You are on page 1of 16

i l..

:ii I
:*
&;dl$iir :,
&A;
"ffiffi1:
ffiffi.+,
a

'x'.
fi'i $e ,:.il:,:- !:,lSi
r#^
E& ffi

. w.
.,6h. as
&#
ffi:
,L&
3" ff48! *ffi
e&
:ffi4
!1S
,ilii!,. -.i;s&
,,:!siili: lliill
i* &: *'lt*
@aq &
& r;j:t: I :t, !
@i?
i -: :i &
.{- "... K

;g I r'

Early Reading Development


Dominic Wyse and Usha Goswami

Written language is one of the supreme alphabet was 'the great invention' because
achievements of human beings. [t has its graphic signs have almost no limita-
enabled our ability to communicate over tions for human communication. The con-
distances, to record history to analyse at tinuing development of writing, forexample
new depths, and to create new artistic through internet and electronic text forms,
forms. It is a cultural invention that in turn is further testament to written language's
has had profound cultural and social effects. extraordinary capacity to adapt to, and be
The creation of alphabetic written lan- part ol cultural change.
guage was a highly significant develop- A more recent debate inspired by the
ment. All alphabets were originally derived alphabet, and its links with speech, has
from the Semitic syllabaries of the second been its place in reading teaching. From
millennium. The developments from both the 1960s onwards this debate became
Greek script and the Roman alphabet can polarised between those who thought that
be seen in the use of the Latinised form reading was best taught bottom-up (put
of the first two letters of the Greek alpha- simply, teaching from phonemes and let-
bet in the word itself, 'alphabet'. Alpha ters to texts) and those who thought that
was derived from the Semitic 'aleph' and reading was best taught top down (from
'beta' from 'beth' (Goody and Watt, 1963). texts to letters and phonemes). Currently
Historically, the alphabet has been at the this debate is complemented by interactive
heart of some of the most enduring debates models that combine bottom-up and top-
about the development of written collmu- down approaches. Bottom-up approaches
nication, for example whether the alphabet to the teaching of reading have a very long
simply emerged from logographic or picto- history. Until the early 1800s the teaching
graphic forms. In Harris'(1986) examina- of reading and the teaching of spelling in
tion of the origins of writing, he called England were indistinguishable. Therefore,
this particular idea of emergence an evolu- evidence on reading teaching at this time
tionary fallacy, arguing instead that the can be found in the hundreds of different
380 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EARLY CHILDHOOD LITERACY

spelling textbooks that were published. key messages from research and suggest
Michael (1984) argued, on the basis of an that reading teaching might be improved
analysis of the spelling books that reading through policy-making that takes account
teaching was for most teachers a bottom- of interdisciplinary evidence.
up approach. As early as 1610 this was The main methodological orientation
clear, for example: Therefore let the schol- that underpins our approach to this chapter
ler, being thus traded (i.e, schooled) from is interdisciplinarity. In a broad sense,
letters to syllables of one Consonant: from interdisciplinarity has been used to refer to
syllables of one Consonant, to syllables of a range of practices, from borrowing and
many Consonants: from syllables of many solving problems across disciplines to the
Consonants, to words of many syllables; actual emergence of an interdiscipline.
proceede to sentences. (1984: 57). Michael But central to the interdisciplinary method-
suggests that the approach was a conse- ology that informs our understanding is
quence of the prevailing view that complex integration, the combining of theoretical
things could only be learned by children perspectives from different fields of knowl-
if they were first broken down into their edge. The process of integration that is
component parts (a theory that still informs key in interdisciplinarity entails a step in
some current opinions of how children which the disciplinary perspectives are
leam to read). In his chapter that exam- seen in a new configuration. The substan-
ines the growth of whole-word methods, tive areas of this methodological orien-
Michael also identifies what he sees as the tation are cognitive, psycholinguistic,
first published reference to whole-word socio-cultural and educational. In part the
teaching. Charles Hoole, in his transla- reasons for selecting these disciplinary
tion of the preface to Comenius' Orbis areas above others relate to our experience
Sensualium Pictus (1659) said, 'reading and interests, but we also think that their
cannot but be learned; and indeed too, integration offers the potential for a sig-
which thing is to be noted, without using nificant contribution to enduring issues.
any ordinary tedious spelling, that most
troublesome torture of wits, which may
be wholly avoyded by this Method [the PHONOTOGICAT AWARENESS
whole word methodl'(1984: 60). IN DIFFERENT TANGUAGES
Our chapter explores the development
ofreading from the perspective ofevidence As Port (2007) has noted, to a literate
across languages, and considers the possi- adult 'it seems intuitively obvious that
ble implications for the teaching of reading speech presents itself to our consciousness
in English and for literacy policy. The first in the form of letter-like symbolic units ...
section of the chapter examines reading when we hear someone say "tomato",
development from a cognitive perspec- we seem to hear it as a sequence of conso-
tive in relation to phonological awareness. nant and vowel sound units' (Port, 2007:
Next, we consider the development of pho- 1434). However, Port goes on to note'...
nological awareness and reading in a wider there is virtually no evidence that supports
socio-cultural educational context, includ- [this] traditional view of linguistic repre-
ing attention to teaching and learning in sentation'. Classically, these letter-like
the context of educational policy-making. symbolic units have been called 'pho-
We conclude by identifying some of the nemes'. Conceptually, phonemes are the
EARLY READING DEVELOPMENT 381

