You are on page 1of 6

Seizure 28 (2015) 26–31

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Seizure
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/yseiz

Menstrual cycle corrupts reliable and valid assessment of language


dominance: Consequences for presurgical evaluation of patients
with epilepsy§
Christoph Helmstaedter *, Christiane Jockwitz, Juri-Alexander Witt
Department of Epileptology, University of Bonn, Bonn, Germany

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Article history: Purpose: Functional transcranial Doppler sonography (fTCD) is a valid and non-invasive tool for
Received 9 October 2014 determining language dominance, e.g. in the context of presurgical evaluations. Beyond this, fTCD might
Received in revised form 29 January 2015 be an ideal tool to study dynamics in language dominance over time. However, an essential prerequisite
Accepted 10 February 2015
would be a high test–retest reliability. This was addressed in the present study.
Methods: Test–retest reliability of hemispheric hemodynamics during open speech was determined in
Keywords: 11 male and 11 female healthy volunteers using the Animation Description Paradigm. Expressive
Functional transcranial Doppler sonography
language dominance was assessed weekly over an interval of 4–5 weeks.
Language dominance
Wada test
Results: Internal consistency of the four measurements was excellent (split-half reliability 0.85–0.95),
Gender but test–retest reliability of the lateralization index was poor to moderate (rtt = 0.37–0.74). Controlling
Menstrual cycle for gender, test–retest reliabilities were better in men (rtt = 0.67–0.78) as compared to women
Stability (rtt = 0.04–0.70). When arranging the assessments in women around day one of menstruation – all were
on contraceptives – a significant shift from left hemisphere dominance toward bilaterality (t = 2.2
p = 0.04) was evident around menstruation with significant reversal afterwards (t = 3.4 p = 0.005).
Conclusion: A high intraindividual variability of language dominance patterns is indicated in women
when assessed repeatedly by fTCD. Menstrual cycle appeared to be the source of inconsistency. The
finding challenges the use of non-deactivating methods for language dominance assessment in epilepsy.
Support for this is demonstrated with a female patient with epilepsy in whom language dominance
assessed by repeated fMRI and fTCD varied concordantly with cycle but not so the repeated
intracarotidal amobarbital test.
ß 2015 British Epilepsy Association. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction is injected via a femoral catheter which terminates within the left/
right arteria carotis interna. Because of its invasiveness, the IAT is
In the context of epilepsy surgery the intracarotidal amobarbi- neither suited for research questions beyond its strict clinical
tal test (IAT; also Wada test [1]) still represents the gold standard indication, nor for routinely performed follow-up assessments.
for determination of the language dominant hemisphere [2–4]. Repeated measurement would for example be appreciated to
This procedure, however, is invasive and today it appears mainly answer questions regarding changes in language dominance along
indicated if brain surgery may directly affect suggested eloquent with language development or processes of functional recovery and
cortex subserving language function. During the IAT a barbiturate plasticity in the context of chronic or reversible cerebral pathology
(in epilepsy: lesions, surgery vs. seizures, interictal activity).
Among the non-invasive methods discussed as alternatives for
§
One of the authors of this paper is a member of the current editorial team of the IAT functional MRI (fMRI) in particular meets all the criteria for
Seizure. The supervision of the independent peer review process was undertaken utilization in clinical settings [5]. Another non-invasive alternative
and the decision about the publication of this manuscript were made by other to the IAT is the functional transcranial Doppler sonography (fTCD),
members of the editor.
which had been introduced by Aaslid and colleagues in 1982
* Corresponding author at: Department of Epileptology, University of Bonn,
Sigmund-Freud-Str. 25, 53105 Bonn, Germany.
[6]. FTCD like fMRI assesses task-dependent changes of cerebral
Tel.: +49 228 287 16108; fax: +49 228 287 90 16108. blood flow and is therefore an indirect measure of brain activation.
E-mail address: C.Helmstaedter@uni-bonn.de (C. Helmstaedter). During cognitive performance (e.g. word generation, picture

