You are on page 1of 6

Borders of Multiple Visual Areas in Humans Revealed by Functional Magnetic Resonance

Imaging
Author(s): M. I. Sereno, A. M. Dale, J. B. Reppas, K. K. Kwong, J. W. Belliveau, T. J. Brady, B.
R. Rosen and R. B. H. Tootell
Reviewed work(s):
Source: Science, New Series, Vol. 268, No. 5212 (May 12, 1995), pp. 889-893
Published by: American Association for the Advancement of Science
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/2887976 .
Accessed: 28/02/2013 05:51

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

American Association for the Advancement of Science is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and
extend access to Science.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded on Thu, 28 Feb 2013 05:51:11 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
v REPORTS

expression inEscherichiacoi as described by X. Liao,


K. R. Clemens, J. Cavanagh, L. Tennant, and P. E. Borders of MultipleVisual Areas in Humans
Wright[J. Biomol. NMR4, 433 (1994)]. We performed
the NMR experiments at 300 K at a 1H frequency of Revealed by Functional Magnetic
500 MHz,using a 0.5 mM solution of ZF1-3 in 30 mM
phosphate buffer (pH 6.5) in a mixtureof 90% H20 Resonance Imaging
and 10% D20 containing 30 mM NaCI,5 mM deuter-
ated dithiothreitol,and 50 ,uMZnCI2.
7. Analysis of the data, accounting for both dipolar re-
M. 1.Sereno,* A. M. Dale, J. B. Reppas, K. K. Kwong,
laxation of the 15N spin mediated by its directly at- J. W. Belliveau,T. J. Brady, B. R. Rosen, R. B. H. Tootell
tached proton and relaxation caused by chemical
shielding anisotropy, was based on the classical ex-
pressions (8) The borders of human visual areas Vl, V2, VP, V3, and V4 were precisely and noninvasively
T,' = ZajJ(,) determined. Functional magnetic resonance images were recorded during phase-en-
coded retinal stimulation. This volume data set was then sampled with a cortical surface
reconstruction, making it possible to calculate the local visual field sign (mirror image
versus non-mirror image representation). This method automatically and objectively out-
lines area borders because adjacent areas often have the opposite field sign. Cortical
N
Ti=1+-=1b,Wj) magnification factor curves for striate and extrastriate cortical areas were determined,
which showed that human visual areas have a greater emphasis on the center-of-gaze
with constant coefficients a., b, and c. The power than their counterparts in monkeys. Retinotopically organized visual areas in humans
spectral density J(w) reflects molecu(ar dynamics
processes (intramoleculardynamics as well as over- extend anteriorly to overlap several areas previously shown to be activated by written
all rotational.tumbling)and is sampled at frequencies words.
x>,which are combinations of the 15Nand 1H Larmor
frequencies wN and wH'
8. L. E. Kay, D. A. Torchia, A. Bax, Biochemistry 28,
8972(1989).
9. M. J. Stone et al., ibid. 31, 4394 (1992). Over half of the neocortex in nonhuman malshave been requiredto define arealbor-
10. P. Debye, Polar Molecules (Dover, New York, 1929), primatesis occupiedby visualareas.At least ders with confidence(7). Here we demon-
chap. 5; F. Perrin,J. Phys. Radium 5,497 (1934); ibid.
7, 1 (1936); L. D. Favro,Phys. Rev. 107, 7 (1960). 25 visual areasbeyond the primaryvisual stratea techniqueforgeneratingretinotopic
11. G. Lipariand A. Szabo, J. Am. Chem. Soc. 104, cortex (Vi) have been identified with a maps of visual cortex in humans with a
4546 (1982). combination of microelectrodemapping, precision similar to that obtained in the
12. W. H. Press, B. P. Flannery,S. A. Teukolsky, W. T.
Vetterling,NumericalRecipes in C: The Art of Scien- tracer injections, histological stains, and most detailed invasive animal studies.Re-
tificComputing (Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, functionalstudies (1). The analysisof this sponses to phase-encodedretinal stimula-
1988). data has been greatlyaided by the use of tion (8) were recordedwith echo-planar
13. Structures were calculated from 1284 NOE distance
constraints and 45 dihedralangle constraints by vari-
flattenedrepresentations of the corticalsur- functional magnetic resonance imaging
able target function distance geometry and re- face madefromconventionalsectionswith (MRI) (9) and analyzedwith a Fourier-
strained molecular dynamics (X. Liao and P. E. graphicaltechniques(2) and flattenedwire based method. The resultingvolume data
Wright, unpublished data). sets were sampledwith a cortical surface
14. M. S. Lee, G. P. Gippert,K.V. Soman, D. A. Case, P.
