You are on page 1of 14

School Psychology Review

ISSN: (Print) 2372-966x (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/uspr20

Students with ADHD in School Classrooms:


Teacher Factors Related to Compatibility,
Assessment, and Intervention

Ross W. Greene

To cite this article: Ross W. Greene (1995) Students with ADHD in School Classrooms: Teacher
Factors Related to Compatibility, Assessment, and Intervention, School Psychology Review, 24:1,
81-93, DOI: 10.1080/02796015.1995.12085753

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/02796015.1995.12085753

Published online: 22 Dec 2019.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 7

View related articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=uspr20
School Psychology Review
Volume 24, No. 1 1995, pp. 81-93

STUDENTS WITH ADHD IN SCHOOL


CLASSROOMS: TEACHER FACTORS RELATED
TO COMPATIBILITY, ASSESSMENT, AND
INTERVENTION
Ross W. Greene
Massachusetts General HospitaUHarmard Medical School
Abstract: Perhaps the most striking omission of school-based treatment outcome re-
search for students with ADHD has been the examination of teacher factors that may
have a significant bearing upon outcome. As a result, the conceptualization of ADHD
and its treatment is incomplete. A perspective incorporating goodness-of-fit/systems
theory may be a more adequate model from which to view interactions between stu-
dents with ADHD and their teachers and a more predictive model from which to hy-
pothesize anticipated treatment outcomes. Two compatibility issues are explored in
some detail: teacher-treatment compatibility (i.e., the match between teacher charac-
teristics and commonly recommended behavior management procedures) and
teacher-student compatibility (i.e., the match between teacher characteristics and
student characteristics). Potential teacher factors which might be important to con-
sider in such compatibility equations are discussed, along with the implications of
these variables for school-based assessment and intervention processes and future re-
search.

A substantial literature on children with (e.g., Abramowitz, Eckstrand, O’Leary, &


attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder Dulcan, 1992; Hoza, Pelham, Sams, & Carl-
(ADHD) has accumulated over the past 2 son, 1992; Swanson, Cantwell, Lerner,
decades. An impressive portion of this liter- McBurnett, & Hanna, 1991). Hence, suc-
ature has addressed the difficulties students cessful school outcomes for students with
with ADHD experience in school class- ADHD depend upon the degree to which
rooms and the efficacy of interventions to treatment components and potencies match
minimize these difficulties. Medication and the needs of particular children. This
behavior management procedures remain “match” between treatment and student
the two most commonly employed school- may best be conceptualized as a “compati-
based interventions for children with ADHD bility equation” (in this case, studerzt-treat-
(Rapport, 1992). Due to well-documented ment compatibility] in which characteris-
limitations associated with both strategies, tics of both child and treatment are taken
some combination of the two is generally into consideration in planning treatment
accepted as the optimal approach to treat- and evaluating outcome.
ment (e.g., Pelham, 1993). Nevertheless, an Studies examining student response to
increasingly common finding in studies treatment can be criticized for an almost
which examine the efficacy of such a com- singular focus on treatment ingredients, as
bined approach to treatment is the consid- well as a relative neglect of student charac-
erable variability in treatment response teristics that may mediate treatment out-
among individual students (Pelham, 1993). come. Yet perhaps the most striking limita-
As such, a case-by-case assessment of treat- tion of school-based treatment outcome re-
ment ingredients and potencies is necessary search has been the lack of consideration of

The author would like to acknowledge the following individuals for their invaluable feedback on previous drafts
of this manuscript: Russell A. Barkley, Caryn L. Carlson, Susan G. O’Leary, Thomas H. Ollendick, Mark D. Rap-
port, Gary N. Siperstein, and Melissa S. Tomback.
Address all correspondence concerning this article to Ross W. Greene, PhD, Pediatric Psychopharmacology Unit,
WACC-725, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, MA 02114.

81
82 School Psychology Review, 1995, Vol. 24, No. 1

teacher characteristics that may have a sig- central to both perspectives. Rather than
nificant bearing on outcome. Two addi- seeing the cause of a problem in a single
tional compatibility equations - person’s behavior (a unidirectional explana-
teacher-treatment compatibility (the tion), the systems viewpoint attempts to un-
match between different teachers and dif- derstand behavior as the product of various
ferent treatment ingredients and potencies) forces that reciprocally influence each
and student-teacher compatibility (the other (Papero, 1983). In systems thinking,
match between a particular teacher and a the emphasis is on both the characteristics
particular student with ADHD with regard of an individual and the contexts that may
to a variety of factors) - have been virtu- influence the nature of the individual’s be-
ally ignored. This article suggests that a havior (as well as the manner in which each
more adequate and predictive model from influences the other). No single event or
which to conceptualize classroom outcome piece of behavior causes another; each is
for students with ADHD will be one in linked in a reciprocal manner to other
which “teacher factors” are given greater events and pieces of behavior (Papp, 1983).
consideration. As such, issues of teacher-setting compati-
bility (e.g., job dissatisfaction) may influ-
Goodness-of-Fit and Systems Theory ence, and be influenced by, issues of stu-
dent-teacher compatibility.
The terms compatibility and match can Goodness-of-fit and systems theory are
be traced most directly to the goodness-of- useful frameworks for conceptualizing the
fit literature (e.g., Thomas, Chess, & Birch, classroom difficulties of students with
1968). Goodness-of-fit is a concept that has ADHD because they compel focus not only
been applied productively to child-care- upon the manner in which the student with
giver interactions (e.g., Bell & Harper, 1977; ADHD is compatible or incompatible with
Thomas & Chess, 1980); several authors the classroom environment, but also on the
have noted that the concept may be relevant manner in which the classroom environ-
to child-teacher interactions involving a va- ment is compatible or incompatible with
riety of special student populations, includ- the student with ADHD. As a “situation-spe-
ing those diagnosed with ADHD (e.g., cific” disorder (e.g., Barkley, 1989; Whalen
Brooks, 1984; Conners & Wells, 1986; Shay- & Henker, 1980; Zentall, 1985), ADHD char-
witz & Shaywitz, 1988; see Whalen & acteristics are apt to be more or less prob-
Henker, 1980 for a comprehensive discus- lematic depending on certain environmen-
sion of the “social ecology” of ADHD). tal contexts. If a student’s ADHD-related be-
Goodness-of-fit can be defined as the de- havior is exacerbated or improved in cer-
gree to which the capacities, motivations, tain situations - such as interactions with
and style of behaving of an organism (the certain teachers, classroom environments,
child) are compatible with properties, ex- or specific tasks - then a goodness-of-
pectations, and demands of the environ- fit/systems approach necessitates examina-
ment (the teacher and classroom). Conso- tion of those aspects of the environment
nance and dissonance are two general contributing to positive and negative varia-
terms describing this compatibility between tions in behavior. From such a perspective,
an organism and its environment(s). Conso- it is impossible to conceptualize the deficits
nance refers to a situation in which good in attention, self-regulation, and motivation
compatibility exists and under which opti- that characterize ADHD separately from the
mal development and progress is thought to environmental contexts in which they oc-
occur. Dissonance refers to the opposite sit- cur. Accordingly, a unidirectional perspec-
uation; under such circumstances distorted tive in which the difficulties exhibited by
development and maladaptiveness is students with ADHD are viewed solely as a
thought to occur. function of their ADHD status (i.e., their or-
There is a clear overlap between the ganismic status) is apt to be less adequate
concept of goodness-of-fit and constructs and predictive.
associated with systems theory; an avoid- Since classroom teachers are a major
ante of linear, cause-and-effect thinking is component of a child’s school environment,
ADHD in School Classrooms 83

