You are on page 1of 5

Epilepsy & Behavior 112 (2020) 107368

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Epilepsy & Behavior

journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/yebeh

Dissociation during Mirror Gazing Test in psychogenic nonepileptic


seizures and functional movement disorders
Veronica Nisticò a,b, Giovanni Caputo e, Roberta Tedesco a,c, Andrea Marzorati a, Roberta Ferrucci a,b,
Alberto Priori a,b,d, Orsola Gambini a,b,c, Benedetta Demartini a,b,c,⁎
a
Dipartimento di Scienze della Salute, Università degli Studi di Milano, Milano, Italy
b
“Aldo Ravelli” Research Center for Neurotechnology and Experimental Brain Therapeutics, University of Milan, Italy
c
Unità di Psichiatria II, A.O. San Paolo, ASST Santi Paolo e Carlo, Milano, Italy
d
III Clinica Neurologica, A.O. San Paolo, ASST Santi Paolo e Carlo, Milano, Italy
e
DISTUM, University of Urbino, Urbino, Italy

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Introduction: Psychogenic nonepileptic seizures (PNES) and functional movement disorders (FMD) seem to rep-
Received 13 July 2020 resent the two ends of a continuum where different clinical phenotypes represent the manifestation of a com-
Revised 21 July 2020 mon framework, involving dissociation. The aim of the present study was to assess dissociation and its
Accepted 21 July 2020 subcomponents through the Mirror Gazing Test (MGT) in these functional neurological disorders (FNDs).
Available online xxxx
Materials and methods: Eleven patients with PNES, 17 with FMD, and 18 healthy controls (HCs) underwent a 10-
minute MGT and completed the Strange Face Questionnaire (SFQ), an ad-hoc questionnaire assessing the sensa-
Keywords:
Functional neurological disorder
tions and perceptions they had looking in the mirror, and a short version of the Clinician-Administered Dissocia-
Psychogenic nonepileptic seizures tive States Scale (CADSS).
Functional motor disorder Results: Patients with PNES, FMD, and HCs did not differ at the total score of the SFQ. Patients with PNES scored
Dissociation higher than HCs at the SFQ-subscale Dissociative Identity/Compartmentalization, at the CADSS Total Score and at
Detachment its subscale Dissociative Amnesia, while patients with FMD scored higher than HCs at the CADSS subscale Deper-
Compartmentalization sonalization.
Conclusions: Patients with FMD reported more sensations falling in the detachment facet of dissociation, while
patients with PNES in the compartmentalization one. We hypothesized that both facets of dissociation might
be important pathophysiological processes for PNES and FMD and that different instruments (self-report clinical
scales vs experimental tasks) might be able to detect different facets in different populations because they assess,
respectively, “trait” and “state” dissociation.
© 2020 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction quite contradictory: some studies highlighted the heterogeneity of the


two disorders [4–6], some others showed more similarities suggesting
Psychogenic nonepileptic seizures (PNES) and functional movement that they should be considered under the same pathological umbrella
disorders (FMD) are two main clinical manifestations of functional neu- [7,8]. A recent review by Erro et al. showed that there is a significant
rological disorders (FNDs), neurological symptoms that are genuine and overlap between PNES and FMD, suggesting that they would represent
not feigned, but not due to an organic cause (DSM-5, A.P.A, 2015) [1,2]. the two ends of a continuum [9], where different clinical phenotypes
In the last decade, few studies have tried to define whether a common (respectively nonepileptic attacks and motor symptoms) represent
framework between PNES and FMD may exist or whether they should the manifestation of a common pathophysiology. In line with this hy-
be considered two separate entities [3]. Results, mainly obtained study- pothesis, in a recent study of our group, we confirmed the traditional
ing epidemiological and clinical features of the two disorders, have been hypothesis according to which dissociation might have a central role
in the pathophysiology of FND, and we showed how different facets of
Abbreviations: CADSS, Clinician-administered Dissociative States Scale; FMD, dissociation – detachment (an altered state of consciousness, character-
functional movement disorders; FNS, functional neurological symptoms; HAM-A, ized by a sense of separation from the self or world) and compartmen-
Hamilton Anxiety Rating Scale; HAM-D, Hamilton Depression Rating Scale; HCs, healthy talization (a reversible loss of voluntary control over apparently intact
controls; MGT, Mirror Gazing Test; SFQ, Strange Face Questionnaire.
⁎ Corresponding author at: Dipartimento di Scienze della Salute, Università degli Studi
processes and functions) – might be relevant respectively for PNES
di Milano, A.O. San Paolo, via A. di Rudinì, 8, Milano 20142, Italy. and FMD [10]. Nevertheless, one of the main limitations of our previous
E-mail address: benedetta.demartini@unimi.it (B. Demartini). study was the exclusive use of self-report questionnaires exploring