smallest elements of
sound that change acquire grapheme-phoneme recoding
a word's meaning (as in 'cat'versus 'cap'). skills (or 'phonic' knowledge) depends
Obviously, a preJiterate child who has on their pre-reading levels of 'phonologi-
represented 'cat' and 'cap' as distinctive cal awareness'.
but complex auditory patterns can distin- What is the form of the 'prosodic
guish between these two words when templates' that infants are acquiring?
listening, and can use them appropriately For English, the dominant template is a
when speaking. Many children can also rhythmic pattern comprising two syllables,
leam which sequences of letters are used with stronger stress on the first syllable.
systematically to represent these auditory Examples are 'mummy', 'daddy', 'baby'
pattems when they are taught the alphabet. and 'bottle'. lndeed, child phonologists
However, other children will struggle. This have pointed out that the words that we
is because the sound elements that are use with infants and young children are
symbolised by the alphabet do not have an often adapted to fit this phonological
acoustic correlate in the speech stream. template. We say 'doggie'instead of 'dog',
Take our example of the spoken words and 'milkie' instead of 'milk'. Young
'cat' and 'cap'. Both words comprise a children also seem to find it easier to pro-
single syllable, both words sound very duce this rhythmic pattern, for example
similar at the beginning, but they sound saying 'nana'for 'banana'. The dominant
different at the end. We can either say that prosodic templates differ in different
these words do not rhyme, or we can languages. For example, the dominant pro-
in terms of their final
say that they differ sodic pattern in French is to lengthen the
'phoneme'. In the written form of these final syllable. Whereas in English the
words, the alphabet uses the change from main stress in a word like 'elephant' is on
T to P to signal this difference in sound the first syllable, in French the main stress
structure (or'phonology'). However, in 'elefant' is on the final syllable. Even
whereas the sound symbolised by the very young infants are aware of these
letter P has one acoustic form in the spoken prosodic differences between languages.
word 'cap', it has a different acoustic form Nevertheless, mothers and fathers across
in the spoken word 'spoon'. Beginning languages use a special prosodic register,
readers may well try to write the word called infant-directed speech, to highlight
'spoon' as SBN, and acoustically this these rhythmic cues for their babies. Infant-
makes sense. In order to acquire literacy, directed speech uses auditory features
children have to overlook some acoustic like exaggerated rhythm and intonation,
differences between their prosodic tem- heightened pitch and increased duration.
plates, and highlight some acoustic simi- This prosodic exaggeration seems to serve
larities. Across languages, children become a language-learning function, helping
able to reflect upon the units of sound infants in different languages to extract
that comprise spoken words as their lan- individual words from the speech stream
guage skills develop, and this process of on the basis of pitch and rhythmic cues
reflection is called 'phonological aware- (e.g., Echols, 1996).
ness'. Phonological awareness follows a Prosodic variation works at the syllable
sequential developmental path. Within a level of speech, and is not often marked in
particular language, the ease with which the written form (although it is in some
children will leam about phonemes and languages, like Spanish). Nevertheless, the
382 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EARLY CHILDHOOD LITERACY

syllable is the primary perceptual linguistic Across languages, therefore, pre-reading


unit across languages. As phonological children are aware of the phonological
awareness develops, children first become structure of syllables at the onset-rime
aware of syllables. Children can reflect on level. Cross-language divergence comes
syllables as phonological units by the age only when the development of phoneme
of 34 years. For example, they can count awareness is studied.
the number of syllables in spoken words
(deciding that 'butterfly' has three sylla-
bles, and 'bottle' has two syllables, see PHONOTOG!CAt COMPLEXITY
Liberman, Shankweiler, Fischer, and
Carter, 1974), and they can decide whether The rate of development of phoneme
words share syllables or not (deciding that awareness varies markedly across lan-
'hammer' and 'hammock' share the first guages. Two factors appear to be particu-
syllable, see Treiman and Zukowski, 1991). larly important in explaining this
Soon after, children become aware of intra- cross-language variation: the phonological
syllabic units called 'onsets' and 'rimes'. complexity of syllable structures in differ-
The onset in any syllable is the sound pat- ent languages, and the spelling (ortho-
tern before the vowel. The onset in 'swing' graphic) consistency of the written form of
would be the sound corresponding to the the language. Most languages in the world
letters SW, and the onset in 'string' would have syllables with a simple phonological
be the sound corresponding to the letters structure. In these languages, the dominant
STR. For monosyllabic words, rimes and syllable type is consonant-vowel or CV. In
rhymes are the same level of linguistic languages like ltalian, Finnish and Spanish,
structure. A word like 'cap'rhymes with a most words are made up of syllables with
word like 'tap', and the shared (rime) this pattern, for example 'Mamma', 'raga-
sound unit is the sound corresponding to zza' (Italian girl) and 'casa' (Italian,
the letters AP. However, 'capture' does not house). In spoken English, the dominant
rhyme with 'caption'. Although the rime monosyllable type is CVC. English has
of the first syllable is the same in both lots of words like 'girl', 'house', 'cat' and
words, the rime of the second syllable 'dog', which follow a CVC phonological
differs. Instead, 'capture' rhymes with pattem ('girl' has a CVC pattern as the
'rapture', because these two words share vowel is the single sound made by the
their phonological structure after the first letters IR). In fact, 43 per cent of monosyl-
onset (linguists call this shared phonolo- lables correspond to this pattem (De Cara
gical unit a 'super-rime', see Davis, 1989). and Goswami, 2002). English also has
For our pu{poses, it is enough that any many CCVC monosyllables ('skip', 'pram',
spoken syllable can be divided into two 'black'; 15 per cent of monosyllables),
units, onset and rime, by segmenting at CVCC monosyllables ('past','bump',
the vowel. Children are able to reflect on 'build'; 2l per cent of monosyllables),
the onsets and rimes that comprise spoken and CCVCC monosyllables ('crust',
syllables by the age of 4-5 years (e.g., 'stamp', 5 per cent of monosyllables). The
Bradley and Bryant, 1983; Ho and Bryant, CV syllable only represents 5 per cent of
1997). They are aware of rhyme even English monosyllables ('see', 'go', 'do').
earlier (e.g., Chukovsky, 1963; Bryant, So English is rather different from many
Bradley, Maclean, and Crossland, 1989). languages in terms of the phonological
EARLY READING DEVELOPMENT