identifying relevant aspects of teacher be- Barkley, 1990; DuPaul, Anastopoulos, Shel-
havior affecting students with ADHD is crit- ton, Guevremont, & Metevia, 1992; Greene
ical. Research suggesting that some teach- & Barkley, in press-a). Thus, the ADHD di-
ers are more effective in working with diffi- agnosis is frequently based on a child’s
cult students (Kauffman & Wong, 1991; Lar- standing on behavior checklists which com-
rivee, 1985; Lloyd, Kauffman, & Kupersmidt, pare a child’s behavior to what normatively
1990) supports the broader, more compre- is expected given their age and gender
hensive goodness-of-fit/systems conceptu- (Costello, Loeber, & Stouthamer-Loeber,
alization. Such findings oblige us to frame 1991). On such checklists, teachers and par-
the problems of students with ADHD differ- ents rate the degree to which various be-
ently: outcome is a function not only of haviors are present or problematic. These
characteristics associated with the child but ratings provide the most objective data re-
also of characteristics associated with the garding the degree to which a child’s behav-
teacher. If we ignore teacher factors influ- ior meets the criterion of developmental de-
encing treatment efficacy (teacher-treat- viance central to the diagnosis of ADHD.
ment compatibility) and the general rela- The limitations of behavior checklists
tionship between a teacher and student are fairly well-documented (e.g., Schaugh-
(student-teacher compatibility), then we ency & Rothlind, 1991) and include reporter
will capture only a fraction of the variance bias (in which the rater over- or under-esti-
associated with outcome. Further, if we are mates the occurrence of particular behav-
to begin conceptualizing characteristics of iors), inaccurate reporter recall regarding
students with ADHD and their teachers as certain behaviors, and reporter misinterpre-
components in a compatibility equation, it tation of checklist items. Checklist ratings
may be necessary to revise our definition of also may be influenced by various charac-
outcome. Outcome no longer merely refers teristics of the informants, including their
to the question, “What was the response of education, intelligence, and emotional sta-
this child with ADHD to this teacher and tus at the time ratings are completed
those treatment procedures in this class- (Barkley, 1990). Thus, there is some sugges-
room environment?” Rather, it also refers to tion that a teacher’s ratings of a particular
the question, “What was this teacher’s re- child are influenced by characteristics of
sponse to this child with ADHD and those both child and teacher and are partially re-
treatment procedures in this classroom en- flective of their compatibility. Although
vironment?” data indicate that teachers are able to dis-
In the following section, two important tinguish between children with and without
issues are discussed to underscore the no- symptoms of ADHD (e.g., Atkins, Pelham, &
tion that failing to consider the impact of Licht, 1985), teachers’ detection thresholds
situational factors increases the likelihood for perceiving children as deviant or defi-
of linear, unidirectional thinking, thereby cient and in need of special help may influ-
reducing the adequacy and predictive utility ence the likelihood that a child will be diag-
of our conceptualizations about children nosed. Little empirical information is avail-
with ADHD. The first issue of unidirection- able about the potentially powerful role of
ality relates to the assessment and diagnos- individual differences in these person-per-
tic process for ADHD, and the second to the ception processes (Whalen, 1989).
(sole) use of medication in reducing the There is, however, significant variability
severity of ADHD-related behaviors. in the way different teachers (and parents)
perceive and respond to various child be-
Linear/Unidirectional haviors; behaviors considered to be a major
Conceptualizations problem by one teacher may not be viewed
as such by another (e.g., Walker & Rankin,
Although still a debated issue, none of 1983). Presumably, some teachers perceive
the direct measures developed to quantify ADHD-related behaviors as more trouble-
difficulties with attention span, impulse some, are more reactive to such behaviors,
control, and vigilance have thus far proven and respond to these behaviors less effec-
to have satisfactory diagnostic utility (e.g., tively than others. These gerceptions and
84 School Psychology Review, 1995, Vol. 24, No. 1

