You are on page 1of 12

Received: 24 December 2017 Revised: 22 November 2018 Accepted: 23 November 2018

DOI: 10.1002/acp.3501

RESEARCH ARTICLE

Conceptual foundations and validation of the Pseudoscientific


Belief Scale
Angelo Fasce1 | Alfonso Picó2

1
Department of Philosophy, University of
Valencia, Valencia, Spain Summary
2
Department of Psychobiology, University of In this study, we present the Pseudoscientific Belief Scale (PSEUDO; α = 0.90).
Valencia, Valencia, Spain
The conceptual foundations of the scale include (a) a philosophically grounded and
Correspondence
Angelo Fasce, University of Valencia,
functional demarcation criterion between science and pseudoscience; (b) an analysis
Department of Philosophy, Facultad de of the pseudoscientific status of science denialism, which is integrated into the scale;
Filosofía, Avd. Blasco Ibañez, 30, 46010
Valencia, Spain.
and (c) a bibliographic justification to back up the inclusion of each item. The validation
Email: angelofasce@hotmail.com process is carried out based on two studies. Study 1 (N = 3,416) uses both exploratory
factor analysis and cluster analysis: Conceptually congruent factors are not reported.
Study 2 (N = 292) includes a total of 21 sociodemographic, personality, and cognitive
independent variables, as well as the two thematic subscales of PSEUDO as dependent
variables, in order to analyze the psychological profile of both sets of believers.
Results show high similarity between both groups, confirming construct validity.

KEY W ORDS

PSEUDO, pseudoscience, pseudo‐theory promotion, science denialism, unwarranted beliefs

1 | I N T RO D U CT I O N correlation and regression analysis to explore the interactions


between the two thematic subscales of PSEUDO and 21 independent
Pseudoscience is a phenomenon that has accompanied science variables: The study shows that the interactions of both subscales are
throughout its increasing social and epistemological prestige, seeking congruent, confirming construct validity.
legitimacy, and protection by the exploitation of its aesthetics, its We already know well the dynamics of other types of unwar-
media, its pedagogical spaces, and so on. In this study, we will develop ranted beliefs such as the paranormal or conspiracy theories by the
the theoretical foundations and validate a psychometric tool for an use of reliable psychometric tools (Brotherton, French, & Pickering,
adequate measurement of pseudoscientific beliefs. The Pseudoscien- 2013; Tobacyk, 2004), but the psychological and sociological charac-
tific Belief Scale (PSEUDO; Appendix A) is the first tool of its type that teristics of pseudoscientific deceit have been scarcely studied from
presents sophisticated conceptual foundations and a high level of an academic and reliable point of view (to consult the predictors of
reliability. To present PSEUDO's most basic foundations, we will the phenomenon and the sociodemographic and psychological
briefly analyze the problem of demarcation, adopting the demarcation differences between these three groups of beliefs, see Lobato,
criterion developed in Fasce (2018a) in order to elucidate the nature Mendoza, Sins, & Chin, 2014; Fasce & Picó, 2018a). The main reason
of pseudoscience and to assure its correct screening during the for this situation is due to the underdeveloped theoretical and
construction of the scale, in which we will justify the inclusion of each psychometric framework of pseudoscience in psychological contexts.
of its items. We will also assess the existence of two types of pseudo- For example, in the scale used by Majima (2015), we find as pseudo-
science: pseudo‐theory promotion (PP) and science denialism (SD)—a science paranormal ideas or myths such as precognition, belief in
distinction that has been theoretically postulated (Hansson, 2017) God, or listening to Mozart to increase the intelligence of children.
but has never been empirically tested. To validate the scale, we None of these issues could be characterized as a complete and explicit
conducted two studies. In Study 1 (N = 3,416), we use exploratory case of pseudoscientific rhetoric (Blancke, Boudry, & Pigliucci, 2016;
factor and cluster analysis: No factor model was satisfactory, but high Fasce, 2017). The same can be said about Lundström and
reliability is reported (α = 0.90). In Study 2 (N = 292), we use Jakobsson (2009; 40% of their scale of pseudoscientific beliefs is

Appl Cognit Psychol. 2019;33:617–628. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/acp © 2018 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 617
618 FASCE AND PICÓ

nonpseudoscientific); Tseng, Tsai, Hsieh, and Hung (2014; 60% of propositions, and so on. For example, paranormal thinking meets
nonpseudoscientific items); Franz and Green (2013; 100% paranormal (1), as, by definition, it appeals to phenomena outside the domain of
items); or Johnson and Pigliucci (2004; 80% of nonpseudoscientific science (Tobacyk, 2004), but it would not be pseudoscience because
such as aliens in Area 51, voodoo, or the belief that breaking a it does not meet (4). Parapsychology, in contrast, can present an
mirror brings bad luck). optimal methodology and does not necessarily fulfill (2), but it
Nevertheless, other studies have handled more appropriate scales meets (1) and (3), because the existence of the so‐called “psi phenom-
for the measurement of pseudoscientific beliefs, although always with ena” has never been demonstrated, and parapsychological statements
theoretical problems and without proper empirical validation. For are not backed up by confirmatory evidence, and (4), therefore, it
example, Lobato et al. (2014) used a 37‐item scale of unwarranted is pseudoscience. Conspiracy theories could avoid (1) but not (2),
beliefs that includes pseudoscience as a thematic 12‐item subscale. and depending on whether they meet (4) will or will not be
Nonetheless, within these items, we find controversial cases such as pseudoscience.
“A person chooses to be homosexual, bisexual, or heterosexual,” or This criterion is the theoretical basis of PSEUDO and its most
items that show ambiguity regarding the interpretation of responses, important feature is (4), so it is worth discussing this item in some
such as “Homeopathic treatments are just as valid as traditional detail. Several theorizations have been carried out on the most used
medical treatments for serious illnesses”—the interpretation depends tactics by nonscience in order to disguise itself as science (Blancke
on the unknown beliefs of subjects about the effectiveness of tradi- et al., 2016; Fasce, 2018b). Because the epistemic vigilance about
tional medical treatments and about when an illness is serious or scientific claims is not usually carried out in relation to the message
not. A similar case can be found in Bensley, Lilienfeld, and Powell but instead to the reliability of the source of information, its apparent
(2014), where a 14‐item scale is used, with seven of its items referring honesty, and competence, pseudoscientists slide science from its usual
to pseudoscience and the rest referring to well‐established scientific standards of epistemic authority seeking, instead, an evaluation of
fields (reversed encoded). However, within those seven items, we find scientificity considering an informal notion of authority. This informal
two as very controversial, such as alchemy, which is usually classified notion of authority is characterized by some recognizable features
as a protoscience, and astrology, already considered by the most used (Goldman, 2001) whose exploitation constitutes the proximal causes
paranormal belief scale (Tobacyk, 2004). of pseudoscience.

a. Dialectical superiority, based on pseudoprofound logorrhea that


generates illusion of explanatory depth in the use of
2 | A DE M A R C A T I O N C R I T E RI O N
decontextualized scientific language.