complexity of its syllable structure. English same letter may correspond to four or more
has syllables that are comprised of a sounds. For example, the letter A makes
number of phonemes (e.g., 'stamp'has five a different sound in CAP, FAIHER, SAW,
phonemes, 'past' has four phonemes). MAKE and BARE. Although most incon-
English is not unique, as languages like sistency occurs with vowels, it also occurs
German, Welsh and Czech also have com- with consonants (consider G in MAGIC
plex syllables. Note that when the domi- versus BAG, or C in CAKE versus
nant syllable type for the language is CY CIRCLE). In fact, the degree of ortho-
a child who is aware of onsets and rimes graphic inconsistency in English is
is also in effect aware of phonemes. A CV much higher than in other orthographies
syllable has a single-phoneme onset and (Berndt, Reggia, and Mitchum, l9B7;
a single-phoneme rime (e.g., 's-ee', 't-oo', Ziegler, Stone, and Jacobs, 1997). So chil-
's-igh'). Note that for English, a CV pho- dren learning to read in English have
nological structure does not necessar- to face two barriers that make learning
ily correspond to a CV spelling pattem. to recode words to sound 'bottom-up'more
So phonological complexity is one cross- challenging. They have to learn ortho-
language factor that affects the develop- graphic patterns for complex syllables that
ment of phonemic awareness when the contain lots of different phonemes, and
alphabet is taught. they have to learn these phonemes even
though the target sounds that they are
learning can be represented by lots of dif-
ORTHOGRAPHIC CONSISTENCY ferent letters. As phonemes do not have
acoustic correlates in the speech stream,
Orthographic (spelling) consistency is the the degree of orthographic inconsistency in
other cross-language factor that affects the English is especially problematic.
development of phonological awareness.
In many alphabetic languages, there is 1:l
consistency between letters and sounds. ACQUIRING READING IN TANGUAGES
The same letter always corresponds to USING THE ALPHABET
the same sound, or phoneme. Examples
of highly consistent writing systems are Perhaps unsurprisingly, therefore, English
Finnish, Italian, Spanish, German, Czech children leam to recode letters to sound
and Welsh. Furthermore, some languages fairly slowly in comparison to children
have a smaller number of phonemes than who are learning to read other languages.
others. Finnish has 21 phonemes, and The term 'phonological recoding' is fre-
when loan words are considered (i.e., quently used to refer to the efficient and
words adopted from other languages, like automatic mapping of print to sound (see
the word 'pizza' from Italian), it has 25 Share, 1995). Phonological recoding is a
phonemes. A language like English has 44 critical component of reading development
phonemes. In other alphabetic languages, across languages. In orthographically con-
there is a 1:many correspondence between sistent languages with simple syllable
letters and sounds. The same letter can cor- structures, phonological recoding is taught
respond to more than one sound. Examples very efficiently via systematic phonics
are French, Danish, English and Portuguese. instruction. Children are taught the sound
Indeed, for the English orthography the made by each letter, they are taught to
384 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EARLY CHILDHOOD LITERACY

blend these sounds together, and they learn and 9 years two types of nonword to read,
to recode words to sound. Children learn- items like DAIK and items like FAIK.
ing to read languages with complex sylla- The children were matched for their ability
ble structures also acquire phonological to read the real words that were used as
recoding skills rapidly once tuition in a basis for the pseudohomophones. The
phonics begins, as long as these languages two types of nonword were matched
have high orthographic consistency. As within-language for orthographic familiar-
Landerl notes, in most consistent orthogra- ity. Goswami et al. (2001) found that while
phies 'reading is typically taught via a German children read aloud items like
straightforward phonics teaching regime ... DAIK as fast and as accurately as items
[with] heavy emphasis on teaching of let- like FAIK, English children were signifi-
ter-sound correspondences' (2000: 240). cantly more accurate at reading aloud the
Given its high utility in other languages, pseudohomophones (EAIK) than the con-
should we then endorse Rose's (2006) trol items (DAIK). However, the opposite
claim for English that for English, too, happened when the children had to make a
synthetic phonics offers 'the vast majority 'lexical decision'about whether items like
of beginners the best route to becoming FAIK and DAIK were real words or not.
skilled readers' (Rose, 2006: 19)? In our When recoding to sound was not involved,
view, the experimental data do not support the English children performed better.
this claim. They found it easy to reject items like
Experimental data suggest instead that FAIK very quickly, whereas the German
phonological recoding in English occurs children struggled. Goswami et al. (2001)
at more than one linguistic level or 'psy- suggested that this was because the English
cholinguistic grain size'. To become an children had learned about specific orthog-
efhcient reader of English, the data suggest raphy-phonology connections at the
that children acquire orthography-phonol- word level. They knew that orthographic
ogy connections at the psycholinguistic patterns like FAIK were not real words.
level of whole words and at the psycholin- The German children, who could recode
guistic level of onsets and rimes as well any item to sound very efficiently by using
as at the psycholinguistic level of the grapheme-phoneme recoding, had not
phoneme. One experimental marker of the developed these kinds of psycholinguistic
acquisition of the whole-word grain size is units.
the pseudohomophone effect. A pseudo- An experimental marker for the acquisi-
homophone is an item that sounds like a tion of psycholinguistic units at the grain
real word, but is not a real word, for exam- size of the rime is switching costs in non-
ple FAIK. tf children are faster and more word reading. Nonwords like DAKE are
accurate at recoding an item like FAIK to analogous in spelling at the rime level
sound than a matched nonword item like to many real words (e.g., MAKE, CAKE,
DAIK, this is indicative of accessing BAKE, LAKE). Nonwords like DAIK,
whole-word phonology. Goswami, Ziegler, which require exactly the same recoding
Dalton, and Schneider (2001) demonstra- to sound, have no analogies at the rime
ted that pseudohomophone effects were level - the spelling pattern AIK is not used
stronger for English children than for in English. Goswami, Ziegler, Dalton, and
German children. They gave English and Schneider (2003) devised lists of these two
German children with reading ages of 7, 8, types of nonwords for English versus
EARLY READING DEVELOPMENT 385

