You are on page 1of 13

Journal of Family Psychology Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

2003, Vol. 17, No. 1, 147–159 0893-3200/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0893-3200.17.1.147

Mothers’ Scaffolding of Children’s Problem Solving: Establishing a


Foundation of Academic Self-Regulatory Competence
Carin Neitzel and Anne Dopkins Stright
Indiana University

The study examined relations between dimensions of mothers’ scaffolding and children’s
academic self-regulatory behaviors in school. Mothers and their preschool children (68
dyads) were visited in their homes the summer before the child entered kindergarten.
Mothers’ metacognitive content and manner of instruction, emotional support, and transfer of
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

responsibility were coded as mothers provided assistance to their children during 4 problem-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

solving tasks. Children’s self-regulatory behaviors were assessed the following school year.
Metacognitive content and manner of instruction were predictors of child behaviors related to
cognitive awareness and management: metacognitive talk, monitoring, and help seeking.
Emotional support and transfer of responsibility were related to children’s task persistence
and behavior control in school. Mothers’ scaffolding appears to lay the foundation for
children’s subsequent academic self-regulatory competence.

Children’s abilities to actively regulate their learning heavily on their parents for assistance when confronted with
have been credited for enhancing school performance (Feld- problems or tasks beyond their own abilities (Rogoff, 1990;
man, Martinez-Pons, & Shaham, 1995). Children who are Vygotsky, 1978). Parents scaffold their children’s problem-
good academic self-regulators have good metacognitive solving efforts until the child acquires or internalizes the
awareness, adjust cognitively and emotionally to the de- skills that have been demonstrated and is capable of man-
mands of school, exercise control over their classroom aging the task independently (Bruner, 1986). However, an
behavior, monitor their academic task performances, and explicit model of the function of these supportive behaviors
seek assistance when it is needed. However, not all children is needed (Bickhard, 1992).
are successful academic self-regulators. Some children may Scaffolding is defined as an instructional interaction that
fail to self-regulate because they do not have the metacog- is aimed at extending the child’s knowledge, reducing task
nitive tools to do so (Schraw, 1994). Some children may fail complexity, and transferring responsibility while providing
to self-regulate because they are unwilling to do so (Pintrich emotional support (Bruner, 1986). However, there are dif-
& Schrauben, 1992) or because they do not realize it is their ferences in the amounts and types of information parents
role or responsibility to be active agents in the learning provide their children (Sternberg, 1994), the degree to
process (Zimmerman, 1995). The knowledge, dispositions, which parents simplify tasks for their children (Rogoff,
and sense of responsibility children need to become self- 1990), the suitability of parents’ responses to their chil-
regulated learners in school may be established early during dren’s emotional needs (Grusec & Goodnow, 1994), the
parent– child interactions within the family. extent that parents encourage their children’s active cogni-
tive participation in the learning process (Sternberg, 1994),
Family: A Fundamental Foundation for Children’s and parents’ abilities to retain or relinquish their assistive
Development of Self-Regulation roles appropriately (Freund, 1990).
The quality of parent– child scaffolding interactions is
Children develop abilities to self-regulate from interac- jointly determined, influenced by children’s current cogni-
tions overseen by more competent social partners, in set- tive abilities (Wood, Bruner, & Ross, 1976) along with
tings that are abundant in communication and supportive parents’ perceptions of their children’s temperaments, par-
(Wertsch, 1985). Sociocultural theory suggests that the fam- ents’ personal adjustment and education, and parents’ atti-
ily is a social institution within which a culture of educa- tudes toward the problem context (Neitzel & Stright, 2001).
tional competence is born and nurtured (Cole, 1985). Until In sum, scaffolding may represent an artifact of the family,
children become proficient problem solvers, they rely reflecting the personalities of its members, resources, goals,
and values. In other words, scaffolding is the product of
numerous attributes of the family that have been linked to
children’s school outcomes previously in independent lines
Carin Neitzel and Anne Dopkins Stright, Department of Coun-
seling and Educational Psychology, Indiana University. of research. Differences in patterns of behavior across the
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to dimensions of scaffolding (cognitive support, emotional
Carin Neitzel, Department of Counseling and Educational Psy- support, and transfer of responsibility) may affect in com-
chology, Indiana University, 201 North Rose Avenue, Blooming- plex ways the likelihood of children developing and utiliz-
ton, Indiana 47405. E-mail: cneitzel@indiana.edu ing effective academic self-regulatory behaviors in school

147
148 NEITZEL AND STRIGHT

(Grolnick & Slowiaczek, 1994; Stright, Neitzel, Sears, & tion provided (Wertsch, 1985). Providing instruction in
Hoke-Sinex, 2001). small steps, with frequent review also may serve as a model
Although previous research has established a connection of task management strategies that can be used by the child
between different facets of parents’ scaffolding and chil- in the classroom (Rogoff, Mistry, Goncu, & Mosier, 1993)
dren’s general competencies in self-regulation (Grolnick & and, in addition, may determine whether children view
Ryan, 1989; Pianta, Smith, & Reeve, 1991; Stright et al., others as resources for information that will have value to
2001), a thorough study that simultaneously examines the them. In each of these ways, the content and manner of
contributions of all of the major aspects of scaffolding to the parents’ instruction may provide a foundation of metacog-
prediction of children’s specific academic self-regulatory nitive competence children need in order to regulate their
behaviors is lacking. It is hypothesized that parents’ content own learning, to discuss their own thinking, monitor
and manner of instruction may furnish information about progress, and seek help in the classroom.
strategies that the child needs for managing his or her own
learning (monitoring and help seeking), parents’ emotional Parents’ Emotional Support of Children’s
support may affect the child’s dispositions toward learning
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Problem-Solving Efforts
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

such as willingness to put forth effort and persistence, and


parents’ level of control and recruitment of children’s active Children’s academic self-regulatory behaviors are con-
involvement in the problem-solving process may foster the tingent on motivational as well as metacognitive factors
child’s understanding of self as the responsible agent for (Zimmerman, 1995). According to Bandura’s model of
cognitive and behavior control in the classroom. children’s self-regulation (Bandura, 1986), self-regulated
behavior is the product of the balance between a child’s
Parents’ Cognitive Support: Metacognitive Content “skill and will” (Schunk, 1995). It is expected that parents’
and Manner of Instruction emotional support of their children’s problem-solving ef-
forts plays an important role in relation to all self-regulatory
Children’s metacognitive development has been associ- behaviors even after the content and manner of parents’
ated with academic self-regulation (Flavell, 1987). Parents’ instruction are taken into consideration. Cognitive manage-
instruction during scaffolded problem-solving interactions ment is an effortful process, and parents’ emotional support
is a means by which metacognitive knowledge is shared and may cultivate the motivational dispositions needed to secure
information about strategies and general task management children’s self-regulated behavior in the classroom (Eccles,
routines is conveyed (Vygotsky, 1978). Therefore, parents’ Wigfield, & Schiefele, 1998; Ryan & Pintrich, 1997). Par-
abilities to dispense good metacognitive information as they ents’ emotional support is instrumental in the development
provide instructional assistance may influence their chil- of children’s sense of personal competence and control
dren’s metacognitive development and subsequently chil- (Ginsburg & Bronstein, 1993), and children’s sense of per-
dren’s use of cognitive management strategies such as mon- sonal competence and control is related to their engagement
itoring progress and seeking assistance in school. in self-regulated behaviors (Brown, 1988; Schunk, 1995).
Parents’ provision of metacognitive information to their Emotional support provided by parents has been related
children during problem-solving interactions is related to directly to children’s academic self-regulation (Feldman,
children’s metacognitive talk in their third-grade classrooms Martinez-Pons, & Shaham, 1995). Specifically, Stright et al.
(Stright et al., 2001). Parents who provide more metacog- (2001) found that parents’ emotional support during parent–
nitive information have children who are more likely to child problem-solving interactions was related to children’s
discuss how an answer was reached and why they approach metacognitive talk and monitoring in their third-grade class-
a problem a certain way or to make evaluative comments rooms. Parents who provided encouragement and displayed
about their performance. In addition, children who have positive attitudes about their children’s abilities to complete
information about strategies and about the general nature of difficult problem-solving tasks had children who discussed
academic tasks are best equipped to cognitively manage their thinking in the classroom. Even if parents provided
their learning (Schraw, 1994). Specifically, parents who high-quality instruction rich in metacognitive content, if
provide metacognitive information during problem-solving they did not emotionally support their children during prob-
interactions have children who check their work, recognize lem solving, then their children were less likely to monitor
when they make an error and self-correct, and adjust their their progress in the classroom. However, in the same study,
strategy use in school (Stright et al., 2001). emotional support was not related uniquely to children’s
However, parents’ provision of metacognitive informa- help seeking or active participation in their classrooms. It is
tion does not effectively predict children’s metacognitive possible that the absence of encouragement and praise may
talk and monitoring in school if parents do not reduce the not necessarily dissuade children from self-regulated behav-
cognitive demands of the task for their children by breaking ior as long as their efforts have not been rejected, criticized,
down the task and presenting information in small steps, or otherwise responded to negatively. For this reason, the
reviewing the steps, and discussing progress in relation to current study will examine patterns of inappropriate re-
the overall task goal (Stright et al., 2001). Parents’ abilities sponses to children’s emotional needs during scaffolded
to deliver instruction in a manner sensitive to their chil- interactions as well as positive aspects of emotional support.
dren’s cognitive needs may determine whether children The overall quality of parents’ emotional support may be
understand and can make use of the metacognitive informa- related to children’s willingness to put forth effort and
MOTHERS’ SCAFFOLDING AND CHILDREN’S SELF-REGULATION 149