A necessary preliminary step to the construction of PSEUDO was b. External support superficially similar to that of scientific theories.
the adoption of a functional and well‐founded demarcation criterion. For this, they seek to imitate and/or to introduce themselves in
For a technical discussion on the problems of classical demarcation the publications, the education, the organization, or the practice
proposals (Popper's, Lakatos', Kuhn's, multicriterial and axiological of science. Pseudoscientists often create “scientific” journals,
approaches) and on recent failed demarcation attempts (such as the such as NLP Research Journal and Journal of Psychiatric Orgone
family resemblance approach), see Fasce (2018a, 2018b). On the basis Therapy, or try to elude the control mechanisms of prestigious
of all this previous philosophical analysis, we have defined pseudosci- scientific journals.
ence using the demarcation criterion developed in Fasce (2018a; for c. Ethical integrity and epistemic superiority. The main features of
the philosophical framework or “metacriterion” that underlies this the sense of belonging of pseudoscientists are, in addition to their
criterion, see Fasce, 2018b), a tool based on the critical analysis of ambivalent relationship with science, victimhood and the claim of
21 multicriteria and capable of offering a well‐grounded and useful being an ethical and an epistemological step above “official”
definition of pseudoscience. The use of this criterion is justified for science.
several reasons, such as its minimum level of philosophical commit-
ments and its capacity to differentiate pseudoscience from other These traits provide pseudoscience with an aura of sophistication
forms of nonscience. and exclusivity, which is why its consumers are mostly people of
(Pseudoscience) (1 and/or 2 and/or 3) and (4). medium‐high or high socioeconomic status (McCright & Dunlap,
2011; MSPSI, 2011). Furthermore, all these rhetorical mechanisms
1. Refers to entities and/or processes outside the domain of science. lead to a motivational state of epistemic negligence that inhibits
2. Makes use of a deficient methodology. detailed scrutiny of beliefs among subjects who prefer distorted but

3. Is not supported by evidence. simple versions of scientific theories (Talmont‐Kamisnki, 2013) to


keep on their lay cognition.
4. Is presented as scientific knowledge.

It being necessary to fulfill any of the first three items, and 3 | TWO TYPES OF PSEUDOSCIENCE
sufficiently in conjunction with (4), to be considered pseudoscience,
this demarcation criterion can demarcate between science and It has been proposed from a theoretical point of view that there would
pseudoscience any epistemological product—theories, hypothesis, be two types of pseudoscience: PP and SD. Hansson (2017) has
FASCE AND PICÓ 619

substantiated this distinction that would characterize SD as a subclass Regarding the writing of the items, it presents two characteristics
of pseudoscience, but the differences between both subclasses have that try to solve problems of previous attempts to measure pseudosci-
not been explored in depth from an empirical point of view. On the entific beliefs:
one hand, PP would be the classic form of pseudoscience, the one
that has traditionally dominated the debate on the phenomenon, a. The items try to include all the dimensions of the pseudoscience
characterized by the promotion of a certain pseudo‐theory by an to which they refer, including its nuances and, thus, being
organized group. These pseudo‐theories would be presented as inclusive regarding internal dissidence. For example, “There is
well‐established science, so the attitude of this type of pseudoscien- no conclusive evidence that climate change is real or that
tists towards science would be ambivalent: Without the support of human activity is its cause,” includes two different dimensions
the scientific community that they need to simulate to be part of, of climate change denial: anthropogenesis and the factual reality
they find themselves in a love/hate relationship with scientists, of global warming. The same could be said of items such as
their methods, their institutions, and their publications. Examples of “Quantum mechanics has great implications in the explanation
this form of pseudoscience would be homeopathy, iridology, of consciousness and/or in the treatment of diseases,” which
anthroposophy, psychoanalysis, or orgone. includes two dimensions of quantum mysticism, or “There are
On the other hand, science deniers (Oreskes & Conway, 2010) areas of our body surface, such as the feet, hands and/or ears
would be motivated by their enmity towards a specific scientific in which we find representations of our entire anatomy,”
theory. Examples of SD would be the denial of climate change, the which includes two types of pseudoscience that incorporate
antivaccination movement, or the denial of diseases caused by magical‐representative thought—reflexology and auriculotherapy;
tobacco. In these cases, we find a motivated rejection—lucrative, emo- we have left chiropractic out of this item because it is already
tional, or ideological—of well‐established scientific theories, simulating mentioned in Item 5.
from a pseudoskeptical standpoint a false controversy among scien- b. The terminology used in the items emphasizes the semantic
tists (Torcello, 2016). In this regard, PP would be pseudoscience at field of science, repeatedly using terms such as “evidence,”
the level of theories, with complex theoretical content and avoiding “scientifically proven,” or “is part of science,” because PSEUDO
direct confrontation with science. Science denialism would be pseudo- aims to measure beliefs acquired due to pseudoscientific deceit.
science regarding the soundness of well‐established scientific theories, For example, “There is scientific evidence that confirm the
being openly hostile towards them based on fake controversies. possibility of repressing traumatic memories as a defense mech-
Of course, we can find hybrid cases of pseudoscience whose rhetoric anism” does not measure the belief in this pseudophenomenon
mixes the promotion of a pseudo‐theory and science denial—for understood as an urban legend or as a psychomyth but
example, German new medicine combines the denial of oncology the belief that it is backed up by scientific evidence. This gives
and the promotion of a very complex theory to support the alleged rise to the measurement of pseudoscience as a specific
emotional etiology of cancer. phenomenon.
The problem of these studies on SD in relation to its characteriza-
tion as a subclass of pseudoscience is that all are focused on a
single case of science denial—mainly antivaccination (e.g., Browne,
Thompson, Rockloff, & Pennycook, 2015) and climate change denial 5 | STUDY 1
(e.g., Lewandowsky, Gignac, & Oberauer, 2013). For this reason, it is
not possible to consider whether the conjunction of these cases of
5.1 | Sample and procedure
science denial work or not as a valid psychological construct (Study
1 of this article). Furthermore, both subclasses have never been
The validation of PSEUDO is based on a heterogeneous sample of
explicitly compared in order to assess whether their correlations with
3,416 subjects, recruited by social networks (Facebook and Twitter)
some sociological, personality, and/or cognitive variables diverge
for an online fulfilment of the scale. No credits or rewards were
(Study 2 of this article).
offered to our participants in exchange for their collaboration;
39.9% were women, and 60.1% were men, with an average age of
38.3 and with 25.8% of the sample in the 14–30 years range,
48.6% in the 31–45 range, 23.1% in the 46–60 range, and 2.3% in
the 60–75 range. Regarding educational level, 1.3% have primary
4 | C ON S T RU CT I O N O F T H E S C A L E education, 5.2% secondary, 12.9% preuniversity, 48.1% university,
and 32.5% postuniversity.
In the process of selecting the items of PSEUDO, we have tried to use
only explicit, well‐founded and studied in‐depth cases of both types of
pseudoscience. For this task, borderline, doubtful, or cases that 5.2 | Statistical analysis
already appear in scales of other types of unwarranted beliefs have
been left out. Table 1 includes PSEUDO's thematic spectrum, the Software R, version 3.3.2 and its psych package was used throughout
pseudoscience expressed by its items, and the key references in order the data analysis process. Due to the nonnormal distribution of a large
to justify their inclusion as instances of pseudoscience. part of the variables, we used the principal axes factorization method
620 FASCE AND PICÓ

