You are on page 1of 8

The Denver Developmental

Screening Test

A Normative Study
KEITH E. BARNES
ANNETTE STARK

The developmental norms of preschoolers from an urban


population, collected for the Denver Developmental
Screening Test, were compared with norms of
preschoolers from a Canadian rural and semirural
population.

Introduction The South Okanagan Health Unit has long been


concerned with the problem of screening preschoolers in a
In 1967 Frankenburg and Dodds introduced a new variety of functional areas (vision, hearing, general personal
developmental screening test for infants and preschoolers development). When the Denver Developmental Screening
between the ages of 2 weeks and 6.4 years.' The major Test (DDST) became available in Canada, the professional
purpose of this new screening instrument was to detect staff at the Health Unit quickly recognized that it could
slow development in four functional areas of development: provide an important addition to their screening program.
gross motor, fine motor, language, and personal-social. The However, as the DDST had been standardized on an urban
test was devised as a clinical tool simple enough in fornat population and as much of the South Okanagan Health
that it could be administered and scored by people who had Unit covers a rural or semirural population which is
no prior training in psychological testing. Essentially, it was significantly different (occupationally) from the Denver
to be used in order to alert professional personnel to the population (X2 = 53.40, df = 4, p < 0.001), it was necessary
possibility of developmental delay and the need for special to look at the need for local norms in order to determine
professional diagnostic investigation. A delay in any one of the validity of using the DDST in the South Okanagan
the four subtest areas was indicated when the child was not region. The need for standardization has been emphasized
able to pass an item which more than 90 per cent of further by the recent study of Bryant et al.,3 who reported
children his chronological age were able to do.* on age/performance differences found between Denver
The test was standardized "on 1,036 presumably children and Newcastle (England) children in the subtest
normal children (2 weeks to 6 years of age) whose families areas of gross motor and language development.
reflect the occupational and ethnic characteristics of the
population of Denver."' Method
Subjects
Dr. Barnes is with the South Okanagan Mental Health
Centre, and Ms. Stark is with the South Okanagan Health Subjects consisted of 122 male and 104 female,
Unit, Kelowna, British Columbia, Canada. Requests for caucasian preschoolers ranging in age from 2 weeks to 6.4
reprints should be sent to Dr. Barnes. years. There were 25 age categoriest all together; subjects
* Since the present study was carried out the scoring
method has been revised2 ; however, the criteria for scoring t Age categories were taken from the Frankenburg and
"delay" have remained unchanged. Dodds study.'

DENVER DEVELOPMENTAL SCREENING TEST 363


TABLE 1-intercorrelations between the Denver Norms of the Table 3 includes the means and standard deviations for
DDST and Norms Collected in the South Okanagan the four subtest areas by occupational class.
Health Unit Region (N = 226) The distribution of scores according to mean, standard
deviations, and range for each subtest area by age groups is
Denver Region
presented graphically in Figures 1 to 4.
South Okanagan
Health Unit Gross Fine Personal-
Region Motor Motor Language Social Discussion
Gross Motor 0.996* In the present normative study it has been demon-
Fine Motor 0.998*
Language 0.996*
strated that the DDST can validly be used on a rural and
Personal-Social 0.996* semirural Canadian population. The correlations between
the two sets of norns (Denver and South Okanagan
*
Significant at the p < 0.005 level. regions) were of very high magnitude in each of the four
subtest areas. Such a finding generally indicates very good
were selected on the basis of a stratified random sample of separation across age groups; however, as can be seen from
the general population residing in the South Okanagan Figures 1 to 4, there is enough overlap between age groups
Health Unit region. All those children who were considered that the DDST does appear to be serving a useful function
to be of high risk developmentally (premature, breech in measuring preschool child development, i.e., the test
deliveries, etc.) were excluded from the study. does seem to be differentiating something over and above
The sample population was stratified according to the simple chronological age.
occupational status of the head of the family as indicated in
the 1966 Canadian census. The occupational breakdown TABLE 2-Correlational Matrix by Age and Subtest Area
was as follows: professional, 8.00 per cent; managerial,
16.44 per cent; sales, 7.56 per cent; craftsman, 28.89 per Subtest Areas
cent; and semi- and unskilled, 39.11 per cent. A representa- Fine Personal-
tive sample for each of these occupational groups was Ages No. Motor Language Social
selected from each of the six school district areas in the
South Okanagan region. months
Gross Motor
Procedure 1-3 28 0.820 0.790 0.572
4-6 28 0.465 0.277 0A20
7-9 25 0.398 0.436 0.482
Each subject was assessed on the DDST by a trained 10-12 26 0.239 OA87 0A94
and experienced examiner. Inter-rater reliability for the five 13-16 25 0.607 0.337 0.324
examiners varied between 0.93 and 0.95. 17-26 27 0.438 0.535 0.732
The norms were computed by determining the mean 27-45 31 0.538 0.387 0.553
scores for each age category in the sample population and 46-78 36 0.376 0.510 0A92
then comparing these scores with the mean scores for the All ages 226 0.972 0.961 0.971
same age categories collected by Frankenburg and Dodds in
the original standardization study. Fine Motor
1-3 0.708 0.665
4-6 0.299 0.803
7-9 OA34 0.516
Results 10-12 0.445 0.506
13-16 0.596 0.629
The correlations between the two sets of norms for the 17-26 0.534 0.627
four major functional areas of the DDST are shown in 27-45 0.602 0.794
Table 1. 46-78 OA94 0.161
Table 2 includes the intercorrelations of the four All ages 0.962 0.970
subtest areas for the South Okanagan Health Unit sample
by age groups. Frankenburg and Dodds' do not report the Language
1-3 0.747
subtest intercorrelations nor the intercorrelations between 4-6 0.303
subtest scores and occupational class, school district, and 7-9 0.379
sex; hence, it was not possible to compare the two sets of 10-12 0.598
norms in these areas.* 13-16 0.448
17-26 0.572
* The computer program for the original Denver 27-45 0.512
sample is no longer available so this information cannot be 46-78 0.690
recalled. Frankenburg, W. K., personal communication, All ages 0-955
1970.

