You are on page 1of 5

REVIEW

Interviewing witnesses with


learning disabilities for
legal purposes
Rebecca Milne, Institute of Criminal Justice Studies, University of Portsmouth, Portsmouth,
Hampshire PO1 2DY, UK and Ray Bull Department of Psychology, University of
Portsmouth, Portsmouth, Hampshire PO1 2DY, UK

Summary The present paper gives an overview of the research literature examining how best to
obtain the maximum quality and quantity of information from people with learning
disabilities (LDs) in a non-biasing manner. First, the authors outline the research
concerning the memory performance of people with LDs and then they go on to
describe the specific strategies which have been recommended for use with this
group. The relative merits of different types of questions are examined. It is argued
that the main difficulty in interviewing people with LDs concerns errors of omission
and that research should find ways to enhance recall, especially since detail is the
currency of both the criminal and civil justice systems. One such method that has been
found to gain greater recall is the cognitive interview. The paper finishes by briefly
describing this technique and its efficacy for use with vulnerable groups.
Keywords Cognitive interview, crime, learning disability, questioning, victims, witnesses

Introduction witnesses. It has been believed that their memory systems are
inherently defective, and therefore, it has been assumed that
Many people with learning disabilities (LDs) live indepen-
they are susceptible to suggestion and lack the skills accu-
dently in the community as a result of changes in social care
rately to report events (Perlman et al. 1994). Indeed, Endicott
policies (e.g. NHS and Community Care Act 1990; Kebbell &
(1992) noted in a technical report to the Canadian Depart-
Hatton 1999) and the group as a whole is becoming more
ment of Justice that:
visible. Unfortunately, because of their vulnerability, people
with LDs are often victims of crime (see Mencap 1999), ‘. . .[T]he law has traditionally concentrated on ways to
perhaps because perpetrators perceive members of these establish formally the things that a person with disabil-
populations as less able to report an offence, reducing their ity cannot do. All too often a person’s perceived inability
fear of retribution (Endicott 1992). Furthermore, people with to do some things is translated by legal processes into a
LDs are often the only witnesses to crimes against other finding of inability to do anything. The law has not demon-
people with LDs, such as other residents in a group home strated much capacity to find ways in which the person’s
(Sanders et al. 1997), and may live in socially disadvantaged special needs can be accommodated so that he or she can
areas with high crime rates (Kebbell & Hatton 1999). Society participate in ordinary human activities, including the
has a responsibility to investigate such matters (Bull 1995, activity of doing justice in society.’ (Endicott 1992, p. 5)
1999). Nevertheless, it has been argued that people with LDs
are those most often denied access to the criminal justice There is an urgent need for research into the validity of
system (Milne 1999). these perceptions and it is important to determine whether,
Historically, justice systems, and particularly the criminal and in what situations, people with LDs can be considered
justice system, have regarded people with LDs as unreliable reliable witnesses (Perlman et al. 1994).