models (3), or more directlyfrom sections
E. Wright,Science 245,635 (1989); R. E. Klevit,J. R. of physicallyflat-mountedcortex (4). reconstructionmade from high-resolution
Herriott,S. J. Horvath,Proteins 7, 215 (1990). A largeportionof the neocortexin hu- structuralMRI imagescollected separately
15. Y. Nakaseko, D. Neuhaus, A. Klug, D. Rhodes, J. mansis likelyto be occupiedby visualareas for each participant(10). The corticalsur-
Mol. Biol. 228, 619 (1992).
16. J. G. Omichinski et al., Biochemistry 31, 3907 too. It has been difficult,however,to out- face containingthe data was then unfolded
(1992). line unambiguouslyany human cortical andanalyzedwith the visualfieldsignmeth-
17. Contributionsfrom aggregation can be excluded as areawith noninvasivetechniques.Previous od to distinguishmirrorimage from non-
a result of the excellent agreement found between
the effective rotationaltumbling correlationtime ob- studieshave mappedonly a few locationsin mirrorimage representations(7). By com-
tained by tryptophan fluorescence depolarization the visual field or have relied on stimulus bining these four techniques (multislice
measurements at a ZF1-3 concentration of 50 ,uM featuresto activatedifferentareas(5), and functional MRI, stimulus phase-encoding
and that derived from NMR relaxation measure-
ments at more than 10-fold higher concentration (X.
the tortuous convolutions of the human and Fourieranalysis,corticalsurfacerecon-
Liao, R. Bruschweiler, D. Millar,P. E. Wright,unpub- neocortexhave defiedpreviousattemptsto struction,and visualfield signcalculations),
lished data). see activity across all of its surface area at it waspossibleto reconstructthe retinotopic
18. A. G. Palmer, M. Rance, P. E. Wright,J. Am. Chem.
Soc. 113, 4371 (1991).
once. organizationof visualareasVl, V2, VP, V3,
19. G. Barbato, M. Ikura,L. E. Kay, R. W. Pastor, A. Bax, Many of the cortical visual areas in non- andV4 in humansin two dimensionsandto
Biochemistty 31, 5269 (1992). human primates are retinotopically orga- accuratelytrace out the bordersbetween
20. It is not possible to specify uniquely the relative ori- nized to some degree (3, 6). These areas are these areasin the living humanbrain.
entations of the diffusion tensors as a consequence
of the transformation properties of the responsible irregularlyshaped and somewhat variable in To mappolarangle (anglefromthe cen-
spin interactions. The relaxation data cannot distin- location; consequently, recordings from ter-of-gaze),we obtained 128 asymmetric
guish between orientations that relate individualdo- many locations (400 to 600) in single ani- spin echo MRI images(11) of 8 to 16 ob-
mains by an orthorhombic symmetry transformation
of the diffusion tensor ellipsoids. This means that a liquesectionsperpendicular to the calcarine
180? rotationof any of the domains about the x, y, or M. I. Sereno, Cognitive Science 0515, Universityof Cali- sulcus(1024 to 2048 total) in a 512-ssession
fornia, San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093-0515, USA.
z axes of their diffusion frames leaves the relaxation
A. M. Dale, Cognitive Science 0515, Universityof Califor- (-8.5 min) while participants(n = 7)
parameters unchanged.
21. I.Radhakrishnan,D. Millar,P. E. Wright,unpublished nia, San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093-0515, USA, and De- vieweda slowlyrotating(clockwiseor coun-
data. partment of Neurophysiology, University of Oslo, 0316 terclockwise), semicircular checkerboard
Oslo, Norway.
22. K. R. Clemens et al., J. Mol. Biol. 244, 23 (1994).
J. B. Reppas, Harvard-Massachusetts Instituteof Tech- stimulus. Eccentricity (distance from the
23. We thank T. Macke for making Fig. 1 (with graphics center-of-gaze)was mapped with a thick
program AVS) and D. Millarand J. Gottesfeld for nology Divisionof Health Sciences and Technology, Bos-
helpfuldiscussions.X.L.is a recipientof the Cancer ton, MA 02115, USA. ring (dilatingor contracting)instead of a
K. K. Kwong, J. W. Belliveau,T. J. Brady, B. R. Rosen, R.
Research Institute-Miriam and BenedictWolf Fel-
B. H. Tootell, Massachusetts General Hospital Nuclear
semicircle.These fourkindsof stimulielicit
lowship.Thisworkwas fundedby grantGM36643 periodicexcitation at the rotationor dila-
fromthe NationalInstitutesof Health. Magnetic Resonance Center, 149 13th Street, Charles-
town, MA 02129, USA. tion-contractionfrequencyat each point
8 November1994; accepted 21 February1995 *To whom correspondence should be addressed. in a corticalretinotopicmap (8, 12). The
SCIENCE * VOL.268 * 12 MAY1995 889