therefore may influence children’s cognitive management, their own learning. The content and manner of mothers’
task persistence, and behavior control in the classroom. instruction may influence whether children are equipped for
the complex metacognitive demands of cognitive manage-
Parents’ Support of Children’s Autonomy ment (monitoring and help-seeking), and mothers’ support
of their children’s efforts and control or advocacy of their
Parents’ scaffolding is anticipated to culminate in transfer children’s autonomy may influence whether children invest
of responsibility as children develop abilities to function effort in the learning process and act as active agents in their
independently in the problem-solving situation. Parents own cognitive and behavior control in school.
must gauge when it is appropriate to resign their assistive
roles and hand over responsibility to their children. Parents Method
may support their children’s transition to autonomous
problem-solving through their timely withdrawal of assis- Participants
tance or through their choice of instructional techniques.
Parents who use prompts, questions, and hints to assist their Sixty-eight family dyads consisting of preschool children and
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

children rather than simply stating the answer, directing their mothers participated in this study. All of the families were
their children’s actions, or performing the task for their from a Midwest university city and its neighboring communities.
children may be encouraging their children to assume an Participants were recruited via letters sent home through the com-
munity’s preschools during the summer prior to the children be-
active role in the learning process.
ginning kindergarten. Four of the mothers had less than a high
Parents’ strategies for controlling interactions and en- school education, 16 mothers had a high school education, 15 had
couraging their children’s autonomy in the problem-solving some college, 15 had college degrees, and 18 mothers had some
process have been related to children’s behavior control in graduate school education. Fifty-five mothers were married and 13
their classrooms (Pianta et al., 1991) and academic achieve- of the mothers were the single head of household. Fifty-two of the
ment in school (Grolnick & Ryan, 1989). Parents’ patterns mothers were employed. Thirty-two of the children were male and
of control or support of their children’s autonomy influence 36 were female. The children ranged in age from 4.75 to 6.75
the child’s view of self as a responsible agent for behavior years; the mean age was 5.6 years. Most of the children were
control (Hess, Holloway, Dickson, & Price, 1984). White (60 White, 3 African-American, 2 Hispanic, 1 Asian-
The adequacy of parents’ transfer of responsibility for American, 2 mixed race).
problem-solving interactions to their children may have
implications for children’s cognitive management and task Procedure
persistence as well. High levels of control may lead children
to view themselves as passive recipients rather than active Each mother– child dyad was visited in their home for approx-
participants in the learning process; although they may be imately an hour and a half during the summer prior to the child’s
capable of self-regulation, they may fail to do so because entrance into kindergarten. Mothers’ scaffolding behaviors were
assessed through observations of the mother– child pair while the
they do not realize it is their responsibility. Therefore, mother provided instructions and any assistance she believed nec-
parents’ patterns of control may render good cognitive and essary as her child completed four difficult problem-solving tasks.
emotional support ineffective. This study examines whether The four tasks were designed by the study authors to create a
parents’ proficiency in transferring responsibility for prob- context in which the mother would have to cognitively and emo-
lem solving to their children is important beyond their tionally support her child’s efforts in a potentially frustrating
cognitive and emotional support of their children. situation. Because previous studies have reported significant vari-
In summary, parent– child joint problem-solving interac- ation in parents’ scaffolding behaviors across multiple tasks
tions within the family provide a context for early develop- (Gonzalez, 1996), a variety of tasks were used to ensure that
ment that may affect significantly the formulation of the mothers’ scaffolding behaviors were assessed thoroughly. Prior to
child’s knowledge base, dispositions, and habits (Vygotsky, beginning each task, the mother was given directions and an
opportunity to ask questions. The mother then explained the task
1978) and consequently the child’s academic self-regu- and provided assistance as her child attempted each task for 5 min.
lation. Because the dimensions of scaffolding do not occur For the first task, a revision of Green’s (1995) referential com-
in isolation, all are examined in a hierarchical model of the munication task, the child was asked to leave the room and the
effects of scaffolding on children’s self-regulation. It is mother was shown a picture of a cartoon-like person made from
hypothesized that it may not be enough for mothers to simple geometric shapes of different colors. The mother was asked
provide high-quality metacognitive information; the manner to record on a tape recorder the directions necessary for her child
of instruction may moderate the effects of the content of to reproduce the same picture from a set of geometric shapes
instruction. In addition, even if the parent provides good identical to those used to create the original picture. When the
metacognitive instruction communicated in a developmen- recording was done, the child was called back into the room. The
tally appropriate manner, the child may not be motivated to child was asked to follow the tape recorded instructions to make
the picture. None of the participating children was able to success-
use his or her abilities in school if the child has not been fully complete the task from the recorded instructions only. At the
emotionally supported in his or her efforts within the home. conclusion of the tape or after 5 min, the mother provided help and
Finally, in the last step of the model, even mothers’ cogni- instruction for an additional 5 min.
tive and emotional support may not be sufficient for solic- A traditional problem-solving task, a block puzzle, was used for
iting children’s self-regulated behavior if mothers have not the second task. The child was asked to build a tower using a set
encouraged their children to become active participants in of nine animal blocks. The child was told that only one animal
150 NEITZEL AND STRIGHT

