You are on page 1of 9

EUR MED PHYS 2006;42:59-67

Attention rehabilitation following stroke

A
and traumatic brain injury
A review

C
T ® DI
J. A. MICHEL, C. A. MATEER

H E
Attentional capacities, which are frequently impaired Department of Psychology
IG M
following brain injury, have also been found to be University of Victoria, Victoria, Canada
amenable to rehabilitation. This review discusses vari-
ous approaches to attention rehabilitation in adult clients
following stroke and traumatic brain injury. Attention
process training has been accepted by many as a prac-
R A

tice standard in postacute clients, however, its ability


to generalize to new situations and to functional capac-
ities is unclear. There is evidence for the use of psy- an effective utilization of higher functions may take
Y V

chostimulant medication, which may be most helpful place. Without attending to information and being
when prescribed in combination with attention training. able to hold information in mind, one is unlikely to be
Biofeedback is a new avenue for intervention and is able to remember or to use that information to help
P R

beginning to show some promising results. Rather than solve problems and guide appropriate behavior.
train underlying processes, another approach which
shows promising results in a few small studies is train-
Following brain injury, basic attentional processes
O E

ing clients on specific functional skills, such as driving (e.g. focused attention) recover in the great majority
or vocational duties. Finally, modifications to the envi- of patients, while problems with higher-order atten-
ronment, implementation of strategies, provision of tional processes may persist.3 These continuing prob-
C IN

emotional support, and introduction of external sup- lems may include: orienting to novel stimuli, vigi-
ports/aids are important parts of a rehabilitation pro- lance, speed of processing, shifting set, divided atten-
gram, especially as the client returns to their home envi- tion and working memory (which can be considered
ronment. to be an impairment of attentional control).
M

Key words: Attention - Rehabilitation - Stroke - Truma - The goal of this paper is to summarize the
Brain injureis. approaches to and review the evidence for remedia-
tion of attention deficits in individuals with stroke
and traumatic brain injury (TBI). Ipsilateral neglect
T he most common sequelae following brain injury
in both children and adults are deficits in attention,
concentration, memory and executive function.1 The
will not be included due to the specific assessment and
treatment considerations for the disorder. Three broad
categories for remediation will be included: 1) direct
idea that deficits in attention impeded the recovery of
remediation of attention processes (including training
other cognitive and functional abilities was first pro- attentional processes, medication and biofeedback);
posed and investigated by Ben-Yishay et al.2 This the- 2) specific skills training (intensive training on a spe-
ory proposes that intact attention is required so that cific functional task); 3) modification of the environ-
ment, self-management, and supports. While for the
Address reprint requests to: C. Mateer, University of Victoria purposes of this review the evidence for each of these
Department of Psychology PO Box 3050 Victoria, BC V8W 3P5
Canada. E-mail: cmateer@uvic.ca areas will be examined separately, in practice, most

Vol. 42 - No. 1 EUROPA MEDICOPHYSICA 59


MICHEL ATTENTION REHABILITATION FOLLOWING STROKE AND TRAUMATIC BRAIN INJURY

clinicians combine interventions, for example com- question, their study involved 38 patients with focal
bining direct attention training with external aids, lesions of vascular origin who were trained in one of
modifications to the environment, and training in cog- the aspects of attention found to be deficient in that
nitive and emotional self-regulation. particular participant. Before and after fourten 1-h
training periods, the change in performance on the
specifically trained attention function was compared

A
Direct remediation of attention deficits
to the change in performance for all other (untrained)
Training attentional processes attention functions. They concluded that specific train-

C
ing effects followed training in specific aspects of
Beginning in the late 1980’s, there was an increasing attention. Interestingly, training at higher levels in
interest in providing direct training to rehabilitate atten- those with significant deficits did not improve atten-

T ® DI
tion functions following acquired brain injury.1, 4 tion, and, in some cases, resulted in a decline in atten-
Training attentional process usually involves a series tional functioning.
of repetitive drills or exercises which increase hierar- These findings highlight the importance of tailoring
chically in attentional demand as the client progress- the remediation approach to the individual’s particu-
es. The theoretical basis of this approach is that repeat-