# 2001 Blackwell Science Ltd, British Journal of Learning Disabilities, 29, 93–97 93
94 R. Milne and R. Bull

was said to ensure that she or he has understood (Bull 1995,


Memory performance and people
1999).
with learning disabilities
At present, there are few experimental studies which have
Psychologists make a three-part distinction when discussing examined the extent to which accurate information can be
the processes underlying memory, depending on the phase elicited if people with LDs are questioned appropriately. The
of memory being referred to. Encoding concerns the process classic work of Loftus (e.g. 1979) has demonstrated that the
through which information is entered into memory. A phy- particular wording of questions can affect the later recall of
sical input, such as the appearance of a perpetrator, is an event from adults in the general population. If people with
transformed into a representation received into memory. LDs have to rely more heavily than their counterparts in the
The perpetrator’s appearance that has just been encoded is general population on external cues to aid recall, more care
stored in memory. At interview, a few hours or days later, an may be needed not to influence their recall by careless
interviewee may need to describe the perpetrator, so she or interviewing techniques (Bull 1995; Milne & Bull 1999).
he attempts to retrieve this information (Milne & Bull 1999). However, research to date has provided little clear gui-
Recalling information (i.e. remembering) can often be dance about the most appropriate types of questions to use
difficult for people with LDs because they tend to take longer with adults with LDs. This poses particular problems in light
to encode, understand and store information than their peers of the new Youth Justice & Criminal Evidence Act 1999 in
in the general population. As a consequence, their free recall which the interviewing of adults with LDs is to be of crucial
is often incomplete (Bull 1995; Milne & Bull 1999). However, importance and the skills of the interviewers are to be open to
the information that is reported is not necessarily less accu- public scrutiny for the first time (Bull 1999). The Memoran-
rate (Bull 1995, 1999). The limited research to date does not dum of Good Practice (MOGP; Home Office 1992) for inter-
suggest that people with LDs are more likely to fabricate viewing children is currently being updated and extended to
or distort information (i.e. make errors of commission; include guidance for interviewing all vulnerable witnesses.
Gudjonsson & Clare 1995), as long as they are interviewed Both the original and updated versions advocate the phased
appropriately. Instead, it is the recall of detail that is parti- approach to interviewing and involve: (1) rapport develop-
cularly problematic for this population (i.e. errors of omission; ment, (2) free narrative, (3) questioning and (4) closure (for a
Sanders et al. 1997; Milne et al. 1999). full description, see Bull 1996). However, practitioners using
In addition to possibly less efficient encoding, it is the acces- this phased approach have found problems applying it for
sibility of information which may be more limited in people use with children with LDs. This is because of errors of
with LDs. In other words, forgetting may be more often caused omission, with the children’s free report often being limited
by having problems retrieving information, as opposed to (Milne et al. a, in preparation). This provides little for the
the information being lost altogether. These difficulties may interviewer to go on in terms of follow-up questioning (for a
be also exacerbated by limited communication skills. more in-depth view of the criticisms of this approach for this
Problems in accessibility suggest that information which is group of children, see Marchant & Page 1997). Since the free
initially irretrievable could be elicited through the use of narrative stage of the interview tends to contain the most
appropriate cues and prompts (e.g. Winters & Semchuk 1986). reliable information, at least from adults with LDs (88%
Therefore, interviewing techniques which aid communica- accuracy rate, i.e. proportion of correct recall; Milne et al.
tion and aim to assist memory using non-biasing retrieval 1999), research is needed to develop ways of enhancing the
strategies should enhance recall. In addition, questioning is recall elicited from people with LDs in this interview phase.
required to elaborate upon adults with LDs’ often skeletal What types of questions should be used? It has been found
free narratives to elicit specific detail for legal purposes (Bull (Heal & Sigelman 1995) that people with mild LDs are
1995, 1999). What questioning techniques should be used? significantly more likely than their peers to acquiesce to
It is this question to which the present authors now turn. closed yes/no questions, with acquiescence increasing at
the more severe levels of disability. Repeated questions
should also be avoided because an interviewee with LDs
Questioning
may change her or his answer because she or he assumes that
In general, questions should be kept as simple and concrete the first response was wrong. Like their peers in the general
as possible, and abstract concepts and double negatives population, people with LDs also tend to have a last option
should not be used (Perlman et al. 1997; Bull 1999). As stated bias when answering forced-choice questions (Milne et al. b,
by people with LDs themselves, ‘Law people . . . don’t in preparation), perhaps because of a difficulty in remember-
[shouldn’t] use big words, police should keep things simple’ ing the choice of replies (Prosser & Bromley 1998). There is
(Brennan & Brennan 1994, p. 143). Interviewers should tailor some evidence that either/or questions (especially pictorial
the lengths of their sentences to those of the interviewees and ones) elicit more reliable responses (Sigelman et al. 1981).
use plain English. Furthermore, the interviewee should be Even ‘short-answer’ questions (i.e. those which require a
asked, where possible, to repeat back to the interviewer what one- or two-word answer from the interviewee) have been

# 2001 Blackwell Science Ltd, British Journal of Learning Disabilities, 29, 93–97
Interviewing witnesses with learning disabilities 95