responses are likely to influence the nature dren with ADHD do not evince a significant
of student-teacher interactions, a teacher’s medication treatment effect (Abikoff & Git-
ratings of a given child, and referrals for telman, 1985; Pelham, 1993; this is still a de-
special education assistance and to mental bated issue, as some research teams have
health professionals outside the school sys- reported very low percentages of nonre-
tem. For example, researchers have found spondents; e.g., Elia, Borcherding,
clear individual differences among teachers Rapoport, & Keysor [1991]).
not only in their views about what consti- When prescribed in the absence of
tutes unacceptable classroom behavior but other intervention, pharmacotherapy may
also in their willingness to accept responsi- represent a less comprehensive approach to
bility for students who exhibit unacceptable resolving the difficulties exhibited by chil-
behavior without technical assistance (e.g., dren with ADHD in the classroom because
Kauffman, Lloyd, & McGee, 1989). it targets for change only one component
Viewing a teacher’s ratings of a child as (the child) of the compatibility equation. AI-
a de facto portrayal of the child (rather than though the effects of medication have been
as a description of this child’s interactions shown to extend beyond the child - for ex-
with this teacher in this classroom environ- ample, studies have shown that teachers
ment) ignores the possible impact of these come to interact with and view students
differences in teachers’ perceptions and re- with ADHD in a more positive manner after
sponses. Little attention has been paid to in- the children are medicated (e.g., Whalen,
terteacher variability in rating the presence Henker, & Dotemoto, 1980) - such an ap-
and severity of ADHD behavior and the pos- proach implies that the problem emanates
sible explanatory mechanisms of this vari- from within the child (Henker & Whalen,
ability. Given the importance of making ac- 1980). In the context of a goodness-of-
curate ADHD diagnoses, this would appear fit/systems conceptualization, this is at best
to be an important area of inquiry. The vari- an incomplete and at worst a counterpro-
ability in teachers’ perceptions and re- ductive way in which to view the student
sponses also strengthens the argument that with ADHD. The problem can instead be
a more adequate and predictive diagnostic conceptualized as a product of a variety of
process should include assessment not only factors, including characteristics of the
of the child but also of characteristics of the child, teacher(s), parent(s), classroom and
teacher (as described below). school environments, task demands, and so
The second issue of unidirectionality forth. Are there teacher characteristics
relates to the use of medication, which has which may mediate medication effects? If
oecome common practice for intervening we focus exclusively on the medication
with students with ADHD. As many as 88% component of treatment are we apt to se-
of children with ADHD receive Ritalin or verely limit our ability to predict outcome
other stimulants at some point in childhood accurately?
(Wolraich et al., 1990). Numerous investiga- Before examining teacher-treatment
tive teams have found that the classroom and student-teacher compatibility issues
functioning and academic performance of more closely, it must be acknowledged that
many children with ADHD improves in re- two related and important compatibility is-
sponse to stimulant medication (e.g., Dou- sues are not addressed in detail in the fol-
glas, Barr, O’Neill, & B&ton, 1986; Pelham, lowing discussion: teacher-setting compat-
Bender, Caddell, Booth, & Moorer, 1985; ibility (the match between characteristics
Rapport, Stoner, & Jones, 1986). Nonethe- of the teacher and characteristics of the
less, debate continues regarding the extent teaching setting) and child-setting compat-
to which medication “normalizes” the be- ibility (the match between characteristics
havior and performance of children with of the child and characteristics of the learn-
ADHD (e.g., Pelham, 1993; Swanson et al., ing setting). The latter area has already
1991) especially when medication effects been the focus of several studies. For ex-
are examined at the individual level (Du- ample, factors such as a high level of noise
Paul & Rapport, 1993). Further, researchers has been shown to have detrimental effects
have concluded that a large minority of chil- on the behavior of students with ADHD
ADHD in School Classrooms 85

(e.g., Whalen, Henker, Collins, Finck, & (Abikoff & Gittelman, 1984; Gittelman et al.,
Dotemoto, 1979), while extra- and intratask 1980; Pelham, Schnedler, Bologna, & Con-
novelty stimulation have been found to re- teras, 1980).
duce activity and improve performance, es- Consideration of teacher-treatment
pecially on easy, repetitive tasks (e.g., Zen- compatibility should improve our under-
tall & Meyer, 1987). Still, our understanding standing of these treatment outcome issues.
of the child-setting compatibility issue can Are commonly recommended behavior
hardly be characterized as complete; other management strategies compatible with the
setting variables (e.g., size of class, ratio of expectations, capacities, motivations, and
hyperactive to nonhyperactive children, behavioral styles of teachers? The treat-
seating arrangements, and open versus ment acceptability literature suggests a va-
closed classes) are worthy of additional ex- riety of factors related to characteristics of
ploration (Pfiffner & O’Leary, 1993). With both teachers and treatments relevant to
regard to teacher-setting compatibility, is- this question including: (a) perceived sever-
sues such as job satisfaction, teaching grat- ity of a child’s problem, (b) nature of treat-
ification, grade preference, student popula- ment, (c) time demands involved in imple-
tion preference, and general school envi- menting a treatment, (d) perceived effec-
ronment are potential areas of inquiry. Bor- tiveness of a treatment, and (e) teacher
rowing terminology from Carlson and La- background variables (see Elliott [1988] for
hey (1988), our ability to “engineer settings a comprehensive review of these factors).
that optimize adaptation” for students with For example, research has suggested
ADHD, and our concepts of interactions be- that teachers tend to prefer more time-effi-
tween students with ADHD and their teach- cient behavioral treatments (e.g., Witt,
ers, will remain incomplete until these addi- Martens, & Elliott, 1984), that greater
tional person-environment compatibility is- knowledge of and familiarity with behav-
sues are more closely examined. ioral strategies increase treatment accept-
ability among teachers (Elliott, 1988), and
Teacher-Treatment Compatibility that teachers’ acceptability ratings are
higher for positive behavioral treatments
Classroom-based behavior modifica- (e.g., praise, token economies) than for re-
tion procedures typically involve a combi- ductive treatments (e.g., time-out, response
nation of token reinforcement and mild cost; Elliott, Witt, Galvin, & Peterson, 1984;
punishment strategies. Fairly detailed de- Witt et al., 1984). More recently, the latter
scriptions of such methods have been pro- finding has been replicated as specifically
vided by numerous authors (e.g., related to interventions for students with
Abramowitz, 1994; DuPaul, 1991; Pfiffner & ADHD (Power, Hess, & Bennett, 1993).
Barkley, 1990; Pfiffner & O’Leary, 1993). Teachers also may be more concerned with
Others have proposed various teaching group outcomes, whereas clinicians and
strategies and classroom modifications to consultants may tend to focus on individual
minimize the difficulties exhibited by stu- outcomes (Fantuzzo & Atkins, 1992). This
dents with ADHD (e.g., Algozzine & Ys- may explain teachers’ preference for behav-
seldyke, 1992; McCarney, 1989). ioral interventions employing group contin-
Although behavior management pro- gencies and relative lack of enthusiasm for
grams enhance the academic performance individually administered contingencies.
and behavior of students with ADHD, con- Recent findings further suggest that teach-
tingency management procedures, even ers view combined behavioral and pharma-
more than medication, have not been found cological interventions as more effective
to normalize children’s classroom behavior and acceptable for students with ADHD
(e.g., Abikoff & Klein, 1992). Studies have than medication alone (Power et al., 1993).
shown that even when behavioral treat- Other possible teacher factors related
ments produce clinical improvements, chil- to successful implementation can be ex-
dren with ADHD may still be significantly trapolated from the literature regarding
more deviant than normal classroom con- parental implementation of behavior man-
trols on a number of crucial dimensions agement strategies. As summarized by
86 School Psychology Review, 1995, Vol. 24, No. I