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.seizure.2015.02.010
1059-1311/ß 2015 British Epilepsy Association. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
C. Helmstaedter et al. / Seizure 28 (2015) 26–31 27

description), the increased need for energy (glucose and oxygen) in fluctuations in language dominance patterns as assessed by fMRI
the activated brain regions leads to increased regional blood flow using a semantic decision task [16]. In the study by Fernandez
that is accompanied by blood flow velocity changes within major there was greater right hemisphere activation during the luteal
cerebral arteries. As a result, change in neuronal activity can be phase. Similarly another fMRI study showed different fMRI
indirectly assessed by experimentally measuring changes in blood activation at the luteal phase and menstruation using a word
flow velocity along with different mental states. FTCD is non- generation task [17]. Therefore, we asked all women about the
invasive, inexpensive, and easily applied in both children and time of menstruation during the study phase.
adults. FTCD has proven to be a valid tool for the assessment of
language dominance and is increasingly used in the presurgical 2. Methods
evaluation of patients with epilepsy [7–10].
In a newer activation paradigm, the so called Animation 2.1. Participants
Description Paradigm, which was developed by Bishop et al. [11],
the subject is asked to watch brief cartoon clips which have to be Twelve male and 12 female healthy volunteers were recruited
described in a fixed time interval immediately after presentation. The to participate in this prospective longitudinal study. Past
description period is followed by a silent resting phase (see Fig. 1). neurological psychiatric diseases led to an exclusion from the
Different from the classical word generation paradigm, this task is study. An inclusion criterion for women was hormonal contra-
easy to perform and eligible for adults, children and disabled persons ceptives to eliminate fluctuations of dominance patterns in women
as well, there is no right or wrong, the resting intervals are short, and as described by Fernandez et al. [16]. Taking this nevertheless into
overt speech allows for direct monitoring of the requested behavioral consideration, all female participants had to indicate the first day
response. In a still ongoing validation study in patients with epilepsy of their menstruation. From the original sample of 24 participants,
the fTCD results obtained with this Animation Description Paradigm one subject had to be excluded because of insufficient quality of
are compared to those of the IAT and/or language fMRI. Preliminary the fTCD signal, probably due to a deficient temporal bone window.
results had shown perfect correspondence between fTCD and IAT Another subject was not able to attend all sessions because of
(100%, n = 10) and good concordance between fTCD and fMRI (89%; illness and therefore had to be excluded as well. This resulted in a
n = 27) and fMRI and IAT (90%; n = 10) [12]. total sample of 22 participants. The participantś chronological age
The present study was set up to determine the internal ranged from 20 to 55 years (mean: 27.3 years; SD: 7.3). Twenty-
consistency and test-retest reliability of fTCD lateralization indices three of the participants were right-handed and one participant
in healthy subjects. Following the reliability study applying the was left-handed. German was the first language for all subjects.
classical silent word generation paradigm [9] the a-priori The participants took part in the study on a voluntary basis and did
hypothesis was that the results should be stable over time. Due not receive any monetary compensation. Written informed
to its low demanding properties, we did not expect practice or consent was given in the first session by all participants.
habituation effects.
We did, however, control for the possibility of gender effects. In 2.2. Design and procedure
this regard there is an ongoing discussion that women may have
more bilateral language representation as compared to men All subjects attended four sessions (T1–T4). In women a fifth
[13,14]. Own evidence from IATs in patients with epilepsy session was added to have a session before during and after the
indicated that women might be more prone to have bilateral menstruation. The assessments were carried out within four/five
language than men [15]. Apart from this, it has been demonstrated weeks; every week at the same day (plus minus one day). We
in healthy subjects that menstrual cycle causes intraindividual employed the Animation Description paradigm which comprises

Fig. 1. Mean blood flow velocity changes within the left (red) and right (green) middle cerebral artery registered during the Animation Description paradigm (fTCD-LI:
+4.0  0.39).
28 C. Helmstaedter et al. / Seizure 28 (2015) 26–31