This content downloaded on Thu, 28 Feb 2013 05:51:11 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
phase of the periodicresponseat the rota- (V4 ventral) (18). In particular,we would sentationsnear the fundusof the calcarine
tion or dilation-contractionfrequency,mea- expect verticalmeridianrepresentationsat sulcusin VI, at the dorsalV2-V3 border,at
sured with the (complex-valued)Fourier the dorsaland ventral V1-V2 border,the the ventralV2-VP border,and at the ante-
transformof the responseprofileover time ventralVP-V4v border,and the dorsalV3- rior borderof ventralV4v (4, 6, 7). Can-
at each voxel, is closelyrelatedto the polar V4 border,and horizontalmeridianrepre- didatesfor all of the bordersare visible in
angleor eccentricityrepresentedat that cor-
tical location(13). This techniqueresultsin
high signal-to-noiseratios (becauseat any
one point in time,approximately one-halfof
each visualfield map will be activated)yet
providesfine spatial resolution.Common
(for example, retinal) and between-area
phasedelayscan be removedand examined
by consideringclockwise-counterclockwise
rotation and expansion-contractionpairs
(14, 15).
Figure 1A shows a color plot of the
responseto a dilatingringon a medialview
of the cortical surfaceof the brain of this
participant(A.M.D.) (16). The hue of the
colorat each corticalsurfacepoint indicates
the responsephase, which is here propor-
tional to the eccentricityof the local visual
field representation.In Fig. LBthe cortical
surfacewas unfolded.This processis similar
to inflatinga crumpledballoon except that
the surfacehas not been stretched.In Fig.
IC, the occipitallobe regioncontainingthe
activated area has been cut off and the
resultingapproximatelyconical surfacecut Fig. 1. Isoeccentricity and isopolar angle maps of human visual areas. The top row shows isoeccentricity
again along the fundus of the calcarine coded by color [red (fovea) -* blue -* green (parafoveal)-*> yellow -- red (periphery)]displayed on the
sulcusto allowit to be flattenedcompletely originalcortical surface (A),the unfolded cortical surface (B), and the cut and flattened cortical surface (C).
(17). The bottom row shows polar angle [red (lower vertical meridian)-- blue (horizontalmeridian)-- green
There is a systematicincreasein eccen- (upper vertical meridian)]plotted on the same three surfaces (D), (E), and (F), respectively. Local eccen-
tricity(redto blue to greento yellowto red) tricityand polar angle were determined by considering the phase of the response to a slowly dilatingring
moving anteriorlyalong the medialwall of or a slowly rotatinghemifieldat the dilationor rotationfrequency. The unfolded representations in (B)and
the occipitalcortex.Linesof isoeccentricity (E)were made by relaxingthe curvature while approximately preserving local area and ocal angles (the
run approximatelyin the coronal plane, sulcal cortex is dark gray and the gyralcortex lightgray).The flattened representations in (C)and (F)were
made with the same algorithmafterthe occipital lobe was cut off and an additionalcut inthe fundus of the
cuttingacrossseveralareas,as shownbelow.
calcarine sulcus was made.
Ventrally, the region showing substantial
retinotopyextends almost to the anterior-
posteriormidpointof the unfoldedventral Fig. 2. Analysis of the data in Fig. 1 by
temporallobe. visual field sign (mirror image versus
A parallel treatment of data from the non-mirror image visual field represen-
rotatinghemifield stimuluscollected a few tation). Mirrorimage areas (yellow; for
minutes later is shown in Fig. 1, D and E. example, Vi), and non-mirrorimage ar-
The color again indicatesthe phase of the eas (blue; for example, V2) are shown in
periodicresponse,which is now proportion- a medial view on the folded (A) and un-
al to the polarangleof the local visualfield folded surface (B) and in a ventral view,
folded (C) and unfolded (D).The incision
representation. The pictureof polarangleis in the fundus of the calcanne is visible in
morecomplex,altematingbetweenvertical (B). Ventral Vl, V2, VP, and V4v (18),
and horizontalmeridiansboth dorsallyand comprising four rerepresentations of the
ventrally.The upperfield verticalmeridian upper visual field, are visible below the
is red, the horizontalmeridianis blue, and incision, whereas lower visual field Vl
the lower field vertical meridianis green. and V2 are visible above the incision.
Several altemationsbetween red and blue The complex folding pattern of the oc-
stripesare visible ventrally,whereasseveral cipitallobe coupled with the weak corre-
alternationsbetweengreenand blue stripes lation between sulci and areal bound-
arevisibledorsally.Mappingexperimentsin aries underscores the need for an un-
folded representation.
monkeyssuggestthat severaladditionalre-
representations of the lowervisualfield ad-
join V1 dorsally, including V2 (second vi-
sual area) and V3 (third visual area), where-
as several rerepresentations of the upper
visual field adjoin V1 ventrally, including
V2, VP (ventroposterior area), and V4v

