Girotto, V. (1995). Relevance theory explains the selection task.
Cognition, 57 , 31–95
Sperber, D. , & Girotto, V. (2002). Use or misuse of the selection task? Rejoinder to Fiddink,
Cosmides and Tooby. Cognition, 85 , 277–290
Spinath, F. M. , Spinath, B. , & Plomin, R. (2008). The nature and nurture of intelligence and
motivation in the origins of sex differences in elementary school achievement. European Journal
of Personality, 22 , 211–229
Squire, S. , Davies, C. , & Bryant, P. (2004). Does the cue help? Children’s understanding of
multiplicative concepts in different problem contexts. British Journal of Educational Psychology,
74 , 515–532
Stafylidou, S. , & Vosniadou, S. (2004). The development of students’ understanding of the
numerical value of fractions. Learning and Instruction, 14 , 503–518
Stanic, G. M. A. (1986). The growing crisis in mathematics education in the early twentieth
century. Journal for Research in Mathematlcs Education, 17 , 190–205
Stankov, L. , Lee, J. , Luo, W. , & Hogan, D. J. (2012). Confidence: a better predictor of
academic achievement than self-efficacy, self-concept and anxiety? Learning and Individual
Differences, 22 , 747–758
Stankov, L. , Morony, S. , & Lee, Y. P. (2014). Confidence: the best non-cognitive predictor of
academic achievement? Educational Psychology, 34 , 9–28
Stanovich, K. E. (2004). The Robot’s Rebellion: Finding Meaning in the Age of Darwin. Chicago:
Chicago University Press
Stanovich, K. E. (2009). What Intelligence Tests Miss: The Psychology of Rational Thought.
New Haven, CT: Yale University Press
Star, J. R. (2005). Reconceptualizing procedural knowledge. Journal for Research in
Mathematics Education, 36 , 404–411
Starkey, G. S. , & McCandliss, B. D. (2014). The emergence of ‘groupitizing’ in children’s
numerical cognition. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 126 , 120–137
Starkey, P. , & Cooper, R. G. (1995). The development of subitizing in young children.
British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13, 399–420
Starr, A. , Libertus, M. E. , & Brannon, E. M. (2013). Number sense in infancy predicts
mathematical abilities in childhood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110 ,
18116–18120
St Clair-Thompson, H. L. , & Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Executive functions and achievements in
school: shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory. The Quarterly Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 59 , 745–759
Steiner, M. (1978). Mathematical explanation. Philosophical Studies, 34 , 135–151
Stern, E. (1992). Spontaneous use of conceptual mathematical knowledge in elementary school
children. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 17 , 266–277
Stylianides, A. , & Stylianides, G. (2009). Proof constructions and evaluations. Educational
Studies in Mathematics, 72 , 237–253
Suinn, R. M. , & Edwards, R. W. (1982). The measurement of mathematics anxiety: the
Mathematics Anxiety Rating Scale for Adolescents MARS–A. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 38
, 576–580
Suinn, R. M. , Taylor, S. , & Edwards, R. W. (1988). Suinn Mathematics Anxiety Rating Scale
for Elementary School Students (MARS-E): psychometric and normative data. Educational and
Psychological Measurement, 48 , 979–986
Sutherland, W. J. , Armstrong-Brown, S. , Armsworth, P. R. , Brereton, T. , Brickland, J. ,
Campbell, C. D. , . . . Watkinson, A. R. (2006). The identification of 100 ecological questions of
high policy relevance in the UK. Journal of Applied Ecology, 43 , 617–627
Sutherland, W. J. , Bellingan, L. , Bellingham, J. R. , Blackstock, J. J. , Bloomfield, R. M. ,
Bravo, M. , . . . Zimmem, R. L. (2012). A collaboratively-derived science-policy research
agenda. PloS ONE, 7, e31824
Svenson, O. , & Sjöberg, K. (1983). Speeds of subitizing and counting processes in different
age groups. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 142 , 203–211
Swanson, H. L. , & Jerman, O. (2006). Math disabilities: a selective meta-analysis of the
literature. Review of Educational Research, 76 , 249–274
Szűcs, D. , Devine, A. , Soltesz, F. , Nobes, A. , & Gabriel, F. (2013). Developmental
dyscalculia is related to visuo-spatial memory and inhibition impairment. Cortex, 49 , 2674–2688
Szűcs, D. , Devine, A. , Soltesz, F. , Nobes, A. , & Gabriel, F. (2014). Cognitive components of
a mathematical processing network in 9-year-old children. Developmental Science, 17 ,
506–524
Szűcs, D. , & Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2017). Empirical assessment of published effect sizes and
power in the recent cognitive neuroscience and psychology literature. PLoS Biol, 15, e2000797
Szűcs, D. , & Myers, T. (2017). A critical analysis of design, facts, bias and inference in the