364 AJPH APRIL, 1975, Vol. 65, No.4


TABLE 3-Means and Standard Deviations for the Four Subtest Areas by Occupational Class
Gross Motor Fine Motor Language Personal-Social

Occupational Class No. X S.D. X S.D. X S.D. X S.D.

1. Unskilled 23 17.39 8.07 16.87 7.27 10.09 5.19 13.04 6.78


2. Semiskilled 67 17.28 721 17.05 7.09 10.45 5.16 13.63 6.84
3. Craftsman 62 17.93 9.88 17.87 9.02 11.35 6.47 1327 7_86
4. Sales 16 14.56 7.03 15.00 6.36 9.50 4.50 11.75 6.69
5. Managerial 37 17.16 8.51 17.00 7.69 10.40 6.15 13.16 7.09
6. Professional 21 13.81 6.88 14.24 6.72 8.09 4.84 10.A3 6.27

Gross Motor Scores


Ages
(in months)
0 5, 10 15 20 25 A30

1- 3

4- 6

7- 9

10 - 12
_- --~

13 - 16

17 - 26

27 - 45

46 - 78 Mean 'II
1cr +l s .j

lowes aihet
score score

FIGURE 1 Distribution of Gross Motor scores by age groups.

The intercorrelations of the four subtest areas are quite small town autonomy, wage levels, rural school support,
substantial across most of the age groupings, which raises community growth, social malaise, and tax rates.4 The lack
the question as to whether there is sufficient differentiation of sex differences, especially in the language area in the first
to justify four separate scores. 3 years of development, is particularly puzzling when one
Unfortunately, it was not possible to compare the two takes into account the variety of studies in which strong
normnative samples in other areas such as occupational class support has been found for superior performance in
or sex. However, in the present investigation, except for the girls.510 This finding could be a reflection on the
age category 10 to 12 months (t 2.20, df = 24,p < 0.05),
=
educational program being emphasized in the South
there were no significant sex differences, nor were there Okanagan region by the Public Health Unit; for the past 7
any significant differences between the six school districts years fathers of preschoolers have been strongly encouraged
in the geographic region studied. This lack of significant to involve themselves more with their children, especially
differences across school districts is rather an interesting their sons, and the attendance by future fathers at the
finding when one realizes that the six school districts differ prenatal clinics now runs between 70 and 75 per cent.
on such factors as suburb conditions, population density, In the analysis of occupational class differences, there

DENVER DEVELOPMENTAL SCREENING TEST 365


Age3 Fine M5otor Scores
(in nmnths) 5 10 15 20 25 30

1 - 3

4- 6
_~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~
i-LZ4ZH
*-E4Zii

7 - 9

10 - 12

13 - 16

17 - 26

27 - 45

46 - 78 i ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ oI -
Mean

lowest -ig ieat


score score

FIGURE 2 Distribution of Fine Motor scores by age groups.