890 SCIENCE * VOL. 268 * 12 MAY 1995

This content downloaded on Thu, 28 Feb 2013 05:51:11 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
am l REPORTS

Fig. 1, D and E. Mapping experiments in likely to be of any fundamentalfunctional with this technique(20). Dorsaland ante-
monkeys have also revealed a number of significance,but it providesa convenient rior to V3 there are additionalnon-mirror
small visual areas beyond those mentioned way to drawbordersbetweenareasbecause image areas (blue) containing both upper
so far (including the parietal-occipital adjoiningareasoften have the oppositevi- and lower visual field representationsthat
area), which may help to explain the pres- sualfield sign. It is a local measurethat can maycorrespondin partto V3A (V3 acces-
ence of several small patches of lower field be calculatedfor each smallpatchof cortex sory) and dorsal V4 (not labeled). Even
dorsally as well as upper field ventrally. given dense retinotopicmappingdata like furtheranteriorly,areas are less obviously
Isoeccentricity and isopolar angle maps those obtainedhere. modulatedat the stimulusrotationor dila-
define two independent coordinates of reti- A map of visual field sign is shown on tion-contractionfrequency,indicatingthat
notopy. Areal borders in either one of these the foldedand unfoldedsurfacein a medial they are either foveal (<0.50), peripheral
maps, however, are often subtle; isoeccen- view (Fig.2, A and B) and in a ventralview (>120), or less well organizedretinotopi-
tricity lines can extend straight across sev- (Fig. 2, C and D) (19). VI is now clearly cally.Similarresultswereobtainedfromsix
eral areas, whereas polar angle maps often outlinedas a largemirrorimagepatch (yel- other individuals.
show only a shallow maximum or minimum low) dividedby ourincisionat the fundusof The areasrevealedin these experiments
at the border of an area. For example, the the calcarine sulcus. V2 forms two non- show a numberof similaritiesto areasorig-
red stripe at the ventral upper field vertical mirrorimage patches (blue) dorsallyand inallydiscoveredin nonhumanprimates.A
meridian border of VI-V2 actually extends ventrally.Two more areasare visible ven- schematiccomparisonof the humanvisual
across both VI and V2. In contrast, the tral and anteriorto ventralV2: mirrorim- areas with macaque monkey visual areas
visual field sign technique (7) provides an age VP (yellow) and, finally, non-mirror (21) and owl monkey visual areas (7) is
objective means to draw borders between imageV4v (blue). shown in Fig. 4. At the top, the three
areas on the basis of an analysis of the local In Fig.3, the cortexhas been completely flattened cortices are drawn at the same
relation between the directions of the fast- flattened, exposing the dorsal and lateral absolutescale, whereasat the bottom,they
est rate of change in these two coordinates. areasconcealed in Fig. 2. DorsalV2 is ad- are approximately scaledby the areaof VI.
Regions of the cortex that contain a reti- joined anteriorlyby a thin band of mirror It has long been knownthat VI in humans
notopic map, however distorted, can be di- imageV3 (yellow).just ventralandanterior is shiftedmediallyaroundthe occipitalpole
vided into two categories when viewed from to V3 the visualfield sign pattem degener- when comparedwith the locationof VI in
the cortical surface: those that contain a ates into noise near the center-of-gazerep- monkeys.The largersize of some human
mirror image representation of the visual resentationsof Vl and V2, likely the result extrastriateareasrelativeto humanVI may
field (like VI) and those that contain a of the difficultyof mappingfoveal cortex have partiallycompensatedfor this shift
non-mirror image representation of the vi-
sual field (like V2). This distinction is un-
Fig. 4. Schematic summa-- -

ry of retinotopic visual ar-- A 7


eas in the owl monkey (7), . N i
1 cm cm 1 cm
the macaque monkey (21),
and the human (present
study) at the same scale
(A)and approximatelynor-
malized by the area of Vl _ Owl
(B). (HumanVi is twice the / monkey
area of macaque Vi, with
larger ocular dominance
columns and cytochrome /
oxidase blobs, but a similar Macaque
v- 5 number of cells.) Visualar- Human monkey
eas in humans show a A/
close resemblance to visu- N

al areas originallydefined ~
B >. 1c
in monkeys. The anterior . cm
VI+.
border of the visual cortex-
in humans was estimated
by using the superiortem- -

VI+ poral sulcus and intrapari-


etal sulcus as landmarks.
In (C), the mapping func-
tions (heavy lines; scale is
on the leftaxis) and magni-
wihdme olrtomarersnthcnrl fication factor functions
(lightlines; scale is on the Human - Macaque Owl
Fig. 3. Visualfieldsigndisplayedon the complete- nght axis) are shown for monkey monkey
flattened
ly foe (2)o:hoccipital cortex (brightnessindicates
iulfedeod10ecnrc the upper field representa- 60 vi H V2 V3 V4 45
signmfcance of the response). Uppervisualfield tions of humanVi, V2, VP,8
iyortey a elsrrtntpc
Vi, V2,VP,and V4vand lowervisualfieldVl, V2, and V4 (24). The Vi map- ; 40 30
and V3 are now all visible.Thereare severalro- ping functions for owl
bustlyrespondingnon-mirror imageregionsante- monkeys (OM*, dotted) 'a U.
riorto dorsalV2 and V3 that may correspondin and macaque monkeys E 00 O
partto V3Aand dorsalV4 (not labeled).Regions (MM*,dashed) shown at 8
U )0 4 8 120 4 8 120 4 8 120
Eccentricity(degree)
4 8
the left were scaled up to
match the overall size of human Vi (25). An increased emphasis on the center-of-gaze in human Vi is
evident. CMF,cortical magnificationfactor.