could be standing on the table; all the other animal blocks had to overall manner of instruction score was created by averaging the
be stacked in a tower on top of the block chosen to be on the two ratings. A high composite score indicated that the mother
bottom. The different sizes and shapes of the blocks resulted in provided meaningful instruction in manageable steps with consis-
different weights for each block that needed to be taken into tent review of the steps.
consideration when stacking them. For the third task, an open- Two aspects of emotional support were coded. Rejection was
ended planning task, the child was asked to plan a birthday party assessed by rating how rejecting the mother was of the child’s
for a stuffed bear. The mother assisted as the child decided whom problem-solving efforts. Rejection included criticism, disapproval,
a bear might like to invite, a game a bear might like to play, a gift dismissal of the child’s efforts, or other negative or inappropriate
a bear would like, and the kind of cake a bear would like. For the reactions to the child (“I knew you wouldn’t be able to do it
final task, a creative language task, the child was asked to make up because you never listen!” “See, you’re not even trying! Your
a story using six puppets: a firefighter, a police officer, a wizard, a picture doesn’t look anything like this!”). Encouragement was
nurse, a doctor, and a baker. assessed by rating the supportive comments that mothers gave
During each problem-solving task, a trained observer noted and their children such as words of encouragement and positive com-
tallied behaviors, and at the conclusion of each problem-solving ments about the task or the child’s ability to do the task. Because
interaction made final overall ratings of each scaffolding behavior. the two ratings were moderately correlated (r ⫽ .65), an overall
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Two educational psychology doctoral students were trained to use score of emotional support was created by reverse coding and
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

the scaffolding coding system over a 1-month period. At the averaging the two ratings. A high score indicated that the mother
conclusion of the visit, the parents and children were given a provided high levels of positive support to the child.
modest monetary remuneration for their participation in the study. Two aspects of transfer of responsibility were coded. The moth-
In order to assess the children’s self-regulation of their learning er’s overcontrol of the child was assessed by rating the degree to
at school, children were observed in their kindergarten classrooms which the mother exercised control over her child’s problem-
for 48 min (24 min during teacher-directed activities, 24 min solving attempts beyond what appeared necessary for the child to
during independent work). Two educational psychology doctoral do the task. The degree to which the mother encouraged her child’s
students were trained to use the coding system over a 1-month active cognitive involvement in the problem-solving task was the
period. The observer (blind to the data from the home visits and the second aspect of the mother’s transfer of responsibility assessed.
hypotheses of the study) coded each child’s self-regulatory behav- Mothers who used prompts, questions, and hints while providing
iors after observing for a 4-min interval. No more than three 4-min assistance to their child rather than simply stating the answer,
intervals of data were collected for a child during one classroom directing their child’s actions, or doing the task for the child were
visit. Consequently, data were collected for each child over at least credited with encouraging the child to take an active role cogni-
4 classroom visits (2 during the fall semester, 2 during the spring tively (“Winnie-the-Pooh is a bear too. What presents does he like?
semester). So, what presents do you think Buddy Bear might like?” “If the
baker ate too much cake and is sick, what should happen next?
What do we do when you get sick?”). Because the two ratings were
Measures moderately correlated (r ⫽ .70), an overall score of transfer of
responsibility was created by reverse coding and averaging the two
Mother’s Scaffolding Behaviors ratings. A high score indicated that the mother relinquished control
of the task when appropriate and encouraged the child’s autonomy.
The mother’s scaffolding behaviors were assessed with an ob- To assess intercoder agreement, a second coder was used for
servational coding system developed by the first author on the 24% of the interactions spaced out equally across the home visits
basis of the work of Wood et al. (1976) and Rogoff (1990).1 (n ⫽ 64: 16 home visits ⫻ 4 tasks). The two coders observed
Aspects of the mother’s cognitive support, emotional support, and together and independently coded the mother’s scaffolding behav-
transfer of responsibility were coded. Each aspect of mother’s iors after each of the four problem-solving tasks. Pearson correla-
scaffolding was rated on a 5-point scale ranging from low (1) to tions between the two coders for the seven codes ranged from .81
high (5). to .96 and a version of Cohen’s kappa appropriate for ratings (K. J.
Two aspects of cognitive support were coded. The first aspect of Berry & Mielke, 1988) ranged from .81 to .90.
cognitive support, metacognitive information, was assessed with a
rating of the degree to which the mother provided metacognitive
information to her child that made salient the thinking behind the Children’s Academic Self-Regulatory Behaviors
problem-solving process. Any information that (a) covered ideas
for task management techniques or strategies, (b) served to ad- An observational coding system using frequency counts and
vance the child’s understanding of how the task worked, or (c) 3-point ratings was created by the first author on the basis of the
provided a rationale for the use of a particular strategy was counted coding system used by Stright et al. (2001) to assess the child’s
as metacognitive information. An example is, “If only one block academic self-regulatory behaviors in the classroom (see Footnote
can touch the table then you have to start with the biggest one on 1). Five areas of self regulation were assessed: metacognitive talk,
the bottom so it can hold all the others.” A second aspect of task persistence, behavior self-control, monitoring progress, and
cognitive support, mother’s manner of instruction, was coded seeking assistance.
using two 5-point ratings. First, a rating was made of the mother’s First, metacognitive talk was assessed by the use of a frequency
regulating task difficulty, the degree to which the mother gave count of instances when the child discussed his or her own think-
instructions in small, manageable steps (“Let’s plan one part of the ing (e.g., shared information about how an answer was reached or
party at a time. First, . . . ”). Second, a rating was made of the why he or she approached a problem a certain way or made
extent to which the mother reviewed the steps of the task and comments about his or her performance). Second, a rating was
discussed progress in relation to the overall goal of the task (“OK,
you finished his hat; now, let’s make his face. Remember, it should
1
look like a silly clown looking sideways when we are all done”). Information about the coding system is available from the
Because the two ratings were moderately correlated (r ⫽ .74), an authors on request.
MOTHERS’ SCAFFOLDING AND CHILDREN’S SELF-REGULATION 151

made of the child’s task persistence during class activities. A high of the mothers’ instructional behaviors were quite different
rating (3) was made if the child sustained effort and maintained depending on the task with ratings that spanned nearly the
interest in the task throughout most of the observation interval. A full range of the scale for all of the instructional behaviors
low rating (1) was made if the child was off-task and unenthusi- (Table 1). One-sample t tests indicated that the ranges in
astic for the majority of the observation interval. Third, a rating
was made of the child’s lack of behavior self-control. A high rating mothers’ behaviors across the four tasks were statistically
(3) was given when the child consistently did not follow instruc- significantly different from zero for each of the instructional
tions, disrupted other students who were trying to work, inter- behaviors (Table 1).
rupted the teacher, or failed to adhere to class routines. Fourth, the The alpha coefficient was used to estimate the stability of
child’s monitoring progress on classwork was assessed by the use mothers’ instructional behaviors across the four tasks to
of a frequency count of the child’s checking work, detecting errors, determine whether the four tasks consistently separated
and correcting work. Finally, the child’s seeking help from the mothers who were “high” from mothers who were “low” on
teacher was assessed with a frequency count. each instructional behavior. Despite the variability in her
To assess the intercoder agreement, a second observer coded
own instructional behaviors across the four problem-solving
20% of the classroom observations. The coders observed together
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

and independently coded 155 of the 816 four-minute intervals of tasks, each mother’s instructional behaviors across tasks
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