were some rather surprising results. For example, in all four with the Denver DDST norms, and the authors recommend
subtest areas, children of professional families consistently the use of caution when using published norms for lower
obtained the lowest mean scores of all the occupational class preschoolers located in urban areas.
groups in the study while children of craftsman families The fact that the present study did not find significant
consistently scored the highest. Although this finding was differences in either the language or fine motor areas
not significant statistically for a number of the occupa- between middle and lower class children could be attribut-
tional classes, it was true in all four subtest areas between able to two possible factors: one is that all of the
professional and craftsman families (t = 2.11, df = 81,p < preschoolers were caucasian (there are no black or Puerto
0.025 for Gross Motor; t = 1.95, df = 81,p < 0.05 for Fine Rican preschoolers living in the South Okanagan region);
Motor; t = 1.81, df = 81, p < 0.05 for Language; and t = the other factor is that the South Okanagan Valley has no
1.82, df = 81,p < 0.05 for Personal-Social). A similar result urban areas within its boundaries. Hence, the relatively
was found in the personal-social area between the children poor levels of language and fine motor functioning found in
of professional and those of semiskilled families (t = 2.00, the Sandler et al. study may be due primarily to conditions
df = 86, p < 0.025). of urban living and ethnic characteristics rather than lower
These findings suggest that, contrary to popular socioeconomic class.
opinion, children of working class and lower middle class One possible explanation of these present findings may
parents may well be developmentally equal or superior to have something to do with the changes in occupational
children of professional and managerial parents. This conditions and educational levels for the average working
relationship is particularly surprising in the language area, class person that have taken place in the last 20 years.
for most reports on the development of language show a There appears to be little doubt that educational level has
marked positive relationship between language skills and risen substantially for the average working person in the last
middle and upper class occupations.' 1-19 two decades; furthermore, the work week for these same
These findings also differ substantially from the results people has been reduced substantially, while the work week
reported by Sandler et al.20 In that study considerable for professional and managerial occupations has increased
differences were found in the language and fine motor areas substantially.
for black and Puerto Rican preschoolers when compared As Milner's2l findings on the relationship of language