SCIENCE * VOL. 268 * 12 MAY 1995 891

This content downloaded on Thu, 28 Feb 2013 05:51:11 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
[seefor examplethe width of VP in Fig.4B resultsof experimentsdesignedto examine scan (voxel size: 1 mm by 1 mm by 1 mm) used for
surface reconstruction that were collected in a sep-
and the region between V2 and MT in cortical responses to more complex and arate session with a head coil.
(22)1;retinotopicareasextendnearlyto the meaningfulstimulisuch as pictures,words, 12. The visual stimuli were generated in real time on a
anterior-posterior midpoint of the ventral scenes, and sentences. It is interestingto Silicon Graphics Onyx computer with GL graphics
temporallobe, justas they do in nonhuman note that several of the foci identified in libraryfunctions. The high-resolution red green blue
(RGB)video output signal was subsampled and con-
primates.The anteriorextent of retinotopic previousnoninvasive and invasive studies verted to a composite video signal that was used to
visualareasis most strikingin the unfolded of responsesto complex stimuli such as drive a Sony 2000 color liquid crystal display (LCD)
ventral view (Fig. 3D). Comparisonswith words(28) may overlapretinotopicallyor- projector. Informed consent to these experiments
was obtained from the participants, several of whom
the foldedventralview (Fig.3C) show that ganizedareasalreadyknown from nonhu- were also authors (M.I.S., A.M.D., R.B.H.T.).Supine
morecortex is hidden in the posteriorfolds man primates.The techniques used here participantslooked up into an adjustable angled mir-
of the ventral occipital lobe than in the could be adaptedto map tonotopyand am- ror that allowed them to comfortably view a frosted
back-projection screen arranged perpendicular to
moreanteriorventraltemporalfissures. plitopy in the auditorycortex and somato- the bore just below their chin. Residual head move-
The internalstructureof a visualcortical topy in the somatosensorycortex. ments were controlled through the use of a dental
map can be characterizedwith the linear impression bite bar made before the scan for each
REFERENCESAND NOTES participant and supported by an adjustable plastic
corticalmagnificationfactor,M(r), millime- yoke. Care was taken to block the participant's di-
tersof cortexper degreeof visualangle as a 1. M. I. Sereno and J. M. Allman,in The Neural Basis of rect view of the screen. Participants were given the
functionof eccentricity,r (23). We estimat- Visual Function, A. G. Leventhal, Ed. (Macmillan, opportunityto take ibuprofen (200 to 600 mg) before
the scan as a prophylacticagainst the minordiscom-
ed the valueof the parameters in a standard London, 1991), pp. 160-172; J. H. Kaas and L. A.
forts that typically accompany attempts to remain
equationformagnificationfactorby measur- Krubitzer, Neuroanatomy of Visual Pathways and
their Retinotopic Organization, B. Dreher and S. R. very still for extended periods of time. Images were
ing eccentricityon the corticalsurfacepar- Robinson, Eds. (Macmillan,London, 1991), pp. 302- projected onto the screen through a collimating lens
allel to the average direction of fastest 359; D. J. Felleman and D. C. Van Essen, Cereb. and subtended a maximum visual angle 350 wide
Cortex 1, 1 (1991); M. G. P. Rosa, J. G. Soares, M. and 260 tall. Stimuli consisted of black and white
change of eccentricityin each upper and checkerboard patterns counterphase flickeringat 4
Fiorani Jr., R. Gattass, Visual Neurosci. 10, 827
lower visual field representationdescribed (1993). Hz (that is, one cycle consists of 1/8 s of black and
above.The resultingmeasurements werefit 2. D. C. Van Essen and S. M. Zeki, J. Physiol. (London) 1/8 s of white), which is near the optimal temporal
frequency for drivingcortical neurons [(5);K. H. Fos-
with a corticalmappingfunction,D(r) [the 277,193 (1978); D. C. Van Essen and J. H. Maunsell,
ter, J. P. Gaska, M. Nagler, D. A. Pollen, J. Physiol.
J. Comp. Neurol. 191, 255 (1980).
distanceon the cortex as a functionof ec- 3. R. Gattass, A. P. B. Sousa, C. G. Gross, J. Neurosci. 365, 331 (1985)]. The checkerboard patterns were
centricity(the integralof the magnification 8,1831 (1988). scaled with eccentricity. These patterns stimulated a
circle inthe visual fieldwith a radius of 100 to 130. The
factorequation)]by adjustingthe parameters 4. R. B. Tootell, M. S. Silverman, R. L. De Valois, Sci-
ring and hemifield stimuli were designed to activate
ence 214, 813 (1981); C. G. Cusick, H. J. Gould, J.
(24). Figure4C showsthe dataand estimat- H. Kaas, J. Comp. Neurol. 230,311 (1984); R. B. H. approximately half of the ganglion cells in the central
ed mapping functions (points and heavy Tootell, S. L. Hamilton,M. S. Silverman,J. Neurosci. 100 to 130 of the two retinae at any one point in time.
13. The time series of activation (across the 128 images
lines) and corresponding corticalmagnifica- 5, 2786 (1985); J. Olavarriaand R. C. Van Sluyters, J.
of each slice) was examined on a pixel-by-pixel ba-
tion factor functions (light lines) for the Neurosci. Methods 15, 191 (1985); L. A. Krubitzer
and J. H. Kaas, VisualNeurosci. 5, 165 (1990). sis. The lineartrend and baseline offset for each pixel
upperfield representations in humanvisual 5. P. T. Fox, F. M. Miezin, J. M. Allman, D. C. Van time series was firstremoved by subtracting off a line
areasVl, V2, VP, and V4v. Essen, M. E. Raichle, J. Neurosci. 7, 913 (1987); S. fitted through the data by least squares. The discrete