30 trials and takes 15 min to complete [11] (Fig. 1). Since we had An ANOVA did not reveal significant differences across all LIs for
found a higher concordance with language dominance results the factor gender (F = 0.18 p = 0.197).
derived from IAT and fMRI, we chose the classical resting phase Split-half reliability was calculated across all subjects for each
instead of the watching phase (as done by Bishop) as the baseline session (T1–T5) using the Spearman-Brown formula. The results
for evaluating speech activation. [12]. indicated reliability coefficients between 0.85 and 0.95 for the
At T1 demographic data and handedness were assessed. As individual sessions (Table 1).
already mentioned female participants were also asked to indicate The test–retest reliability results for all subjects are shown in
the first day of their menstruation along the test series. Table 2(a). Although the results reached significance for each
Test sessions were as follows: The participant was placed in combination, the coefficients were moderate (rtt = 0.37–0.74)
front of the computer. Then, the headset with the probes was rather than high. Table 2(b and c) shows the test-retest reliabilities
attached to the participants head. If needed, glasses were put on split up for gender. The results in men revealed high reliabilities
over the fixation device. After confirmation that each probe (rtt = 0.67–0.78) between all sessions, this being confirmed by
showed the characteristic envelope curve of the middle cerebral repeated measures ANOVA which did not reveal a significant
artery (MCA), the participant had to count from one to ten to verify impact of the factor time. (F = 0.05, p = 0.98). The men’s mean LIs
a stable signal during speech. Afterwards, the Animation Descrip- ranged from 1.4 to 1.78. (Fig. 2a)
tion paradigm was explained. The participant was asked to avoid In women, except for one combination (T2–T3), none of the
unnecessary movements to reduce artifacts induced by muscle test-retest coefficients reached statistical significance indicating
activation. Furthermore, the relevance of continuous speech insufficient reliability over time (Table 1). Repeated measures
production during the description phase was stressed. ANOVA with 5 tests revealed no statistical significance (F = 1.47
p = 0.22). Women showed mean LI’s in the range from 1.9 to 3.2.
2.3. Apparatus (Fig. 2b).
Following hints from the literature that changes of language
The blood flow of the left and the right MCA was measured by a dominance in women may follow hormonal variation across the
Doppler ultrasonography device (Doppler Box by DWL, Singen, menstrual cycle, the women’s session order was arranged around
Germany). Two 2-MHz ultrasound probes were attached to the the time of the reported menstruation. The session closest to time
fixation device. The stimuli were presented on a PC, using E-prime of the first day of menstruation was defined as well as one session
software (Psychology Software Tools). the week before and one week afterwards (Fig. 2c). Whether the
time point of assessment within the menstrual cycle has an
2.4. Data analysis influence on language lateralization was tested using paired
t-tests. The results indicated that the session around menstruation
The data collected from each session were analyzed using the differed significantly from the session before (t = 2.19; p = 0.047)
Average 1.85 software [18]. The first step was an automated and after (t = 3.383; p = 0.005), i.e. the lateralization index around
standardized post-processing of the blood flow data. Then the menstruation showed a reversible trend toward bilaterality). The
absolute blood flow velocities were normalized according to the
baseline and filtered for outliers in the non-physiological range. After
Table 1
averaging the relative blood flow velocities of all accepted epochs, a
Split-half reliability coefficients for each session.
mean relative blood flow velocity curve for the left and right artery as
well as the lateralization index were calculated. The lateralization Session 1 2 3 4 5
index (LI) describes the direction and degree of lateralization and its Reliability coefficient 0.92 0.85 0.95 0.90 0.86
calculation is based on the maximum difference in cerebral blood
flow velocity change between the left and right MCA during the
speech versus baseline phase [8]. A positive LI indicated left Table 2
hemisphere dominance, a negative LI right hemispheric dominance. Pearson’s reliability coefficients.
If the 95% confidence interval around a given LI includes zero, a
T1 T2 T3 T4 T5
bilateral language representation is indicated.
(a) All subjects
T1 – 0.45 0.41 0.37 –
2.5. Statistics (0.02) (0.03) (0.04)
T2 – 0.74 0.41 –
The statistical analyses across all subjects was performed across (<0.01) (0.02)
4 sessions using SPSS 19. Analyses separate for men and women T3 – 0.64 –
(<0.01)
took 4 versus 5 tests into consideration. Split-half reliability was
calculated to verify consistency across the up to 30 trials that (b) Men
underly the lateralization index. Test-retest reliabilities (Pearson T1 – 0.71 0.67 0.76 –
(0.01) (0.02) (0.01)
product-moment correlations) were calculated for all combina- T2 – 0.74 0.72 –
tions of assessments (T1–T5). (0.01) (0.01)
The within-subject stability was evaluated with repeated T3 – 0.78 –
measures ANOVA. Furthermore, repeated measures ANOVA with (0.01)
gender as between-subject factor and retests as within-subject (c) Women
factor was calculated. Afterwards, men and women were analyzed T1 0.27 0.14 0.04 0.08
separately. Finally, in women, menstruation was taken into (0.201) (0.333) (0.454) (0.797)
T2 0.70 0.24 0.18
account by using paired t-tests.
(0.006) (0.231) (0.589)
T3 0.50 0.27
3. Results (0.068) (0.421)
T4 0.22
Fig. 1 depicts a typical change in regional cerebral blood flow (0.615)