McMahon and Wells (1989), family compo- ful implementation of a school-based be-
sition (single- versus two-parent house- havior management program. It is unclear,
holds), socioeconomic status, personal and however, whether a teacher’s implementa-
marital distress (including parenting tion of such a program is the most critical
stress), parent personality traits, and component of overall school outcome for
parental knowledge, attitudes, and beliefs students with ADHD. Other teacher charac-
may be important to the successful imple- teristics may bear even greater influence on
mentation of behavior management pro- both student-teacher interactions and the
grams with children exhibiting problems broader behavioral and academic prospects
similar to those seen in ADHD. Several of of students with ADHD in classrooms.
these characteristics can be reframed for The notion that school outcome for stu-
application to teachers and their ability to dents with ADHD may be related to stu-
implement classroom behavior manage- dent-teacher compatibility has been sug-
ment programs. For example, classroom gested by several authors (Whalen et al.,
composition and structure (e.g., presence of 1979), and similar suggestions have been
an aide, inclusion of other special needs stu- made regarding generically defined emo-
dents), personal distress (including the tionally or behaviorally challenging popula-
stress of having a given child in the class), tions (Kauffman & Wong, 1991; Landrum,
teacher personality traits, and knowledge, 1992). Theorists have posited that the diffi-
attitudes, and beliefs about children with culties of students with EBD (emotional or
ADHD (e.g., the degree to which certain behavioral disorders) are often due in part
ADHD-related behaviors are interpreted as to incompatibilities between pupils and
“intentional”) may all be related to effective teachers and mismatches in their behav-
implementation of behavior management ioral styles, perceptions, or expectations
procedures. There has been little systematic (Wong, Kauffman, & Lloyd, 1991). Unfortu-
evaluation of these teacher and setting vari- nately, there has been little systematic study
ables. of which teacher characteristics are most
Although clearly nonexhaustive, the influential in facilitating successful interac-
above discussion strongly suggests that the tions with students with ADHD.
notion of treatment potency - defined ear- Good and Brophy (1991) have sug-
lier as the practice of applying variable in- gested that a teacher’s classroom behavior
tensities of specific treatment components management skills/practices may con-
on a case-by-case basis depending on the in- tribute to student-teacher interchanges and
dividual needs of a given child with ADHD compatibility. Much of the research examin-
- also must be applied as it relates to the ing interchanges between children and
needs and characteristics of the teacher adults has centered on parents and their
charged with implementation. The charac- children, and it may again be helpful to ex-
teristics and potency of a behavior manage- trapolate from this research to generate hy-
ment program deemed consonant for a potheses regarding interchanges between
child may produce a high degree of disso- students and teachers (Landrum, 1992). As
nance in a teacher whose capacities, moti- summarized by Kendziora and O’Leary
vations, style of behaving, and teaching de- (1992), interactions between parent and
mands have not been taken into account. child are apt to be dysfunctional when a
Such a program would be expected to have parent is (a) uninvolved and not responding
low compliance, effectiveness, and mainte- to the child with sufficient warmth and
nance (see Reimers, Wacker, & Koeppl stimulation; (b) overly harsh and control-
[1987] for a model of treatment acceptabil- ling; (c) unable to establish reasonable ex-
ity incorporating this perspective). pectations and limits for the child; (d) inad-
vertently attentive to inappropriate behav-
Student-Teacher Compatibility ior; (e) vague or attacking in communicat-
ing with the child; (f) inconsistent and gen-
The above discussion suggests that erally inept in handling situations calling for
teacher factors may play a critical role in discipline; and (g) too gentle, lengthy, or de-
determining the acceptability and success- layed in dealing with misbehavior. Teachers’
ADHD in School Classrooms

problem-solving skills (i.e., facility at gener- practices influences school outcome for
ating and implementing strategies) for deal- students with ADHD.
ing with various ADHD-related behaviors A final category of teacher characteris-
also may be an important factor. Many of tics is suggested by the literature on child
these characteristics have possible applica- temperament and parent-child goodness-
tion to interactions between teachers and of-fit. As noted by Seifer (unpublished man-
children with ADHD, but this possibility has uscript), goodness-of-fit consists of objec-
not been well-explored. At this juncture the tive and subjective constructs. Objectively-
literature is neither sufficiently comprehen- defined goodness-of-fit can include expec-
sive nor sophisticated for definitive conclu- tation-behavior matching (a process of
sions to be reached regarding the behavior comparing adult expectations and child
management practices or nature of characteristics for degree of fit). Subjec-
teacher-student interchanges that may opti- tively-defined goodness-of-fit can include
mize behavior of students with ADHD. an individual’s cognitive appraisal of the de-
Good and Brophy (1991) have further gree of fit (i.e., how is a behavior inter-
suggested that teaching practices - and the preted?) and affective response to the per-
classroom “climate” emanating from these ceived degree of fit (i.e., how do the ap-
practices - may also impact upon stu- praisals feel?).
dent-teacher compatibility. A recent study Some of these goodness-of-fit compo-
involving a nationwide panel of experts rep- nents have been studied in relation to teach-
resenting general and special education ers and students, though typically not in di-
identified and validated 96 teaching prac- rect examination of student-teacher com-
tices as being essential to the effective patibility. For example, with regard to ob-
teaching of students with mild handicaps in jectively-defined goodness-of-fit, re-
regular classrooms (Cannon, Idol, & West, searchers have shown that most classroom
teachers have similar expectations and atti-
1992). These practices encompass the
tudes regarding successful student adjust-
broad areas of assessment and diagnosis
ment. Such adjustment is characterized by a
(i.e., of student learning needs and pat- behavioral repertoire that (a) facilitates
terns), instructional content, instructional academic performance (i.e., listening to the
practices, management of student behavior, teacher, following instructions and direc-
planning and managing the teaching and tions, working on assigned tasks, complying
learning environment, and monitoring and with teacher requests, etc.) and (b) is
evaluation procedures (i.e., for gauging stu- marked by the absence of disruptive and/or
dent progress). Within these categories are unusual behaviors that challenge the teach-
a variety of specific teacher behaviors, in- ers’ authority and disrupt classroom atmos-
cluding adjustment and modification of cur- phere or are objectionable to teachers and
ricular materials to meet individual student difficult for them to cope with (Hersh &
needs; providing content to students Walker, 1983; Kerr & Zigmond, 1986). Chil-
through student-teacher interaction rather dren with ADHD clearly do not fit this de-
than relying on curricular materials to con- scription of the model behavioral profile. As
vey information; and minimizing student er- such, their style of behaving elevates the
rors by choosing tasks students can handle risk for heightened tension in their interac-
without frustration and monitoring perfor- tions with most teachers. There is, however,
mance to provide immediate help and cor- little evidence to suggest that the disparity
rective feedback when needed. Although it between teachers’ behavioral expectations
is unclear whether practices identified as and the behavior of students with ADHD in-
essential to the teaching of mildly handi- evitably leads to negative outcomes. At pre-
capped students generalize to the disruptive sent, we lack information regarding other
behavior disorders, these practices may related, but more variable, aspects of
nonetheless be relevant to the current dis- teacher expectations (e.g., cflexibility of ex-
cussion. To date, however, no studies di- pectations) that may be more potent con-
rectly examine the extent to which the tributors to objectively-defined goodness-
presence (or absence) of specific teaching of-fit.
88 School Psychology Review, 1995, Vol. 24, No. I