H E
ed activation and stimulation of attentional systems
facilitates changes in cognitive capacity, and results in
lar pattern of deficits. Similarly, decisions regarding
when to increase or decrease the difficulty level, or
when to start or stop a therapy program, should be
IG M
improved attentional processes. The treatment activ- dictated by client performance. Therefore, a standard
ities are, at least at the start, typically not functional set of intervention activities is likely less useful than
activities (e.g. meal planning, vocational tasks), as one tailored to the specific areas of weakness demon-
functional activities involve multiple cognitive process- strated by a particular client. Activities should be hier-
R A

es. The activities resemble laboratory tasks, such as archically organized, such that as the client progress-
detecting targets with the presence of a distracter es, the same components of attention are stimulated
noise, engaging in more and more demanding work- at higher levels.1 For example, a sustained attention
Y V

ing memory tasks, working on tasks that involve shift- task may begin by requiring detection of particular tar-
ing of set, and engaging in more than one task at get numbers presented auditorily, and progress to
once (to practice divided attention). As improvements
P R

detection of ascending or descending sequences of


are made in these tasks, the focus of treatment shifts numbers, then to pairs of numbers that add to a par-
to practicing the skills in real life activities, and in ticular sum, and finally to holding in mind and
O E

more naturalistic settings. sequencing sets of auditorily presented numbers.


With training attentional processes, it is important More complex forms of attention (such as divided
to use a treatment model that is grounded in attention attention) should not be trained until more basic
C IN

theory.1 The model which drives the attention program processes are trained. For those who have milder
dictates the aspects of attention that are to be deficits, the APT-II program 1 provides a more chal-
addressed. For example, the Attention Process Training lenging experience, with higher level tasks that require
(APT) program 5 is based on the clinical model,1 which more executive aspects of attentional control (work-
M

divides attention into 5 components: focused attention, ing memory, divided attention, set shifting).
sustained attention, selective attention, alternating Another important aspect of training attentional
attention, and divided attention. These 5 components process is that each task should provide sufficient
provide a framework for organizing attention treatment repetition to stimulate improved attentional process-
activities. Other training approaches may be based ing. An investigation of the factors associated with a
on a different model, therefore clinicians should positive response to cognitive remediation in 48 adult
attempt to ascertain what type of attention a particu- psychiatric outpatients revealed that there was a
lar task addresses within the broader framework of threshold of treatment intensity below which there
attention theory. was no treatment effect.7 Repetition may be carried on
Sturm et al.6 suggest that specific deficits (e.g. sus- in therapy sessions, or with the cooperation of care-
tained attention) require specific training, and that givers outside of clinical hours. For example, a recent
training in more basic functions can improve both study by Boman et al.8 evaluated the effectiveness of
basic and more complex functions. To examine this cognitive rehabilitation in adults with mild to moder-

60 EUROPA MEDICOPHYSICA March 2006


ATTENTION REHABILITATION FOLLOWING STROKE AND TRAUMATIC BRAIN INJURY MICHEL

ate acquired brain injury in their own home or voca- severe brain injury.1 However, Park and Ingles 4 suggest
tional environment. Treatment involved APT, imple- that if the only evidence is that individuals improve
mentation of a generalization plan, and teaching com- on a similar task to the training task, what has occurred
pensatory strategies for self-selected cognitive prob- is the learning of a specific skill (e.g. mentally manip-
lems. It was a 9 h program conducted over 3 weeks. ulating numbers) rather than improving an aspect of
The study suggested some improvement at the level attention. A more rigorous approach is to look for gen-

A
of specific attentional impairments, but not for activ- eralization of skills to untrained tasks (including mea-
ities of daily living or general everyday functioning. sures of attention or other cognitive abilities). In addi-

C
However, this study suggests that, even when con- tion, the goal of any rehabilitation program is to pro-
ducted in the patient’s natural environment, with a duce changes in the person’s ability to function in their
strong focus on generalization, improvement may not everyday life. Functional changes have been the most

T ® DI
be seen in functional activities. This study was quite difficult to demonstrate experimentally.
brief, and, therefore, does not rule out the possibili- A Cochrane review 11 examined the evidence for the
ty of success of a more intensive program. It does, effectiveness of attention training following stroke.
however, highlight the importance of attention to gen- Only 2 studies, with a total of 56 participants, met