approximate number system training literature: a systematic review. Trends in Neuroscience
and Education, 6 , 187–203
Szűcs, D. , Nobes, A. , Devine, A. , Gabriel, F. , & Gebuis, T. (2013). Visual stimulus
parameters seriously compromise the measurement of approximate number system acuity and
comparative effects between adults and children. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 444
Tall, D. O. (1980). The notion of infinite measuring numbers and its relevance in the intuition of
infinity. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 11 , 217–284
Tall, D. O. (2001). Natural and formal infinities. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 48 ,
199–238
Tall, D. O. , & Schwarzenberger, R. L. E. (1978). Conflicts in the learning of real numbers and
limits. Mathematics Teaching, 82 , 44–49
Temple, C. M. (1991). Procedural dyscalculia and number fact dyscalculia: double dissociation
in developmental dyscalculia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 8 , 155–176
ten Hoopen, G. , & Vos, J. (1979). Effect on numerosity judgment of grouping of tones by
auditory channels. Perception & Psychophysics, 26 , 374–380
Thomas, G. , & Dowker, A. (2000). Mathematics anxiety and related factors in young children.
In Paper Presented at British Psychological Society Developmental Section Conference. Bristol
Thompson, P. W. , & Saldanha, L. (2003). Fractions and multiplicative reasoning. In J. Kilpatrick
& G. Martin (Eds.), Research Companion to the NCTM Standards (pp. 95–114). Reston, VA:
NCTM
Thorndike, E. L. (1922). Instruments for measuring the disciplinary value of studies. Journal of
Educational Research, 5 , 269–279
Thorndike, E. L. , & Woodworth, R. S. (1901). The influence of improvement in one mental
function upon the efficiency of other functions: II The estimation of magnitudes. Psychological
Review, 8 , 384–395
Tirosh, D. (1991). The role of students’ intuitions of infinity when teaching the Cantorian theory.
In D. O. Tall (Ed.), Advanced Mathematical Thinking (pp. 199–214). Dordrecht: Kluwer
Torbeyns, J. , Ghesquière, P. , & Verschaffel, L. (2009). Efficiency and flexibility of indirect
addition in the domain of multi-digit subtraction. Learning and Instruction, 19 , 1–12
Torbeyns, J. , Verschaffel, L. , & Ghesquière, P. (2004a). Strategic aspects of simple addition
and subtraction: the influence of mathematical ability. Learning and Instruction, 14 , 177–195
Torbeyns, J. , Verschaffel, L. , & Ghesquière, P. (2004b). Strategy development in children with
mathematical disabilities: insights from the choice/no-choice method and the chronological-
age/ability-level—match design. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 37 , 119–131
Tosto, M. G. , Haworth, C. M. , & Kovas, Y. (2015). Behavioural genomics of mathematics. In R.
Cohen Kadosh & A. Dowker (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Numerical Cognition (pp.
993–1017). Oxford: Oxford University Press
Tosto, M. G. , Hanscombe, K. B. , Haworth, C. M. , Davis, O. S. P. , Petrill, S. A. , Dale, P. S. ,
Malykh, S. , Plomin, R. , & Kovas, Y. (2014). Why do spatial abilities predict mathematical
performance? Developmental Science, 17 , 462–470
Towse, J. , & Saxton, M. (1997). Linguistic influences on children’s number concepts:
methodological and theoretical considerations. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 66 ,
362–375
Trick, L. M. , & Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1994). Why are small and large numbers enumerated
differently? A limited-capacity preattentive stage in vision. Psychological Review, 101 , 80–102
Tsamir, P. (1999). The transition from comparison of finite to the comparison of infinite sets:
teaching prospective teachers. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 38 , 209–234
Uller, C. , Carey, S. , Huntley-Fenner, G. , & Klatt, L. (1999). What representations might
underlie infant numerical knowledge? Cognitive Development, 14 , 1–36
Uttal, D. H. , Scudder, K. V. , & DeLoache, J. S. (1997). Manipulatives as symbols: a new
perspective on the use of concrete objects to teach mathematics. Journal of Applied
Developmental Psychology, 18 , 37–54
Vamvakoussi, X. , Christou, K. P. , Mertens, L. , & Van Dooren, W. (2011). What fills the gap
between discrete and dense? Greek and Flemish students’ understanding of density. Learning
and Instruction, 21 , 676–685
Vamvakoussi, X. , & Vosniadou, S. (2004). Understanding the structure of the set of rational
numbers: a conceptual change approach. Learning and Instruction, 14 , 453–467
Van Bendegem, J. P. (2012). A defense of strict finitism. Constructivist Foundations, 7 ,
141–149
Vanbinst, K. , Ansari, D. , Ghesquière, P. , & De Smedt, B. (2016). Symbolic numerical
magnitude processing is as important to arithmetic as phonological awareness is to reading.