along with the Promax rotation. It should be noted that for the extrac- 5.3 | Results
tion of the appropriate number of factors, various methods were
checked—Kaiser's criterion, parallel analysis, and very simple struc- An exploratory factor analysis was conducted on the 30 items of
ture—that yielded different results. Depending on the method chosen PSEUDO with Promax rotation. The Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin measure
and taking into account a fit based on off‐diagonal values higher than verified the sampling adequacy for this analysis (KMO = 0.94), with
0.95, there could be up to five different models, from the most conser- all individual values >0.70, above the commonly recommended value
vative model with five factors to a model with a single factor. We of 0.6. In addition, correlations between items were assessed using
present a three‐factor model that represents the balance with a good Bartlett's test of sphericity, which showed to be significant with
fit, with Tucker Lewis Index of 0.904 and better Cronbach's alphas in χ2(435) = 30,695.19, p < 0.001. The pattern matrix for the three‐factor
each factor. model can be seen in Table 2, and Table 3 represents the correlation
Additionally, a cluster analysis with the same data was carried out matrix among factors.
in order to confirm the apparent lack of coherence in the factors Initial eigenvalues indicated that those three factors explained
formed. This method consists of six steps extracted from (a) find 16%, 11%, and 6% of the variance, respectively. Models from one to
the proximity matrix; (b) identify the most similar pair of items; five factors were each examined using Varimax and Promax rotations
(c) form a cluster with the combination of this most similar pair of of the factor loadings matrix. We preferred the three‐factor solution
items; (d) assess the similarity of this cluster with other clusters and because of (a) greater internal consistency from Factor 1 to Factor 3,
items; (e) repetition of second and third steps until reach some (b) lack of theoretical ground to support multifactor model, (c) it was
criterion, whether only one cluster remains or these a failure to the model with the least number of factors that maintained the maxi-
increase reliability coefficients; and (f) reassigning items to the most mum fit according to different tests. As shown in Table 3, correlations
similar cluster. between factors were moderated, from r = 0.46 between Factors 2

TABLE 1 Tematic spectrum of PSEUDO and theoretical justification of its items


Type Number Pseudoscience Key reference

PP 1 Body memory (Lynn, Lock, Loftus, Krackow, & Lilienfeld, 2003)


PP 2 Morphic fields (Rose, 1992)
PP 3 Quantum quackery (Stenger, 1997)
PP 4 Homeopathy (Mathie et al., 2017)
PP 5 Osteopathy and chiropractic (Ernst, 2008; Posadzki & Ernst, 2011)
PP 6 Reflexology (Ernst, Posadzki, & Lee, 2011)
PP 7 Acupuncture (Colquhoun & Novella, 2013)
PP 8 Law of attraction (Shermer, 2007)
PP 9 Polygraph (Iacono, 2001)
PP 10 Intelligent design (Boudry, Blancke, & Braeckman, 2010)
PP 12 Parapsychology (Hines, 2003)
PP 13 Repressed memories (McNally, 2007)
PP 14 Emotional carcinogenesis (Ahn, Bae, Anh, & Hwang, 2016)
PP 16 NLP (Witkowski, 2010)
PP 18 Psychoanalysis (de Maat et al., 2013)
PP 21 Blood type diet (Cusack, 2013)
PP 22 Pseudocosmetics (Mohammadi, 2017)
PP 23 Scientific racism (Paludi & Haley, 2014)
PP 26 Pseudoarqueology (Fagan, 2006)
PP 27 Reiki (Lee, Pittler, & Ernst, 2008)
PP 28 Magnet therapy (Finegold & Flamm, 2006)
SD 11 Holocaust denial (Shermer & Grobman, 2002)
SD 15 Climate change denial (Dunlap & McCright, 2011)
SD 17 Flat earth (Gardwood, 2008)
SD 19 Animal suffering denial (Wicks, 2011)
SD 20 Antipsychiatry (Whitley, 2012)
SD 24 GMOs opposition (Blancke, Van Breusegem, De Jaeger, Braeckman,
& Van Montagu, 2015)
SD 25 HIV/AIDS denialism (Chigwedere & Essex, 2010)
SD 29 Chemophonia (Ropeik, 2015)
SD 30 Antivaccination (Wolfe & Sharp, 2002)

Note. PP: pseudo‐theory promotion; SD: science denialism.


FASCE AND PICÓ 621

TABLE 2 The three‐factor model show for its balance between good fitness and high reliabilities in their factors

Promax rotated factor loadings


No. Type Item Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3
1 PP Body memory 0.04 −0.02 0.83
2 PP Morphic fields 0.01 −0.04 0.90
3 PP Quantum quackery 0.30 0.12 0.35
4 PP Homeopathy 0.24 0.31 −0.14
5 PP Osteopathy and chiropractic 0.67 0.03 −0.05
6 PP Reflexology 0.62 0.27 −0.03
7 PP Acupuncture 0.58 0.11 −0.06
8 PP Law of attraction 0.26 0.48 −0.09
9 PP Polygraph 0.35 0.04 −0.06
10 PP Intelligent design 0.20 0.29 0.20
12 PP Parapsychology −0.03 0.29 0.06
13 PP Repressed memories 0.41 0.46 0.00
14 PP Emotional carcinogenesis 0.42 −0.15 0.09
16 PP NLP 0.42 0.37 0.02
18 PP Psychoanalysis −0.11 0.34 0.07
21 PP Blood type diet 0.52 −0.06 0.16
22 PP Pseudocosmetics 0.00 0.29 0.03
23 PP Scientific racism 0.60 −0.07 0.05
26 PP Pseudoarcheology −0.13 0.18 0.02
27 PP Reiki −0.16 0.41 0.05
28 PP Magnet therapy 0.20 0.50 −0.04
11 SD Holocaust denial 0.30 0.25 0.05
15 SD Climate change denial −0.02 0.43 0.12
17 SD Flat earth 0.61 0.16 −0.07
19 SD Animal suffering denial 0.03 0.29 0.02
20 SD Antipsychiatry 0.33 0.42 0.03
24 SD GMOs opposition 0.30 0.26 −0.10
25 SD HIV/AIDS denialism 0.30 0.18 0.05
29 SD Chemophobia 0.60 0.09 0.00
30 SD Antivaccination 0.19 0.60 −0.12
Eigenvalues 4.65 3.27 1.82
% of Variance 16 11 6
Cronbach's α 0.84 0.74 0.81

Note. In bold factor loadings over 0.40. PP: pseudo‐theory promotion; SD: science denialism.

TABLE 3 R‐matrix of factor correlations Finally, no item has been eliminated because it would be useless
Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3 in order to elucidate properly conceptualized factors. According
to our criteria, the resulting factors and clusters do not correspond,
Factor 1 1 0.49 0.59
at least in taking into account the current literature on pseudoscience,
Factor 2 1 0.46
with any congruent classification system—either by topic, by sociolog-
Factor 3 1
ical stratum, by rhetorical tools, and so on—so we discourage the
use of these factors for psychometric purposes.
and 3, r = 0.49 between Factors 1 and 2, and r = 0.59 between first
and third factors. With regard to reliability, the scale had a general
Cronbach's alpha of 0.90 and 0.84, 0.74, and 0.81 for every factor, 6 | STUDY 2
respectively. As a complementary analysis, we try to ratify the results
obtained through a hierarchical analysis of clusters. This analysis found 6.1 | Prior research findings
two clusters (C27 and C28) with purified alphas of 0.91 and 0.50,
respectively, and, again, no conceptual unification was found in both Considering pseudoscience as a whole, there are no significant gender,
sets of items. They count with 23 and seven items, and their cluster religiosity, and political orientation differences (Fasce & Picó, 2018a).
loadings can be seen in Table 4. Believers in pseudoscience show personality traits related to greater
622 FASCE AND PICÓ