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yebeh.2020.107368
1525-5050/© 2020 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
2 V. Nisticò et al. / Epilepsy & Behavior 112 (2020) 107368

lifetime general feelings of dissociation. In the last decades, several ex- 2.2.1. Mirror Gazing Test
perimental techniques, such as dot staring, mirror staring, spiral staring, The MGT was conducted in a darkened room, 5 m × 5 m, whose
strobe light, hyperventilation, audio stimulation, and stimulus depriva- walls were gray-painted and the windows obscured. A large mirror
tion [11–14] have been developed with the aim of inducing and investi- (0.5 m × 0.5 m) was mounted on a tripod in the center of the room.
gating dissociation in a laboratory setting. Moreover, in the last years, a Each subject seated at a distance of 0.4 m in front of the mirror and
growing body of literature describing a dissociative phenomenon called was instructed to keep staring into his/her own eyes for 10 min. The
“strange-face in the mirror illusion” has emerged: if an observer steadily room was lit only by a halogen light bulb (20 W), mounted on a spot-
looks at his/her image reflected in a mirror, in a dimly lit room for 8/10 light placed 1.2 m behind the participant, on the floor (and therefore,
min – a procedure called Mirror Gazing Test (MGT) – several visual illu- out of his/her visual field and out of the mirror reflection). Illumination
sions occur, such as deformations of one's own face, a relative's face of the face (‘incident light’) was about 1 lx (measured with TES-1330A
with some changed features or an unknown person's face; an archetypal luxmeter). For further details about the procedure, see Caputo et al. [17].
face (i.e., a numinous child, a young androgyne, a very old woman, an At the end of the session, participants completed the Strange Face
ancestor or a shaman), an animal face, and/or monstrous beings [15]. Questionnaire (SFQ), an ad-hoc questionnaire assessing the sensations
This dissociative phenomenon physiologically occur in healthy subjects and perceptions they had looking in the mirror [22] and a short version
[15,16]; moreover, it seems to be stronger in individuals with schizo- of the Clinician-Administered Dissociative States Scale (CADSS) [23].
phrenia [17] and reduced in patients with depression [18] when com- The SFQ, in its new version used in the present study, is composed of
pared with healthy controls (HCs), suggesting a crucial role of specific a total of 28 items, each one describing a possible sensation or percep-
psychopathological features in the subjective response to the MGT. tion that might occur during the MGT. The items were selected and de-
The aim of the present study was to assess dissociation and its sub- veloped basing on the sensations reported by a large number of subjects
components through the MGT in a group of patients with PNES, a who performed the MGT in the last decade [16,22,24]. By staring in their
group of patients with FMD, and a group of HCs. own eyes for a few minutes, participants began to perceive different vi-
sual phenomena: (strange deformations of facial traits, apparitions of a
new face in place of his/her own face, changing in brightness changes).
2. Materials and methods These apparitions were accompanied by a sense of dissociation (a) from
the mirror reflection, i.e., the feeling that the new faces did not belong to
2.1. Participants them but to another, strange person (compartmentalization facet); and
(b) from their own body and from the environmental reality (detach-
Patients affected by PNES and FMD were recruited from the psychi- ment facet) [16]. Participants had to evaluate, on a 5-point Likert
atric outpatient clinic at San Paolo Hospital in Milan. Eleven consecutive scale, how often they experienced the perception described (where 0
patients affected by PNES took part in the study, and they were com- meant “never” and 4 meant “almost always”). Item 19 is a response con-
pared with seventeen consecutive patients affected by FMD and eigh- trol item: therefore, the questionnaire has been considered valid only if