found to increase incorrect responding by adults with mild themselves have recommended ‘talking to you not too fast’
LDs (Perlman et al. 1994). The reason may be that this group (Brennan & Brennan 1994, p. 143).
prefer to give any answer, regardless if it is the correct, rather
than admit that they ‘don’t know’. However, in contrast with
Interview setting
these results, Cardone & Dent (1996) found that non-leading
specific questions did not lead to a greater reporting of People with LDs do not always adapt well to unusual
inaccurate information. Indeed, these questions resulted in situations, and thus, police stations and court, which are
more accurate information than was obtained from a ‘free unusual to most people, are likely to be harder to cope with
report only’ group. Nevertheless, supporting the study by (Bull 1995). Poor adaptation has a deleterious effect on
Perlman et al. (1994), Milne et al. (1999) found that it was the memory and communication, and increases stress levels.
questioning phase of the interview, even when containing Furthermore, the interview location should be free of
predominantly open-ended questions and appropriate distractions (Perlman et al. 1997).
closed questions, which resulted in the least reliable informa-
tion (73% accuracy, 88% free report accuracy rate). Although
Interview expectations
the data are confusing, it seems that it might be beneficial to
follow the phased approach to interviewing (as recom- People with LDs may have had negative experiences in the
mended in the MOGP). This will provide information in a past regarding interviews (e.g. concerning changing resi-
hierarchy of reliability (Bull 1996). dence) and may be worried that something ‘bad’ will happen
All interviewees are vulnerable to suggestion (Milne & again. It is imperative to outline the reasons for the interview
Bull 1999), but people with LDs tend to be even more and to check the interviewees’ understanding of what is
susceptible when poorly questioned (e.g. Milne et al. 1999, taking place (Prosser & Bromley 1998).
Milne et al. b, in preparation). Misleading questions had a
deleterious effect on the adults with LDs in the study by
Rapport
Cardone & Dent (1996). The above authors attributed this
effect to increased susceptibility to compliance, as the inter- Rapport is essential for a successful interview, but people
viewees’ non-verbal behaviour intimated that they realized with LDs may need extra time to feel comfortable with
that something was wrong with the questions. According to someone (Bull 1995; Milne 1999). Indeed, 30–40 min of rap-
many authors, the vulnerability to suggestion of people with port building may not be unusual (Marchant & Page 1997).
LDs results from a number of factors, including their sus- People with LDs also differ extensively from one another in
ceptibility to influence by authority figures, acquiescence, their level of social, emotional and cognitive development,
problems understanding the language used, lack of knowl- communication skills, degree of understanding and particu-
edge of appropriate words and an inability to concentrate lar needs. If it has not already happened, assessment of these
(Gudjonsson 1992; Sanders & Young 1994). Therefore, this skills needs to take place during this phase of the interview.
type of question, which may prompt suggestible responding, Brennan & Brennan (1994) strongly advocated that inter-
should be avoided as far as is possible, since information viewers should adopt an interactive model of questioning.
elicited from such questions has limited evidential value. This requires interviewers to take on much more responsi-
Therefore, because the main concern is errors of omission bility for the acquisition of accurate information by changing
when adults with LDs are interviewed appropriately, mem- their own behaviour (verbal and non-verbal) to suit the needs
ory enhancing techniques need to be developed which utilize of the interviewee. In addition, Brennan & Brennan (1994)
non-biasing retrieval mnemonics. The cognitive interview emphasized a need to match the demands of the interview
might be one such technique which helps to overcome the and questioning format to the communication ability of the
difficulties of memory accessibility among people with LDs. interviewee. For example, people with LDs often adapt
Therefore, the next two sections of the present paper concern common words and phrases, and ascribe unusual meanings
the manner and style in which to interview. to them. Therefore, rapport can help to identify language
idiosyncrasies as well as helping the interviewer to assess
the likely competence of the witness.
Interviewing behaviour
Furthermore, since people with LDs often suffer high
social anxiety, low self-esteem and a lack of assertiveness,
Interview duration
it is even more important to put the interviewee at ease
The Canadian Law Reform Commission consultations pro- (Perlman et al. 1997). Allowing an interviewee with LDs to
posed that interviewers could make the process of interview- exert some control in the interview (e.g. determining breaks)
ing less overwhelming if the interviewers just slowed down should reduce the perceived power differential between
their pace of conversation a little, and thus, lengthened interviewer and interviewee. This should help to create a
the duration of the interviews. Indeed, people with LDs psychologically comfortable environment that may have