TABLE 4 Summary statistics from the cluster analysis

Cluster Structure matrix


No. Type Item C27 C28 Cluster
1 PP Body memory 0.61 0.36 C27
2 PP Morphic fields 0.61 0.35 C27
3 PP Quantum quackery 0.61 0.36 C27
4 PP Homeopathy 0.35 0.28 C27
5 PP Osteopathy and chiropractic 0.60 0.28 C27
6 PP Reflexology 0.76 0.43 C27
7 PP Acupuncture 0.58 0.29 C27
8 PP Law of attraction 0.54 0.41 C27
9 PP Polygraph 0.32 0.19 C27
10 PP Intelligent design 0.54 0.45 C27
12 PP Parapsychology 0.23 0.40 C28
13 PP Repressed memories 0.74 0.49 C27
14 PP Emotional carcinogenesis 0.34 0.09 C27
16 PP NLP 0.69 0.45 C27
18 PP Psychoanalysis 0.20 0.40 C28
21 PP Blood type diet 0.54 0.29 C27
22 PP Pseudocosmetics 0.24 0.33 C28
23 PP Scientific racism 0.53 0.22 C27
26 PP Pseudoarcheology 0.03 0.16 C28
27 PP Reiki 0.18 0.35 C28
28 PP Magnet therapy 0.54 0.50 C27
11 SD Holocaust denial 0.50 0.38 C27
15 SD Climate change denial 0.40 0.48 C27
17 SD Flat earth 0.65 0.31 C27
19 SD Animal suffering denial 0.26 0.32 C28
20 SD Antipsychiatry 0.66 0.42 C27
24 SD GMOs opposition 0.40 0.28 C27
25 SD HIV/AIDS denialism 0.46 0.29 C27
29 SD Chemophobia 0.62 0.32 C27
30 SD Antivaccination 0.55 0.50 C27
Eigenvalues 6.8 2.1

Note. Cluster analysis shows one cluster conformed by 24 items and another of 6 items. PP: pseudo‐theory promotion; SD: science denialism.

extraversion and need for socialization (Fasce & Picó, 2018a), and, 6.2 | Participants and procedure
based on current evidence, the most effective cognitive predictors of
pseudoscientific beliefs are ontological confusions, faith in intuition, The participants in the study were 292 undergraduate students, all
cognitive reflection (Fasce & Picó, 2018a), scientific literacy (Fasce & students of the University of Valencia, Spain (106 Psychology
Picó, 2018b), other types of unwarranted beliefs (Fasce & Picó, students, 40 from Philosophy, 23 from Biochemistry, 45 from Social
2018a; Lobato et al., 2014), and misconceptions about psychology Work, 41 from Education, and 35 from other university degrees).
(Bensley et al., 2014). None of the groups were offered credits in exchange for their par-
Considering just instances of SD, they are strongly negatively ticipation. Two of the participants were excluded due to incomplete
correlated with mistrust in science (Dunlap & McCright, 2011; data. Of the final sample (N = 290), 199 were women and 91 were
Nadelson & Hardy, 2015). We know also about the especially close men, with an average age of 22.04 (SD = 6.50). Religious affiliation
connection between science denial and conspiracy theories has been grouped into two: (a) Religious (practicing Catholics,
(Lewandowsky, Gignac, & Oberauer, 2013; Jolley & Douglas, 2014), its nonpracticing Catholics, Jewish, Evangelicals, Muslims, Bahá'is,
association with conservatism and economic liberalism (Lewandowsky, Hindus, and others) and (b) nonreligious (agnostics, atheists, and
Gignac, & Oberauer, 2013; Lewandowsky & Oberauer, 2016; nonbelievers). One hundred twenty‐three participants were religious,
Lewandowsky, Oberauer, & Gignac, 2013; Nadelson & Hardy, 2015), and 167 were nonreligious. The political tendency (PT) of the sub-
male dominance, and lack of critical thinking among deniers (Browne jects has also been measured. Subjects were able to choose between
et al., 2015; McCright & Dunlap, 2011). the four major Spanish political parties (PP, CD's, Podemos, and
FASCE AND PICÓ 623

PSOE), plus an option of “other”—all participants who indicated the For personality, we used the Mini‐International Personality Item
option “other” were left‐wing voters (PACMA and Compromís). Pool (Mini‐IPIP; Donnellan, Oswald, Baird, & Lucas, 2006), a short ver-
These results were encoded as “right‐wing” (PP and CD's) and sion of the Five‐Factor Inventory. It includes 20 statements and five
“left‐wing” (Podemos, PSOE, and other). One hundred twenty‐seven personality factors: extraversion, conscientiousness, agreeableness,
of the participants were right‐wing voters, and 163 were left‐wing openness, and neuroticism (α = 0.85, 0.72, 0.70, 0.69, and 0.70).
voters. Participants indicated on a 5‐point scale to what extent they agree
Additionally, the study included the assessment of the vital satis- or disagree with each statement. For cognitive style regarding the dual
faction of the subjects (VS), with a single item ranging from 1 to 10—1 process theory, we used the classic 40‐items REI (Epstein, Pacini,
indicating minimum level of vital satisfaction and 10 indicating maxi- Denes‐Raj, & Heier, 1996). The REI has two 20‐item subscales: “Need
mum level. The average vital satisfaction was 7.02, with a standard for Cognition” (NC; α = 0.87) assesses the extent to which an individ-
deviation of 1.59. Finally, we collected information about the ual enjoys rational, analytic, effortful, affect‐free, and logical thinking
academic performance of the participants in two different academic and “Faith in Intuition” (FI; α = 0.88) measures intuitive thinking, the
periods. First, we asked for their university access score, which extent to which a person enjoys and engages preconscious, nonverbal,
indicates the academic performance in preuniversity courses (PAP), associative, and holistic thinking. Subjects answered from 1 to 5, with
characterized by multidisciplinarity. And, second, we asked for their 5 being maximum agreement with the statement and 1 being
average score in university evaluations (UAP), characterized by maximum disagreement, in relation to their own personal tendencies.
being focused on a specific field of study. We have measured different variables related to scientific literacy.
The set of scales and tests were filled by the participants in The Factual Knowledge of Science Scale (FKSS), a 15‐item scale that
person and in class hours due to the kindness of their professors, measures knowledge about scientific theories. The FKSS has been
with the exception of 17 who completed the scales in a nonpresential used by the National Science Board since 1992 (NSB, 2016), facilitat-
way. The scales and tests were always presented in the same order ing the comparison between different samples. Nevertheless, the scale
(sociodemographic data, VS, Mini‐International Personality Item Pool, presents small variations from one study to another. This 15‐item
Rational‐Experiential Inventory [REI], Cognitive Reflection Test [CRT], version includes all the questions that usually appear on all its
Generic Conspiracy Ideation Scale, PSEUDO, CORE, Need for Closure versions. To measure trust in science, we used the Trust in Science
Scale, Revised Paranormal Belief Scale [R‐PBS], and PT), in a 1 hr and a and Scientists Inventory (TSSI; Nadelson & Hardy, 2015), a 21‐item
half session and under the surveillance of two researchers. The set of scale with a good level of reliability (α = 0.86). Its items are presented
scales and tests were completed using paper and then the data on a 5‐point Likert scale ranging from “1” (strongly disagree) to “5”
manually introduced into the data matrix. The data entry process has (strongly agree).
been assured to the maximum, being carried out by two researchers We have measured also two dimensions of critical thinking, the
at the same time—one dictating the data and the other one introduc- willingness to put it into practice and the factual ability to carry out
ing it—and later carefully reviewed. cognitive reflections. For the first dimension, we used the Critical
Thinking Disposition Scale (CTDS; α = 0.80; Sosu, 2013), an 11‐item
scale with two factors: Critical Openness and Reflective Skepticism.
6.3 | Measures The CTDS works with a scale ranging from 1 to 5, “1” meaning that
the subject strongly disagrees that the statement represents her dis-
All scales, except PSEUDO, which was originally written in Spanish, position and “5” that she strongly agrees with it. For the practical
have been translated from English to Spanish. The scales were dimension of critical thinking, we used the CRT (Frederick, 2005), a
independently translated by two persons with a high level of English, three‐item test whose items present mathematical calculations that
and their translations were later compared with a third person seem to have a simple solution but, after a longer and deeper reflec-
assisting as a referee. No problems were detected regarding the tion, turns out to have a counterintuitive one. It is a test whose
translations throughout the study. For this study, we divided PSEUDO answers are right or wrong, so the subject receives a score from 1
into two subscales according to the thematic spectrum of its items— to 3 depending on his performance.
21 items for PP and nine for SD. Answers range from 1 to 5, express- The short version of the Need for Closure Scale (NFC; α = 0.84;
ing 1 as a strong disagreement with the statement and 5 as a strong Roets & Van Hiel, 2011) has also been used, a 26‐item scale that
agreement. measures the level of rejection of individuals towards situations of epi-
For paranormal beliefs, we used the R‐PBS (Tobacyk, 2004), a stemic uncertainty and irregularity in behavioral patterns. Participants
26‐item questionnaire focused on seven dimensions of paranormal were asked to answer on a scale of 1 to 6. Finally, we measured
beliefs (traditional religious beliefs, witchcraft, psi, precognition, Core Ontological Confusions (CORE; Lindeman, Svedholm, Takada,
superstition, extraordinary life forms, and spiritualism; α = 0.92). The Lönnqvist, & Verkasalo, 2011). An ontological confusion consists in
respondents indicated the extent of their agreement or disagreement the attribution of a specific feature of some stratum of reality, for
on a 7‐point scale. For conspiracy ideation, we used an abbreviated example, the psychological, to an entity belonging to a different stra-
15‐item version of the Generic Conspiracy Ideation Scale (GCIS; tum, such as the physical (“stones live in the forest” or “the moon
α = 0.93; Brotherton et al., 2013). Participants indicated on a scale aspires to move forward”). The version used in this study of the scale
of 1 to 5 how true they consider each item, with 1 as definitely false of ontological confusions has 37 items. Participants should indicate
and 5 as definitely true. whether they consider each item to be either metaphorical or literal.
624 FASCE AND PICÓ