data spaced out equally across the school year. For the two codes were consistent relative to the other mothers. Cronbach’s
assessed on a 3-point rating scale, Pearson’s correlations between alpha for scores on the four tasks was .78 for metacognitive
the two coders were .90 and .94 and a version of Cohen’s kappa for information, .80 for regulating task difficulty, .78 for mon-
ratings (K. J. Berry & Mielke, 1988) was .88 and .90. For the codes itoring and review of the task, .82 for encouragement, .70
assessed with frequency counts, a variation of Cohen’s kappa for for rejection of the child’s problem-solving efforts, .73 for
frequency data (Scott & Hattfield, 1985) ranged from .83 to .96. overcontrol of the problem-solving interaction, and .77 for
recruiting the child’s active role in the problem-solving
Results process.
For each of the tasks, the correlations and part correla-
Preliminary Analyses tions between the mothers’ instructional behaviors and chil-
Prior to the main analyses, each of the variables was dren’s classroom behaviors were examined to determine
examined to determine whether the assumptions of normal- whether there were differences in patterns of relations
ity, linearity, and independence had been upheld. All as- across the four tasks that would confound the relations
sumptions associated with linear regression analyses were between the predictor and outcome variables if scores for
met. Next, preliminary analyses were conducted to deter- the four tasks were aggregated. In general, the patterns of
mine whether there were differences in the scaffolding correlations and part correlations between the variables of
behaviors of mothers of boys versus girls. There were no interest were similar for each of these tasks.
significant differences in mothers’ scaffolding behaviors by Because the mothers’ behaviors were consistent across
child gender, multivariate F(4, 63) ⫽ 0.36, p ⫽ .83. Also, tasks, and because the patterns of correlations did not
there were no significant differences between boys and girls change across the tasks, and because the modest sample size
for the five academic self-regulation behaviors, multivariate places constraints on the number of analyses that can be
F(5, 62) ⫽ 0.95, p ⫽ .46. Therefore, child gender was conducted, a decision was made to average the ratings of
collapsed for all further analyses. mothers’ instructional behaviors across the four tasks (see
Preliminary analyses of the relative variability and sta- Table 2 for means, standard deviations, and ranges for the
bility in each mother’s personal instructional behaviors overall scores). These scores were used in all subsequent
across the different tasks also were conducted. The majority analyses.

Table 1
Means, Standard Deviations, and Distribution of Ranges in Ratings of Each Mother’s
Instructional Behaviors Across the Four Problem-Solving Tasks
Range in ratings across tasks/
Range in % of mothers with each range
ratings of ratings
Mothers’ instructional behaviors M SD 0 1 2 3 4 t(67)
Metacognitive content 2.22 1.14 9 14 32 33 12 16.01*
Regulating task complexity 1.87 0.71 0 30 52 18 0 22.35*
Monitoring/review of the task 2.15 0.95 3 25 35 33 6 18.63*
Encouragement 1.68 0.95 7 41 31 18 3 14.50*
Rejection 1.23 0.94 22 44 26 6 2 10.55*
Overcontrol 1.64 0.84 5 41 40 12 2 16.09*
Encouraging child’s active
cognitive involvement 1.79 0.74 4 25 58 13 0 20.44*
Note. N ⫽ 68.
*p ⬍ .01.
152 NEITZEL AND STRIGHT

Table 2 seek help when they needed it (see Table 3). However, an
Means, Standard Deviations, and Ranges for Mothers’ examination of part correlations revealed that after mothers’
Scaffolding Behaviors and Children’s scaffolding behaviors were taken into consideration, moth-
Self-Regulatory Behaviors ers’ education was not related significantly to any of the
Behavior M SD Range child’s self-regulatory behaviors, suggesting that the rela-
tion of mothers’ education to children’s self-regulatory be-
Mothers’ scaffolding haviors may be understood best as an indirect influence;
Metacognitive information 2.11 0.70 1.00–4.00
Manner of instruction 2.54 0.66 1.13–4.25 mothers’ education impacts mothers’ scaffolding, and in
Regulating task complexity 2.96 0.74 1.25–4.75 turn, mothers’ scaffolding behaviors influence children’s
Monitoring/review of task 2.42 0.70 1.00–4.00 self-regulatory behaviors. When a set of variables are op-
Emotional support 3.27 0.53 2.13–4.25 erating as mediators of the relations between other vari-
Encouragement 2.10 0.69 1.00–3.50
Rejection 1.60 0.54 1.00–3.00 ables, path analysis is the most appropriate method of anal-
Transfer of responsibility 3.38 0.66 2.00–4.50 ysis (Hoyle & Smith, 1994); however, the modest size of the
sample precluded the use of path analysis in this study.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Overcontrol 2.36 0.78 1.00–4.00


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Encouraging child’s active role 3.11 0.70 1.00–4.25 Mothers’ education was not included in subsequent analyses
Children’s self-regulatory behaviors because the guiding research questions were focused on the
Metacognitive talk 0.74a 0.16 0.48–1.21
Task persistence 2.22 0.32 1.50–2.83 role of the different dimensions of mothers’ scaffolding
Lack of behavior control 1.69 0.45 1.00–3.00 behaviors in predicting children’s academic self-regulatory
Monitoring progress 1.00a 0.17 0.64–1.42 behaviors.
Help seeking 0.80a 0.12 0.52–1.09
Note. N ⫽ 68.
a
The average frequency of children’s self-regulatory behavior dur- Mothers’ Scaffolding and Children’s Academic
ing 4-min coding intervals. All other mean values are average Self-Regulatory Behaviors
ratings of mothers’ and children’s behaviors (5-point scales for
mothers’ scaffolding behaviors; 3-point scales for children’s self-
regulatory behaviors). An examination of bivariate correlations revealed that
mothers who provided more metacognitive information to
their children also tended to provide instruction in a manner
Mothers’ Education, Scaffolding Behaviors, and that reduced cognitive complexity and were more likely to
Children’s Self-Regulatory Behaviors offer emotional support as their children completed the
problem-solving tasks (see Table 3). Mothers who provided
Education was an important resource for mothers’ scaf- instruction in a manner sensitive to their children’s cogni-
folding (see Table 3). Mothers who had higher levels of tive needs typically gave more emotional support as well.
education were more likely to provide metacognitive infor- Mothers who reduced cognitive complexity and were sup-
mation to their children and to regulate task complexity by portive were less likely to be overcontrolling.
giving instructions in small steps with frequent review. Typically in the classroom, children who talked more
Mothers with more education also were more likely to about their thinking were more likely to exhibit increased
emotionally support their children and transfer responsibil- task persistence and better behavior control and also were
ity for the problem-solving interaction to their children. more likely to monitor their progress and ask for help when
Children whose mothers were more educated were more needed (see Table 3). Children who lacked self-control were
likely to talk about their thinking, be persistent in their less likely to persist on school tasks or to monitor their
work, have better behavior control, monitor their work, and progress. Children who were more persistent during their

Table 3
Correlations Between Mothers’ Scaffolding Behaviors and Children’s Self-Regulatory Behaviors
Behavior 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Demographics
1. Mother education —
Mother scaffolding
2. Metacognitive information .53** —
3. Manner of instruction .46** .51** —
4. Emotional support .26* .28* .57** —
5. Transfer of responsibility .29* .20 .40** .47** —
Child self-regulation
6. Metacognitive talk .33** .44** .54** .34** .23 —
7. Task persistence .29* .31** .34** .51** .47** .47** —
8. Lack of behavior control ⫺.25* ⫺.20 ⫺.21 ⫺.35** ⫺.43** ⫺.45** ⫺.61** —
9. Monitoring progress .34** .32** .50** .18 .20 .57** .44** ⫺.50** —
10. Help seeking .29* .31** .36** .42** .41** .54** .46** ⫺.46** .42** —
N ⫽ 68.
*p ⬍ .05. **p ⬍ .01.
MOTHERS’ SCAFFOLDING AND CHILDREN’S SELF-REGULATION 153

work were more likely to monitor their progress. Children The relative roles of mothers’ metacognitive content of
who were more persistent, had better behavior control, or instruction, manner of instruction, emotional support, and
monitored their work more carefully were more likely to transfer of responsibility for predicting each of the child’s
seek help. Although there were moderate correlations self-regulatory behaviors were examined using five hierar-
among children’s self-regulatory behaviors, the magnitudes chical multiple regression equations (see Table 4). The
of these correlations also were indicative of sizable portions content of instruction is the foundation of instructional
of unshared variance, so each of the self-regulatory behav- support (Vygotsky, 1978), as information is conveyed about
iors was explored separately in subsequent analyses, mak- processes and strategies that the young child may be lack-
ing it possible to examine the relative contributions of the ing; therefore, metacognitive content was the first aspect of
different dimensions of scaffolding for predicting the dif- scaffolding entered into the regression model. However,
ferent aspects of self-regulation. even high-quality information may not be beneficial if it is
Pearson correlations were calculated between the aspects not given in a manner sensitive to the young child’s cogni-
of mothers’ scaffolding and the child self-regulatory behav- tive needs (Wertsch, 1985). The next step examined the
iors (see Table 3). The metacognitive content of mothers’ importance of the manner in which instructions were deliv-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