Zeki etal., ibid. 11, 641 (1991); W. Schneider, D. C. Fourier transform of the time series for each pixel
Human visual areas show a prominent was then computed to give 128 complex values. In
Noll, J. D. Cohen, Nature 365,150 (1993).
emphasison central vision, as reflectedin 6. W. T. Newsome, J. H. R. Maunsell, D. C. Van Essen, retinotopicallyorganized areas, the amplitude spec-
the steepness of the mapping function, J. Comp. Neurol. 252,129 (1986); D. J. Felleman, J. trum showed a sharp peak at the stimulus rotationor
J. Knierim,D. C. Van Essen, Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. dilation-contraction frequency (-0.015 Hz) 5 to 30
which corresponds to a largecorticalmagni- times as large as the peaks at other frequencies. The
12,1182 (1986); R. Desimone and L. G. Ungerleider,
fication factor near the center-of-gaze.To J. Comp. Neurol. 248,164 (1986); D. R. Boussaoud, phase angle of the response at the stimulus rotation
comparethese resultswith those fromnon- R. Desimone, L. G. Ungerleider, ibid. 306, 554 (or dilation-contraction)frequency at a voxel is relat-
ed to the polar angle (or eccentricity) represented
humanprimates,we firstscaledthe mapping (1991); S. Neuenschwander, R. Gattass, A. P. B.
there. The phase angles for the pixels in a slice were
Sousa, M. C. G. P. Pihon, ibid. 340, 65 (1994).
functions for macaque monkey and owl 7. M. I. Sereno, C. T. McDonald, J. M. Allman, Cereb. then corrected to take account of delays resulting
monkeyVl to compensatefor the differenc- Cortex 4, 601 (1994); Cereb. Cortex, in press. from the known order in which the slices were col-
lected (1, 3, 5... .2, 4, 6. . .). Finally,the phase an-
es in the overall size of Vl (humanVl is 8. S. A. Engel et al., Nature 369, 525 (1994); E. A.
gles were mapped to differenthues whose intensity
DeYoe, P. Bandettini,J. Neitz, D. Miller,P. Winans, J.
approximately twice the areaof macaqueVl Neurosci. Methods 54,171 (1994). was a function of the ratio between the amplitude at
and aboutsix timesthe areaof owl monkey 9. J. W. Belliveau et al., Science 254, 716 (1991); K. K. the stimulus frequency and the average amplitude of
all other frequencies (this ratio can reach 25 to 30 in
Vi) (25). The resulting scaled mapping Kwong et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 89, 5675
V1). Our basic procedure is closely related to corre-
functionsareshownin the leftmostgraphin (1992).
10. A. M. Dale and M. I.Sereno, J. Cog. Neurosci. 5,162 lation of the signal with a sinusoid [compare P. A.
Fig. 4C. The macaque (dashed) and owl (1993).
Bandettinietal., Magn. Reson. Med. 25, 390 (1992);
monkey(dotted)curvesareconsiderablyless 11. Echo-planar images were collected on a 1 .5-T scan- P. A. Bandettini, A. Jesmanowicz, E. C. Wong, J. S.
ner (GE Signa) with full-body gradient coils (Ad- Hyde, ibid. 30, 161 (1993)].
steepthan the humancurve,indicatingthat 14. Ingeneral, the phase of the periodic response willbe
vanced NMR) by using an asymmetric spin echo
humanvisualareashave an extremeempha- sequence [AT(offset time) = -25 ms, TE(echo time) delayed because of a finite vascular response time.
sison the center-of-gaze,surpassingthatseen = 70 ms, TR(repetitiontime) = 3000 to 4000 ms) [G. The basic stimulus frequency was low enough so
that there was no whole-cycle phase ambiguity. Re-
not only in the nocturnalowl monkeybut L. Wismer et al., J. Comp. Assist. Tomogr. 12, 259
sidual phase delays were canceled by considering
(1988); J. R. Baker et al., Proc. Soc. Mag. Reson.
also that in the diurnalmacaquemonkey Med., 12, 1400 (1993); R. B. H. Tootell et al., J. the circularaverage of the phase angles obtained for
(26). It remainsto be seen whetherthis will Neurosci., in press]. A 5-inch receive-only surface opposite directions of stimulus motion (contraction
versus expansion, clockwise versus counterclock-
be explainedby an increasedemphasison radio frequency (RF)coil or a receive-only wrap RF
wise) at each voxel after the phase angles for the
coil was positioned near the occipital pole. Eight to
the center-of-gazein the retina,in the reti- 16 contiguous oblique 4-mm slices (voxel size: 3 mm opposite stimulus direction were reflected around 0.
nogeniculocorticalprojection, or in both by 3 mm by 4 mm) were chosen to lie approximately 15. We rejected scans in which substantial movement
(27). Muchhas been madeof the changesin perpendicular to the calcarine sulcus. Before and (-2 mm) had occurred. This was easily recognized
after the set of functional scans for a participant,we by viewing animations of the 128 images for each
areasbeyondthe primarycortex in humans recorded a Ti -weighted series of echo-planar imag- slice. Nevertheless, even in the presence of such
in comparisonwithnonhumanprimates.It is es [spin echo inversion recovery sequence: TE = 40 movements, a sharp peak at the basic stimulus fre-
now apparentthat the humanprimaryvisual ms, TI(inversiontime) = 1050 ms, TR = 20,000 ms, quency (-0.015 Hz)was usually largerthan the low-
NEX (number of excitations) = 2] with the same ori- er frequency peaks due to movement. Note that we
cortex is distinct as well. have discretelysampleda continuousfunction:the
entation, slice thickness, and field of view as the
These studies of retinotopy in humans functional scans. We used the Ti series'to align the intensityof a given voxel over time. Because that
provide a framework for understanding the functional scans with the higher resolution structural signal has not been explicitlylimitedin bandwidth