6.4 | Results TABLE 5 Correlations between 18 independent variables and the


two different kinds of pseudoscience
Mean scores along with their standard errors, taking into account PSEUDO
gender, religious beliefs, or PT, can be found in Figure 1. No significant Independent variables PP SD
differences are reported.
GCBS 0.385*** 0.323***
Pearson product–moment correlations were carried out, in which
R‐PBS 0.555*** 0.370***
PSEUDO has been divided into PP and SD, as shown in Table 5.
MINI‐IPIP
Both types of pseudoscience show very similar associations with the
Extraversion 0.290*** 0.122
rest of the variables. Perhaps the most outstanding differences
Neuroticism 0.032 0.068
have been found regarding NC, extraversion, and CTDS. NC and
Openness −0.055 −0.060
extraversion were correlated just with PP (r = −0.161, p < 0.05 and
Conscientiousness 0.008 −0.120
r = 0.290, p < 0.001, respectively) and CTDS just with SD
Agreeableness 0.106 0.009
(r = −0.216, p < 0.01).
NC −0.161* 0.110
Hierarchical multiple linear regression analyses were conducted
FI 0.200** 0.175**
for each of the two types of pseudoscience. Independent variables
CORE 0.305*** 0.266***
have been selected in relation to their level of confirmation as
FKSS −0.348*** −0.352***
predictors of pseudoscience. In Model 1 were entered variables with
TSSI −0.325*** −0.463***
high level of confirmation such as R‐PBS (Fasce & Picó, 2018a;
CTDS −0.111 −0.216**
Lobato et al., 2014), GCBS (Lewandowsky, Gignac, & Oberauer,
CRT −0.198** −0.180**
2013; Lewandowsky, Oberauer, & Gignac, 2013; Jolley & Douglas,
NFC 0.102 −0.071
2014; Lobato et al., 2014; Fasce & Picó, 2018a), CRT (Browne
UAP −0.416*** −0.277***
et al., 2015; Fasce & Picó, 2018b), and Trust in Science and Scien-
PAP −0.153* −0.159*
tists Inventory (Fasce & Picó, 2018b; Nadelson & Hardy, 2015),
VS 0.119 −0.075
and in Model 2 were entered variables with less level of confirma-
Note. Pearson product–moment correlations with Holm–Bonferroni
tion such as CORE (Fasce & Picó, 2018a), FI (Fasce & Picó, 2018a)
correction. PP: pseudo‐theory promotion; SD: science denialism.
and FKSS (Fasce & Picó, 2018b). We present those results in
*p < 0.05. **p < 0.01. ***p < 0.001.
Table 6.
Altogether, predictors from Model 1 show an explained variance
of 39.3% ( F (4, 267) = 43.34, p < 0.001) to predict the promotion of 6.5 | Discussion
pseudo‐theories. Three first predictors, with F values between 13.5
and 136.5, show to be statistically significant. The addition of The results of Study 2 show that believers in both types of pseudo-
predictors in Model 2 increase the variance explained by 1.7% science are, even showing some interesting differences, two of a
( F (7, 264) = 26.27, p < 0.001). Although none of the predictors added kind. Both types of pseudoscience have equivalent associations
in Model 2 are significant on their own, Model 2 shows a high good- regarding paranormal beliefs and conspiracy theories—although with
ness of fit. Regarding SD, Model 1 explains a 26% of its variance a greater effect size between PP and paranormal beliefs. Something
( F (4, 268) = 23.51, p < 0.001), and each predictor is statistically similar happens with respect to personality factors: the behavior of
significant ( F values between 8.5 and 41.34 and at least p < 0.01). PP and SD is almost identical, with the exception of extroverted
When adding predictors in Model 2, the explained variance increased personality, which only correlates with PP. This can indicate
to 28.8% ( F (7, 265) = 15.36, p < 0.001), with FKSS as a significant that one of the differential variables among believers in PP and in
predictor ( F (1, 265) = 7.51, p < 0.01). SD may be some of the characteristics associated with this

FIGURE 1 Bar charts of pseudo‐theory promotion and science denialism regarding religious affiliation, political tendency, and gender difference
FASCE AND PICÓ 625

TABLE 6 Hierarchical linear regression analysis into two models on pseudo‐theory promotion and science denialism