instructions and mothers’ manner of instruction were related ered after metacognitive content was taken into account.
significantly to children’s metacognitive talk, task persis- Although the content and manner of instruction are funda-
tence, and monitoring progress in the classroom. Mothers’ mental, the objective of instructional support is the child’s
manner of instruction also was related to children’s help- eventual autonomous cognitive and regulatory activity, and
seeking behaviors. Mothers’ emotional support of their chil- models of the socialization process suggest that information
dren’s problem solving and appropriate transfer of respon- transmitted and procedures or routines modeled are not
sibility for the problem-solving interaction to their children likely to be internalized if instruction also is not sensitive to
were related to children’s task persistence and behavior the emotional needs of the child (Eccles et al., 1998; Grusec
control in the classroom. Mothers’ emotional support also & Goodnow, 1994). The third step tested the unique con-
was related to children’s metacognitive talk and help tribution of mothers’ emotional support and possible role in
seeking. modifying the relations between the content and manner of

Table 4
Hierarchical Regressions Predicting Children’s Self-Regulatory Behaviors
Metacognitive Task Lack of Monitoring Help
Variable talk persistence control progress seeking
Step 1
2
Model R (adjusted) .18** .09** .02 .09** .08**
Metacognitive content .44** .31** ⫺.20 .32** .31**
Step 2

Change in R2 .13** .05 .02 .16** .07*


Metacognitive content .22 .19 ⫺.12 .08 .16
Manner of instruction .42** .25 ⫺.15 .46** .31*
Model R2 (adjusted) .30** .12** .03 .24** .14**
Step 3

Change in R2 .00 .15** .08* .01 .09**


Metacognitive content .22 .20 ⫺.12 .08 .16
Manner of instruction .39** ⫺.02 .04 .55** .10
Emotional support .05 .48** ⫺.34* ⫺.10 .37**
Model R2 (adjusted) .29** .26** .09* .24** .22**
Step 4

Change in R2 .00 .06* .09** .00 .04


Metacognitive content .22 .20 ⫺.13 .08 .16
Manner of instruction .39** ⫺.08 .11 .54** .05
Emotional support .05 .37** ⫺.21 ⫺.13 .29*
Transfer of responsibility .00 .29* ⫺.35** .05 .22
Final
2
Final R (adjusted) .28** .32** .18** .23** .25**
Note. N ⫽ 68. Changes in R at each step are in boldface. Other values are standardized regression
2

coefficients (betas).
*p ⬍ .05. **p ⬍ .01.
154 NEITZEL AND STRIGHT

instruction and children’s self-regulatory behaviors. Finally,


scaffolding culminates in the transfer of responsibility for
the task as children develop abilities to function indepen-
dently in the problem-solving situation. In the terminal step,
the role of mothers’ transfer of responsibility was tested
while controlling for mother’s metacognitive content, man-
ner of instruction, and emotional support.
Metacognitive talk. The overall model of mothers’ scaf-
folding was a significant predictor of children’s metacogni-
tive talk in school (adjusted R2 ⫽ .28, p ⬍ .01). Mothers
who provided more metacognitive information had children
who were more likely to discuss how an answer was
reached or why they approached a problem a certain way or
to make evaluative comments about their performance.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Mothers’ manner of instruction also contributed uniquely to


the prediction of children’s metacognitive talk. A change in
the magnitude of the regression coefficient for mother’s
metacognitive content of instruction was noted when moth-
er’s manner of instruction was introduced into the equation,
an indication of a possible interaction between these vari- Figure 1. Moderating effect of mothers’ manner of instruction on
ables (Falk & Miller, 1992). Nonadditive effects were con- the relations between mothers’ metacognitive content of instruc-
firmed through a separate exploratory regression analysis tion and children’s metacognitive talk in the classroom. The slope
predicting children’s metacognitive talk in the classroom of the line representing high-quality manner of instruction is
using the following procedure, appropriate for small sam- significantly different from zero (␤ ⫽ .41, p ⫽ .02), indicating a
ples (W. D. Berry & Feldman, 1985). Mother’s metacogni- significant relation between mother’s metacognitive content of
tive content of instruction was entered in the first step, and instruction and children’s metacognitive talk for children who
the interaction term, Metacognitive Content ⫻ Manner of received high-quality instruction (instruction given in small steps
with frequent review in relation to the task goal).
Instruction, was added in the second step. A significant
change in R2 at the second step indicated that the interaction
was significant (Pedhazur, 1982). significantly to predicting children’s task persistence, and
In order to interpret the meaning of this interaction, low
although mothers’ manner of instruction did not contribute
and high groups for each of the predictor variables (meta-
uniquely to predicting children’s task persistence, once
cognitive content and manner of instruction) were created
again, mothers’ manner of instruction appeared to moderate
with the use of median splits (the split was checked and
the relation between metacognitive content and children’s
adjusted for meaningfulness), and the interaction was plot-
task persistence. Nonadditive effects were confirmed, the
ted (see Figure 1). In addition, follow-up exploratory tests
interaction was plotted, and the slope of each regression line
of the interaction were performed with a procedure de-
(representing low- and high-quality instruction) was tested
scribed by Aiken and West (1991). For each regression line
with the use of the exploratory procedures described above.2
in the figure, the slope was tested to determine whether it Mothers’ provision of metacognitive information predicted
was significantly different from zero. A significant slope
children’s task persistence only when the content was de-
reflects significant relations between mother’s metacogni-
livered in an understandable manner (␤ ⫽ .37, p ⫽ .04).
tive content and children’s metacognitive talk in school for
However, metacognitive information was not related to
the group of children represented by that line (children
children’s task persistence when the information was not
whose mothers provide instruction in small steps with re-
presented in a developmentally appropriate manner (␤ ⫽
view at either low or high levels). The line representing .14, p ⫽ .39). In addition, mothers’ emotional support
high-quality manner of instruction had a slope significantly appeared to moderate the relation between mothers’ manner
different from zero (␤ ⫽ .41, p ⫽ .02), but the line repre- of instruction and children’s task persistence. The interac-
senting low-quality manner of instruction did not (␤ ⫽ tion was confirmed and plotted, and post hoc tests of the
⫺.20, p ⫽ .25). Even high-quality metacognitive informa- slopes of the regression lines for high and low levels of
tion was not effective in predicting children’s metacognitive emotional support were completed (see Footnote 2). When
talk if the information was not presented in a way that mothers provided high levels of emotional support, manner
children could understand. Finally, neither mothers’ emo- of instruction was not related to children’s task persistence:
tional support nor transfer of responsibility made significant Their children were likely to sustain effort and interest in
unique contributions toward predicting children’s metacog- their schoolwork regardless of the mothers’ manner of in-
nitive talk. struction (␤ ⫽ .03, p ⫽ .89). When mothers provided low
Task persistence. The overall model of mothers’ scaf-
folding was related to children’s task persistence in the
kindergarten classroom (adjusted R2 ⫽ .32, p ⬍ .01). The 2
Figures illustrating these interactions are available from the
metacognitive content of mothers’ instruction contributed authors on request.
MOTHERS’ SCAFFOLDING AND CHILDREN’S SELF-REGULATION 155