velocity that was initiated by the Animation Description paradigm. Correlation coefficients and p-values (one-tailed) in brackets.
C. Helmstaedter et al. / Seizure 28 (2015) 26–31 29

concordance between fTCD and IAT [12]. The original aim of this
study was the assessment of the reliability of expressive language
dominance results derived from fTCD, when applying the
Animation Description paradigm which allows for open speech.
Stability of fTCD results is a prerequisite for longitudinal follow-up
assessments.
Until now test-retest reliability of fTCD in children or adults has
mainly been demonstrated by the stability across two assessments
[19,20]. Only one study assessed language dominance not only
twice but at 10 different points over a 5 month period in a 9-year-
old child [9]. The lateralization indices were very stable.
In the present study, language dominance was assessed weekly
over 4–5 weeks. Test-retest reliability of the lateralization indices
across all subjects was poor to moderate with reliability
coefficients ranging from 0.37 to 0.74. Lack of reliability was
mainly caused by the great variability seen in women. In men
stable results with satisfactory to high reliability were obtained
(rtt = 0.67–0.78). This observation raised the question whether
fundamental differences between men and women led to this
difference and whether such a measure can really be used as IAT
replacement in patients with epilepsy.
As indicated in the introduction, we took into consideration that
women might show different language lateralization than men.
Averaging the lateralization index across all sessions both groups
were left dominant. As discussed in the literature the sex difference
may be less an absolute one than one which depends on the time of
testing [14,21,22]. Language dominance as a function of menstrual
cycle was not the core issue of this study, but dominance changes
along the menstrual cycle obviously need to be discussed in order
to understand the results obtained here. We had simply asked for
the time of menstruation and did neither assess hormones, nor did
we intend to contrast the luteal phase with menstruation. Post hoc
arrangement of the women’s assessments in relation to the
assessment closest to day one of the menstruation resulted in a
distinctive pattern which indicated less marked left hemispheric
dominance during this time, i.e a shift toward bilaterality. Before
and after the assessment at menstruation the lateralization indices
clearly indicated left dominance. Previous studies [16,21] indicat-
ed more bilateral language and worse mental rotation in the
midluteal as compared to the menstruation phase. The gonadal
hormones estradiol and progesterone have been claimed to be
responsible for interhemispheric coupling and decoupling
Fig. 2. Lateralization indices (a) across all sessions (men and women) (b) across
[21,23]. During low levels of estradiol and progesterone the left
sessions broken down for men and women (c) the three sessions before, next to, and
after menstruation in women.
hemisphere seems to inhibit the right hemisphere. In contrast
during high levels of estradiol and progesterone the inhibition is
diminished leading to a more symmetric cerebral function.
remaining two sessions did not significantly differ from each other Different from these two studies, the women in our study were
(t = 0.811; p = 0.432). Additional reliability calculations showed all on contraceptives. According to theoretical considerations
that the sessions one week before and one week after the time of contraceptives eliminate hormonal peak levels due to the artificial
testing during menstruation showed a significant correlation harmonization [17]. This plus the vague timing of the assessments
(r = 0.45 p = 0.05) whereas the tests before and after menstruation in regard to the menstrual cycle may account for the different
did not correlate with the test during menstruation (pre/during observations.
r = 0.20, p = 0.244, during/post r = 0.19 p = 0.254). The design of this study clearly does not allow any in-depth
analysis of the observation that menstrual cycle corrupted the
4. Discussion reliability of language fTCD in women. In conjunction with the
studies just mentioned, four major questions arise:
In times when the IAT is discussed to be no longer justified for
routine assessment of patients undergoing epilepsy surgery, 1. First one would like to know the driving factor behind the
alternative non-invasive tools for language dominance assessment variation seen in women. Is it hormonal change or a biorhythm
gain increasing interest. At our department fTCD and fMRI are and why is the language activation shift not seen in all women?
currently used in patients with epilepsy and repeated fTCD seems 2. Second, and this is maybe more important, one would like to
particularly attractive for the monitoring of language development know the behavioral consequences of such shifts. In clinical
and processes of functional plasticity along with brain damage or settings (i.e. lesion driven) atypical language dominance has
surgery. Preliminary data from an ongoing cross-sectional valida- cognitive consequences and these are in part different for
tion study had shown good concordance of fTCD using the women and men. In left lateralized epilepsies, atypical language
Animation Description Paradigm with language fMRI, and perfect dominance and preservation of language often goes along with
30 C. Helmstaedter et al. / Seizure 28 (2015) 26–31