German. One list contained familiar ortho- psycholinguistic grain size. Indeed, Brown
graphic pattems for rimes (DAKE, MURN), and Deavers (1999) suggested that children
while the second did not (DAIK, MIRN). learning to read in English developed both
Children who have developed psycholin- 'small unit' and 'large unit' strategies in
guistic units for rimes via leaming to read parallel. In our view, these data show that
real words should find the lists that con- the brain naturally develops this combina-
tained DAKE-type nonwords easy to read tion of strategies because of the nature
aloud, as they can apply a 'rhyme analogy' of the orthography that it is learning to
strategy. They can also, of course, use a s)m- read, and the complex phonology that this
thetic phonics strategy, applying grapheme- orthography represents. The'strategies'
phoneme relations (here involving the are not explicit and are not used purposely
'magic e'for DAKE). However, the DAIK- by children; rather the brain has devel-
type nonwords can only be recoded to oped these internal psycholinguistic units
sound by using a synthetic phonics strat- on the basis of incremental reading experi-
egy. Two predictions follow. The first is ence of English words. The brain is then
that ifchildren show an advantage (greater naturally faster and more efficient for some
accuracy) in reading nonwords like DAKE kinds of words than for others, and this can
than in reading nonwords like DAIK, they be explored using methods from cognitive
must have developed psycholinguistic units psychology, such as nonword reading stud-
at the grain size of the rime. The second is ies. However, 'psycholinguistic grain size
that if the two types of nonword are mixed theory' (Ziegler and Goswami, 2005) also
together in a list (e.g., DAIK, MURN ...), raises important questions about learning
then it should slow children down, and and teaching. For example, do we best
perhaps make them more inaccurate as teach young children the efficient and
well, because they might try to alternate automatic mapping of print to sound in
between a rhyme analogy recoding strat- English using only one grain size, as in a
egy and a synthetic phonics recoding strat- synthetic phonics approach, or do we try to
egy.7-,8- and 9-year-old readers in English teach children aboutrimes and about whole
and German were again tested. The data words as well?
revealed that blocking word lists by grain
size increased recoding accuracy for the
English children but not for the German PSYCHOTINGUISTIC GRAIN SIZE
children. Furthermore, switching costs THEORY: IMPLICATIONS
for mixed lists were found for the English FOR LEARNlNG
children but not for the German children.
Goswami et al. (2003) argued that the With respect to learning, we can use exist-
German children were already relying exclu- ing data sets to show that children learning
sively on synthetic phonics sftategies - that to read English acquire phonological recod-
is, they were recoding words to sound at ing skills more slowly than children learn-
the smallest psycholinguistic grain size of ing to read in other alphabetic languages.
the phoneme. For example, a large-scale and carefully
One way of thinking about such data is controlled cross-language reading study
to suggest that children who leam to read reported by Seymour, A.ro, and Erskine
English are developing'phonological (2003) involved participating scientists
recoding' strategies at more than one from 14 European Community countries,
386 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EARLY CHILDHOOD LITERACY

who developed matched sets of items of learning to read (reading 34 per cent of
simple real words (BALL, BOY) and non- real word items correctly, and 29 per cent
words (DEM, FIP) for use across these 14 of nonword items). Danish children
languages. The items were given to chil- (71 per cent correct), Portuguese children
dren from each country during their first (73 per cent correct), and French chil-
year of reading instruction (for further dren (79 per cent correct) also showed
details, see Seymour et al., 2003). All par- lower levels of accuracy than Greek and
ticipating schools followed a reading Finnish children during the first year of
instructional prograrnme based on graph- acquisition. This would be expected given
eme-phoneme level phonics (the schools the reduced orthographic consistency of
contributing the English data were in these languages. Smaller experimental
Scotland). The results of this cross-lan- studies comparing fewer languages have
guage study are shown inTable22.l. obtained very similar results. For example,
The data show a clear relationship when French, Spanish, and English are
between orthographic consistency and the compared, then Spanish children reach
speed of acquiring efficient and automatic ceiling levels much faster than French chil-
mapping of print to sound. Children who dren, who in turn are faster than English
were leaming to read languages with children (Goswami, Gombert, and de
consistent spelling systems (such as Greek, Barrera, 1998). When English and German
Finnish, German, Italian and Spanish) are compared, the German children outper-
performed at almost ceiling levels, for form the English children until the age of
both word and nonword reading, by the around 9-10 years (Frith, Wimmer, and
middle of first grade (irrespective of age). Landerl, 1998).
English-speaking children performed However, it might be objected that there
extremely poorly in their first year of are many socio-cultural differences across
languages which have not been taken into
Table 22.1 Data (0/o correct) from the large- account by cross-language psychology
scale study of reading skills at the end of studies such as these. For example, there
grade I in 14 European languages (adapted
are likely to be differences in school sys-
from Tables 5 and 6 of Seymour, Aro, and
Erskine, 2003) tems, school curricula, teaching methods
Language Faniliar real words Nonwords
and demographic distributions between
different countries, and such differences
Greek 98 92
cannot be controlled systematically in
Finnish 98 95
experimental cross-language studies.
German 98 94
Austrian German 97
One way to get around these inherent
92
Italian 95
problems with cross-language studies is to
89
Spanish 95 89
comparc beginning reading skills in chil-
Swedish 95 88 dren who live in the same communities but
Dutch 95 82 are learning to read in different languages.
lcelandic 94 86 English and Welsh provide a good exam-
Norwegian 92 9l ple. Particularly in North Wales, English
French 79 85 and Welsh are often spoken and read
Portuguese 73 77 within the same communities, and there
Danish 71 54
may be two primary schools in such com-
Scottish English 34 29
munities, one for Welsh-speakers and one
EARLY READING DEVELOPMENT 387

for English-speakers. These schools will phonological units, i.e., syllables, onset-
serve the same geographical catchment rimes, onset-nucleus-coda, phoneme. For
area, they will be administered by the same the smallest units (phonemes), orthographic
local educational authorities, and they will structure affects the speed of learning, with
follow similar curricula and teaching advantages for children learning more
approaches. The difference will be in the transparent orthographies.
language of instruction. Both English and The first part of this chapter has mainly
Welsh have complex syllable structures, addressed children's phonological aware-
but whereas English has a highly incon- ness and understanding of spelling sound
sistent orthography, Welsh has a highly correspondences. However although these
consistent orthography. A series of studies are important aspects of reading develop-
by Hanley and his colleagues (Hanley, ment there are a considerable range of
Masterson, Spencer, and Evans, 2004; other factors that have to be taken account
Spencer and Hanley, 2003; see also Ellis of when considering how children's devel-
and Hooper, 2001) have demonstrated that opment can be supported through teaching.
English-speaking children's reading skills The second half of the chapter addresses
develop at the same slow pace in Wales some of these factors.
as is found everywhere else. In contrast,
Welsh-speaking children learn more like
Finnish children. In Hanley's studies, the FROM GRAINSIZE !N LEARNING
Welsh-speaking children read more than TO CONTEXTS FOR TEACHING
twice as many words accurately as the
English-speaking children after the same Children naturally encounter written lan-
amount of reading instruction. After 3 years guage and the alphabetic code through
of reading instruction, the English children engagement with texts. These texts are
'caught up'. Again, this mirrors the results often only single words, for example the
found in the other cross-language studies child's name, or short phrases, for example
that have been discussed. The slow rate packaging and signs, but children are also
of learning of the English children is only likely to encounter written language in
temporary. After around 3 or 4 years of books. The context that surrounds the
reading instruction, they are indistinguish- reading of a book, often a story book, is of
able in reading achievement from children prime importance to scholars working in
who have learned to read orthographically the socio-cultural educational tradition.
consistent languages. One aspect of this is the nature of the
Psycholinguistic grain size theory text itself. For example, the controlled
accounts for the developmental sequence of vocabulary of a reading scheme book or
phonological awareness across languages, basal reader will lead to a rather different
and for initial differences in learning spell- experience for the child than an encounter
ing-sound correspondences. In spite of with a 'trade' book written by an author
differences in the phonological and ortho- with the intent of engaging young readers
graphic structure of words in different (Levy, 2009). This is one of the reasons
languages, the developmental sequence that Meek argued that Texts Teach What
for phonological awareness is the same: Readers Learn (Meek 1988).
early sensitivity to larger phonological An important aspect of children's
units and subsequent awareness of smaller encounters with texts is the extent to which
388 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EARLY CHILDHOOD LITERACY