Other research seems germane to sub- ADHD in school settings, and are adapted
jectively-defined goodness-of-fit. For in- from the various references cited above.
stance, while there is general consensus 1. Assessments must be systems-ori-
among teachers regarding the unacceptabil- ented and must incorporate the possibility
ity of maladaptive pupil behavior that of situational influences on behavior be-
threatens classroom control, there is far cause behavior may not be consistent
less agreement among teachers about the across time and settings. Thus, school-
degree to which various adaptive behaviors based assessments for ADHD should be
are eriticaZ to a student’s success in their broad enough to include not only the child
classrooms (Lloyd, Kauffman, Landrum, & but also the multiple adults (e.g., parents
Roe, 1991). This suggests that teachers have and teachers) who interact (and have previ-
different tolerances for various student be- ously interacted) with the child in multiple
haviors, and that low teacher tolerance (in settings (e.g., various contexts within the
this case, for ADHD-related behaviors) is school and home). Diagnostic conclusions
likely to result in negative outcomes for will be more definitive if there is stability in
both teacher and student (Hersh & Walker, the child’s behavior across time and con-
1983; Walker & Rankin, 1983). As noted ear- texts. In instances of inconsistency across
lier, teachers may also interpret and react to situations - for example, the child’s ADHD-
ADHD-related behaviors in diverse ways related behavior is more frequent and inten-
(e.g., Whalen, 1989), and these interpreta- sive in interactions with a particular class-
tions and reactions may contribute to the room environment - the evaluation
compatibility that exists between a student process must focus on finer compatibility
with ADHD and his or her teacher (Brooks, issues that may account for the inconsis-
1984). These goodness-of-fit issues also tency. As discussed above, this expanded
await exploration. assessment process will by definition in-
clude examination of teacher characteris-
Implications for School-Based tics that may be impacting upon a child’s be-
Assessment and Intervention havior and vice versa. Such a process
should increase understanding of the com-
It may be useful at this point to summa- plex, reciprocal factors affecting child and
rize and expand upon many of the points teacher behaviors and facilitate appropriate
made above as they apply to school-based interventions in the specific contexts where
assessment and intervention processes for incompatibility exists. Targets for interven-
students with ADHD. Readers are referred tions may subsequently encompass specific
to other sources for reviews of the complex characteristics of the child, teacher, class-
array of factors that must be considered in room environment, tasks, and so forth.
the assessment of ADHD (e.g., &Paul & 2. Assessments must be multimodal, in-
Stoner, 1994; Greene & Barkley, in press-b) cluding different informants and methods.
and other childhood behaviors (e.g., Mash & These should include naturalistic observa-
Terdal, 1988; Ollendick & Greene, 1990). tion (e.g., direct observation of the child in
These sources underscore the necessity of different contexts), rating scales completed
assessing the contexts in which behaviors by multiple sources, interviews with multi-
occur; however, they typically do not pro- ple sources, and review of other relevant in-
vide detail about how to assess teacher formation (e.g., psychoeducational testing).
characteristics that may represent a major Although the necessity of naturalistic obser-
aspect of such contexts. The assessment lit- vation has been questioned, it is difficult to
erature can, nevertheless, provide a useful imagine circumstances under which a com-
framework for discussing mechanisms by prehensive understanding of ADHD-related
which the assessment process can be ex- behavior can be achieved in the absence of
panded to include teacher characteristics such observation. Inconsistency among
and other contextual variables. sources would once again compel focus on
The following are suggested guidelines finer issues of incompatibility, as described
for assessments and interventions for above, thereby allowing appropriate inter-
ADHD in School Classrooms 89

ventions in the specific situations (and with child, are charged with implementation of
specific individuals) where incompatibility interventions, and/or are targets of inter-
is evident. vention to monitor and maintain the ongo-
3, A diagnosis should be viewed as ing assessment process and facilitate refor-
merely descriptive, not explanatory. Thus, mulation of “the problem” as necessary.
assessments must focus less on assigning a 5. Interventions should be guided not
child to a category and more on obtaining only by consideration of the needs of the
information that is directly relevant to treat- student with ADHD, but also by the degree
ment. “Relevance for treatment” refers to to which recommended strategies are ac-
the usefulness of information in pinpointing ceptable to the teachers charged with im-
treatment goals, the selection of targets for plementation. Selection of intervention
intervention, the design and implementa- strategies should be viewed as a separate
tion of interventions, and the evaluation of assessment process; teachers should be as-
intervention outcomes. By itself, conclud- sessed along the various dimensions dis-
ing that a child has ADHD provides no use- cussed above prior to design of an interven-
ful information about the contexts in which tion program and should be active partici-
the disorder is most problematic. Pinpoint- pants in the design process. Assessment
ing situations in which ADHD-related be- should continue after intervention has been
havior is exacerbated (e.g., transitions be- initiated to determine responses of both
tween classes or activities, group discus- child and teacher. Finally, intervention
sions, standing in line for lunch, playing should include not only traditional child-tar-
softball, interacting with certain teachers) geted strategies (medication, behavior man-
helps identify the compatibility issues that agement programs, accommodations, etc.)
should be targeted for intervention and but also teacher-targeted strategies (e.g., ed-
thereby allow more educated and fme- ucation and training).
tuned selection of intervention options. Fur
thermore, assessment should be viewed as Implications for Applied Research
an ongoing, fluid process, especially in The above discussion directly and indi-
school settings where contexts can change rectly suggests a fairly extensive agenda for
dramatically from class to class and year to future research. With regard to commonly
year. recommended classroom behavior manage-
4, Assessments must be conducted by ment strategies, our knowledge of the de-
persons with the training and experience in gree to which behavioral programs are im-
ADHD to execute them in an educated fash- plemented by teachers as instructed is still
ion and take into consideration the complex quite limited. Also lacking is information re-
compatibility issues discussed above. Such garding the amount of training required for
persons must have the skills to analyze, or- successful implementation, the points dur-
ganize, integrate, and communicate the vast ing actual implementation at which teach-
array of information gathered in the assess- ers begin to abandon the use of such strate-
ment process for the purposes of (a) arriv- gies, and the teacher characteristics (e.g.,
ing at a comprehensive understanding of a stress) associated with successful or unsuc-
child’s interactions with their environ- cessful implementation and teachers’ will-
ment(s); (b) requiring that additional infor- ingness to initiate and maintain the use of
mation be collected when such an under- various intervention approaches. Further,
standing has not been achieved; (c) making much of the recent research in this area has
accurate judgments regarding the develop- been conducted in experimental settings
mental deviance of a child’s behavior; (d) (e.g., summer camps), and it is unclear
determining the most appropriate persons whether results from such settings general-
and behaviors to be targeted for change and ize to typical school classrooms and teach-
the interventions most likely to produce ers,
these desired changes; and (e) maintaining The treatment acceptability research
contact over the long term with the various has provided extremely useful information
adults who continue to interact with the regarding the limits of behavioral interven-
90 School PsychologyReview, 1995, Vol. 24, No. 1