H E
eralization. Clinicians must plan for and measure gen-
eralization from attention process tasks to real-world
activities. Throughout treatment, clients should be
the stringent criteria required of a Cochrane review.14, 15
These studies both showed a benefit of training on
measures of alertness and sustained attention com-
IG M
encouraged to apply the trained skills to everyday pared to control treatments 14 or in a crossover
activities which involve multiple cognitive process- design.15 Only one study 14 examined measures of
es. For example, the APT program includes General- functional independence, and these showed no sig-
ization Sheets where clients can track their perfor- nificant improvement with attention training. This
R A

mance on everyday tasks which are specific to the review concluded that there appears to be some sup-
type of attention they are training. port for treating attentional deficits in stroke patients
In examining the literature on the effectiveness of to improve alertness and sustained attention. However,
Y V

attention training, factors relating both to the indi- as neither study was conducted with the assessor
vidual and to the nature of the training must be tak- blind to the intervention category, the authors sug-
P R

en into account. Individual factors include: 1) injury gested that the routine use of attentional training can-
severity; 2) time postinjury (e.g. acute vs subacute); 3) not be supported or refuted.
location and nature of injury (e.g. left vs right, focal vs A review by Park and Ingles 4 highlighted the impor-
O E

diffuse), and other patient characteristics, including tance of including control groups, and suggested that
education, motivation, age, and comorbidity. the findings from attention training studies in acquired
Important characteristics of the training include: 1) brain injury may be primarily due to practice effects
C IN

type of skills trained; 2) duration/intensity; 3) fre- rather than true gains acquired from training. They
quency; 4) setting. Given the myriad of individual found that while all measures of cognitive function sig-
and injury characteristics and variety of training nificantly improved from pre to post-test, none sig-
approaches, “When does it work best and for whom?” nificantly improved when control estimates were
M

may be a better question than “Does it work?”.9, 10 accounted for. Of those 12 studies which included a
There have been a number of recent reviews of the control condition, 6 reported no statistically significant
effectiveness of training attentional processes.4, 9-13 The improvement after training. In those 6 studies which
effectiveness of attention training may be evaluated at did report improvement, Park and Ingles suggest that
multiple levels including changes on: 1) the training the improvement seems to be more attributable to
task itself; 2) other psychometric measures of attention; the acquisition of specific skills rather than improve-
3) psychometric measures of other abilities (e.g. mem- ment in attention per se. Criticisms of this review
ory); 4) functional or everyday tasks; 5) neurophysi- include the fact that its inclusion criteria were overly
ological measures (e.g. electroencephalography, EEG, broad, for example including studies which included
or magnetic resonance imaging, MRI). participants with severe brain injury who would not
At the first level (changes in performance on the be expected to significantly benefit from training. In
training task), improvements have consistently been addition, the types of programs and what was mea-
shown across studies even in studies of patients with sured varied extensively between studies included in

Vol. 42 - No. 1 EUROPA MEDICOPHYSICA 61


MICHEL ATTENTION REHABILITATION FOLLOWING STROKE AND TRAUMATIC BRAIN INJURY

the review. occurs.


In another review, Cicerone et al. reported the find- A task force of the European Federation of
ings of a subcommittee of the American Congress of Neurological Societies also evaluated the effective-
Rehabilitation Medicine. They reviewed the evidence ness of cognitive rehabilitation.12 This report specifi-
for effectiveness of cognitive rehabilitation in people cally reviewed the evidence for acute (1 Class 1 and
with TBI and stroke. One article reviewed the evi- 2 Class 2 studies) versus the evidence for postacute (2

A
dence up to 1997,16 followed by an updated review of Class 1 and 2 Class 2 studies) attention training. They
the literature from 1998 through 2002.13 The original concluded that while evidence is not able to distin-

C
review selected 13 studies which examined the effec- guish the effects of attention training from sponta-
tiveness of remediation of attention deficits. Three neous recovery in the acute phase of recovery, there
met criteria for a Class 1 study (well designed, prospec- is sufficient Class 1 evidence to support attention

T ® DI
tive, randomized clinical trials). The others met crite- training in the postacute phase.
ria for less stringent Class 2 and Class 3 studies. This Given the evidence that specific aspects of attention
review supported the effectiveness of attention train- can be improved through training, the next question
ing during the postacute stage. The most recent review is: what are the neurophysiological correlates of these

H E
found 2 additional Class 1 studies, 1 on attention train-
ing 17 and 1 on strategy training,18 1 small Class 2
study, and 2 Class 3 studies. The authors concluded
changes? This question is beginning to be addressed
by researchers who examine changes in brain func-
tioning before and after attention training. For exam-
IG M
that these recent studies support the earlier conclusions ple, Sturm 21 conducted functional magnetic reso-
regarding the effectiveness of attention training. nance imaging (fMRI) before and after alertness train-
Attention training was recommended as a practice ing in 8 postacute subjects with right hemisphere vas-
standard for individuals in the postacute stage of TBI cular lesions, in order to test the hypothesis that
R A