teen HCs. The diagnosis of PNES was done on the basis of the the answer was 0, never. First, number of answers “never” and number
consensus of at least two epilepsy specialists based on the clinical his- of answers ranging from 1 to 4 were counted, as an index of how many
tory and video-electroencephalogram (EEG) monitoring. Typical different apparitions and sensations occurred during the MGT to each
nonepileptic attacks had been captured by video-EEG for all patients participant. Second, a total score, ranging from 0 (I have never experi-
with PNES, and PNES were therefore “documented” according to the di- enced any illusion and/or feeling of dissociation) to 108 (I have almost
agnostic certainty levels described in LaFrance et al. [19]. Panic attacks always experienced all the described illusions and feelings of dissocia-
as an alternative explanation of the paroxysmal symptoms of PNES tion), has been calculated. Finally, three subscales have been calculated
were excluded by psychiatric examination. Patients with FMD were in- and analyzed: (i) Derealization (summing items: 1, 4, 5, 6, 8, 10, 11, 16;
cluded if they had “clinically established” FMD according to Fahn & Wil- total score ranging from 0 to 32); (ii) Depersonalization (summing
liams [20] and Gupta & Lang [21] criteria. The diagnosis was ascertained items: 14, 17, 18, 20, 22, 23, 24; total score ranging from 0 to 28); and
by a neurologist and psychiatrist on the basis of clinical presentation (iii) Dissociative Identity/Compartmentalization (summing items: 2, 7,
and appropriate investigations. The group of HCs was recruited from 9, 12, 13, 21, 25, 26, 27; total score ranging from 0 to 36).
staff members, their friends, and relatives. Psychiatric, neurological, The short version of the CADSS is composed of 19 items, to be replied
and medical disorders were excluded by means of a complete anamnes- on the same 5-point Likert scale (0 being “never” and 4 being “almost
tic questionnaire and a clinical interview. Participants of the three always”). An overall total score, ranging from 0 to 76, has been calcu-
groups were Caucasian. lated by summing each single item's response; moreover, the three fol-
Exclusion criteria were as follows: (i) age less than 18 years, (ii) in- lowing subscales have been calculated and analyzed: (i) Derealization
ability to understand the aim and the steps of the project, (iii) any other (summing items: 1, 2, 8, 9, 10, 11, 12, 13, 16, 17, 18, 19; total score rang-
serious neurological (epilepsy included) or medical illnesses, and (iv) ing from 0 to 48); (ii) Depersonalization (summing items: 3, 4, 5, 6, 7;
overlay between functional and organic movement disorders. total score ranging from 0 to 20); and (iii) Dissociative Amnesia (sum-
One HC was excluded as outlier (performing two standard devia- ming items: 14, 15; total score ranging from 0 to 8).
tions above the average in the experimental variables). Every partici-
pant had the opportunity to ask for clarification and explanation 2.2.2. Psychological assessment
during each stage of the study and was free to interrupt and leave the Participants have been assessed by a psychiatrist for their levels of
experiment at any moment. The study was approved and registered depressive and anxiety symptoms, respectively through the Hamilton
by the local ethics committee. Participants gave their informed written Depression Rating Scale (HAM-D) and the Hamilton Anxiety Rating
consent. The experiment was conducted in accordance with the Decla- Scale (HAM-A).
ration of Helsinki.
2.3. Data analysis

2.2. Experimental protocol Data have been analyzed with the software SPSS (Statistical Package
for Social Science), Version 25.
After the preliminary neurological and psychiatric assessment, par- Univariate analysis of variance (ANOVA) was run to assess whether
ticipants took part in an experimental session lasting about 60 min dur- the three groups (PNES, FMD, and HCs) were different with respect to
ing which they underwent the MGT and a psychological assessment. demographical, experimental, and psychological variables; when a
V. Nisticò et al. / Epilepsy & Behavior 112 (2020) 107368 3