PloS ONE, 11, e0151045
van der Sluis, S. , de Jong, P. F. , & van der Leij, A. (2004). Inhibition and shifting in children
with learning deficits in arithmetic and reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87 ,
239–266
van der Ven, S. H. , Kroesbergen, E. H. , Boom, J. , & Leseman, P. P. (2012). The development
of executive functions and early mathematics: a dynamic relationship. British Journal of
Educational Psychology, 82 , 100–119
van Dijck, J.-P. , Gevers, W. , & Fias, W. (2009). Numbers are associated with different types of
spatial information depending on the task. Cognition, 113 , 248–253
Van Dooren, W. , De Bock, D. , Hessels, A. , Janssens, D. , & Verschaffel, L. (2004).
Remedying secondary school students’ illusion of linearity: a teaching experiment aiming at
conceptual change. Learning and Instruction, 14 , 485–501
Van Dooren, W. , Lehtinen, E. , & Verschaffel, L. (2015). Unraveling the gap between natural
and rational numbers. Learning and Instruction, 37 , 1–4
Van Hoof, J. , Janssen, R. , Verschaffel, L. , & Van Dooren, W. (2015). Inhibiting natural
knowledge in fourth graders: towards a comprehensive test instrument. ZDM Mathematics
Education, 47 , 849–857
Van Hoof, J. , Vandewalle, J. , Verschaffel, L. , & Van Dooren, W. (2015). In search for the
natural number bias in secondary school students’ interpretation of the effect of arithmetical
operations. Learning and Instruction, 37 , 30–38
Van Hoof, J. , Verschaffel, L. , & Van Dooren, W. (2015). Inappropriately applying natural
number properties in rational number tasks: characterizing the development of the natural
number bias through primary and secondary education. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 90
, 39–56
VanLehn, K. (1982). Bugs are not enough: empirical studies of bugs, impasses and repairs in
procedural skills. The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 3 , 3–71
van Loosbroek, E. , Dirkx, G. S. , Hulstijn, W. , & Janssen, F. (2009). When the mental number
line involves a delay: the writing of numbers by children of different arithmetical abilities. Journal
of Experimental Child Psychology, 102 , 26–39
Van Opstal, F. , Gevers, W. , De Moor, W. , & Verguts, T. (2008). Dissecting the symbolic
distance effect: comparison and priming effects in numerical and nonnumerical orders.
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15 , 419–425
van’t Noordende, J. E. , van Hoogmoed, A. H. , Schot, W. D. , & Kroesbergen, E. H. (2016).
Number line estimation strategies in children with mathematical learning difficulties measured
by eye tracking. Psychological Research, 80 , 368–378
Vasilyeva, M. , Laski, E. V. , Ermakova, A. , Lai, W.-F. , Jeong, Y. , & Hachigian, A. (2015).
Reexamining the language account of cross-national differences in base-10 number
representations. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 129 , 12–25
Vergnaud, G. (1982). A classification of cognitive tasks and operations of thought involved in
addition and subtraction problems. In T. P. Carpenter , J. M. Moser , & T. A. Romberg (Eds.),
Addition and Subtraction: A Cognitive Perspective (pp. 39–59). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates
Vergnaud, G. (1997). The nature of mathematical concepts. In T. Nunes & P. Bryant (Eds.),
Learning and Teaching Mathematics: An International Perspective (pp. 5–28). Hove:
Psychology Press
Verschaffel, L. , & De Corte, E. (1997). Word problems: a vehicle for promoting authentic
mathematical understanding and problem solving in the primary school? In T. Nunes & P.