# 2001 Blackwell Science Ltd, British Journal of Learning Disabilities, 29, 93–97
96 R. Milne and R. Bull

the added benefit of helping to reduce compliance later in interview ¼ 72; (general population) cognitive interview ¼
the questioning phase of the interview (Perlman et al. 1997). 201; structured interview ¼ 160]. However, for the group
with LDs, it also produced a disproportionate increase in
the reporting of person confabulations [mean number of
Cognitive interview
confabulations: (adults with LDs) cognitive interview ¼ 23,
The cognitive interview was developed in the USA by R. E. structured interview ¼ 12; (general population) cognitive
Geiselman and R. P. Fisher (for a full description of the seven interview ¼ 9; structured interview ¼ 8]. Nevertheless, the
phases of the cognitive interview, see Fisher & Geiselman accuracy ratios were similar across interview types (80%
1992; Milne & Bull 1999), and aims to increase the quantity for the cognitive interview and 82% for the structured inter-
and quality of recall elicited from cooperative witnesses and view). Indeed, when interviewed with a cognitive interview,
victims, and possibly suspects of crime. It consists of tech- some of the participants with LDs performed better than
niques to improve memory retrieval and dyadic communi- their counterparts in the general population who were inter-
cation. The cognitive interview has been found to increase viewed with a structured interview (Milne et al. 1999).
the recall of correct information by approximately 35–45% for Conboy-Hill (1998) has also conducted research on the use
adult witnesses (for a meta-analysis, see Köhnken et al. 1999) of the cognitive interview with adults with LDs. In a small
and is now recommended for use with children from the scale case study, the above author found that a cognitive inter-
age of 7 years and upwards. Since the cognitive interview view again increased the reporting of correct details and also
involves the use of non-biasing retrieval techniques, it should reduced the effects of suggestion to post-interview mislead-
also increase the recall of people with LDs. ing questions (for similar results with children and children
In the first ever study examining the applicability of the with LDs, respectively, see Milne et al. 1995, Milne et al. c, in
cognitive interview for use with adults with mild LDs preparation). In addition, Conboy-Hill (1998) noted many
(Brown & Geiselman 1990), the cognitive interview was possible applications of the cognitive interview in LD ser-
found to enhance recall of correct information by 32% com- vices; for example, around issues of consent and evaluation
pared with a control interview, with no significant effect of challenging behaviour (for more details, see Conboy-Hill
upon the number of incorrect details reported (i.e. informa- 1998).
tion discrepant with the event such as saying the car was blue
when it was brown). However, the cognitive interview was Conclusion
also associated with significantly more confabulations (i.e. The present authors hope that this brief review will leave the
details not present in the event), although the numbers were reader with the message that people with LDs, if questioned
very small. Moreover, compared with the general population appropriately, can produce accurate reports of events which
(from Geiselman et al. 1985), adults with LDs recalled less have occurred. Therefore, the focus should be on the abilities
correct information, regardless of the style of interview. of the interviewer rather than the capabilities of the inter-
Nevertheless, the performance of the two populations was viewee (Milne & Bull 1999). What is urgently needed is the
similar in terms of the overall accuracy (i.e. the percentage development of techniques which enhance such accounts,
of total information reported which was correct). Unfortu- particularly when detail is required. This is especially the
nately, this comparison between populations needs to be case now that people with LDs should be afforded greater
viewed very cautiously since it is based on the findings from access to justice through the implementation of the Youth
different studies. Subtle differences in the way in which Justice & Criminal Evidence Act 1999. The cognitive inter-
each study was conducted (e.g. different interviewers) view may provide one such technique, although more
might account for the results. research is necessary (e.g. using live events). Research should
Milne et al. (1999) conducted a study with 47 adults with now also turn its attention to adults with LDs whose intel-
mild LDs attending day centres and 38 adults from the lectual development is very limited.
general population who viewed a videotape recording of a
street accident. A day later, the participants were inter- Acknowledgements
viewed using either a cognitive interview or a structured
interview (a control interview). The structured interview We would like to thank two anonymous reviewers for their
was very similar to the MOGP interview described above. extremely helpful comments on an earlier draft of the manu-
Compared with their counterparts with LDs, adults from the script and Isabel Clare for her invaluable assistance in the
general population recalled more correct information, and writing of this review.
made fewer confabulations about persons and objects.
The type of interview had an impact. For both groups, the References
cognitive interview was more effective than the structured Brennan M. & Brennan R. (1994) Cleartalk: police responding to
interview in enhancing recall [mean numbers of details: intellectual disability. Wagga Wagga, School of Education, Charles
(adults with LDs) cognitive interview ¼ 103, structured Sturt University.

# 2001 Blackwell Science Ltd, British Journal of Learning Disabilities, 29, 93–97
Interviewing witnesses with learning disabilities 97