questionnaire presented more than one subscale, multivariate ANOVA with PNES having higher scores than HCs (p = 0.029) but not than
(MANOVA) was used, with the subscales as dependent variables (and FMD (p = 0.841); no differences between FMD and HCs (p = 0.208)
Groups as factor); in both cases, Bonferroni post hoc analysis was used were detected. Specifically, differences were found at the CADSS sub-
to verify specific differences between two of the three groups. Categor- scales Depersonalization (F (2, 43) = 3.509, p = 0.039), with FMD pre-
ical variables were analyzed via Pearson Chi Square (χ2) test. Moreover, senting higher scores than HCs (p = 0.043), and Dissociative Amnesia
Pearson's correlational analysis was used to assess correlations between (F (2, 43) = 4.057, p = 0.024), with PNES presenting higher scores
the variables assessed. than HCs (p = 0.025). No differences between the three groups were
found at the subscale Derealization (F (2, 43) = 1.978, p = 0.151).
3. Results
3.3. Psychological assessment
3.1. Demographic data
3.3.1. HAM-D and HAM-A
The three samples did not differ in terms of gender (χ (2) = 2.737, p Groups were different for their levels of (i) anxiety (F (2, 43) =
= 0.255) and age (F (2, 43) = 0.891, p = 0.418). They differed for psy- 10.933, p b 0.001), with FMD being more anxious than HCs (p b
chiatric comorbidities (χ (2) = 7.926, p = 0.019), but not for psychiatric 0.001) and PNES (p = 0.005), and (ii) depression (F (2, 43) = 8.169,
family history (χ (2) = 0.126, p = 0.939). p = 0.001), with FMD being more depressed than HCs (p = 0.001)
Mean duration of illness for patients with PNES was 3.2 years ± 2.64, and PNES (p = 0.028).
while for patients with FMD, it was 4.35 years ± 4.26; duration of illness
did not differ between the two groups of patients (t (26) = 0.797, p = 3.4. Correlational analysis
0.433 — with Levene's F = 2.223, p = 0.148).
Demographic, experimental, and psychological variables are re- Considering the whole sample, a positive correlation was found be-
ported in Table 1. tween the duration of illness and the CADSS, both at the total score (r =
0.334, p = 0.025) and at its subscales Derealization (r = 0.325, p =
3.2. Mirror Gazing Test 0.029) and Dissociative Amnesia (r = 0.399, p = 0.007). Focusing on
the correlations within each group, only a positive correlation between
Patients with PNES, patients with FMD, and HCs did not differ at the the duration of illness of patients with FMD and the CADSS subscale Dis-
total score of the SFQ (F (2, 43) = 2.378, p = 0.105). Moreover, the sociative Amnesia (r = 0.556, p = 0.020) emerged.
number of times participants had misperceptions by looking in the mir- Additionally, within group with PNES, a negative correlation was
ror did not differ between the three groups (F (2, 43) = 1.501, p = found between the levels of anxiety (HAM-A) and the CADSS subscale
0.234). A difference between the three groups emerged at the subscale Dissociative Amnesia (r = − 0.692, p = 0.018). Levels of depression
Dissociative Identity/Compartmentalization (F (2, 43) = 3.88, p = (HAM-D) did not correlate with any of the MGT questionnaires in any
0.028), with PNES scoring higher than HCs (p = 0.024) but not than group.
FMD (p = 0.424); no differences between FMD and HCs (p = 0.44)
were detected. Furthermore, no differences between the three groups 4. Discussion
emerged at the two other subscales (Derealization: F (2, 43) = 1.335,
p = 0.274; Depersonalization: F (2, 43) = 0.92, p = 0.406). Groups The aim of the present study was to assess dissociation and its sub-
also differed at the CADSS total score (F (2, 43) = 3.933, p = 0.027), components through the MGT in a group of patients with PNES, a

Table 1
Values for demographical, psychological and experimental variables. Bold values are significant values (p b 0.05).