Bryant (Eds.), Learning and Teaching Mathematics: An International Perspective. Hove:
Psychology Press
Verschaffel, L. , Depaepe, F. , & Van Dooren, W. (2015). Individual differences in word problem
solving. In R. Cohen Kadosh & A. Dowker (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Numerical Cognition
(pp. 953–976). Oxford: Oxford University Press
Vetter, P. , Butterworth, B. , & Bahrami, B. (2008). Modulating attentional load affects
numerosity estimation: evidence against a pre-attentive subitizing mechanism. PLoS ONE, 3,
e3269
Viswanathan, P. , & Nieder, A. (2013). Neuronal correlates of a visual ‘sense of number’ in
primate parietal and prefrontal cortices. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110
, 11187–11192
Viswanathan, P. , & Nieder, A. (2015). Differential impact of behavioral relevance on quantity
coding in primate frontal and parietal neurons. Current Biology, 25 , 1259–1269
Vogel, S. E. , Goffin, C. , & Ansari, D. (2015). Developmental specialization of the left parietal
cortex for the semantic representation of Arabic numerals: an fMR-adaptation study.
Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 12 , 61–73
von Aster, M. G. , & Shalev, R. S. (2007). Number development and developmental dyscalculia.
Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 49 , 868–873
Vorderman, C. , Porkess, R. , Budd, C. , Dunne, R. , Rahman-Hart, P. , Colmez, C. , & Lee, S.
(2011). A World Class Mathematics Education for All Our Young People. London: Conservative
Party
Vosniadou, S. , & Brewer, W. F. (1987). Theories of knowledge restructuring in development.
Review of Educational Research, 57 , 51–67
Vuilleumier, P. O. , & Rafal, R. D. (2000). A systematic study of visual extinction. Brain, 123 ,
1263–1279
Vukovic, R. K. , & Lesaux, N. K. (2013). The language of mathematics: investigating the ways
language counts for children’s mathematical development. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 115 , 227–244
Wagenmakers, E.-J. , & Dutilh, G. (2016). Seven selfish reasons for preregistration. APS
Observer, 29(9)
Wagner, J. B. , & Johnson, S. C. (2011). An association between understanding cardinality and
analog magnitude representations in preschoolers. Cognition, 119 , 10–22
Wagner, S. , & Walters, J. (1982). A longitudinal analysis of early number concepts: from
numbers to number. In G. Foreman (Ed.), Action and Thought: From Sensorimotor Schemes to
Symbolic Operations (pp. 137–161). New York: Academic Press
Wakeley, A. , Rivera, S. , & Langer, J. (2000). Can young infants add and subtract? Child
Development, 71 , 1525–1534
Walsh, V. (2003). A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(11), 483–488
Wang, Z. , Hart, S. A. , Kovas, Y. , Lukowski, S. , Soden, B. , Thompson, L. A. , Plomin, R. ,
McLoughlin, G. , Bartlett, C. W. , Lyons, I. M. , & Petrill, S. A. (2014). Who is afraid of math?
Two sources of genetic variance for mathematical anxiety. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 55 , 1056–1064
Wason, P. C. (1966). Reasoning. In B. Foss (Ed.), New Horizons in Psychology (pp. 135–151).
Harmondsworth: Penguin Books
Wason, P. C. (1968). Reasoning about a rule. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20
, 273–281
Watts, I. (1801). The Improvement of the Mind, or a Supplement to the Art of Logic. Berwick:
Jennings & Doddridge
Weber, K. (2001). Student difficulty in constructing proofs: the need for strategic knowledge.
Educational Studies in Mathematics, 48 , 101–119
Weber, K. (2008). How mathematicians determine if an argument is a valid proof. Journal for
Research in Mathematics Education, 39 , 431–459
Weber, K. (2010). Mathematics majors’ perceptions of conviction, validity and proof.
Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 12 , 306–336
Weber, K. (2015). Effective proof reading strategies to foster comprehension of mathematical
proofs. International Journal for Research in Undergraduate Mathematics Education, 1 ,
289–314
Weber, K. , & Alcock, L. (2004). Semantic and syntactic proof productions. Educational Studies
in Mathematics, 56 , 209–234
Weber, K. , & Alcock, L. (2005). Using warranted implications to understand and validate proofs.