or stroke. improvement on an attention task is related to reor-


Sohlberg et al. provided a review of the evidence ganization of attentional processes in the brain. Four
and practice guidelines for the implementation of subjects were given computerized attention training
Y V

direct attention training in individuals with TBI.9 As an using a car-driving paradigm. Four control subjects
alternative to answering the binary question, does it were trained in verbal and topographical memory.
P R

work or does it not work, a template of 5 key ques- Fourteen 45-min sessions were conducted with pre
tions are proposed which can be used to evaluate and post fMRI and PET scans. Three of the 4 in the
the literature. These questions are as follows: 1) who treatment group improved and showed the expected
O E

are the participants who received the intervention? change of activation pattern in the right fronto-parietal
2) What comprises the attention training? 3) What are area. The one who did not improve showed an
the outcomes of the intervention? 4) Are there method- increase of activation only in the left hemisphere. In
C IN

ological concerns? Are there other explanations for contrast, only 1 of the 4 in the memory group
given outcomes, and may results be either exaggerated improved, while another participant in the memory
or hidden? 5) Are there clinically applicable trends group showed a change in the activation pattern sim-
across different attention remediation studies? The ilar to the treatment group. This suggests that an
M

authors use these questions to evaluate the studies increase in attention can only occur if at least part of
identified by Cicerone et al.16 and Park and Ingles,4 as the right hemisphere network (and especially the
well as 3 well designed studies published after those frontal region), is reactivated.
reviews.17, 19, 20 In turn, they developed a set of clini-
cal recommendations/practice guidelines which follow Medication
the above questions: 1) mildly injured, postacute
clients with intact vigilance are best suited for atten- Methylphenidate (MP) and other psychostimulant
tion training; 2) attention training should include com- medication have been successfully used to treat atten-
plex attention tasks, be conducted in conjunction with tion deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). A recent
metacognitive training, and should be individualized; meta-analysis of the last 40 years of research con-
3) the outcomes which can be expected are task spe- cluded that MP consistently improves the core clini-
cific, at the impairment level. It is unknown whether cal features of ADHD.22
generalization to untrained or functional level tasks Whyte et al.23 conducted a detailed review and cri-

62 EUROPA MEDICOPHYSICA March 2006


ATTENTION REHABILITATION FOLLOWING STROKE AND TRAUMATIC BRAIN INJURY MICHEL

tique of research into the use of psychostimulant med- hemispherectomy) received 3 to 5 thirty-min biofeed-
ication in TBI. They found 10 controlled studies of the back sessions per week. Successful alterations of EEG
effects of MP in brain injury (2 of these were in the parameters were found in patients with mild to severe
pediatric population). Of these 10, 6 reported some brain damage. Symptom improvement (based on
measure of attentional function. However, large dif- agreement between client, caregiver and physician
ferences in sample characteristics, experimental rigor, report) was also found, but did not correlate with

A
statistical power, dependent measures and other fac- EEG changes.28
tors made the studies difficult to compare. Overall, Stathopoulou and Lubar 29 suggested that atten-

C
they concluded that the evidence indicates that psy- tional processes training may work in the same way
chostimulant medication does not have a very strong as biofeedback, by training individuals to modify their
effect on aspects of attention such as sustained atten- brain waves. They examined 5 subjects with TBI

T ® DI
tion or resistance to distraction. In contrast, medica- before and after 22 sessions of computerized APT.
tion does seem to have a positive effect on process- While results showed an improvement in attention
ing speed. A recent double-blind, placebo-controlled on psychometric measures in all participants, the
study of 34 adults in the postacute stage of moderate expected EEG changes were not consistently found.

H E
to severe TBI supported this conclusion.24 Three vari-
ables showed statistically significant improvement in
both the pilot and the replication samples: speed of
Only a reduction in alpha frequency was consistent
with the hypothesis in most of the participants. Follow-
up with a larger sample size and a nontreated control
IG M
information processing, attentiveness during individ- group is necessary.
ual work tasks and caregiver ratings of attention. Effect Another study 30 administered 6 months of stan-
sizes were small to medium. Whyte et al.24 have sug- dardized, computerized cognitive remediation to 21
gested that medication may provide incremental ben- adults with severe closed head injury. Subjects were
R A

efit when combined with training attentional process- compared to 22 matched head-injured controls who did
es, as medication affects speed of processing, an area not receive training. While the groups showed some sig-
typically unaffected by attentional training. nificant differences on neurophysiological measures
Y V

following treatment, the authors concluded that the


pattern of differences was consistent with increased
Biofeedback
motivation, attentional effort and improved stimulus
P R