children are motivated and interested to knowledge and understandings that flow
engage with them. Evidence about the from reader to textual interpretation. As
nature of young children's interest in lan- Rosenblatt says, 'we need to see the read-
guage and literacy learning has come from ing act as an event involving a pafiicular
ethnographic case studies of individual individual and a particular text, happen-
children (e.g., Bissex, 1980) but also from ing at a particular time, under particular
studies of early-years settings (Harste, circumstances, in a particular social and
Woodward, and Burke, 1984) which have cultural setting, and as part of the ongoing
revealed the creative ways that young chil- life ofthe individual and the group' (1985:
dren engage with language and literacy. 100). Rosenblatt argued that her notion
Research has also shown the disparities of the transaction was not the same as the
that exist between literacy in formal educa- separation of text and reader that is a fea-
tional settings and home literacy, including ture of some cognitive views of transac-
children's motivation for different kinds tion, including information processing
of texts than those preferred in formal models, because of the decontextualisation
educational settings (Marsh, 2003; Wells, of processes that she felt was a feature of
1986). Wigfield and Guthrie (1997) found such models.
that children's reading motivation pre- The idea of reading embedded in a
dicted the amount and breadth of their social and cultural setting is also of central
reading. They also found that intrinsic concern to scholars in the new literacies
motivation predicted more strongly amount tradition. Prior to the 1970s the word 'lit-
and breadth than extrinsic motivation. eracy' was not typically used in relation
The implications from a range of studies to schooling in the countries of the West;
point to the importance of encouraging reading and writing were the preferred
children's motivation, for example by pro- terms. But increasingly politicians and
viding texts that are likely to interest them. policy-makers began to talk about crises in
However, there is of course a dilemma in relation to levels of literacy. At the same
relation to intrinsic motivation. When does time sociocultural work on the study of
encouragement to read by teachers become language was increasingly focusing on
extrinsic motivation? This implies a subtle literacy (Lanksheare and Knobel, 2006).
balanced between the requirement to read, A classic line of thinking in the new litera-
and encouragement to read. cies tradition was portrayed by the New
Once appropriate texts have been London Group's (M-G) concept of multili-
selected by the teacher in order to motivate teracies. In particular the NLG recom-
children and to best support their deve- mended a new pedagogy based on situated
lopment of reading (taking due account practice, overt instruction (of a nontradi-
of children's interests) there is a need to tional kind to include a metalanguage of
understand the nature of the 'interaction' design) critical framing, and transformed
between text and reader. Rosenblatt (1985) practice. Their position was built on the
theorised the idea of transaction to explain idea that traditional pedagogy represents
what happens between reader and text. 'page-bound, official, standard ... formal-
Reading is a two-way process that includes ized, monolingual, monocultural, and rule
the processes of decoding and literal inter- governed forms of language'(New London
pretation that flow from text to reader, but Group, 1996: 61). The NLG's contribution
also the reader bringing their experiences, to understanding of literacy pedagogy was
EARLY READING DEVELOPMENT 389

in three main areas: 1 a necessary empha- affected by policy. Policy in many coun-
sis on cultural and linguistic diversity; tries in the last 20 years has increasingly
2 the recognition oflocal diversity interact- located reading within drives to improve
ing with global connectedness; and 3 the literacy. But where policy makers have
idea of situated practice that included often reduced the complexities of reading
the important concept of a community of to a limited focus on literacy, socio-cultural
leamers. The NLG were critical of what educational work has provided critiques of
they called 'mere literacy'that they claim such foci.
involves a focus on language only, based In spite of the promising lines of research
on rules and correct usage that leads and theory on the place of phonological
to 'more or less authoritarian pedagogy' awareness and alphabetic knowledge in
(1996:64). learning to read, and the appropriate con-
Working in the new literacies tradition, text for such leaming, educational policy
Street (2003) argued that there was an in many countries fails to adequately reflect
important additional distinction to be made such developments. In particular policy
between autonomous and ideological seems to favour simple and narrow forms
models of literacy. Autonomous models of instruction in favour of approaches that
assume that the improvement of literacy, build on the kind of research and theory
for example in economically disadvan- reviewed in this chapter. Typically the
taged groups of people, will automatically complexities of grainsize and the contexts
lead to societal and material advancement. for text transactions are reduced to a dgbate
Ideological models require consideration about a bottom-up approach such as syn-
of the social and economic conditions that thetic phonics. Brief examples from the
led to disadvantage in the first place. Street US, Australia and England illustrate this.
also makes a distinction between liter- In the United States the National Reading
acy events and literacypractices.A literacy Panel (NRP) (National Instirure of Child
event takes place when written language is Health and Human Development, 2000)
integral to the participants' interactions. concluded that reading teaching should
Literacy practices account for literacy not focus too much on the teaching of lef
events but also add the social models of ter-sound relations at the expense of the
literacy that are brought to bear by partici- application of this knowledge in the con-
pants. Literacy practices in this sense share text of reading texts. Also that phonics
some of the features of Rosenblatt's (1985) should not become the dominant compo-
transactions. nent in a reading program, so educators
'must keep the end in mind and insure
that children understand the purpose of
READING PEDAGOGV AND POLICY leaming letter-sounds' (2000: 2-96). The
importance of the cautions about phonics
For teachers the socio-cultural context for becoming a dominant component are given
texts and transactions is the professional added weight if we consider the findings of
one. It is in the professional context where Camilli, Vargas, and Yurecko (2003).
the pedagogies that teachers might adopt, Camilli et al. replicated the meta-analysis
including reading teaching, are mediated from the NRP phonics instruction report
by policy, and the early-years curriculum and found a smaller but still significant
is an area that continues to be strongly effect for systematic phonics (d = 0.24)
390 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EARLY CHILDHOOD LITERACY