For the Learning of Mathematics, 25 , 34–38
Weber, K. , Inglis, M. , & Mejía-Ramos, J. P. (2014). How mathematicians obtain conviction:
implications for mathematics instruction and research on epistemic cognition. Educational
Psychologist, 49 , 36–58
Weber, K. , & Mejía-Ramos, J. P. (2011). Why and how mathematicians read proofs: an
exploratory study. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 76 , 329–344
Weber, K. , & Mejía-Ramos, J. P. (2015). On relative and absolute conviction in mathematics.
For the Learning of Mathematics, 35 , 15–21
Wellman, H. M. , & Miller, K. F. (1986). Thinking about nothing: development of concepts of
zero. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4 , 31–42
Whalen, J. , Gallistel, C. R. , & Gelman, R. (1999). Nonverbal counting in humans: the
psychophysics of number representation. Psychological Science, 10 , 130–137
Wilson, A. J. , & Dehaene, S. (2007). Number sense and developmental dyscalculia. In D. Coch
, G. Dawson , & K. W. Fischer (Eds.), Human Behavior, Learning, and the Developing Brain:
Atypical Development (pp. 212–237). New York: The Guilford Press
Wood, G. , Nuerk, H.-C. , Moeller, K. , Geppert, B. , Schnitker, R. , Weber, J. , & Willmes, K.
(2008). All for one but not one for all: how multiple number representations are recruited in one
numerical task. Brain Research, 1187 , 154–166
Wood, G. , Willmes, K. , Nuerk, H.-C. , & Fischer, M. H. (2008). On the cognitive link between
space and number: a meta-analysis of the SNARC effect. Psychology Science Quarterly, 50 ,
489–525
Wood, J. N. , & Spelke, E. S. (2005). Infants’ enumeration of actions: numerical discrimination
and its signature limits. Developmental Science, 8 , 173–181
World Health Organization . (2016). ICD-10: International Statistical Classification of Diseases
and Related Health Problems 10th Revision. Geneva: World Health Organization
Wu, S. , & Menon, V. (2012). Scale for Early Mathematics Anxiety (SEMA) in Young Children.
Retrieved from http://mathbrain.stanford.edu
Wynn, K. (1990). Children’s understanding of counting. Cognition, 36, 155–193.
Wynn, K. (1992a). Addition and subtraction by human infants. Nature, 358 , 749–750
Wynn, K. (1992b). Children’s acquisition of the number words and the counting system.
Cognitive Psychology, 24 , 220–251
Wynn, K. (1998). Psychological foundations of number: numerical competence in human
infants. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2 , 296–303
Wynn, K. , & Chiang, W.- C. (1998). Limits to infants’ knowledge of objects: the case of magical
appearance. Psychological Science, 9 , 448–455
Xenidou-Dervou, I. , De Smedt, B. , van der Schoot, M. , & van Lieshout, E. C. D. M. (2013).
Individual differences in kindergarten math achievement: the integrative roles of approximation
skills and working memory. Learning and Individual Differences, 28 , 119–129
Xu, F. , & Arriaga, R. I. (2007). Number discrimination in 10-month-old infants. British Journal of
Developmental Psychology, 25 , 103–108
Xu, F. , & Spelke, E. S. (2000). Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants. Cognition,
74 , B1–B11
Xu, F. , Spelke, E. S. , & Goddard, S. (2005). Number sense in human infants. Developmental
Science, 8 , 88–101
Yang, K.-L. , & Lin, F.- L. (2008). A model of reading comprehension of geometry proof.
Educational Studies in Mathematics, 67 , 59–76
Yeniad, N. , Malda, M. , Mesman, J. , van IJzendoorn, M. H. , & Pieper, S. (2013). Shifting
ability predicts math and reading performance in children: a meta-analytical study. Learning and
Individual Differences, 23 , 1–9
Yeo, D. (2008). Dyslexia, Dyspraxia & Mathematics. London: John Wiley & Sons
Zettle, R. D. (2003). Acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT) vs. system desensitization in
treatment of mathematics anxiety. The Psychological Record, 53 , 197–215
Zuber, J. , Pixner, S. , Moeller, K. , & Nuerk, H.- C. (2009). On the language specificity of basic
number processing: transcoding in a language with inversion and its relation to working memory
capacity. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102 , 60–77

You might also like