FMD PNES HCs p

Sex [M/F] 4/14 0/11 3/14 0.255


Age [mean (S.D.)] 43.6 (16.1) 36.1 (16.8) 38.1 (13.8) 0.418
Psychiatric family history [(Y/N)] 6/12 4/6 6/11 0.939
Psychiatric comorbidities [(Y/N)] 11/7 8/3 4/13 0.019
Duration of illness (years) [mean (S.D.)] 4,4 (4,3) 3,2 (2647) NA NA
SFQ — total score [mean (S.D.)] 12.6 (11.8) 20 (14.4) 11.06 (6.9) 0.105
% of misperception (SFQ) 22% 32% 21% 0.234
SFQ — Derealization [mean (S.D.)] 3.3 (4.1) 5.2 (4.5) 2.8 (3) 0.274
SFQ – Depersonalization [mean (S.D.)] 2.8 (3.1) 4.5 (4.6) 3.1 (2.8) 0.406
SFQ — Dissociative Identity – Compartmentalization [mean (S.D.)] 2.6 (2.9) 4.3 (4.6) 1.1 (1.3) 0.028
CADSS — total score [mean (S.D.)] 15 (12.9) 19.7 (12.6) 7.8 (8.2) 0.027
CADSS — Derealization [mean (S.D.)] 9.4 (7.7) 10.9 (6.3) 6.2 (5) 0.151
CADSS — Depersonalization [mean (S.D.)] 6.9 (7.3) 5.9 (6.1) 1.9 (3.3) 0.039
CADSS — Dissociative Amnesia [mean (S.D.)] 2.1 (2.3) 2.9 (2.0) 0.9 (1.1) 0.024
HAM-A [mean (S.D.)] 14.6 (10) 5.5 (4.3) 4.1 (4.1) b0.001
HAM-D [mean (S.D.)] 11.2 (7.8) 5 (4) 3.4 (4.4) 0.001

FMD = patients with functional movement disorders.


PNES = patients with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures.
HCs = healthy controls.
M = male.
F = female.
S.D. = standard deviation.
Y = yes.
N = no.
CADSS = Clinician-administered Dissociative States Scale.
SFQ = Strange Face Questionnaire.
HAM-A = Hamilton Anxiety Rating Scale.
HAM-D = Hamilton Depression Rating Scale.
4 V. Nisticò et al. / Epilepsy & Behavior 112 (2020) 107368

Fig. 1. A Dissociation model for FNS: we speculate that (i) FMD are primarily a compartmentalization phenomenon (measured by a trait dissociation instrument such as the Somatoform
Dissociation Questionnaire) and detachment (measured by a “state” dissociation instrument such as the MGT) is often part of the ongoing clinical picture and may contribute to the
development and maintenance of FMD; (ii) PNES are primarily a detachment phenomenon (measured by a “trait” dissociation instrument such as the Dissociative Experience Scale)
and the compartmentalization (measured by a “state” dissociation instrument such as the MGT) represent a state dissociative epiphenomenon, in terms of loss of executive control of
mental processes.