892 SCIENCE * VOL. 268 * 12 MAY 1995

This content downloaded on Thu, 28 Feb 2013 05:51:11 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
ommm REPORTS
before sampling, there is also the danger that fre- ogy await futurestudies of the functionalproperties of upper visual field Vl, V2, VP, and V4 were Mv1(r)=
quencies higher than the Nyquist critical frequency these retinotopicallydefined regions in humans. 20.05 [r+ 0.08]-126e; Mv2(r)= 25.19 [r+ 0.09]-1s53;
(f c-1/2A 0.1 2 - 0.25 Hz for TR = 2000 to 4000 19. The gradients in retinal eccentricity and polar angle Mvp(r)= 18.28 [r + 0.24]-1 75; and Mv4Vr)= 18.17
ms) could alias back into the spectrum we intended with respect to cortical x and y, Vr and VO, are [r + 0.24]-1l55
to measure. Changes in the signal due to blood ox- vector quantities whose norms have units of de- 25. The magnification factor at a particulareccentricity
ygenation are unlikely to be fast enough to cause grees per millimeter (the reciprocal of the magnifi- will be proportional to the size of VI if the shape of
such a problem. Artifactualsignals due to brain mo- cation factor). They were computed by fitting a the mapping function (cortical distance versus ec-
tion caused by rapid head motion were controlled plane to the r (or 0) values of the current vertex and centricity) does not change. To compare the map-
through the use of a bite bar. Brain movement arti- its immediate neighbors with least squares. The ping function for humans to that for owl monkeys
facts induced by cardiac pulsation are potentially clockwise angle, X, between Vr and VOwas then (OM)and macaque monkeys (MM),we first scaled
more problematic; however, there is enough varia- measured to determine whether the local represen- up the mapping functions for those animals to a
tion in frequency and phase of the cardiac signal to tation of the visual field at that vertex was non- human-sized Vl using values for B and C from the
avoid the generation of large aliased peaks in the mirrorimage (A near nr/2)or mirrorimage (X near literature (23). The resulting equations for cortical
Fourier amplitude spectrum given the number of -nr/2). Finally,the intensity of the field sign coloring magnification factor were MOM*(r)= 19.83 Vr+
stimulus cycles used in the present study. at each point was scaled by the average signifi- 2.5)-1i and MMM*(r) = 19.51 [r + 0.94]-1 0.
16. To paint the functionaldata onto the cortical surface, cance of the eccentricity and polar angle data sets. 26. Cortical magnification factor equations are some-
blinkcomparison in three orthogonal planes was first 20. The limited resolution of the video projector made it times constrained to have C = 1, which results in a
used to align the echo-planar inversion recovery im- difficultto present checkerboard patterns scaled with particularlysimple mapping function: D(r) = Alog[r
ages (3 mm by 3 mm by 4 mm, taken in the plane of eccentricity near the center-of-gaze while still cover- + B]. This equation fits nonhuman primate data
the functional images) with the high-resolution data ing a reasonable amount of visual angle peripherally. quite well. Our data could also be fit with this equa-
set (1 mm by 1 mm by 1 mm) used to reconstruct the Small imperfectionsin fixationalso h[avea much more tion, but only if we allowed B to be negative, which
corticalsurface (17). The functionaldata set was then deleterious effect at the center-of-gaze than they do in results in a singularity (infinitemagnification factor)
sampled with the cortical surface. By using a surface the near periphery.Inorder not to occlude the fixation before the center-of-gaze is reached. A good fit
nearerto the gray-whitematterborderthan to the pial cross, the central 0.50 of the ringand semicircle stim- without a singularity could only be achieved with C
surface, we were able to accurately assign activa- uliwere omitted. These considerations lead us to ex- above 1. The combinations of parameters given
tions to the correct bank of a sulcus despite the fact pect the periodic signal to falloff in the foveal parts of here fit the cortical distance data very closely but
that the individualfunctionalvoxels were comparable the visual field. This region of the visual field would be still give unrealistically large estimates of cortical
in size to the thickness of the cortex. Our success in expected to be represented across several square magnification at the exact center of the fovea (ex-
this endeavor is demonstrated by the lack of ambigu- centimeters of cortex just anteriorto the occipital pole trapolation of the dotted portion of cortical magni-
ityin recoveringthe closely apposed upper and lower on the lateralsurface of the brain.Mappingthis region fication curve in Fig. 4C), indicating that the stan-
visualfield representations situated on opposite sides would requirehigher resolution stimuliand additional dard equation for M is inadequate to accurately
of the calcarine sulcus. The lower resolution activa- averaging to overcome fixationjitter. describe cortical magnification in the very center of
tion data (one sample 3 mm by 3 mm) was 21. D. C. Van Essen, C. H. Anderson, D. J. Felleman, the fovea in humans even with C > 1. The data
smoothly interpolated onto the high-resolution sur- Science 255, 419 (1992). presented in (8) are consistent with ours in that D(r)
face reconstruction(one polygon _ 1 mm by 1 mm). 22. J. D. Watson etal., Cereb. Cortex 3, 79 (1993); R. B. is almost vertical near the center-of-gaze; accu-
17. The folded, unfolded, and flattened surfaces were H. Tootell and J. B. Taylor,ibid. 5, 39 (1995). rately fittingtheir cortical distance data at 20 to 100