366 AJPH APRIL, 1975, Vol.65, No.4


Ages Language Scores
(in months) 0 10 M 201

1- 3

4 - 6

7 - 9

10 - 12

13 - 16

17 - 26
~~~~~~-i4m~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~i

27 - 45

45 - 78

-1 Mean +i r

lowes hghst
score score

FIGURE 3 Distribution of Language scores by age groups.

development and length of time spent with parents in exception as noted above. That there are essentially no sex
conversation and at meal times are probably as valid for differences in any of the four subtest areas and that there
nonblack families as for black ones, and since Irwin S222 are some unexpected significant differences between chil-
findings indicate that working class mothers can be dren of different occupational classes are both findings
educated to help develop their preschool children's lan- contrary to previous studies on early child development.
guage through reading, then the present findings are not Furthermore, how well the DDST actually predicts signifi-
necessarily contradictory to the results of studies carried cant developmental delay needs further investigation, for
out 20 and 30 years ago. Certainly, practically all mothers although Frankenburg et al.3 report relatively high levels
are better educated in infant care than mothers were two of agreement between the revised method of scoring and
decades ago, and in this particular geographic region the the DDST and such external criterion measures as the
educational program offered by the public health depart- Bayley Infant Scale for Children and the Stanford Binet
ment to young mothers is especially comprehensive and Form LM, these comparisons are essentially concurrent
intensive. validation studies and do not indicate the longitudinal
It is indeed unfortunate that the normative data from predictability of DDST scores, especially where the
the original DDST study were not retrievable, especially the abnormal and questionable ones might be compared to
sex and occupational class variables. However, in an internal pediatric and neurological follow-up studies, i.e., to
analysis of the test's ability to discriminate across age levels, deternine exactly how many false positives and false
it stood up exceedingly well. Only one subtest area failed to negatives might actually exist over an extended period of
discriminate adequately and that was in the fine motor area time.* Furthermore,, although the comparison between
for age groups 7 to 9 versus 10 to 12 months (t = 1.268, df DDST scores and scores on the Bayley Infant Scale for
= 49, N.S.). All other age groupings in all four subtest areas Children constitutes a reasonably appropriate concurrent
were significantly different from each other. It is possible validity test, the comparison between DDST and Stanford
that this one nonsignificant finding is due to a chance Binet Form LM scores does not, if it is indeed true that
factor only.
In general, the DDST appears to be an adequately * The present authors are currently involved in such a
constructed screening device in that it discriminates across follow-up study and will report on the results at a later
the age groupings in all four subtest areas with one date.

DENVER DEVELOPMENTAL SCREENING TEST 367


Ages Personal-Social Scores
(in months) 0 5 10 15 2,0
1-3

4-6

7 -9 p_j

10 -12 t

13 - 16

17 - 26

46 -78 .

Mean ,

lowes g est
score score

FIGURE 4 Distribution of Personal-Social scores by age groups.

"the D.D.S.T. is not an intelligence test" (p. 190 in 5. Harms, I. E., and Spiker, C. C. Factors Associated with
Reference 1). the Performance of Young Children on Intelligence
Scales and Tests of Speech Development. J. Genet.
Psychol. 94:3-22, 1959.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 6. Irwin, 0. C., and Chen, H. P. Development of Speech
during Infancy: Curve of Phonemic Types. J. Exp.
The authors wish to express their sincere appreciation Psychol. 36:431-436, 1946.
to Lucille Amundsen, Stella Barnes, Pat Brown-Clayton, 7. Irwin, 0. C., and Chen, H. P. Infant Speech: Vowel and
Betty Found, and Rosemarie Yates for their invaluable Consonant Frequency. J. Speech Disord. 11:123-125,
assistance in assessing all of the children in the present 1946.
study on the DDST. The authors also wish to express their 8. McCarthy, D. A. Language Development in Children.
appreciation to Dr. Lorne Kendall of Simon Fraser In Manual of Child Psychology, Ed. 2, edited by
University for his statistical guidance in the analysis of the Carmichael, L. Wiley, New York, 1954.
data and for his careful evaluation of the manuscript 9. McCarthy, D. A. Research in Language Development:
preparation. Retrospect and Prospect. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev.
24:3-24, 1959.
10. Moore, T. Language and Intelligence: A Longitudinal
Study of the First Eight Years. I. Patterns of
References Development in Boys and Girls. Hum. Dev.
10:88-106, 1967.
1. Frankenburg, W. K., and Dodds, J. B. The Denver 11. Beckey, R. E. A Study of Certain Factors Related to
Developmental Screening Test. J. Pediatr. Retardation of Speech. J. Speech and Hear. Disord.
71:181-191, 1967. 7:223-249, 1942.
2. Frankenburg, W. K., Dodds, J. B., and Fandal, A. The 12. Bernstein, B. Social Class and Linguistic Development:
Revised Denver Developmental Screening Test Manual. A Theory of Social Learning. In Economy, Education
University of Colorado Press, Denver, 1970. and Society, edited by Halsey, A. G., et al. Free Press,
3. Bryant, G. M., Davies, K. J., Richards, F. M., and New York, 1961.
Voorhees, S. A Preliminary Study of the Use of the 13. Davis, E. A. The Development of Linguistic Skill in
Denver Developmental Screening Test in a Health Twins, Singletons with Siblings, and Only Children
Department. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 15:33-40, 1973. from Age Five to Ten Years. In Institute of Child