so called crowding or suppression of originally right hemi- dominance might be indicated. Whether a false lateralization
spheric functions. This can cause a pattern of material-specific into the left hemisphere is possible in genuine right dominant
cognitive impairments which is misleading in regard to the women would be interesting to see.
typical left vs. right and verbal vs. nonverbal dichotomy [24– 4. In clinical settings, i.e. in the rare conditions where it is still
26]. In this regard further research should address menstrual- indicated, the results of the IAT still guide surgical interventions
cycle dependent changes in language dominance parallel to and the indication of additional, in part invasive diagnostic
other cognitive domains, whilst taking into account hormone procedures (e.g. electrocortical stimulation via implanted
levels of naturally cycling women as well as contraceptive using electrodes or intraoperatively) [2,3,27]. Different from fTCD,
women. fMRI and most other non-deactivating procedures which derive
3. The third question regards the consequences for the interpreta- language dominance patterns from the relative contribution of
tion of findings from previous neuroimaging studies using single both interacting hemispheres, IAT and transcranial magnetic
non-deactivating assessments of language dominance. The stimulation (TMS) conclude language dominance from deacti-
present data indicate that dependent on which phases of the vation of the suggested language relevant hemisphere or brain
menstrual cycle are included, findings based on a single region, respectively. In this regard it would also be essential to
assessment in women as a group may sum up to more bilateral know whether results from deactivating procedures do fluctuate
language dominance patterns. In single cases even false right with menstrual cycle.

Fig. 3. Single case study of a patient with right frontal dysplasia who, along the menstrual cycle, demonstrated variation of language dominance when repeating non-
deactivating procedures (fTCD and fMRI: 1st test time left dominant, 2nd test time bilateral, 3rd test time left dominant) and right hemisphere involvement when repeating
deactivating (right sided) IAT (2nd test time: expressive and receptive, 3rd test time primarily expressive).
C. Helmstaedter et al. / Seizure 28 (2015) 26–31 31