made from high-resolution Ti -weighted images (1 23. R. B. H. Tootell, M. S. Silverman, E. Switkes, R. L. De eccentricity will give equally extreme estimates of
mm by 1 mm by 1 mm) optimized for gray-white Valois, Science 218, 902 (1982); D. C. Van Essen, cortical magnification at the exact center-of-gaze.
matter contrast with techniques similarto those pre- W. T. Newsome, J. H. Maunsell, VisionRes. 24, 429 27. It is possible that differences at the level of
sented in (10) [compare E. L. Schwartz, in Compu- (1984); D. J. Tolhurstand L. Ling,Hum. Neurobiol. 6, the human retina (or lateral geniculate nucleus)
tational Neuroscience, E. L. Schwartz, Ed. (Wiley, 247 (1988); J. C. Horton and W. F. Hoyt, Arch. Oph- may partly account for the differences in the
New York, 1990), pp. 295-315; and G. J. Carman, thalmol. 109,816 (1991); E. L. Schwartz, in Cerebral cortical magnification factor we observed. For ex-
thesis, CaliforniaInstituteof Technology (1990)]. The Cortex, vol. 10, Primaty VisualCortex in Primates, A. ample, human OFF midget retinal ganglion cells
skull was first automatically stripped off by "shrink- Peters and K. S. Rockland, Eds. (Plenum, New York, have dendritic fields with diameters 70% those of
wrapping" a stiff deformable template onto the 1994), pp. 359-41 1. their ON counterparts, a contrast not seen in the
brain. The gray-white matter boundary for each 24. The falloffin the linearcortical magnificationfactor, M macaque monkey retina [D. M. Dacey, J. Neurosci.
hemisphere was then estimated with a region-grow- (millimetersof cortex per degree of visual angle), with 13, 5334 (1993)]; and the fibers of Henle are espe-
ing method. The result was tessellated to generate a increasing eccentricity has often been described cially long in the human retina [C. A. Curcio and K.
surface (- 150,000 vertices) that was refined against with an equation of the form M(r)= A[r + B-c (23), A. Allen, J. Comp. Neurol. 300, 5 (1990)]. These
the MRIdata by using a deformable template algo- where r is the eccentricity and A, B, and C are con- observations are consistent with an increase in the
rithm,inspected for topological defects (forexample, stants (with C sometimes constrained to be 1). To foveal density of human midget ganglion cells with
"handles"), and remade if necessary. Itwas unfold- estimate M(r),we firstmarked activated regions (ver- respect to macaque monkeys, which could explain
ed by area-preserving curvature reduction. A com- tices) of the flattened visual field sign map by visual our result.
pletely flattened cortical surface was obtained by area and by whether they contained upper or lower 28. S. E. Petersen, P. T. Fox, A. Z. Snyder, M. E. Raichle,
cutting off the occipital lobe and incising the fundus field representations, omitting regions of ambiguous Science 249,1041 (1990); A. C. Nobre, T. Allison, G.
of the calcarine by disconnection of vertices. The visual field sign. The average direction of the cortical McCarthy,Nature 372, 260 (1994).
resulting surface fragment was then pushed onto a eccentricity gradient (the vector sum of Vr across 29. We thank M. Kutas, R. Buxton, L. AnIlo-Vento, and
coronal plane in one step and unfurledon the plane. every vertex assigned to an area) was estimated G. Ganis at University of California, San Diego
The vertex update rulewas modified from that in (10) independently for each upper and lower visual field (UCSD); T. Davis, J. Baker, C. Stern, R. Savoy, R.
to include both local area-preserving and shear-min- representation. The mapping function, D(r) = Benson, T. Campbell, and M. Vevea at the Massa-
imizing terms. Beautiful images of the cortical sur- fM(r)dr, where D(r)is the distance in millimeterson chusetts General Hospital Nuclear Magnetic Res-
face can be generated by rendering stacked slices the cortex from the center-of-gaze, was determined onance Center; S. Engel, B. Wandell, and D.
[H. Damasio and R. Frank, Arch. Neurol. 49, 137 for each area by measuring distances from the most Rumelhart at Stanford University;T. DeYoe and E.
(1992)]. Such images, however, lack explicit informa- central response zone (representing approximately Wong at Medical College of Wisconsin; J. Allmanat
tion about the cortical surface orientation and con- 0.50 eccentricity) along the direction of the average California Institute of Technology; and D. Dacey at
nectivity required for cortical unfolding. It is possible eccentricity gradient for each labeled vertex. The University of Washington for help, comments, and
to trace the cortical ribbon in individualslices and measurements were combined into 30 eccentricity discussions. Supported by Office of Naval Re-
manually connect them into a surface [M. L. Jouan- bins. The resultingpoints were then fitwith the equa- search NOOOI4-94-1-0856, the UCSD McDonnell-
det et al., J, Cog. Neurosci. 1, 88 (1989)], but these tion for the mapping function Pew Cognitive Neuroscience Center, the Norwe-
techniques are difficult to implement in regions gian Research Council, NIH grants MH47035,
where sulci run nearly parallelto slices, as they do in NICHD22614 (M. Kutas), EY07980, MH50054,
any slice plane through the occipital lobe. D(r) = fM(r)dr= A(r + B)c
1 -C C
IC= 1 HL3981 0, P01 CA48729, and by the Human Fron-
18. AreaVP has sometimes been labeled V3v (ventralV3), tier Science Program.
and area V4v has sometimes been labeled VA (ven- by adjusting the parameters A, B, and C. The result-
troanteriorarea). More definitivestatements of homol- ing cortical magnificationfactor equations for human 9 January 1995; accepted 13 March 1995

SCIENCE ? VOL.268 * 12 MAY1995 893

This content downloaded on Thu, 28 Feb 2013 05:51:11 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

You might also like