Brown C.L. & Geiselman R.E. (1990) Eyewitness testimony of the Köhnken G., Milne R., Memon A. & Bull R. (1999) The cognitive
mentally retarded: effect of the cognitive interview. J Police interview: a meta-analysis. Psychology, Crime Law, 5: 3–28.
Criminal Psychol, 6: 14–22. Loftus E.F. (1979) Eyewitness testimony. Cambridge, MA, Harvard
Bull R. (1995) Interviewing witnesses with communicative University Press.
disability. In: Bull R., Carson D., editors. Handbook of psychology in Marchant R. & Page M. (1997) The memorandum and disabled
legal contexts. Chichester, John Wiley & Sons: 247–60. children. In: Westcott H., Jones J., editors. Perspectives on the
Bull R. (1996) Good practice from video recorded interviews with memorandum: policy, practice and research in investigative
child witnesses for use in criminal proceedings. In: Davies G., interviewing. Aldershot, Arena, Ashgate Publishing: 67–79.
Lloyd-Bostock S., McMurran M., Wilson C., editors. Psychology, Mencap (1999) Living in fear. London, Mencap.
law and criminal justice. Berlin, de Gruyter: 100–17. Milne R. (1999) Interviewing children with learning disabilities. In:
Bull R. (1999) Questioning approach. Solicitors Journal, July: 706–7. Memon A., Bull R., editors. Handbook of the psychology of
Cardone D. & Dent H. (1996) Memory and interrogative interviewing. Chichester, John Wiley & Sons: 165–80.
suggestibility: the effects of modality of information presentation Milne R. & Bull R. (1999) Investigative interviewing: psychology and
and retrieval conditions upon the suggestibility scores of practice. Chichester, John Wiley & Sons.
people with learning disabilities. Legal Criminal Psychology, Milne R., Bull R., Köhnken G. & Memon A. (1995) The cognitive
1: 165–77. interview and suggestibility. In: Clark N.K., Stephenson G.M.,
Conboy-Hill S. (1998) The cognitive interview and adults with editors. Criminal behaviour: perceptions, attributions and rationality.
learning disability. Masters Thesis, University of Leicester, Leicester, British Psychological Society: 21–7.
Leicester. Milne R., Clare I.C.H. & Bull R. (1999) Using the cognitive interview
Endicott E. (1992) Technical report: the impact of bill C-15 on persons with adults with mild learning disabilities. Psychology, Crime Law,
with communication disabilities. Ottawa, Research and 5: 81–101.
Development Directorate, Department of Justice. Perlman N.B., Ericson K.I., Esses V.M. & Isaacs B.J. (1994) The
Fisher R.P. & Geiselman R.E. (1992) Memory-enhancing techniques for developmentally handicapped witness: competency as a function
investigative interviewing: the cognitive interview. Springfield, IL, of question format. Law Human Behavior, 18: 171–87.
Charles C Thomas. Perlman N.B., Ericson K.I. & Isaacs B.J. (1997) Young victims with
Geiselman R.E., Fisher R.P., MacKinnon D.P. & Holland H.L. (1985) developmental disabilities. Surrey Place, Canada, Workshop
Eyewitness memory enhancement in the police interview: Proceedings.
cognitive retrieval mnemonics versus hypnosis. J Appl Psychology, Prosser H. & Bromley J. (1998) Interviewing people with intellectual
70: 401–12. disabilities. In: Emerson E., Hatton C., Bromley J., Caine A.,
Gudjonsson G. (1992) The psychology of interrogations, confessions an editors. Clinical psychology and people with intellectual disabilities.
testimony. Chichester, John Wiley & Sons. Chichester, John Wiley & Sons: 99–103.
Gudjonsson G. & Clare I.C.H. (1995) The relationship between Sanders A., Creaton J., Bird S. & Weber L. (1997) Victims with
confabulation and intellectual ability, memory, interrogative learning disabilities: negotiating the criminal justice system. Oxford,
suggestibility and acquiescence. Personality Individual Differences, Oxford University Centre for Criminological Research.
19: 333–8. Sanders A. & Young R. (1994) Criminal justice. London,
Heal L.W. & Sigelman C.K. (1995) Response biases in interviews of Butterworths.
individuals with limited mental ability. J Intellectual Disability Res, Sigelman C.K., Budd E.C., Spanhel C.L. & Schoenrock C.J. (1981)
39: 331–40. Asking questions of retarded persons: a comparison of yes–no
Home Office and Department of Health (HODH) (1992) and either–or formats. Applied Research in Mental Retardation, 2:
Memorandum of good practice for video recorded interviews with child 347–57.
witnesses for criminal proceedings. London, HMSO. Winters J.J. & Semchuk M.T. (1986) Retrieval from long term store
Kebbell M. & Hatton C. (1999) People with mental retardation as a function of mental age and intelligence. Am J Mental
as witnesses in court: a review. Mental Retardation, 37: 179–87. Deficiency, 90: 440–8.

# 2001 Blackwell Science Ltd, British Journal of Learning Disabilities, 29, 93–97

You might also like