In this respect a single case report may provide first evidence References
of the possibility of a principal and clinically highly relevant
[1] Wada J, Rasmussen T. Intracarotid injection of sodium amytal for the laterali-
difference between functional imaging and deactivating meth-
zation of cerebral speech dominance. 1960. J Neurosurg 2007;106:1117–33.
ods. A 28-year-old female patient suffered from chronic [2] Baxendale S, Thompson PJ, Duncan JS. The role of the Wada test in the surgical
uncontrolled epilepsy with complex partial and infrequent treatment of temporal lobe epilepsy: an international survey. Epilepsia
secondary generalized seizures originating from the right frontal 2008;49:715–20 [discussion 720–715].
[3] Haag A, Knake S, Hamer HM, Boesebeck F, Freitag H, Schulz R, et al. The Wada
lobe. MRI post-processing (MAP 07) revealed a suspicious right test in Austrian, Dutch, German, and Swiss epilepsy centers from 2000 to 2005:
frontal/insular lesion most likely a cortical dysplasia. Because of a review of 1421 procedures. Epilepsy Behav 2008;13:83–9.
the localization of the lesion in the vincinity of the homologue to [4] Helmstaedter C. The role of the Wada test in the surgical treatment of temporal
lobe epilepsy: an international perspective. Epilepsia 2008;49:720–2.
Boca’s area and because of aphasic symptoms during/after the [5] Abou-Khalil B. An update on determination of language dominance in screen-
seizure, the patient underwent fTCD for screening of language ing for epilepsy surgery: the Wada test and newer noninvasive alternatives.
dominance. The result indicated left hemispheric dominance for Epilepsia 2007;48:442–55.
[6] Aaslid R, Markwalder TM, Nornes H. Noninvasive transcranial Doppler ultra-
expressive language dominance. Since this is part of an ongoing sound recording of flow velocity in basal cerebral arteries. J Neurosurg
validation study, she also underwent language fMRI three days 1982;57:769–74.
later which also indicated left hemispheric dominance. It was [7] Lohmann H, Ringelstein EB, Knecht S. Functional transcranial Doppler sonog-
raphy. Front Neurol Neurosci 2006;21:251–60.
decided to go for surgery but not without an IAT for final
[8] Deppe M, Knecht S, Lohmann H, Ringelstein EB. A method for the
evidence. The patient came back three months later and automated assessment of temporal characteristics of functional hemispheric lat-
underwent a unilateral right IAT. Language performance when eralization by transcranial Doppler sonography. J Neuroimaging 2004;14:226–30.
[9] Knecht S, Deppe M, Ringelstein EB, Wirtz M, Lohmann H, Drager B, et al.
testing the isolated left hemisphere, however, revealed that she
Reproducibility of functional transcranial Doppler sonography in determining
was able to show only 38–40% of the baseline performance. This hemispheric language lateralization. Stroke 1998;29:1155–9.
was interpreted as evidence that the right hemisphere might be [10] Knake S, Haag A, Hamer HM, Dittmer C, Bien S, Oertel WH, et al. Language
involved in language. Since this was discordant with the imaging lateralization in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy: a comparison of func-
tional transcranial Doppler sonography and the Wada test. Neuroimage
data (fMRI and fTCD) these tests were repeated in the following 2003;19:1228–32.
two days. Most astonishingly both tests now indicated bilateral [11] Bishop DV, Watt H, Papadatou-Pastou M. An efficient and reliable method for
language dominance consistent with the IAT. Knowing from the measuring cerebral lateralization during speech with functional transcranial
Doppler ultrasound. Neuropsychologia 2009;47:587–90.
ongoing study on test–retest reliability, and having checked for [12] Witt J-A, Prömpler K, Maydych V, Helmstaedter C. Cross-validation of
the test dates in regard to the patient’s menstrual cycle (1st the Animation Description Paradigm applied to determine language
assessment during menstruation phase, 2nd assessment during dominance via functional transcranial Doppler sonography.In: International
Congress on Epilepsy, Brain and Mind. Prag.. Epilepsy & Behavior 2010;17:611.
luteal phase; no intake of hormonal contraceptives; cf. [16]), the [13] Sommer IE, Aleman A, Bouma A, Kahn RS. Do women really have more bilateral