levels of emotional support, children persisted in their work monitor their progress on academic tasks even if their
more if their mothers regulated task difficulty well during mothers provided high levels of metacognitive information
scaffolding (␤ ⫽ .43, p ⫽ .01). Finally, mothers who more (␤ ⫽ ⫺.17, p ⫽ .28). The additional contributions of
adequately relinquished control over the problem-solving mothers’ emotional support and transfer of responsibility
interaction, encouraging their children’s autonomy in the were not significant.
problem-solving process, had children who were better at Help seeking. The overall model of mothers’ scaffold-
sustaining interest and effort in the classroom. ing was a significant predictor of children’s help seeking
Lack of behavior control. The overall model of the (adjusted R2 ⫽ .25, p ⬍ .01). The metacognitive content of
quality of mothers’ scaffolding was a significant predictor mothers’ instruction was a significant predictor. In addition,
of children’s lack of behavior control in the classroom mothers’ manner of instruction was associated significantly
(adjusted R2 ⫽ .18, p ⬍ .01). Neither the metacognitive with children’s help seeking and also moderated the relation
content of mothers’ instruction nor the mothers’ manner of between metacognitive content and children’s help seeking
instruction was related significantly to children’s lack of (see Footnote 2). For children whose mothers provided
behavior control. However, after controlling for the meta- low-quality instruction, the metacognitive content of moth-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

cognitive content and manner of instruction, mothers’ emo- ers’ instructions was positively related to children’s help
tional support was related significantly (negatively) to chil- seeking (␤ ⫽ .35, p ⫽ .05). However, children whose
dren’s lack of behavior control in school. Mothers who mothers presented instructions in small steps with frequent
provided more encouragement and displayed more positive review were likely to seek instructions from their teacher or
attitudes about the problem-solving task and their children’s peers when having difficulty regardless of the metacognitive
abilities to do the task had children who were less likely to content of their mothers’ instructions (␤ ⫽ ⫺.02, p ⫽ .91).
be disruptive or fail to follow instructions and class routines. The contribution of mothers’ emotional support for predict-
Mothers’ transfer of responsibility also made a unique con- ing children’s help seeking also was significant and mod-
tribution to predicting (negatively) children’s lack of behav- erated the relations between mothers’ manner of instruction
ior control in the classroom, and when mothers’ transfer of and children’s help seeking (see Footnote 2). When mothers
responsibility was entered into the equation, emotional sup- provided high levels of emotional support, children who
port was no longer a significant predictor, suggesting that received high-quality instruction from their mothers were
mothers’ transfer of responsibility moderates the relation likely to seek assistance in the classroom (␤ ⫽ .41, p ⫽ .01).
between mothers’ emotional support and children’s lack of If children’s emotional needs had not been supported ade-
behavior control. Nonadditive effects were confirmed quately during problem solving, children were not likely to
through exploratory analyses, the interaction was plotted, seek assistance in the classroom even if they had received
and post hoc tests of the slopes of the regression lines for high-quality instruction from their mothers in a manner that
low and high levels of mother’s transfer of responsibility regulated the difficulty of the task (␤ ⫽ ⫺.02, p ⫽ .92).
were completed (see Footnote 2). When their mothers were Finally, mothers’ transfer of responsibility did not make a
less directive and controlling, children were likely to exhibit significant unique contribution to understanding children’s
behavior control in the classroom, regardless of the moth- help-seeking behaviors in class.
er’s level of emotional support (␤ ⫽ ⫺.07, p ⫽ .65).
However, when mothers had not encouraged their chil- Discussion
dren’s autonomous behavior, emotional support was partic-
ularly important for predicting children’s behavior control The present study extends previous research on the rela-
in the classroom (␤ ⫽ ⫺.40, p ⫽ .02). tions between parents’ scaffolding of their children’s
Monitoring progress. The overall model of mothers’ problem-solving and young children’s adjustment to school
scaffolding was related significantly to children’s monitor- by investigating a complete model of scaffolding interac-
ing progress (adjusted R2 ⫽ .23, p ⬍ .01). Mothers who tions as a multidimensional process by which children’s
provided metacognitive information had children who metacognition, task persistence, behavior control, and cog-
checked their work, recognized when they had made an nitive management processes such as monitoring and help
error, self-corrected, and adjusted strategy use appropriately seeking may be socialized. Vygotsky (1978) believed that
in school. When manner of instruction was entered into the the child’s desire to act independently is innate but that
instructional model, metacognitive content was no longer a access to the means and attitudes for self-regulated behavior
significant unique contributor, again, suggesting that moth- are culturally determined and learned. Parent– child joint
ers’ manner of instruction moderates the relation between problem-solving interactions within the family appear to lay
metacognitive content and children’s self-monitoring. The the foundation for a culture of academic self-regulatory
interaction was confirmed and plotted, and post hoc tests of competence, serving as a process in which cultural artifacts
the slopes of the regression lines for low and high quality such as tools, attitudes or beliefs, and roles may be pro-
manner of instruction were completed (see Footnote 2). cured. More specifically, during scaffolded interactions with
When instruction was given in small steps with frequent their parents, children may acquire important tools for meta-
review, the metacognitive content of mothers’ instruction cognitive understanding and cognitive management, moti-
predicted children’s monitoring (␤ ⫽ .31, p ⫽ .05). How- vational beliefs and attitudes about the learning process, and
ever, when mothers did not present instructions in small conceptions of roles and responsibilities (their own and
steps with frequent review, children were not likely to others) needed for academic self-regulation. In these ways,
156 NEITZEL AND STRIGHT

the family may furnish the fundamental foundation for needed. However, if children’s experiences within the fam-
children’s academic self-regulatory development and, in ily are characterized by instances in which instruction is
turn, affect whether or not children will be able to derive presented in a manner that makes it impossible to under-
maximum benefit from their school experiences. stand, then children may fail to seek assistance in school
because they assume that the instruction will not be helpful.
Children’s Metacognitive Awareness and Cognitive It is important that children view others as important re-
Management in the Classroom sources for information that will have value to them, and
children’s early interactions with their parents may affect
The mothers’ cognitive support (metacognitive content their theories about the usefulness of cognitive strategies
and manner of instruction) was important particularly to the such as seeking assistance.
prediction of children’s self-regulatory behaviors related to Mothers’ emotional support also contributed significantly
cognitive awareness and management: metacognitive talk, to predicting children’s help seeking in the classroom. This
monitoring, and help seeking. Mothers who provided more finding is consistent with previous research which has found
metacognitive information during scaffolded interactions that the quality of personal relationships with the teacher
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