EEG biofeedback is an operant conditioning pro- processing, rather that improved attentional selectivity.
cedure where, through feedback, an individual is Finally, Penkman and Mateer 31 examined both
O E

trained to modify the amplitude, frequency or coher- behavioral and event-related potential (ERP) corre-
ence of his or her brain waves. A number of studies lates of brain activity using the P300 oddball para-
have supported the notion that people with atten- digm. Multiple baseline measures of attentional behav-
C IN

tional problems (e.g. ADHD) have increased power in ior and brain activity were taken before 3 weeks of
delta, theta and alpha frequency bands (the bands attention training, administered in a group format,
involved in drowsy conditions), and decreased pow- and again after training. Although pretreatment base-
er in the beta frequency band (involved in more active line measures were stable, post-treatment, there were
M

cognitive processes) relative to arousal. Biofeedback both improvements on attention measures and
treatment in ADHD typically involves theta-beta train- changes in the pattern of N100 and P200 waveforms
ing, where individuals are trained to increase beta associated with the P300 tasks. The changes differed
and decrease theta waves (see a review of EEG use in across participants, however, suggesting that under-
the assessment and treatment of ADHD 25). A similar lying brain based correlates of attentional problems dif-
beta-increasing biofeedback procedure has been fered across subjects or that training effects had dif-
found to be effective in enhancing attention in normal ferent neural substrates.
subjects.26
A number of investigators have found EEG differ- Specific skills training
ences in patients with acquired brain injuries,27 and
EEG biofeedback has been used as a treatment in Training attentional processes has been criticized for
such individuals. For example, in one study, a group its lack of focus on functional changes,4 and specific
of 27 individuals with brain injury (stroke, TBI or skills training has been proposed as an alternative.

Vol. 42 - No. 1 EUROPA MEDICOPHYSICA 63


MICHEL ATTENTION REHABILITATION FOLLOWING STROKE AND TRAUMATIC BRAIN INJURY

Specific skills training attempts to train (or retrain) skills have begun to develop virtual environments that mim-
of functional significance, such as driving, or specific ic household, community, and office settings in which
vocational tasks. The theoretical rationale behind spe- attentional, memory and planning tasks can be pre-
cific skills training is that it is possible for brain-damaged sented. Practice in these virtual contexts has been
individuals to develop skills that rely on preserved shown to generalize in a number of studies to real
brain areas. When a specific skill is learned in this way, world environments as well.

A
the cognitive processes used in the tasks may be dif-
ferent from the processes a non brain-damaged indi-

C
vidual would use.32 This contrasts with the theory Strategies and supports
behind training attentional processes, in which basic
attentional skills are retrained such that the individual Self management strategies, environmental sup-

T ® DI
uses the same processes as a non brain-damaged indi- ports, and environmental modifications may be imple-
vidual performing the same task. In specific skills train- mented in conjunction with, or as an alternative to,
ing, there is little or no expectation of generalization to training of attentional processes or functional skills.
untrained abilities, whereas in attentional processes They may be most useful later in the recovery phase,

H E
training, the theory is that it is possible to train under-
lying attentional processes, which will in turn improve
higher, more complex cognitive functions.
when an individual is reintegrating into home and
work environments. At these times, specific strate-
gies or supports may be the most practical form of mit-
IG M
In their meta-analysis of attention rehabilitation, igating problems in specific situations. Strategies and
Park and Ingles 4 found 4 studies which focused on supports can be divided into 2 broad categories: 1)
performing a specific functional skill, or a closely external support and modifications (which involve
related skill, that required attention.33-36 The review modification of the environment in some way); 2)
R A

concluded that all specific areas assessed (activities of self-management strategies. Careful attention to assess-
daily living, driving and attention behavior) increased ment of a client’s strengths and weaknesses will help
from pre to post-test. Unlike APT, which yielded non the clinician to choose the most effective strategies. To
Y V

significant results when the control conditions were increase the chances of success, clients should be
accounted for, driving and attention behavior remained involved as much as possible in the selection and
P R

significant even when the control condition was development of strategies and supports.1
accounted for. It should be noted that the sample
sizes for specific skills studies were extremely small. Supports and modifications
O E