tions in school classrooms and the teacher to student responses to treatment. Although
characteristics that may impact upon ac- our understanding of the effects of medica-
ceptability. However, data gathered during tion and behavior management strategies
actual training and implementation of be- on the classroom performance and behav-
havioral programs are needed, as much of ior of children with ADHD may be classified
the information in this area has been ob- as incomplete, our understanding of other
tained via analogue methodology. More- factors influencing outcome - especially
over, only recently have researchers begun those associated with teachers - may best
to examine the treatment acceptability is- be described as embryonic. Medication and
sue as it relates specifically to teachers of, behavior management strategies are impor-
and interventions for, students with ADHD. tant and beneficial interventions, but their
A wide range of teacher characteristics effectiveness may be further enhanced by a
were hypothesized to influence their inter- goodness-of-fit/systems theory perspective
actions with students with ADHD, including incorporating various contextual factors
expectations, attitudes, perceptions, attri- that contribute to outcome.
butions, tolerance thresholds, level of Consideration of various teacher char-
stress, and behavior management and acteristics would improve the assessment
teaching practices. Although some mea- and intervention processes related to stu-
sures of teacher characteristics have ac- dents with ADHD. Unfortunately, such re-
ceptable psychometric properties, addi- search efforts may meet resistance, due to
tional valid and reliable instruments need to sensitivity on the part of teachers and
be developed. The development of such school systems regarding the examination
measurement technology must occur be- of these issues.
fore the effects of these teacher character- The glass ceiling of what can be learned
istics on student outcome can be fully eval- by merely examining student-treatment
uated. compatibility may have been reached -
Although teacher-setting and child-set- many important points beyond may not be
ting compatibility issues were not discussed attainable unless we begin to examine the
in detail, exploration of various setting vari- contribution of teachers more closely. As
ables related to the former (e.g., job satis- noted years ago by two leading ADHD re-
faction, teaching gratification, grade prefer- searchers (Whalen & Henker, 1980), as one
ence, student population preference, and moves from the molecular level of research
general school environment) and the latter (the individual child) to more macrolevels
(e.g., size of class, ratio of hyperactive to of analysis (for example, consideration of
nonhyperactive children, seating arrange- teachers and educational practices) the
ments, and open versus closed classes) phenomena under study require more quali-
would likely yield important information. tative methodologies and more speculative
Finally, teacher factors must be entered inferential processes. Thus, ethnographc
into predictive equations of school-based methods may be useful in the early stages of
treatment outcomes. It would ultimately be such research in identifying relevant vari-
desirable to manipulate such factors to ex- ables and possible relationships among
amine their causal influences. Future inves- them. Once researchers can develop rea-
tigations along these lines might include ex- sonable hypotheses about a more limited
amination of the differential effect of med- set of variables, these hypotheses can be
ication and/or behavioral treatment on chil- tested using quantitative, multivariate meth-
dren with ADHD depending on characteris- ods of research design.
tics of the teacher, and longitudinal evalua-
tion of the differential impact of various REFERENCES
teachers on a child’s outcome in the ele- Abikoff, H., & Gittelman, R. (1984). Does behavior ther-
mentary or middle school years. apy normalize the classroom behavior of hyperac-
tive children? Archives of General Psychiatry, 41,
Summary 449454.
The focus of treatment outcome re- Abikoff, H., & Gittelman, R. (1985). The normalizing ef-
search on ADHD has been primarily limited fects of methylphenidate on the classroom behav-
ADHD in School Classrooms 91