had children who more frequently talked about their think- (Newman & Schwager, 1993) and attention to social and
ing and monitored progress on their classwork. This is emotional needs in the classroom (Ryan, Gheen, & Midg-
consistent with findings from previous research with third- ley, 1998) affect help seeking. Mothers’ emotional support
grade students (Stright et al., 2001). It seems reasonable to also moderated the relations between mothers’ cognitive
conclude that information about task management tech- support and children’s help seeking in school. In order for
niques and suggestions for how and when to use strategies children to seek help, they not only must realize the need
may equip children with important “tools” for cognitive and be motivated to improve their current level of under-
management, and children’s metacognitive talk may pro- standing but also must be willing to receive assistance from
vide a window into their level of metacognitive knowledge others (Ryan & Pintrich, 1997). Children who have not
(Schunk, 1986); however, this study did not examine the received adequate emotional support may recognize that
underlying processes, such as children’s metacognitive adults are resources but may not be willing to use them
knowledge, that would help clarify these relations. because these children may determine that it simply is not
In addition, children’s abilities to derive benefit from the worth the risk. If their problem-solving efforts typically
metacognitive information given by their mothers depended have been responded to negatively within the family, chil-
on the mothers’ manner of instruction, which modified the dren may anticipate rejection and be unlikely to seek help in
relations between the metacognitive content of mothers’ the classroom. Little research has explored the relations
instruction and children’s metacognitive talk and monitor- between models of behavior that the child develops within
ing progress. Again, this finding replicates that of previous the family and the child’s behavior in school. It may be
research with older students (Stright et al., 2001). Mothers’ useful to examine whether children entering school gener-
providing instruction in small steps with frequent review alize models of interaction with their parents to interactions
may reduce the cognitive load of a task and allow the child with their teacher and peers in the new school setting.
to receive maximum benefit from metacognitive informa- Future research on how previous instructional interactions
tion contained in mothers’ instructions. If children’s previ- within the family may affect the child’s internal working
ous experiences within the family are characterized by models about the value of assistance from others would
instruction presented in a manner that makes it impossible provide evidence for the link we are hypothesizing to ex-
to understand, then children may not be able to internalize plain the relations found between mothers’ manner of in-
and use the information to discuss their cognition or monitor struction, emotional support, and children’s help seeking in
progress. Children may fail to talk about their thinking or school.
monitor their progress in class because they lack the re-
sources to engage in these behaviors (Schraw, 1994). Children’s Effort and Self-Control in the Classroom
The metacognitive content and manner of mothers’ in-
struction also were related to children’s seeking help in the Mothers’ emotional support and mothers’ transfer of re-
classroom, contrary to previous findings (Stright et al., sponsibility during problem-solving interactions were im-
2001) which indicated that children’s seeking assistance in portant to the prediction of children’s effort and control in
their third-grade classrooms was related to whether parents’ their classrooms. In particular, mothers’ emotional support
instructions were given in an understandable manner but was important for predicting children’s behavior control if
was not related to the metacognitive content of their par- mothers had not encouraged their children’s autonomous
ents’ instruction. One explanation for these conflicting find- behavior. Whether children believe that completing a task is
ings may be that the metacognitive content of instruction is within their abilities or worth the effort, and therefore per-
more important for young children whose own repertoires sist in their attempts to complete the task or recognize that
of metacognitive skills may not be as well developed as it is their responsibility to do so, may depend on whether
those of older children. If parents provide metacognitive children’s efforts and autonomy have been encouraged in
information to their children, then their children may be the past. Parents’ emotional support may be important for
more cognitively aware and therefore more likely to recog- ensuring that children are personally invested (Grusec &
nize that they do not understand and to realize that help is Goodnow, 1994) and parents’ patterns of control or promo-
MOTHERS’ SCAFFOLDING AND CHILDREN’S SELF-REGULATION 157

tion of children’s autonomy in problem-solving situations support of their children’s autonomy influence whether chil-
may be important for demonstrating that children are re- dren put forth effort in their work and whether children are
sponsible for their own behavior control in the classroom responsible for their own behavior control in the classroom.
(Hess et al., 1984). In turn, children’s metacognitive skill, task persistence, and
A mediating link between aspects of parents’ scaffolding behavior self-control differentially affect their progress
and children’s self-regulation may be the impact of parents’ monitoring and help-seeking behaviors in the classroom. A
scaffolding on children’s long-term attitudes and beliefs. To larger sample would enable examination of these hypothe-
the degree that children’s task persistence and behavior sized mediated models.
control are acceptable indicators of motivational disposi- A number of previous studies that have examined parent-
tions, there is support for this hypothesis; however, to child interactions across multiple tasks report significant
adequately test this link, research is needed to examine how variation across tasks in the parenting behaviors observed
parents’ scaffolding relates to children’s mental models of (Gonzalez, 1996). Likewise, the present study revealed
motivation and control and how these mental models relate large differences in mothers’ instructional behaviors across
to children’s academic self-regulation in school. tasks (although there also was a high degree of stability in
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Parents’ scaffolding also may affect how children define mothers’ personal instructional behaviors across the tasks
their roles and responsibilities in the learning process. Al- relative to the other mothers). Parents are the architects and
though children’s behavior control in school may be one managers of children’s early experiences (Bradley & Cald-
indication that they understand their responsibilities and well, 1995), and mothers’ differing degrees of support de-
roles as active agents in the learning process, research is pending on the task may reflect emphases, expectations, and
needed to examine whether differences in parents’ patterns goals in the home that could lead to differences in experi-
of control or support of autonomy are related directly to ences and opportunities afforded to their children. This issue
children’s definitions of their responsibilities and roles and warrants further attention in future research.
whether the differences in children’s definitions are related For decades, scaffolding interactions within the family
to their self-regulatory behaviors in school. have been recognized as contributing to children’s increased
Although the consistency of relations found between the independent functioning on the tasks being scaffolded
current study and earlier work with third graders lends (Pratt, Green, MacVicar, & Bountrogianni, 1992; Wood et
support for our model, longitudinal research is needed to al., 1976). Scaffolded interactions between parent and child
examine whether developmental differences exist in the also have been credited for children’s more efficient self-
patterns of relations between scaffolding within the family regulated problem-solving behaviors on subsequent tasks
and children’s self-regulatory behaviors in school. It would that resemble the task being scaffolded (Freund, 1990;
be informative to examine whether the effects of early Radziszewska & Rogoff, 1988). The current study provides
socialization within the family on children’s academic self- evidence that scaffolding processes within the family may
regulatory behaviors persist or recede as children spend have more pervasive influences that transcend environmen-
more time in school. tal settings as well as tasks, inducting children into a culture
of academic self-regulatory competence.
Conclusions
Implications for Application and Public Policy
Because of the modest sample size, caution must be
exercised in interpreting the results. However, given the size Beginning school places numerous demands on the child
of the sample, statistically significant results are indicative to assume new roles and meet new expectations for cogni-
of strong effect sizes. The size of the sample did preclude tive self-management (Fernie, 1989). Children’s adjustment
the analysis of a more dynamic model. The dimensions of to these demands is crucial because the ability of the child
scaffolding do not appear to function in additive fashion; to adapt has long-term as well as immediate consequences
however, further research is needed using a larger sample so (Fernie, 1989). Therefore, the current findings have impli-
that interactions between the three dimensions of parental cations for educators as well as parents. It is important for
scaffolding (cognitive support, emotional support, and teachers to be aware that differences in patterns of family
transfer of responsibility) can be explored fully. In addition, interactions mean that children come to school with differ-
research with a larger sample would permit an examination ent levels of metacognitive knowledge and procedures for
of a model of family influences that more adequately rep- academic management. Consequently, some children will
resents its complexity. For instance, parent– child scaffold- be primed for the demands of school better than others.
ing interactions are believed to mediate the relations among Many schools make concerted efforts to incorporate into the
characteristics of the child and parents’ personal adjustment curriculum programs aimed at enhancing children’s meta-
and education. In addition, a more precise model of the cognitive and cognitive management skills. These findings
relations between parent– child scaffolding interactions and suggest the need for approaches that address motivational
children’s self-regulatory behaviors in school may be that components of self-regulation as well as those that intro-
parents’ cognitive support (metacognitive content and man- duce basic procedures for cognitive regulation. Children
ner of instruction) influences children’s metacognition and who are particularly at risk for behavioral problems, lack of
therefore their ability to persist in their classwork. Parents’ effort and persistence, and failure to seek assistance in
emotional support of their children’s efforts and control or school are children whose parents have not emotionally
158 NEITZEL AND STRIGHT

supported the child’s efforts and encouraged the child’s relationship of self-efficacy, self-regulation, and collaborative
autonomy. This finding suggests that in order to establish verbal behavior with grades: Preliminary findings. Psychological
environments supportive of children’s self-regulated behav- Reports, 77, 971–978.
ior, teachers also need to examine the patterns of commu- Fernie, D. E. (1989). Becoming a student: Messages from first
nication and control in their classrooms, emotional support settings. Theory Into Practice, 27, 3–10.
Flavell, J. H. (1987). Speculations about the nature and develop-
mechanisms that are in place in their classrooms, and rou- ment of metacognition. In F. Weinert & R. Kluwe (Eds.), Meta-
tines or procedures used in their classrooms that may either cognition, motivation, and understanding (pp. 21–29). Hillsdale,
sanction or prohibit autonomous behavior. NJ: Erlbaum.
The current findings have implications for the design of Freund, L. S. (1990). Maternal regulation of children’s problem-
parent education or intervention programs. Parents’ beliefs solving behavior and its impact on children’s performance. Child
about their own abilities are related significantly to their Development, 61, 113–126.
instructional behaviors (Hoover-Demsey, Bassler, & Bris- Ginsburg, G. S., & Bronstein, P. (1993). Family factors related to
sie, 1987). Parents may be reluctant to offer assistance to children’s intrinsic/extrinsic motivational orientation and aca-
their children if they do not believe that they have the demic performance. Child Development, 64, 1461–1474.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Gonzalez, M. M. (1996). Tasks and activities: A parent– child