group of patients with FMD, and a group of HCs. Our results showed that compartmentalization facet. These results seem to be in contrast with
patients with PNES, patients with FMD, and HCs reported, at the SFQ, to our previous finding, obtained by self-report questionnaires, according
have had misperceptions during the 10 min of mirror gazing, whose fre- to which patients with PNES presented higher levels of detachment
quency was not different among the groups. Previous results on differ- and patients with FMD of compartmentalization than HCs [10]. How-
ent samples of patients showed that patients with schizophrenia ever, as anticipated before, a strong limitation of our previous study
reported more apparitions than HCs at the MGT [17] and, on the con- was the exclusively use of self-report questionnaires exploring general
trary, patients with depression experienced less apparitions than HCs feelings of dissociation. Differently, here, dissociation was elicited
and patients with schizophrenia [18]. Authors hypothesized that the through a specific experimental task (MGT) and selected questionnaires
lower number of apparitions experienced by patients with depression were used only to explore specific sensations elicited by the exposure.
was explained by the well-known deficits in emotional facial recogni- In addition, previous studies showed that both the facets of dissociation
tion and expression typical of these patients [18]. On the other hand, it (detachment and compartmentalization) might be relevant, from a
has been speculated that one of the crucial differences between HCs' pathophysiological angle, for all the clinical manifestations of functional
and patients' with schizophrenia answers at the MGT questionnaires neurological symptoms (FNS), including PNES and FMD. Kuyk and col-
lay in the feeling of reality of the apparitions [17]. In other words, pa- leagues [26] showed that 17 out of 20 patients with PNES, when hypno-
tients with schizophrenia tended to identify themselves with the appa- tized, were able to recall seizure memories usually unavailable to
ritions in the mirror, contrarily to healthy individuals who consciousness, contrarily than a control group with organic epileptic at-
predominantly felt dissociative experiences during the MGT [16]. In tacks; authors argued that in PNES, unlike in epilepsy, there is no per-
the present study, both patients with PNES and patients with FMD re- manent memory loss due to an encoding deficit occurred during the
ported a similar number of misperceptions during the MGT to the one attack, but a retrieval deficit due to dissociation (or, more specifically,
reported by HCs. This confirms that the MGT is a good instrument to in- compartmentalization) of the ictal memories [27]. Given that
duce typical dissociative symptoms also in clinical samples, where a nonepileptic attacks have been interpreted also as a dissociative re-
psychotic trait is not present and whose depressive symptomatology, sponse to autonomic arousal aimed to reduce intense anxiety [28],
occurring in comorbidity, does not alter the MGT results (as confirmed this is also in line with our results, showing that, only in patients with
by the absence of correlation between the MGT questionnaires and PNES, there was a positive correlation between the levels of anxiety at
the HAM-D levels). the HAM-A and the score at the Dissociative Amnesia CADSS subscale.
Moreover, our results showed that patients with PNES scored signif- Patients with PNES and patients with epilepsy have also been compared
icantly higher than HCs at the SFQ subscale assessing Dissociative Iden- on self-report questionnaires about dissociation, with inconsistent find-
tity/Compartmentalization. This difference was corroborated by the ings: Alper and colleagues [29] found that patients with PNES scored
CADSS results, where patients with PNES reported higher scores at the higher than patients with epilepsy on the Dissociative Experience
Dissociative Amnesia subscale, since dissociative identity is usually as- Scale (DES) subscales measuring depersonalization and derealization,
sociated with dissociative amnesia [25]. On the other hand, patients but this was attributed by the authors to a higher prevalence of child-
with FMD reported higher scores at the Depersonalization CADSS sub- hood abuse in the group with PNES; furthermore, Lawton, Baker, and
scale than HCs. In other words, patients with FMD reported more sensa- Brown [30] found a difference between patients with PNES and patients
tions (such as “feeling like they were looking at things from outside with epilepsy only at the compartmentalization subscale of the DES (for
their own body”, “feeling like being in a dream”, or even “feeling like a review, see Brown [27]).
looking at the things as through a fog”) that would fall in the detach- On the other hand, recent studies suggested that detachment might
ment facet of dissociation, while patients with PNES reported more sen- have a specific role also in patients with FMD: Stone et al. [31], for exam-
sations (such as “recognizing in the mirror another personality that he/ ple, found that 39% of their patients with functional weakness reported
she would not have expected” or “feeling like wandering with their own depersonalization or derealization 24 h prior the onset of the symptom.
thoughts and/or losing track of what was happening”) falling in the Concerning the processes underpinning compartmentalization, there is
V. Nisticò et al. / Epilepsy & Behavior 112 (2020) 107368 5