Pseudo‐theory promotion Science denialism


Model 1 Model 2 Model 1 Model 2
Predictors
B β SE B β SE B β SE B β SE
RPB‐S 4.062*** 0.441*** 0.484 3.527*** 0.383*** 0.530 0.920*** 0.175*** 0.302 0.504*** 0.096*** 0.326
GCBS 3.158*** 0.254*** 0.648 2.798*** 0.225*** 0.667 0.855*** 0.121*** 0.404 0.725*** 0.102*** 0.412
CRT −1.447*** −0.175*** 0.411 −1.162*** −0.140*** 0.427 −0.376** −0.080** 0.256 −0.123** −0.026** 0.265
TSSI −0.253 −0.013 0.972 0.124 0.006 0.994 −0.344*** −0.336*** 0.602 −3.083*** −0.301*** 0.610
CORE 0.110 0.062 0.208 0.084 0.084 0.128
FI 1.702 0.097 0.091 0.552 0.055 0.056
FKSS −0.362 −0.100 0.872 −3.528** −0.168** 0.544
R2 0.393*** 0.410*** 0.260*** 0.288***
Adjusted R2 0.384 0.395 0.248 0.270
2
Change in R 0.017 0.028

Note. Model 1 is composed of the variables confirmed by previous studies on pseudoscience, whereas in Model 2 we add three variables that we think
could predict both types of pseudoscience.
*p < 0.05. **p < 0.01. ***p < 0.001.

personality factor—positive emotions, assertiveness, sociability, and consequences, at least, in these two studies. The items of PSEUDO
talkativeness. that refer to SD are integrated into the scale, being part of several
Gender and PT show no significant differences between both factors and behaving in a similar way than the ones that refer to PP,
dependent variables. We must keep in mind that the only confirmed at least regarding the sociodemographic, cognitive, and personality
case of male dominance in SD has been discovered regarding climate variables considered in Study 2. Nevertheless, PP and SD should be
change denial (McCright & Dunlap, 2011), where White men used as thematic subscales, not as factors or clusters. In any case,
show higher prevalence in the United States and that studies that the differences between PP and SD could be an issue of great interest
have associated right‐wing politics with SD are also focused on for further research—for example, their specific relationship with
climate change denial (Lewandowsky, Gignac, & Oberauer, 2013; paranormal beliefs, conspiracy ideation, and some dimensions of
Lewandowsky & Oberauer, 2016; Lewandowsky, Oberauer, & Gignac, scientific literacy—but these differences, which exist, are not enough
2013)—although some studies have also found this association in the to break the construct into two different sets of ideas.
denial of biological evolution (Nadelson & Hardy, 2015). Therefore,
considering SD as a whole and not just a single case, gender and
political orientation may be neutral or vary from case to case. 7 | C O NC L U S I O NS A N D L I M I T A T I O N S
Regarding scientific literacy, both types of pseudoscience show
strong negative correlations with knowledge of scientific theories Pseudoscientific beliefs are a psychological construct that, being
and with trust in science—although the effect size of this last correla- conceptually complex and presenting a highly variable thematic
tion is greater for SD. In fact, this may be one of the most interesting spectrum, shows validity by internal and external congruence. The reli-
differences between SD and general conspiracy ideation, given that ability of PSEUDO is comparable to the reliability of the scales for
there is no evidence of interaction between the latter and knowledge other types of unwarranted beliefs, which indicates that this set of
about scientific facts (Fasce & Picó, 2018b), but trust in science and beliefs is sufficiently elucidated to give rise to reliable studies.
scientific knowledge are good predictors of SD. Conversely, the Nevertheless, the limitations of PSEUDO must lead to future
behavior of both types of pseudoscience partially diverges in relation work in order to expand and assure the tool: (a) the selection of items
to critical thinking, given that, while maintaining the same negative may present problems of intercultural analysis, given that pseudosci-
correlation with the capacity for cognitive reflection performance, entific manifestations may be highly variable in this regard; (b) it would
only SD is associated with a lower disposition to critical thinking. be desirable to carry out proper validations of both a short version of
Both present negative correlation with both FI and CORE, thus dem- the scale and its translations to other languages; (c) the limitation in its
onstrating that faith in intuition and the proneness to ontological use with children, because the writing of the items has not been
errors are closely related to both types of pseudoscience. Finally, we specifically designed for them and, therefore, could present problems
do not find differences regarding vital satisfaction and academic of reading comprehension; and (d) the instability over time of the
performance—but the effect size of the correlation between PP and proposed belief scale—it is to be expected that the scale will be in
preuniversity performance is greater compared with the one between need of revision in the future, possibly due to new scientific
PAP and SD. knowledge and in all probability due to the rise and decline of pseudo-
In summary, SD can to be considered as a form of pseudoscience scientific claims.
from a psychological standpoint: the differences between SD and PP, In conclusion, PSEUDO is a highly reliable psychometric tool that
even if philosophically valuable, does not have relevant psychometric we can consider as duly grounded on a theoretical level and as
626 FASCE AND PICÓ

empirically validated. It should give rise to a qualitative leap in the Fagan, G. (2006). Diagnosing pseudoarchaeology. In G. Fagan (Ed.),
psychological and sociological study of pseudoscience. Archaeological fantasies: How pseudoarchaeology misrepresents the past
and misleads the public. New York: Routledge. https://doi.org/10.
1017/S0003598X00095363
ORCID
Fasce, A. (2017). Los parásitos de la ciencia. Una caracterización
Angelo Fasce https://orcid.org/0000-0002-5019-4953 psicocognitiva del engaño pseudocientífico [The Parasites of Science. A
Psycho‐cognitive Characterization of a Pseudo‐scientific Hoax]. Theoria.
An International Journal for Theory, History and Foundations of Science,
RE FE R ENC E S
32(3), 347–365. https://doi.org/10.1387/theoria.17775
Ahn, H., Bae, J., Ahn, H., & Hwang, I. (2016). Risk of cancer among patients
Fasce, A. (2018a). What do we mean when we speak of pseudoscience?
with depressive disorder: A meta‐analysis and implications. Psycho‐
The development of a demarcation criterion based on the analysis of
Oncology, 25(12), 1393–1399. https://doi.org/10.1002/pon.4084
twenty one previous attempts. Disputatio. Philosophical Research
Bensley, A., Lilienfeld, S., & Powell, L. (2014). A new measure of psycholog- Bulletin, 6(7), 459–488. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.1433737
ical misconceptions: Relations with academic background, critical
Fasce, A. (2018b). Are Pseudosciences Like Seagulls? Against Statistical
thinking, and acceptance of paranormal and pseudoscientific claims.
Demarcation. Under review.
Learning and Individual Differences, 39(7), 1227–1236. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.lindif.2014.07.009 Fasce, A., & Picó, A. (2018a). Sociodemographic, personality and cognitive
differences among believers in pseudoscience, the paranormal and
Blancke, S., Boudry, M., & Pigliucci, M. (2016). Why do irrational beliefs
conspiracy theories. Under review.
mimic science? The cultural evolution of pseudoscience. Theoria,
83(1), 78–97. https://doi.org/10.1111/theo.12109 Fasce, A., & Picó, A. (2018b). Science as a vaccine. The Relation between
Blancke, S., Van Breusegem, F., De Jaeger, G., Braeckman, J., & Van Scientific Literacy and Unwarranted Beliefs. Science & Education.
Montagu, M. (2015). Fatal attraction: The intuitive appeal of GMO Accepted manuscript.
opposition. Trends in Plant Science, 20(7), 414–418. https://doi.org/ Finegold, L., & Flamm, B. (2006). Magnet therapy. Extraordinary claims,
10.1016/j.tplants.2015.03.011 but no proved benefits. BMJ, 332(7532), 4. https://doi.org/10.1136/
Boudry, M., Blancke, S., & Braeckman, J. (2010). Irreducible incoherence bmj.332.7532.4
and intelligent design: A look into the conceptual toolbox of a pseudo- Franz, T., & Green, K. (2013). The impact of an interdisciplinary learning
science. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 85(4), 473–482. https://doi. community course on pseudoscientific reasoning in first‐year science
org/10.1086/656904 students. Journal of the Scholarship of Teaching and Learning, 13(5),
Brotherton, R., French, C., & Pickering, A. (2013). Measuring belief in con- 90–105.
spiracy theories: The generic conspiracist beliefs scale. Frontiers in Frederick, S. (2005). Cognitive reflection and decision making. Journal of
Psychology, 4, 279. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00279 Economic Perspectives, 19(4), 25–42. https://doi.org/10.1257/0895
Browne, M., Thompson, P., Rockloff, M., & Pennycook, G. (2015). Going 33005775196732
against the herd: Psychological and cultural factors underlying the Gardwood, C. (2008). Flat earth: The history of an infamous idea. New York:
‘Vaccination Confidence Gap’. PLoS One, 10(9), e0132562. https:// Thomas Dunne Books.
doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0132562
Goldman, A. (2001). Experts: Which ones should you trust? Philosophy and
Chigwedere, P., & Essex, M. (2010). AIDS denialism and public health Phenomenological Research, 63(1), 85–110. https://doi.org/10.2307/
practice. AIDS and Behavior, 14(2), 237–247. https://doi.org/10.1007/ 3071090
s10461‐009‐9654‐7
Hansson, S. O. (2017). Science denial as a form of pseudoscience. Studies in
Colquhoun, D., & Novella, S. (2013). Acupuncture is theatrical placebo. History and Philosophy of Science, 63, 39–47. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Anesthesia & Analgesia, 116(6), 1360–1363. https://doi.org/10.1213/ shpsa.2017.05.002
ANE.0b013e31828f2d5e
Hines, T. (2003). Pseudoscience and the paranormal (2nd edition). New York:
Cusack, L. (2013). Blood type diets lack supporting evidence: A systematic Prometheus Books.
review. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 98(1), 99–104.
https://doi.org/10.3945/ajcn.113.058693 Iacono, W. (2001). Forensic ‘lie detection’: Procedures without scientific
basis. Journal of Forensic Psychology Practice, 1(1), 75–86. https://doi.
de Maat, S., de Jonghe, F., de Kraker, R., Leichsenring, F., Abbass, A., org/10.1300/J158v01n01_05
Luyten, P., … Dekker, J. (2013). The current state of the empirical
evidence for psychoanalysis: A meta‐analytic approach. Harvard Johnson, M., & Pigliucci, M. (2004). Is knowledge of science associated
Review of Psychiatry, 21(3), 107–137. https://doi.org/10.1097/ with higher skepticism of pseudoscientific claims? The American Biology
HRP.0b013e318294f5fd Teacher, 66(8), 536–548. https://doi.org/10.2307/4451737