question was raised whether we can trust the IAT. Moreover, language representation than men? A meta-analysis of functional imaging
what would the IAT have shown at the time of the first functional studies. Brain 2004;127:1845–52.
[14] Ortigue S, Thut G, Landis T, Michel CM. Time-resolved sex differences in
assessments that indicated typical left hemispheric dominance? language lateralization. Brain 2005;128:E28.
Therefore, the assessments were repeated at about the same [15] Kurthen M, Helmstaedter C, Elger CE, Linke DB. Sex differences in cerebral
time of the menstrual cycle when the first assessments had been language dominance in complex-partial epilepsy. Die Naturwissenschaften
1997;84:131–3.
conducted, i.e. during menstruation phase. Both functional
[16] Fernandez G, Weis S, Stoffel-Wagner B, Tendolkar I, Reuber M, Beyenburg S,
imaging data consistently indicated clear left dominance. et al. Menstrual cycle-dependent neural plasticity in the adult human brain is
However, the right-sided IAT for testing the isolated left hormone, task, and region specific. J Neurosci 2003;23:3790–5.
hemisphere, which was done one day later, once again indicated [17] Rumberg B, Baars A, Fiebach J, Ladd ME, Forsting M, Senf W, et al. Cycle and
gender-specific cerebral activation during a verb generation task using fMRI:
an atypical pattern with a participation of the right hemisphere comparison of women in different cycle phases, under oral contraception, and
in language function. At this time no expressive and only part of men. Neurosci Res 2010;66:366–71.
receptive functions were possible with the isolated left hemi- [18] Deppe M, Knecht S, Henningsen H, Ringelstein EB. AVERAGE: a Windows
program for automated analysis of event related cerebral blood flow. J
sphere. The intraindividual language dominance results are Neurosci Methods 1997;75:147–54.
illustrated in Fig. 3. [19] Stroobant N, Van Boxstael J, Vingerhoets G. Language lateralization in children A
In concluding the present study deactivation methods functional transcranial Doppler reliability study. J. Neurolinguistics 2011;24:14–24.
[20] Stroobant N, Vingerhoets G. Test–retest reliability of functional transcranial
seem to assess more basic and stable patterns of language Doppler ultrasonography. Ultrasound Med Biol 2001;27:509–14.
dominance, irrespective of menstrual cycle. Results from [21] Hausmann M, Slabbekoorn D, Van Goozen SH, Cohen-Kettenis PT, Gunturkun
activation tasks in contrast appear susceptible to dominance O. Sex hormones affect spatial abilities during the menstrual cycle. Behav
Neurosci 2000;114:1245–50.
changes along with menstrual cycle. For clinical use and also for [22] Hausmann M, Gunturkun O. Sex differences in functional cerebral asymme-
research questions, single measurements in women using tools tries in a repeated measures design. Brain Cogn 1999;41:263–75.
that solely assess brain activation patterns of the two interacting [23] Weis S, Hausmann M, Stoffers B, Vohn R, Kellermann T, Sturm W. Estradiol
modulates functional brain organization during the menstrual cycle: an
hemispheres are thus not sufficient to provide reliable
analysis of interhemispheric inhibition. J Neurosci 2008;28:13401–10.
results. The study hopefully initiates follow up evaluations, [24] Helmstaedter C, Brosch T, Kurthen M, Elger CE. The impact of sex and language
since it raised more questions than having given the expected dominance on material-specific memory before and after left temporal lobe
answers. surgery. Brain 2004;127:1518–25.
[25] Helmstaedter C, Kurthen M, Linke DB, Elger CE. Right hemisphere restitution
of language and memory functions in right hemisphere language-dominant
patients with left temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain 1994;117(Pt 4):729–37.
Conflict of interest statement [26] Helmstaedter C, Kurthen M, Elger CE. Sex differences in material-specific
cognitive functions related to language dominance: an intracarotid amobar-
bital study in left temporal lobe epilepsy. Laterality 1999;4:51–63.
None of the authors has any conflict of interest in regard to the [27] Helmstaedter C, Kurthen M. Validity of the WADA test. Epilepsy Behav
topic and the contents of this article. 2002;3:562–3.

You might also like