ior of ADHD children. Joumzal of Abnormal Child laboratory and in the classroom. Journul of C%iZd
Psychology, 13, 33-44. Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 191-211.
Abikoff, H., & Klein, R. G. (1992). Attention-deficit hy- DuPaul, G. J. (1991). Attention-deficit hyperactivity
peractivity and conduct disorder: Comorbidity and disorder: Classroom intervention strategies. School
implications for treatment. Joumzal of Consulting Psychology International, 12, 85-94.
and Clinical Psychology, 60, 881-892.
DuPaul, G. J., Anastopoulos, A. D., Shelton, T. L,
Abrarnowitz, A. J. (1994). Classroom interventions for Guevremont, D. C., & Metevia, L. (1992). Multi-
disruptive behavior disorders. Psychiatric Clinics method assessment of attention-deficit hyperactiv-
of North America, 3, 343-360. ity disorder: The diagnostic utility of clinic-based
tests. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 21,
Abramowitz, A. J., Eckstrand, D., O’Leary, S. G., & Dul- 394402.
can, M. K. (1992). ADHD children’s responses to
stimulant medication and two intensities of a be- DuPaul, G. J., & Rapport, M. D. (1993). Does
havioral intervention. Behavioral Modification, methylphenidate normalize the classroom perfor-
16, 193-203. mance of children with attention deficit disorders?
Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Algozzine, B., & Ysseldyke, J. E. (1992). Strategies and Adolescent Psychiatry, 32, 190-198.
tactics for effective instruction. Longmont, CO: So-
pris West. DuPaul, G. J., & Stoner, G. (1994). ADHD in the
schools: Assessment and intervention strategies.
Atkins, M., Pelham, W., & Licht, M. (1985). A compari- New York: Guilford.
son of objective classroom measures and teacher
ratings of attention deficit disorder. Journal of Ab- Elia, J., Borcherding, B. G., Rapoport, J. L., & Keysor,
normal Child Psychology, 13, 155-167. C. S. (1991). Methylphenidate and dextroampheta-
mine treatments of hyperactivity: Are there true
Barkley, R. A. (1989). Attention deficit hyperactivity nonresponders ? Psychiatry Research, 36(2),
disorder. In E. J. Mash & R. A. Barkley (Eds.), 141-155.
Treatment of childhood disorders (pp. 39-72). New
York: Guilford. Elliott, S. N. (1988). Acceptability of behavioral treat-
ments: Review of variables that influence treatment
Barkley, R. A. (1990). Attention deficit hyperactivity selection. Professional Psychology: Research and
disorder: A han,dbook for diagnosis and treat- Practice, 19, 68-80.
ment. New York: Guilford.
Elliott, S. N., Witt, J. C., Galvin, G., & Peterson, R.
Bell, R., & Harper, H. (1977). Child effects on adulti. (1984). Acceptability of positive and reductive in-
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. terventions: Factors that influence teachers’ deci-
Brooks, R. B. (1984). Success and failure in middle sions. Journal of School Psychology, 22, 353-360.
childhood: An interactionist perspective. In M. Fantuzzo, J., & Atkins, M. (1992). Applied behavior
Levine & I? Satz (Eds.), Middle childhood: Develop- analysis for educators: Teacher centered and class-
ment and dysfunction (pp. 87-128). Baltimore: room based. Journal of Applied Behavior Analy-
University Park Press. sis, 25, 3742.
Cannon, G. S., Idol, L., & West, J. E (1992). Educating Gittelman, R., Abikoff, H., Pollack, E., Klein, D. E, Katz,
students with mild handicaps in general class- S., & Mattes, J. (1980). A controlled trial of behavior
rooms: Essential teaching practices for general and modification and methylphenidate in hyperactive
special educa.tors. Journal of Learning Disabili- children. In C. K. Whalen & B. Henker (Eds.), Hy-
ties, 25, 300-3 17. per-active children: The social ecology of identifi-
cation and treatment (pp. 221-243). New York:
Carlson, C. L., & Lahey, B. B. (1988). Conduct and at- Academic Press.
tention-deficit disorders. In J. C. Witt, S. N. Elliott,
& F. Gresham (Eds.), The handbook of beha,vior Good, T. L., & Brophy, J. E. (1991). Looking in class-
therapy in education (pp. 653-678). New York: rooms (5th ed.). New York: Harper Collins.
Plenum.
Greene, R. W., & Barkley, R. A. (in press-a). Attention
Conners, C. K., & Wells, K. C. (1986). Hyperkinetic deficit hyperactivity disorder: Diagnostic, develop-
children: A neuropsychosociat approach. Beverly mental, and conceptual issues. In M. Breen & C.
Hills, CA: Sage. Fiedler (Eds.), Behavioral approach to the assess-
ment of emotionally disturbed youth: A handbook
Costello, E. J., Loeber, R., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. for school-based practitioners. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
(1991). Pervasive and situational hyperactivity -
confounding effect of informant: A research note. Greene, R. W., & Barkley, R. A., (in press-b). Clinic-
Journajl of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, based assessment of attention deficit hyperactivity
367-376. disorder. Journal of Psychoeducational Assess-
ment.
Douglas, V. I., Barr, R. G., O’Neill, M. E., & B&on, B. G.
(1986). Short-term effects of methylphenidate on Henker, B., & Whalen, C. K. (1980). The changing faces
the cognitive learning and academic performance of hyperactivity: Retrospect and prospect. In C. K.
of children with attention deficit disorders in the Whalen & B. Henker (Eds.), Hyperactive children
92 School Psychology Review, 1995, Vol. 24, No. 1

social ecology of identification and treatment (Eds.), Handbook of psychological assessment