than the NRP but also found an effect for of policy formation and implementation
systematic language activities (d = 0.29) are also revealed in Australia. The
and an effect for individual tutoring Commonwealth government in Australia
(d = 0.40). Hence the effect for individual carried out a review of research on literacy,
tutoring was larger than the effect for sys- influenced by the work of the NRP, but
tematic phonics and that the effect for effectively restricted its focus to the teach-
systematic language activities was slightly ing of reading. Although the report recom-
larger but comparable with that for system- mended that 'teachers [should] provide an
atic phonics. These findings resulted in integrated approach to reading that sup-
their conclusion that 'systematic phonics ports the development of oral language,
instruction when combined with language vocabulary, grammar, reading fluency,
activities and individual tutoring may triple comprehension and the literacies of new
the effect of phonics alone.' (Camilli et al. technologies.' (Australian Government
2003). DEST, 2005: 14) and'no one approach of
Unfortunately the conclusions of the itself can address the complex nature of
NRP and Camilli et al. seem not have been reading difficulties. An integrated approach
sufficiently reflected in policy on reading requires that teachers have a thorough
pedagogy in the US. Policy on the teach- understanding of a range of effective strat-
ing of reading became strongly influenced egies, as well as knowing when and why
by federal government through the legisla- to apply them' (Australian Government
tion of No Child Left Behind. Phonics DEST, 2005) Sawyer (2010) argues that
instruction frequently received more atten- the synthetic phonics approach was fore-
tion than other important aspects of read- grounded and panicularly favoured by the
ing pedagogy sometimes extremisin report. Of particular concern to Sawyer
(Cummins, 2007). Allington (201 0) argues was the use of the study by Johnston and
that federal education policy adopted a Watson (2005) as the basis for the sugges-
nzrrrow, ideologically defined notion of tion in the report that the case for synthetic
'scientifically-based reliable, replicable' phonics was clearly proven, whereas the
reading research (SBRR). This determined research showing the significance of bal-
the kind of reading pedagogy that states anced reading instruction was 'assertion'.
had to implement in order to receive fed- In 2006, concerns expressed by many in
eral funding. However, as shown in this education that England's National Literacy
chapter, scientifically-based reliable and Strategy (l[S) approach to reading teach-
replicable reading research is much broader ing (implemented from 1997 to 2010) was
than synthetic phonics. To date there is no not working (ironically from both synthetic
compelling evidence that reading stand- phonics advocates and from those commit-
ards have improved as a result of the No ted to a more balanced approach to the
Child Left Behind (NCLB) legislation and teaching of reading) led to a govemment-
the narrow definition of SBRR. In fact, commissioned review into the teaching of
there is some evidence of NCLB leading early reading in England. It was hoped that
to more limited reading curricula and to this might result in a more rigorous analy-
decreased curricular and instructional sis of research evidence as the basis for
coherence (Allington, 2010) policy concerning how to improve reading
The difficulties of maintaining research teaching. This unfortunately was not the
informed reading pedagogy in the context case. The outcome of the review was the
EARLY READING DEVELOPMENT 391

decision to prescribe synthetic phonics (2007: 72), would be a helpful way for-
as the sole method for teaching reading, ward, yet in spite of some researchers'
something that caused controversy (Ellis, direct engagement with govemment on
2007 ; Gouch and Lambirth, 2008; Kershner policy implementation and their under-
and Howard,2006; Lewis and Ellis,2006; standing of the contexts of policy enact-
Wyse and Styles, 2007). As Wyse and ment, the problems with politicisation and
Goswami (2008) pointed out, the report lack of attention to the full range of evi-
did not draw sufficiently upon the large dence remain, suggesting different prob-
amount of high-quality research evidence lems such as increasing political control
available about the teaching ofreading and of education (see Wyse, 2011).
phonological awareness.
Wyse, Andrews, and Hoffman (2010)
summarise what they see as a pattern in the coNcrusroN
kinds of policy responses to the teaching of
reading described above. First, a dubious Scientifically based and reliable research
characterisation by politicians that educa- on how to teach the reading ofEnglish sup-
tion is failing, followed by 'decisive'action ports the direct teaching of all relevant
giving a reason for greater centralisation psycholinguistic grain sizes. Larger units
as the government 'takes responsibility' are important, and for all psycholinguistic
for improving performance; then the inevi- grain sizes children need to see how the
table increase in a battery of measures to decoding of letters is related to the com-
gauge progress and the impact of curricu- prehension and understanding of text. Wyse
lar interventions ('high-stakes testing'); a and Goswami (2008) and Wyse (2010)
gradual seeping in of pedagogical control review evidence from experimental trials
as well as curricular control, leading to comparing different teaching approaches
loss ofteacher autonomy and agency. Such to examine the empirical case for the
control eventually becomes unworkable, approach they call contextualised phonics
uninspiring, and ceases to provide the teaching.
results it is intended to deliver; and so a While new literacy theory accurately
reaction sets in, freeing up teachers to have identifies the risks of politically moti-
more agency within what and how they vated authoritarian pedagogy there is a
teach; allowing more space for creativity need for development of this idea and its
across the whole English and language consequences. New literacy theory could
curriculum; making the curriculum more be enhanced through an interdisciplinary
closely related to the outside world. Perry, analysis of socio-cultwal and psychologi-
Amadeo, Fletcher, and Walkers (2010) caUcognitive work. This would entail more
work confirms the gap between evidence rigorous attention to the links between
and policy making, the politicisation of teaching at the micro level, for example the
decisions on pedagogy, the speed of policy alphabetic code, and teaching at the macro
change, and the role of the media in exac- level, for example the transactions between
erbating some of these problems. Moss readers and texts. The benefits of this
and Huxford (2007) suggested that a focus approach are enhanced theory a stronger
by researchers on understanding the con- empirical case to challenge narrowly-
texts of policy enactment, more than 'find- focused visions of literacy, and a rationale
ing new content for policy to convey' for innovative research-based practice.
392 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EARLY CHILDHOOD LITERACY