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

information necessary to provide effective instruction. Of


course, there may be limits to the extent to which parents interaction analysis. Learning and Instruction, 6, 287–306.
Green, R. J. (1995). High achievement, underachievement, and
can help directly with school skills if they lack the relevant
learning disabilities: A family systems model. In B. A. Ryan,
skills themselves. However, the findings of this study sug- G. R. Adams, T. P. Gullotta, R. P. Weissberg, & R. L. Hampton
gest that parents’ models of general task management tech- (Eds.), The family–school connection: Theory, research, and
niques, emotionally supportive actions, and encouragement practice (pp. 207–249). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
of their children’s independent attempts at problem solving Grolnick, W. S., & Ryan, R. M. (1989). Parent styles associated
aid in children’s academic development by fostering the with children’s self-regulation and competence in school. Jour-
metacognitive awareness and attitudes toward learning nal of Educational Psychology, 81, 143–154.
known to lead to academic competence. Institutional efforts Grolnick, W. S., & Slowiaczek, M. L. (1994). Parents’ involve-
to design and implement parenting education programs are ment in children’s schooling: A multidimensional conceptualiza-
not only warranted but may be invaluable. tion and motivational model. Child Development, 65, 237–252.
Grusec, J. E., & Goodnow, J. J. (1994). The impact of parental
discipline methods on the child’s internalization of values: A
References reconceptualization of current points of view. Developmental
Psychology, 30, 4 –19.
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing
and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Hess, R. D., Holloway, S. D., Dickson, W. P., & Price, G. G.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A (1984). Maternal variables as predictors of children’s school
social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. readiness and later achievement in vocabulary and mathematics
Berry, K. J., & Mielke, P. W. (1988). A generalization of Cohen’s in sixth grade. Child Development, 55, 1902–1912.
kappa agreement measure to interval measurement and multiple Hoover-Demsey, K. V., Bassler, O. C., & Brissie, J. S. (1987).
raters. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 48, 921– Parent involvement: Contributions of teacher efficacy, school
933. socioeconomic status, and other school characteristics. American
Berry, W. D., & Feldman, S. (1985). Multiple regression in prac- Educational Research Journal, 24, 417– 435.
tice. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Hoyle, R. H., & Smith, G. T. (1994). Formulating clinical research
Bickhard, M. H. (1992). Scaffolding and self-scaffolding: Central hypotheses as structural equation models: A conceptual over-
aspects of development. In L. T. Winegar & J. Valsiner (Eds.), view. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 429 –
Children’s development within social context: Vol. 2. Research 440.
and methodology (pp. 33–52). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Neitzel, C., & Stright, A. D. (2002). Parenting behaviors during
Bradley, R. H., & Caldwell, B. M. (1995). The acts and conditions child problem-solving: The roles of child temperament, mother
of the caregiving environment. Developmental Review, 15, 38 – education and personality, and the problem-solving context.
85. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Brown, A. (1988). Motivation to learn and understand: On taking Newman, R. S., & Schwager, M. T. (1993). Students’ perceptions
charge of one’s own learning. Cognition and Instruction, 5, of the teacher and classmates in relation to reported help seeking
311–321. in math class. The Elementary School Journal, 94, 3–17.
Bruner, J. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Pedhazur, E. J. (1982). Multiple regression in behavioral re-
Harvard University Press. search: Explanation and prediction. New York: Holt, Rinehart,
Cole, M. (1985). The zone of proximal development: Where & Winston.
culture and cognition create each other. In J. V. Wertsch (Ed.), Pianta, R. C., Smith, N., & Reeve, R. E. (1991). Observing mother
Culture, communication, and cognition: Vygotskian perspectives and child behavior in a problem-solving situation at school entry:
(pp. 252–272). New York: Basic Books. Relations with classroom adjustment. School Psychology Quar-
Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., & Schiefele, U. (1998). Motivation to terly, 6, 1–15.
succeed. In E. M. Hetherington (Ed.), Handbook of child psy- Pintrich, P., & Schrauben, B. (1992). Students’ motivational be-
chology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development liefs and their cognitive engagement in classroom academic
(pp. 1017–1095). New York: Wiley. tasks. In D. H. Schunk & J. L. Meece (Eds.), Student perceptions
Falk, R. F., & Miller, N. B. (1992). A primer for soft modeling. in the classroom (pp. 149 –183). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Akron, OH: University of Akron Press. Pratt, M. W., Green, D., MacVicar, J., & Bountrogianni, M.
Feldman, S. C., Martinez-Pons, M., & Shaham, D. (1995). The (1992). The mathematical parent: Parental scaffolding, parenting
MOTHERS’ SCAFFOLDING AND CHILDREN’S SELF-REGULATION 159

style, and learning outcomes in long-division mathematics home- include student perceptions. Educational Psychologist, 30, 213–
work. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 13, 17–34. 216.
Radziszewska, B., & Rogoff, B. (1988). Influence of adult and Scott, M. M., & Hattfield, J. G. (1985). Problems of analyst and
peer collaborators on children’s planning skills. Developmental observer agreement in naturalistic narrative data. Journal of
Psychology, 24, 840 – 848. Educational Measurement, 22, 207–218.
Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive develop- Sternberg, R. J. (1994). Answering questions and questioning
ment in social context. New York: Oxford University Press. answers: Mediating children to intellectual excellence. Phi Delta
Rogoff, B., Mistry, J., Goncu, A., & Mosier, C. (1993). Guided Kappan, pp. 136 –138.
participation in cultural activity by toddlers and caregivers. Stright, A. D., Neitzel, C. L., Sears, K. G., & Hoke-Sinex, L.
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, (2001). Instruction begins in the home: Relations between pa-
58(1, Serial No. 236). rental instruction and children’s self-regulation in the classroom.
Ryan, A. M., Gheen, M. H., & Midgley, C. (1998). Why do some Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 456 – 466.
students avoid asking for help? An examination of the interplay Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
among students’ academic efficacy, teachers’ social-emotional
Wertsch, J. V. (1985). Vygotsky and the social formation of mind.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

role, and the classroom goal structure. Journal of Educational


Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Psychology, 90, 528 –535. Wood, D., Bruner, J., & Ross, S. (1976). The role of tutoring in
Ryan, A. M., & Pintrich, P. R. (1997). “Should I ask for help?” problem-solving. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
The role of motivation and attitudes in adolescents’ help seeking 17, 89 –100.
in math class. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 329 –341. Zimmerman, B. J. (1995). Self-regulation involves more than
Schraw, G. (1994). The effect of metacognitive knowledge on metacognition: A social cognitive perspective. Educational Psy-
local and global monitoring. Contemporary Educational Psy- chologist, 30, 217–221.
chology, 19, 143–154.
Schunk, D. H. (1986). Verbalization and children’s self-regulated Received March 20, 2001
learning. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 11, 347–369. Revision received March 15, 2002
Schunk, D. H. (1995). Inherent details of self-regulated learning Accepted March 28, 2002 䡲

You might also like