some evidence that patients with FMD have difficulties when actions References
are explicitly initiated, but not when they are evoked implicitly, which
[1] American Psychiatric Association: diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disor-
is consistent with the dissociated control account [32]. ders (5th Ed.). Washington, DC: Author; 2013.
In this view, we hypothesized that both the facets of dissociation [2] Hallett M, Stone J, Carson AJ. Handbook of clinical neurology, 139 (3rd series) —
(detachment and compartmentalization) might be important patho- functional neurologic disorders Elsevier B.V. ; 2016.
[3] Kanaan RA, Duncan R, Goldstein LH, Jankovic J, Cavanna AE. Are psychogenic non-
physiological processes for PNES and FMD and that different instru- epileptic seizures just another symptom of conversion disorder? J Neurol Neurosurg
ments (self-report clinical scales VS experimental tasks) might be able Psychiatry. 2017;88:425–9.
to detect different facets in different populations because they assess re- [4] Stone J, Sharpe M, Binzer M. Motor conversion symptoms and pseudoseizures: a
comparison of clinical characteristics. Psychosomatics. 2005;45:492–9.
spectively “trait” and “state” dissociation. In fact, self-report question- [5] Reuber M. Are psychogenic non-epileptic seizures an expression of “neurologic” pa-
naires commonly used to assess dissociation in FND, such as the thology? Psychiatric controversies in epilepsy; 2008. p. 153–77.
Dissociative Experience Scale or the Somatoform Dissociation Question- [6] Ekanayake V, Kranick S, LaFaver K, Naz A, Webb AF, LaFrance Jr WC, et al. Personality
traits in psychogenic nonepileptic seizures (PNES) and psychogenic movement dis-
naire, explore a set of symptoms (trait dissociation) and not a process; order (PMD): neuroticism and perfectionism. J Psychosom Res. 2017;97:23–9.
on the other hand, experimental tasks such as the MGT directly explore [7] Driver-Dunckley E, Stonnington CM, Locke DE, Noe K. Comparison of psychogenic
a process, namely induced dissociative experiences (state dissociation). movement disorders and psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: is phenotype clinically
important? Psychosomatics. 2011;52:337–45.
Although still speculative, our hypothesis might add important knowl- [8] Hopp JL, Anderson KE, Krumholz A, Gruber-Baldini AL, Shulman LM. Psychogenic
edge on the pathophysiology of FND and deserves further attention to seizures and psychogenic movement disorders: are they the same patients? Epi-
be fully verified. lepsy Behav. 2012;25:666–9.
[9] Erro R, Brigo F, Trinka E, Turri G, Edwards MJ, Tinazzi M. Psychogenic nonepileptic
It seems, furthermore, that both aspects of dissociation are strictly
seizures and movement disorders: a comparative review. Neurology Clin Prac.
linked to the duration of illness in the whole sample, suggesting a pre- 2016;6:138–49.
dominant role of both detachment (CADSS Derealization subscale) [10] Demartini B, Goeta D, Barbieri V, Ricciardi L, Canevini MP, Turner K, et al. Psycho-
and compartmentalization (CADSS Dissociative Amnesia subscale) in genic non-epileptic seizures and functional motor symptoms: a common phenome-
nology? J Neurol Sci. 2016;368:49–54.
the maintenance of the symptomatology. In conclusion, our data cor- [11] Miller PP, Brown TA, DiNardo PA, Barlow DH. The experimental induction of
roborate the hypothesis according to which both detachment and com- depersonalisation and derealisation in panic disorder and nonanxious subjects.
partmentalization are important for understanding the mechanism Behav Res Ther. 1994;32:511–9.
[12] Leonard KN, Telch MJ, Harrington PJ. Dissociation in the laboratory: a comparison of
underlying PNES and FMD: we might hypothesize that (i) PNES are pri- strategies. Behav Res Ther. 1999;37:49–61.
marily a detachment phenomenon (measured by a “trait” dissociation [13] Lickel J, Nelson E, Lickel AH, Deacon B. Interoceptive exposure exercises for evoking
instrument such as the Dissociative Experience Scale) and the compart- depersonalisation and derealisation: a pilot study. J Cogn Psychother. 2008;22:321 300.
[14] Dorahy MJ, Peck RK, Huntjens RJ. The impact of dissociation on perceptual priming
mentalization (measured by a “state” dissociation instrument such as and intrusions after listening to auditory narratives. J Trauma Dissociation. 2016;
the MGT) represent a state dissociative epiphenomenon, in terms of 17:410–25.
loss of executive control of mental processes; (ii) FMD are primarily a [15] Caputo GB. Strange-face-in-the-mirror illusion. Perception. 2010;39:1007.