Street (2003) argued that 'The next stage increasingly affects the ways in which
of work in this area is to move beyond teachers can teach. No matter how good the
[these] theoretical critiques and to develop research and scholarship on the develop-
positive proposals for interventions in ment of reading is, it will not be realised in
teaching, curriculum, measurement crite- practice unless educational policy-making
ria, and teacher education in both the attends to the full range ofrigorous schol-
formal and informal sectors, based upon arly work.
these principles' (2003: 82). An example of
such practice from Queensland Australia
was given by Street, but since that paper REFERENCES
was written, govemment policy in Australia
appears to have changed. Currently, there Allington, R. (201 0). 'Recent federal education policy in
are movements away from the theory of the United States', in D. Wyse, R. Andrews, and
new literacy studies as an influence on J. Hoffman (ed$, Ihe Routledge lnternational
Handbook of
English, Language and LiteraoJ
policy and practice (see Sawyer, 2010). This
Teaching. (pp.
496-507). London: Routledge.
is part of a continuing trend internationally, Australian Government DEST (Department of
seen particularly in the US, the UK, Australia Education Science and Training) (20051. Teaching
and New Zealand, to more instrumental or Reading. Report and Recommendations National
autonomous forms of literacy. Enquiry into the Teaching of Literacy. Bafton,
Australia: Department of Education, Science and
Over the past 20 years research evidence
Training.
has accumulated to show the important Berndt, R.S., Reggia, J.A., and Mitchum, C.C. (1987).
role of phonological awareness in learn- 'Empirically derived probabilities for grapheme-
ing to read. One of the implications of this to-phoneme conespondences in English'. Behavior
research has been greater emphasis on the Research Methods, lnstruments, & Computers,
19:1-9.
teaching of phonics as part of early read-
Bissex, G. (1980). 6nys at Wrk: A Child Learns to Read
ing teaching. It has also been theorised that
and Write. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
this emphasis in teaching should differ Press.
according to the language that is being Bradley, L, and Bryant, P.E. (1983). 'Categorising
learned. Supporting children's develop- sounds and learning to read: A causal connection',
ment of phonological awareness, and Nature, 310'. 419-21.
Brown, G.D.A. and Deavers, R.P. (1999). 'Units of
teaching them about the alphabetic code,
analysis in nonword reading: Evidence from children
is one important part of reading teaching. and adults', /ourn al of Experimental Child Psychology,
However, teachers have to engage with a 73(31:208a2.
number of other important aspects if they Bryant, P.E., Bradley, 1., Maclean, M., and Crossland, J.

are to support children's development as (1989). 'Nursery rhymes, phonological skills and

readers most effectively. One of these reading', Journal of Child Language, 16: 407 -28.
Camilli, G., Vargas, S., and Yurecko, M. (2003).
aspects concerns texts, and the contexts 'Teaching children to read: The fragile link between
and ways in which children engage with science and federal education policy', Education
texts. But the teaching of reading also Policy Analysis Archives, 11(15). Retrieved 1 March
has to be understood in the wider peda- 2006, from http://epaa.asu.edu/epaa/v1 1 n1 5/
gogical context that requires the teaching Chukovsky, K. (1963). from Two fo Firze, Berkeley, CA:
University of California Press.
of English, language and literacy. The Cummins, J. (2007). 'Pedagogies for the poor?
pedagogical context is subject to influ- Realigning reading instruction for low-income stu-
ences from research but also from the dents with scientifically based reading instruction',
political domain of educational policy that Ed u cati ona I Resea rcher, 36(9): 56+7 2.
EARLY READING DEVELOPMENT 393