Donnellan, M., Oswald, F., Baird, B., & Lucas, R. (2006). The mini‐IPIP Jolley, D., & Douglas, K. (2014). The effects of anti‐vaccine conspiracy the-
scales: Tiny‐yet‐effective measures of the big five factors of personal- ories on vaccination intentions. PLoS One, 9(2), e89177. https://doi.
ity. Psychological Assessment, 18(2), 192–203. org/10.1371/journal.pone.0089177

Dunlap, R., & McCright, A. (2011). Organized climate change denial. Lee, M. S., Pittler, M. H., & Ernst, E. (2008). Effects of reiki in clinical prac-
In J. Dryzek, R. Norgaard, & D. Schlosberg (Eds.), The Oxford tice: A systematic review of randomised clinical trials. The International
handbook of climate change and society. Oxford: Oxford University Journal of Clinical Practice, 62(6), 947–954. https://doi.org/10.1111/
Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199566600.003.0010 j.1742‐1241.2008.01729.x
Epstein, S., Pacini, R., Denes‐Raj, V., & Heier, H. (1996). Individual Lewandowsky, S., Gignac, G., & Oberauer, K. (2013). The role of conspira-
differences in intuitive‐experiential and analytic‐rational thinking cist ideation and worldviews in predicting rejection of science. PLoS
styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 390–405. One, 8(10), e75637. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0075637
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022‐3514.71.2.390 Lewandowsky, S., & Oberauer, K. (2016). Motivated rejection of science.
Ernst, E. (2008). Chiropractic: A critical evaluation. Journal of Pain and Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25(4), 217–222. https://
Symptom Management, 35(5), 544–562. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. doi.org/10.1177/0963721416654436
jpainsymman.2007.07.004 Lewandowsky, S., Oberauer, K., & Gignac, G. (2013). NASA faked the moon
Ernst, E., Posadzki, P., & Lee, M. S. (2011). Reflexology: An update of landing—therefore (climate) science is a hoax: An anatomy of the
a systematic review of randomised clinical trials. Maturitas, 68(2), motivated rejection of science. Psychological Science, 24(5), 622–633.
116–120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.maturitas.2010.10.011 https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612457686
FASCE AND PICÓ 627

Lindeman, M., Svedholm, A., Takada, M., Lönnqvist, J., & Verkasalo, M. (2011). Roets, A., & Van Hiel, A. (2011). Item selection and validation of a brief,
Core knowledge confusions among university students. Science & 15‐item version of the Need for Closure Scale. Personality and Individ-
Education, 20, 439–445. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11191‐009‐9210‐x ual Differences, 50, 90–94. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.09.004
Lobato, E., Mendoza, J., Sins, V., & Chin, M. (2014). Examining the Ropeik, S. (2015). On the roots of, and solutions to, the persistent battle
relationship between conspiracy theories, paranormal beliefs, and between “chemonoia” and rationalist denialism of the subjective
pseudoscience acceptance among a university population. Applied nature of human cognition. Human & Experimental Toxicology, 34(12),
Cognitive Psychology, 28, 617–625. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3042 1272–1278. https://doi.org/10.1177/0960327115603592
Lundström, M., & Jakobsson, A. (2009). Students' ideas regarding science Rose, S. (1992). So‐called “formative causation”—A hypothesis
and pseudo‐science in relation to the human body and health. Nordina, disconfirmed. Response to Rupert Sheldrake. Rivista di Biologia ‐ Bilogy
5(1), 3–17. https://doi.org/10.5617/nordina.279 Forum, 85(3–4), 445–453.