[16] Caputo GB. Apparitional experiences of new faces and dissociation of self-identity
compartmentalization phenomenon (measured by a trait dissociation
during mirror gazing. Percept Mot Skills. 2010;110:1125–38.
instrument such as the Somatoform Dissociation Questionnaire) and [17] Caputo GB, Ferrucci R, Bortolomasi M, Giacopuzzi M, Priori A, Zago S. Visual percep-
detachment (measured by a “state” dissociation instrument such as tion during mirror gazing at one's own face in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2012;
140:46–50.
the MGT) is often part of the ongoing clinical picture and may contribute
[18] Caputo GB, Bortolomasi M, Ferrucci R, Giacopuzzi M, Priori A, Zago S. Visual percep-
to the development and maintenance of FMD (Fig. 1). tion during mirror-gazing at one's own face in patients with depression. Scientific
We acknowledge the limitations of our study. First, the small sample World Journal. 2014;2014:1–4.
size, although in line with other experimental studies on similar topics, [19] LaFrance Jr WC, Baker GA, Duncan R, Goldstein LH, Reuber M. Minimum require-
ments for the diagnosis of psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: a staged approach: a
might limit the generalization of our results. Second, the lack of self-re- report from the International League Against Epilepsy Nonepileptic Seizures Task
port clinical questionnaires on this same cohort of patients does not Force. Epilepsia. 2013;54:2005–18.
allow us to draw definitive conclusions on our hypothesis about the dif- [20] Williams D, Ford B, Fahn S. Phenomenology and psychopathology related to psycho-
genic movement disorders. Adv Neurol. 1995;65:231–57.
ferent tests' sensibility to the facets of dissociation. Third, although the [21] Gupta A, Lang AE. Psychogenic movement disorders. Curr Opin Neurol. 2009;22:
MGT is gaining more and more evidences of reliability in different clin- 430–6.
ical populations, our data would be stronger if confirmed by other ex- [22] Caputo GB. Strange-face illusions during eye-to-eye gazing in dyads: specific effects
on derealisation, depersonalisation and dissociative identity. J Trauma Dissociation.
perimental paradigms aimed to assess the process of dissociation. 2019;20:420–44.
Future researches should expand our preliminary results, evaluating [23] Bremner JD, Krystal JH, Putnam FW, Southwick SM, Marmar C, Charney DS, et al.
patients with FMD and patients with PNES with instruments measuring Measurement of dissociative states with the Clinician-administered Dissociative
States Scale (CADSS). J Trauma Stress. 1998;11:125–36.
both state and trait dissociation. [24] Caputo GB. Dissociation and hallucinations in dyads engaged through interpersonal
gazing. Psychiatry Res. 2015;228:659–63.
Funding [25] Holmes EA, Brown RJ, Mansell W, Fearon RP, ECM Hunter, Frasquilho F, et al. Are
there two qualitatively distinct forms of dissociation? A review and some clinical
implications. Clin Psychol Rev. 2005;25:1–23.
The authors report no funding given to the research. [26] Kuyk J, Spinhoven P, van Dyck R. Hypnotic recall: a positive criterion in the differential
diagnosis between epileptic and pseudoepileptic seizures. Epilepsia. 1999;40:485–91.
[27] Brown RJ, Reuber M. Psychological and psychiatric aspects of psychogenic non-epi-
leptic seizures (PNES): a systematic review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2016;45:157–82.
Declaration of competing interest [28] Goldstein LH, Mellers JDC. Ictal symptoms of anxiety, avoidance behaviour, and dis-
sociation in patients with dissociative seizures. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2006;
77:616–21.
None.
[29] Alper K, Devinsky O, Perrine K, Luciano D, Vazquez B, Pacia S, et al. Dissociation in
epilepsy and conversion nonepileptic seizures. Epilepsia. 1997;38:991–7.
Acknowledgment [30] Lawton G, Baker GA, Brown RJ. Comparison of two types of dissociation in epileptic
and nonepileptic seizures. Epilepsy Behav. 2008;13:333–6.
[31] Stone J, Warlow C, Sharpe M. Functional weakness: clues to mechanism from the na-
The study was partly supported by the “Aldo Ravelli” Research Cen- ture of onset. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2012;83:67–9.
ter on Neurotechnology and Experimental Brain Therapeutics at the De- [32] Roelofs K, Näring GW, Keijsers GP, Hoogduin CAL, van Galen GP, Maris E. Motor im-
partment of Health Sciences, University of Milan, Italy agery in conversion paralysis. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2001;6:21–40.

You might also like