Echols, C. (1996). 'A role for stress in early speech attainment: A Seven Year Longitudinal Study
segmentation', in J.L. Morgan and K. Demuth (eds), Retrieved 10 December, 2006, from http://v'nruw.
Bootstrapping from Speech to Grammar in Early scotland.gov. uUResourcei Doc/36496/0023 582.pdf
Acquisition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Kershner, R. and Howard, J. (2006). The Psychology
Associates. pp. 1 51-70. of Education Review, 30(2): 1-60.
Ellis, S. (2007). 'Policy and research: Lessons from the Landerl, K. (2000). 'lnfluences of orthographic consist-
Clackmannanshire synthetic phonics initiative', ency and reading instruction on the development
lourna I of Early C hildhood Literaq, 7 (3): 28\-97 . of nonword reading skills', European lournal of
Ellis, N.C. and Hooper, A.M. (2001). 'Why learning to Psychology of Education, 15: 239-57 .
read is easier in Welsh than in English: 0rthographic Lanksheare, C. and Knobel, M. (2006). New Literacies:
transparency effects evinced with frequency-matched Everyday Practices and Classroom Learning.
tests' . A p p I i e d P sy ch o I i n g u i sti cs, 22: 57 1 -99. Maidenhead: 0pen University Press.
Frith, U., Wimmer, H., and Landerl, K. (1998). Levy, R. (2009). 'Children's perceptions of reading and
'Differences in
phonological recoding in German- the use of reading scheme texts', Cambridge Journal
and English-speaking children', Scientific Studies of of Education, 39(3): 36 l-77.
Reading,2(1):31-54. Lewis, M. and Ellis, S, (eds). (2006). Phonics: Practice
Goody, J., and Watt, l. (1963). The consequences of Research and Policy. London: Paul Chapman
literacy', Comparative Studies in Society and History, Publishing.
5(3): 30a-a5. Liberman, 1.Y., Shankweiler, D., Fischer, F.W., and
Goswami, U., Gombert, J.E., and de Banera, L.F. Carter, B. (1974). 'Explicit syllable and phoneme
(1998).'Children's orthographic representations segmentation in the young child', lournal of
and linguistic transparency: Nonsense word read- Experimental Child Psychology, 1 8: 201-1 2.
ing in English, French, and Spanish', Applied Marsh, .1. (2003). 'One way traffic? Connections
Psycholinguistics, 1 9(1 ): 1 9-52. between literacy practices at home and in the
Goswami, U., Ziegler, J.C., Dalton, 1., and Schneider, nursery', British Educational Research Journal, 29(3)
W. (2001). 'Pseudohomophone effects and phono- 370-82.
logical recoding procedures in reading development Meek, M. (1988). Hovv Texts Teach what Readers
in English and German', Journal of Memory & learn. Stroud: Thimble Press.
La n g u age, 45(4): 64844. Michael, l. (1984). 'Early evidence for whole
Goswami, U., Ziegler, J.C., Dalton, 1., and Schneider, word methods', in G. Brooks and A.K. Pugh (eds),
W. (2003). 'Nonword reading across orthographies: Studies in the History of Reading. Reading:
How flexible is the choice of reading units?' Applied Centre for the Teaching of Reading, University of
Psycholinguistics, 24: 23547 . Reading.
Gouch, K. and Lambirth, A. (2008). Understanding Moss, G. and Huxford, L. (2007). 'Exploring literacy
Phonics and the Teaching of Reading: Critical policy making from the inside out', in L. Saunders
Perspectives. Maidenhead: McGraw-Hill/0pen (ed.), Educational Research and Policy-Making:
University Press. kploring the Border Country between Research and
Hanley, J.R., Masterson, J., Spencer, 1.H., and Evans, D. Poliq. London: Routledge.
(2004). 'How long do the advantages of learning a National lnstitute of Child Health and Human
transparent orthography last? An investigation of Development (NICHD). (2000). Report of the
the reading skills and reading impairment of Welsh National Reading Panel. Teaching Children to Read:
children at 10 years of age', Quarterly lournal of An Evidence-Based Assessment of the Scientific
Experimental Psychology, 57(81: 1 393-41 0. Research Literature on Reading and its lmplications
Hanis, R. (1986). Ihe Origin of Writing. London: for Reading lnstruction: Reports of the Subgroups.
Duckworth. (NlH Publication no. 00-4754). Washington, DC:
Harste, J.C., Woodward, V.A., and Burke, C.L. (1984). US Government Printing Office.
Language Stories and Literacy Lessons. Portsmouth, New London Group (1996). 'A pedagogy of multilitera-
NH: Heinemann Educational Books. cies: Designing social futures', Hantard Educational
Ho, C.S.-H. and Bryant, P. (1997). 'Phonological skills Reyrew, 66(1): 60-93.
are important in learning to read Chinese', Perry, P., Amadeo, C., Fletcher,M., and Walker, E.
Developmental Psychology, 33:946-5 1 . (2010). lnstind or Reason: How Education Polky is
Johnston, R. and Watson, J. (2005). The Effects of Made and How we Might Make it Better. Reading:
Synthetic Phonics Teaching of Reading and Spelling CfBT Education Trust.
394 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EARLY CHILDHOOD LITERACY

Port, R. (2007). 'How are words stored in memory? Wells, G. (1986). Ihe Meaning Makers: Children
Beyond phones and phonemes', New ldeas in Learning Language and Using Language to Learn.
Psychology, 25: 1 43-7 0. London: Hodder and Stoughton.
Rose, J. (2006). lndependent Review of the Teaching of Wigfield, A. and Guthrie, J, (1997). 'Relations of
Early Reading. Nottingham: DfES Publications. children's motivation for reading to the amount and
Rosenblatt, L, (1985). 'Viewpoints: Transaction versus breadth of their reading', lournal of Educational
interaction: A terminological rescue operation', Psychology, 89(3): 420-32.
Research in the Teaching of English, 19(l): Wyse, D. (2010). 'Contextualised phonics teaching',
96-1 07. in K. Hall, U. Goswami, C. Harrison, S. Ellis, and
Sawyer,W. (2010). 'English teaching in Australia J. Soler (eds), lnterdisciplinary Perspectives on
and New Zealand', in D. Wyse, R. Andrews, and Learning to Read: Culture, Cognition and Pedagogy.
J. Hoffman (eds), Ihe Routledge lnternational London: Routledge.
Handbook of English, Language and Literacy Wyse, D. (201 l). 'The control of language or the lan-
Teaching, (pp.508-51 7). London: Routledge. guage of control? Primary teachers' knowledge in
Aro, M., and Erskine, J.M. (2003).
Seymour, P.H.K., the context of policy', in S. Ellis and E. McCarthy
'Foundation literacy acquisition in European (eds), Applied Linguistics and the Primary School
orthographies', British Journal of Psychology, 94: Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
143-74. Wyse, D. and Goswami, U. (2008). 'Synthetic phonics
Share, D.L. (1995). 'Phonological recoding and self- and the teaching of reading', British Educational
teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition', Research Journal 3a(6): 691-710.
Cognition, 55(2): 1 51-218. Wyse, D. and Styles, M. (2007). 'Synthetic phonics and
Spencer, L.H. and Hanley, j,R. (2003). 'EffeAs of ortho- the teaching of reading: The debate sunounding
graphic transparency on reading and phoneme England's " Rose report"', Literacy, 4 1 ( 1 ): 3 5-42.
awareness in children learning to read in Wales', Wyse, D., Andrews, R., and Hoffman, l. (eds). (2010).
British Journal of Psychology,94(1): 1-28. The Routledge lnternational Handbook of English,
Street, B. (2003). 'What's "new" in new literacy Language, and Literacy Teaching. London:
studies? Critical approaches to literacy in theory and Routledge.
practice', Cunent lssues in Conparative Education, Ziegler, J.C. and Goswami, U. (2005).'Reading acquisi
5(2): 1-14. tion, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading
Treiman, R. and Zukowski, A. (1991). 'Levels of phono- across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size
logical awareness', in S. Brady and D. Shankweiler theory' , Psychological Eulletin, I 3 I (1 ): 3-29.
(eds), Phonological Processes in Literacy. Hillsdale, Ziegler, J.C., Stone, G.0., and Jacobs, A.M. (1997).
NJ; Erlbaum. 'What is the pronunciation for -ough and the spell-
Walton. P.D,, Bowden, M.E., Kurtz, S.1., and Angus, M. ing for ui? A database for computing feedforward
(2001).'Evaluation of a rime-based reading program and feedback consistency in English', Behavior
with Shuswap and Heiltsuk First Nations prereaders', Research Methods, lnstruments & Computerc, 29(4):
Reading & Writing, 14:22944. 600-1 8.

You might also like