Lynn, S., Lock, T., Loftus, E., Krackow, E., & Lilienfeld, S. (2003). The Shermer, M. (2007). The (other) secret. The inverse square law trumps
remembrance of things past: Problematic memory recovery techniques the law of attraction. Scientific American, 296, 39.
in psychotherapy. In S. Lilienfeld, S. Lynn, & J. Lohr (Eds.), Science and Shermer, M., & Grobman, A. (2002). Denying history: Who says the holocaust
pseudoscience in clinical psychology. New York: The Guilford Press. never happened and why do they say it? Oakland: University of
https://doi.org/10.1080/07317107.2016.1172890 California Press.
Majima, Y. (2015). Belief in pseudoscience, cognitive style and science Sosu, E. (2013). The development and psychometric validation of a Critical
literacy. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 29, 552–559. https://doi.org/ Thinking Disposition Scale. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 9, 107–119.
10.1002/acp.3136 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2012.09.002
Mathie, R. T., Ramparsad, N., Legg, L. A., Clausen, J., Moss, S., Davidson, Stenger, V. (1997). Quantum quackery. The Skeptical Inquirer, 21(1).
J. R., … McConnachie, A. (2017). Randomised, double‐blind, placebo‐ Talmont‐Kamisnki, K. (2013). Werewolves in scientists' clothing.
controlled trials of non‐individualised homeopathic treatment: System- Understanding pseudoscientific cognition. In M. Pigliucci, & M. Boudry
atic review and meta‐analysis. Syst Rev, 24(1), 63. https://doi.org/ (Eds.), The philosophy of pseudoscience. Chicago: University of Chicago
10.1186/s13643‐017‐0445‐3 Press. https://doi.org/10.1086/676054
McCright, A., & Dunlap, R. (2011). Cool dudes: The denial of climate Tobacyk, J. (2004). A revised paranormal belief scale. International Journal
change among conservative white males in the United States. Global of Transpersonal Studies, 23(1), 94–98. https://doi.org/10.24972/
Environmental Change, 21(4), 1163–1172. https://doi.org/10.1080/ ijts.2004.23.1.94
23251042.2018.1488516
Torcello, L. (2016). The ethics of belief, cognition, and climate change
McNally, R. (2007). Dispelling confusion about traumatic dissociative pseudoskepticism: Implications for public discourse. Topics in Cognitive
amnesia. Mayo Clinic Proceedings, 82(9), 1083–1090. https://doi.org/ Science, 8, 19–48.
10.4065/82.9.1083 Tseng, Y., Tsai, C., Hsieh, P., & Hung, J. (2014). The relationship between
Mohammadi, D. (2017). The dangers of unregulated stem‐cell marketing. exposure to pseudoscientific television programmes and pseudoscien-
The Lancet, 21(10105), 1823–1824. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140‐ tific beliefs among Taiwanese university students. International
6736(17)32358‐9 Journal of Science Education, 4(2), 107–122. https://doi.org/10.1080/
21548455.2012.761366
MSPSI (2011). Análisis de situación de las terapias naturales.
Retrieved from http://www.mspsi.gob.es/novedades/docs/analisis Whitley, R. (2012). The antipsychiatry movement: Dead, diminishing, or
SituacionTNatu.pdf. developing? Psychiatric Services, 63(10), 1039–1041. https://doi.org/
10.1176/appi.ps.201100484
Nadelson, L., & Hardy, K. (2015). Trust in science and scientists and the
acceptance of evolution. Evo Edu Outreach, 8(9), 1–9. https://doi.org/ Wicks, D. (2011). Silence and denial in everyday life—The case of
10.1186/s12052‐015‐0037‐4 animal suffering. Animals, 1(1), 186–199. https://doi.org/10.3390/
ani1010186
NSB (2016). Science and engineering indicators. Retrieved from https://
www.nsf.gov/statistics/2016/nsb20161/uploads/1/nsb20161.pdf Witkowski, T. (2010). Thirty‐five years of research on Neuro‐Linguistic
Programming. NLP research data base. State of the art or pseudoscien-
Oreskes, N., & Conway, E. (2010). Merchants of doubt: How a handful of tific decoration? Polish Psychological Bulletin, 41(2), 58–66. https://doi.
scientists obscured the truth on issues from tobacco smoke to global org/10.2478/v10059‐010‐0008‐0
warming. London: Bloomsbury Press. https://doi.org/10.1525/
auk.2011.128.2.435 Wolfe, R., & Sharp, L. (2002). Anti‐vaccinationists past and present. BMJ,
325(7361), 430–432. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.325.7361.430
Paludi, M., & Haley, S. (2014). Scientific racism. In T. Thomas (Ed.), Encyclo-
pedia of critical psychology. Berlín: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/
978‐1‐4614‐5583‐7
How to cite this article: Fasce A, Picó A. Conceptual founda-
Posadzki, P., & Ernst, E. (2011). Osteopathy for musculoskeletal pain
tions and validation of the Pseudoscientific Belief Scale. Appl
patients: A systematic review of randomized controlled trials. Clinical
Rheumatology, 30(2), 285–291. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10067‐010‐ Cognit Psychol. 2019;33:617–628. https://doi.org/10.1002/
1600‐6 acp.3501
628 FASCE AND PICÓ

APPENDIX A

PSEUDOSCIENTIFIC BELIEF SCALE

1 All the cells of our body store memories (cellular memories), ours or of our ancestors. 1 2 3 4 5
2 The collective memory inherited and shared by the organisms belonging to the same species (‘morphic field’ or also ‘morphic 1 2 3 4 5
resonance’) explains several biological phenomena.
3 Quantum mechanics has great implications in the explanation of consciousness and/or in the treatment of diseases. 1 2 3 4 5
4 Homeopathy is an ineffective medication. (R) 1 2 3 4 5
5 Osteopathy and/or chiropractic are scientifically backed branches of physiotherapy. 1 2 3 4 5
6 There are areas of our body surface, such as the feet, hands and/or ears in which we find representations of our entire anatomy. 1 2 3 4 5
7 The theoretical basis of acupuncture is incongruent with current knowledge about human anatomy. (R) 1 2 3 4 5
8 It is a proven fact that the enthusiastic repetition of desires or asking them to the universe (law of attraction) could cause them to 1 2 3 4 5
come true.
9 The polygraph (lie detector) is a reliable tool to know if someone is lying. 1 2 3 4 5
10 While it is true that evolution is a fact, there are issues that require an intelligent intervention to be explained. 1 2 3 4 5
11 The Holocaust took place and 11 million people were murdered (of whom about 6 million were Jews). (R) 1 2 3 4 5
12 It has been scientifically proven that some people have extrasensory abilities (such as telepathy or precognition). 1 2 3 4 5
13 There is scientific evidence that confirm the possibility of repressing traumatic memories as a defence mechanism. 1 2 3 4 5
14 Due to well demonstrated biological reasons, negative emotions and unsolved conflicts or traumas increase the probability of having 1 2 3 4 5
cancer.
15 There is no conclusive evidence that climate change is real or that human activity is its cause. 1 2 3 4 5
16 Neuro‐linguistic programming (NLP) is accepted as part of psychology. 1 2 3 4 5
17 The earth is not spherical. 1 2 3 4 5
18 The main ideas of psychoanalysis are supported by scientific evidence. 1 2 3 4 5
19 Animals are incapable of feeling pain and suffering. 1 2 3 4 5
20 Many mental disorders (and a large proportion of psychiatric practice) are not scientifically known to be real. 1 2 3 4 5
21 Food should be chosen according to the blood group of each person. 1 2 3 4 5
22 The use of stem cells and/or DNA improves the effectiveness of facial creams. 1 2 3 4 5
23 We have evidence that different human races behave differently for innate or genetic reasons (for example, that some are more 1 2 3 4 5
intelligent or violent than others).
24 GMOs are medically and ecologically safe. (R) 1 2 3 4 5
25 The human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) is the cause of the acquired immunodeficiency syndrome (AIDS). (R) 1 2 3 4 5
26 There is archaeological evidence of ancient contacts with ‘astronauts’ or ‘space visitors’ (for example, in cultures such as Sumerian, 1 2 3 4 5
Egyptian, Maya or Nazca).
27 It is impossible to cure diseases by the laying on of hands and the channelling of energy (Reiki). (R) 1 2 3 4 5
28 It is demonstrated that, in some contexts and cases, being exposed to magnetic fields is positive for health. 1 2 3 4 5
29 Many of the pesticides and additives used by the food industry are unsafe. 1 2 3 4 5
30 Vaccines are unsafe, some of them cause diseases such as autism. 1 2 3 4 5

Note. (R): reversed encoded.

You might also like