321-364). New York: Academic Press. (2nd ed.; pp. 403422). Ehnsford, NJ: Pergamon.
Hersh, R. H., & Walker, H. M. (1983). Great expecta- Papero, D. V (1983). Family systems theory and ther-
tions: Making schools effective for aII students. Pol- apy. In B. Wohnan & G. Stricker (Eds.), Handbook
icy Studies Review, 2, 147-188. offamily and marital therapy (pp. 137-158). New
York: Plenum.
Hoza, B., Pelham, W. E., Sams, S. E., & Carlson, C.
(1992). An examination of the “dosage” effects of Papp, I? (1983). l%eprocess of change. New York: Guil-
both behavior therapy and methylphenidate on the ford.
classroom performance of two ADHD children. Be-
huvior Modification, 16, 164-192. PeIham, W. E. (1993). Pharmacotherapy for children
with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. School
Kauffman, J. M., Lloyd, J. W., & McGee, K. A. (1989). Psychology Review, 22, 199-227.
Adaptive and maladaptive behavior: Teachers’ atti-
tudes and their technical assistance needs. Journal PeIham, W. E., Bender, M. E., CaddelI, J., Both, S., &
of Special Education, 23, 185-200. Moorer, S. (1985). The dose-response effects of
methylphenidate on classroom academic and social
Kauffman, J. M., & Wong, K. L. H. (1991). Effective behavior in children with attention deficit disorder.
teachers of students with behaviorzil disorders: Are Archives of General Psychiatry, 42,948-952.
generic teaching skiIls enough? Behavioral Disor-
ders, 16,225-237. PeIharn, W. E., SchnedIer, R. W., Bologna, N. C., & Con-
teras, J. A. (1980). Behavioral and stimulant treat-
Kendziora, K. T., & O’Leary, S. G. (1992). Dysfunctional ment of hyperactive children: A therapy study with
parenting as a focus for prevention and treatment methylphenidate probes in a within-subjects de-
of child behavior problems. In T. H. OIIendick & R. sign. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 13,
J. Prinz (Eds.), Advances in child clinical psychol- 221-236.
ogy (Vol. 15; pp. 175-296). New York: Plenum.
Pfiffner, L. J., & Barkley, R. A (1990). Educational
Kerr, M. M., & Zigmond, N. (1986). What do high school placement and classroom management. In R. A.
teachers want? A study of expectations and stan- BarkIey (Ed.), Attention deficit hyperactivity dis-
dards. Education and neatment of Children, 9, order: A handbook for diagnosis and treatment
239-249. (pp. 498-539). New York: Guilford.
Landrum, T. J. (1992). Teachers vs. victims: An interac- PfVfner, L. J., & O’Leary, S. G. (1993). School-based
tional analysis of the teacher’s role in educating psychological treatments. In J. L. Matson (Ed.),
atypical learners. Behavioral Disorders, 17, Handbook of hyperactivity in children (pp.
135-144. 284-255). Boston: A&n and Bacon.
Lanivee, B. (1985). Effective teaching for successful Power, T. J., Hess, L. E., & Bennett, D. S. (August,
mainstreaming. New York: Longxnan 1993). l%e accountability of interventions for
Lloyd, J. W., Kauffman, J. M., & Kupersmidt, J. B. ADHD among elementary and middle school
(1990). Integration of students with behavior disor- teachers. Presented at the annuaI convention of the
ders in regular education environments. In K. D. American Psychological Association, Toronto,
Gadow (Ed.), Advances in learning and behav- Canada.
ioral disabilities (Vol. 6; pp. 225-264). Greenwich, Rapport, M. D. (1992). Treating children with attention-
CT: JAI Press. deficit hyperactivity disorder. Behavior Modifica-
Lloyd, J. W., Kauffman, J. M., Landrum, T. J., & Roe, D. tion, 16, 15&163.
L. (1991). Why do teachers refer pupils for special Rapport, M. D., Stoner, G., & Jones, J. T. (1986). Com-
education? An analysis of referral records. Excep- paring classroom and clinic measures of attention
tionali ty, 2, 115-126. deficit disorder: Differential, idiosyncratic, and
Mash, E. J., & Terdal, L. G. (1988). Behaviora assess- dose-response effects of methylphenidate. Journal
ment of child and family disturbance. In E. J. Mash of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54,
& L. G. TerdaI (Eds.), Behavioral assessment of 334-341.
childhood disorders (2nd ed.; pp. 3-65). New York: Reimers, T. M., Wacker, D. I?, & Koeppl, G. (1987). Ac-
GuiIford. ceptability of behavioral treatments: A review of
McCarney, S. B. (1989). Attention deficit disorders in- the literature. School Psychology Review, 16,
teruention manual. Columbia, MO: Hawthorne Ed- 212-227.
ucational Services.
Schaughency, E. A., & Rothlind, J. (1991). Assessment
McMahon, R. J., & We&, K. C. (1989) Conduct disor- and classification of attention-deficit hyperactive
ders. In E. J. Mash & R. A. BarkIey (Eds.), neat- disorders. School Psychology Review, 20, 187-202.
ment of childhood disorders (pp. 73-134). New
York: GuiIford. Seifer, R. (unpublished manuscript). Temperament fit,
attachment, and depressed mothers. Department of
OlIendick, T. H., & Greene, R. (1990). Behavioral as- Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Brow University
sessment of children. In G. Goldstein & M. Hersen School of Medicine.
ADHD in School Classrooms

Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A (1988). Attention Whalen, C. K, Henker, B., Collins, B. E., Finck, D., &
deficit disorder: Current perspectives. In J. E. Ka- Dotemoto, S. (1979). A social ecology of hyperac-
vanagh & T. J. Truss, Jr. (Eds.), Learning disabili- tive boys: Medication effects in systematically
ties:-k-oceedings of the ~ati&al Confience (pp. structured classroom environments. Journal ofAp-
369-567). Pa&on, MD: York Press. plied Behavioral Analysis, 12, 65-81.
Swanson, J. M., Cantwell, D., Lerner, M., McBurnett, K., Whalen, C. K., Henker, B., & Dotemoto, S. (1980).
& Hanna, G. (1991). Effects of stimulant medication Methylphenidate and hyperactivity: Effects on
on learning in children with ADHD. Journal of teacher behaviors. Science, 208, 12804282.
Learning lkabilities, 24, 219-230. Witt, J. C., Martens, B. K., & Elliott, S. N. (1984). Fac-
Thomas, A, & Chess, S. (1980). 7ke dynamics of psy- tors affecting teachers’ judgments of the accept-
chologicai development. New York: Brunner/Mazel. ability of behavioral interventions: Time involve-
ment, behavior problem severity, and type of inter-
Thomas, A, Chess, S., & Birch, H. G. (1968). Ternperu- vention. Behavior Therapy, 15,20&209.
rnmt a.nd behavior disorders in children. New
York: University Press. Wolraich, M. L., Lindgren, S., Stromquist, A, Milich, R.,
Davis, C., & Watson, D. (1990). Stimulant medica-
Walker, H. M., & Rankin, R. (1983). Assessing the be- tion use by primary care physicians in the treat-
havioral expectations and demands of less restric- ment of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Pe-
tive settings. School Psychology Review, 12, diatrics, 86, 95401.
274-284.
Wong, K. L. H., Kauffman, J. M., & Lloyd, J. W. (1991).
Whalen, C. K. (1989). Attention deficit and hyperactiv- Choices for integration: Selecting teachers for
ity disorders. In T. H. Ollendick & M. Hersen (Eds.), mainstreamed students with emotional or behav-
Handbook of child psychopathology (2nd ed.; pp. ioral disorders. Intermention in School and Clinic,
131-169). New York: Plenum. 27, 108415.
Whalen, C. K., & Henker, B. (1980). The social ecology Zentall, S. S. (1985). Stimulus-control factors in search
of psychostimulant treatment: A model for concep- performance of hyperactive children. Journal of
tual and empirical analysis. In C. K. Whalen & B. Learning Disabilities, 18, 480485.
Henker (Eds.), Hyperactive children: 17ze social
ecology of identification and treatment (pp. S-51). Zentall, S. S., & Meyer, M. J. (1987). Self-regulation of
New York: Academic Press. stimulation for ADD-H children during reading and
vigilance task performance. Journal of Abnormal
Child Psychology, 15,519-536.

Ross W. Greene, PhD, is Director of Behavioral Psychology and Clinical As-


sistant in Psychology at the Pediatric Psychopharmacology Unit at Massa-
chusetts General Hospital and Instructor in Psychology at Harvard Medical
School. His research interests include the influence of teacher characteris-
tics on student behavior, the development of instruments for measuring
teacher characteristics, the effectiveness and acceptability of behavioral
interventions for classroom teachers, and the development and effective-
ness of programs for training teachers in strategies for improving the be-
havior and social functioning of students with ADHD.

You might also like