4. Macurdy, B. A. An Ecological Study of Hospitalized Welfare Monographs, Ser. No. 14. University of
Mental Disorder in British Columbia. Master's thesis, Minnesota Press, Minneapolis, 1937.
Simon Fraser University, 1971. 14. Fisher, M. S. Language Patterns of Preschool Children.

368 AJPH APRIL, 1975, Vol.65, No.4


In Child Development Monographs, No. 15. Teacher's Developmental Study of Language. Genet. Psychol.
College, Columbia University, New York, 1934. Monogr. 23:3-141, 1941.
15. Irwin, 0. C. Infant Speech: The Effect of Family 20. Sandler, L., Van Campen, J., Ratner, C., Stafford, C.,
Occupational Status and of Age on Use of Sound and Weismar, R. Responses of Urban Preschool
Types. J. Speech Hear. Disord. 13:224-226, 1948. Children to a Developmental Screening Test. J. Pediatr.
16. Sampson, 0. C. A Study of Speech Development in 77:775-781, 1970.
Children 18-30 Months. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 21. Milner, E. A Study of the Relationship between
26:194-202, 1956. Reading Readiness in Grade One School Children and
17. Templin, M. C. Certain Language Skills in Children. In Patterns of Parent-Child Interaction. Child Dev.
Institute of Child Welfare Monographs, Ser. No. 26. 22:95-112, 1951.
University of Minnesota Press, Minneapolis, 1957. 22. Irwin, 0. C. Infant Speech: Effect of Systematic
18. Van Alstyne, D. The Environment of Three Year Old Reading of Stories. J. Speech Hear. Res. 3:187-190,
Children: Factors Related to Intelligence and Vocabu- 1960.
lary Tests. In Contributions to Education, No. 366. 23. Frankenburg, W. K., Goldstein, A. D., and Camp. B. W.
Teacher's College, Columbia University, New York, The Revised Denver Developmental Screening Test: Its
1929. Accuracy as a Screening Instrument. J. Pediatr.
19. Young, F. M. An Analysis of Certain Variables in a 79:988-995, 1971.

A TRIBUTE TO DR. SCHLESINGER

The Maternal and Child Health Section Council of the American Public Health Association, at its
annual meeting in New Orleans October 22, 1974, prepared a resolution, published below, in eulogy
to their esteemed colleague, Edward R. Schlesinger, MD, MPH.
A fellow of APHA, Dr. Schlesinger was past Chairman of the Program Area Committee on Child
Health, and had served as Secretary and Vice-Chairman of the Maternal and Child Section. He had
been a member of APHA since 1940.
Born in New York City in 1911, Dr. Schlesinger was a graduate of Columbia University, received
his MD from that University, and his MPH from the Johns Hopkins School of Hygiene and Public
Health. At the time of his death, Dr. Schlesinger was Professor and Head of Maternal and Child
Health, and Associate Dean for Academic Affairs, at the University of Pittsburgh Graduate School of
Public Health. The Edward R. Schlesinger Memorial Fund has been established at the University of
Pittsburgh.
RESOLUTION
At this, the annual meeting of the American Public Health Association, the Maternal and Child
Health Section takes formal note of the untimely loss of our esteemed colleague, Dr. Edward R.
Schlesinger.
Dr. Schlesinger had a profound influence on health workers in maternal and child health. Many
members of the section and of the APHA have been inspired, encouraged, trained, and so enabled to
improve the public health because of their valuable association with him.
Throughout his long career, Dr. Schlesinger was a loyal and devoted member of the section and
added substantially to its strength. During his many years as a member he held a number of positions
of responsibility, including that of Chairman. He was instrumental in the production of a number of
basic studies and contributed extensively to the literature. He was the author, together with Dr.
Pauline Stitt, of The Maternal & Child Health Section: A Political & Social History, which is being
distributed for the first time today.
Administrator, investigator, author, and teacher, Ed Schlesinger made a most significant impact
on the health of mothers and children. We shall miss him.

DENVER DEVELOPMENTAL SCREENING TEST 369


This article has been cited by:

1. Mohammad R. Deemeh, Marziyeh Tavalaee, Mohammad H. Nasr-Esfahani. 2015. Health of Children Born Through
Artificial Oocyte Activation. Reproductive Sciences 22:3, 322-328. [Crossref]
2. M. Van Dam, A. Hallemans, S. Truijen, V. Segers, P. Aerts. 2011. A cross-sectional study about the relationship between
morphology and kinematic parameters in children between 15 and 36 months. Gait & Posture 34:2, 159-163. [Crossref]
3. Emin Pala, Muferet Erguven, Sirin Guven, Makbule Erdogan, Tulin Balta. 2010. Psychomotor Development in Children
with Iron Deficiency and Iron-Deficiency Anemia. Food and Nutrition Bulletin 31:3, 431-435. [Crossref]
4. M. Van Dam, A. Hallemans, S. Truijen, P. Aerts. 2010. A cross-sectional study about the relationship between
morphology and step-time parameters in children between 15 and 36 months. Gait & Posture 32:3, 400-404. [Crossref]
5. M. Van Dam, A. Hallemans, P. Aerts. 2009. Growth of segment parameters and a morphological classification for children
between 15 and 36 months. Journal of Anatomy 214:1, 79-90. [Crossref]
6. Ann Hallemans, Dirk De Clercq, Peter Aerts. 2006. Changes in 3D joint dynamics during the first 5 months after the
onset of independent walking: A longitudinal follow-up study. Gait & Posture 24:3, 270-279. [Crossref]
7. Gerhard Winneke. 1995. Endpoints of developmental neurotoxicity in environmentally exposed children. Toxicology
Letters 77:1-3, 127-136. [Crossref]
8. Gillian M. Bryant, Kathleen J. Davies, Robert G. Newcombe. 1979. Standardisation of the Denver Developmental
Screening Test for Cardiff Children. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 21:3, 353-364. [Crossref]
9. Alan S. Appelbaum. 1978. Validity of the Revised Denver Developmental Screening Test for Referred and Nonreferred
Samples. Psychological Reports 43:1, 227-233. [Crossref]

You might also like