For example, only one subject was included in each


of the 2 categories with pre/post control. However, the Implementation of external modifications, supports
small studies are encouraging, and worthy of increased and strategies to minimize the effects of attention
C IN

attention. deficits should be considered as part of any rehabili-


A major challenge for rehabilitation professionals is tation program. For example, if distracting environ-
how to actually work with individuals in functional set- ments are a problem, the client may be encouraged to
tings. Working in people’s homes, communities, avoid those environments whenever possible.
M

schools, and places of employment might be very Modifications could also include strategies to reduce
valuable, but is often expensive and/or difficult to or eliminate distractions during tasks which require
implement within the limitations of medical rehabil- attention (e.g. facing away from visual distractions,
itation programs. One promising approach in other using earplugs, shopping in less crowded environ-
areas of psychological treatment has been the use of ments). In addition, specific modifications may be
virtual environments. These three-dimensional, immer- made to the client’s environment to reduce attentional
sive environments are achieved through the use of demands. Organizational systems (e.g. filing systems,
specialized helmets or goggles. The technology, once bill paying systems, labeling of cupboards) are one
rather exotic and very expensive, has reduced in price such approach. Another approach is reducing visual
and is much more flexible and available. It has been distractions and clutter.1
used in the treatment of a variety of psychological Some have found the use of external devices help-
disorders (e.g., phobias) and has more recently begun ful. A recent review describes the state of the art in
to be used in rehabilitative contexts. Rizzo et al.37 assistive technology for cognition (ATC) for those with

64 EUROPA MEDICOPHYSICA March 2006


ATTENTION REHABILITATION FOLLOWING STROKE AND TRAUMATIC BRAIN INJURY MICHEL

deficits in a diverse range of cognitive skills.38 ATCs This program was designed to provide cognitive strate-
range from alarms to remind a client to take their med- gies to compensate for deficits in speed of processing
ication to robotic technology utilizing artificial neural during daily tasks such as holding a conversation or
networks to assist with everyday activities. This review preparing a meal. Strategies involved included enhanc-
discusses ATCs for problems with initiation, attention, ing awareness, planning and organization, rehears-
memory, executive function, and sensory processing ing of task requirements or modifying the environ-

A
impairments as well as for social and behavioral prob- ment, all with the goal of preventing or managing
lems. Of note is the high level of customization (and time pressure. The study included 22 individuals in the

C
recustomization as the client’s needs change) required chronic or subacute stage following a severe to very
in order for an ATC device to be of practical use. severe closed head injury. Subjects were randomized
Indeed, today there are a number of assistive devices into 2 conditions: 12 subjects participated in TPM

T ® DI
designed specifically for individuals with cognitive training while 10 participated in concentration train-
deficits. Some programs provide a high level of guid- ing (a control condition). While both groups showed
ance and structure, such as walking a client through the some improvement, TPM produced larger gains and
steps required to make dinner. In addition, a new gen- also appeared to generalize to other measures of

H E
eration of devices provides context-sensitive cues
based on sensors placed in the client’s environment
(e.g. a cue to wash hands when in the bathroom if
speed and memory function.
Another approach to research into the effective-
ness of strategy use is combining it with direct atten-
IG M
the water has not yet been turned on). The clinician tion training. Cicerone 19 took this approach, com-
should note that anyone, especially someone with bining working memory training with encouraging
cognitive deficits, needs time to effectively utilize sup- participants to consciously use strategies to allocate
ports and modifications. Therefore, sufficient time attention and manage the rate of information.
R A

should be set aside for teaching their use. Strategies included verbal mediation, self-pacing strate-
gies, sharing attentional resources during multiple
tasks, self-monitoring of mental effort, and manage-
Y V

Self-management strategies
ment of secondary emotional reactions during task
Sohlberg and Mateer 1 describe 3 types of self-man- performance. Compared to untreated controls, those
agement strategies aimed at helping clients deliber-
P R

who participated in treatment were more likely to


ately focus their attention. The first is orienting proce- exhibit clinically significant improvement on mea-
dures, which encourage clients to consciously monitor sures of attention and reduction of self-reported atten-
O E

their activities in order to avoid lapses in attention. tional difficulties in their daily functioning. However,
This may be a general orienting procedure (e.g. to in this study, strategy use was confounded with atten-
focus them on the task at hand), or may be designed tion training. Further research is required which would
C IN

for a specific task (e.g. to avoid forgetting their desti- examine the incremental benefit of adding strategy
nation while they are driving). Second, pacing strate- training to attention training.
gies may be helpful for clients who experience fatigue It is well acknowledged that awareness of and
or difficulty maintaining concentration over an extend- insight into the nature of one’s own difficulties may be
M

ed period. These may involve setting realistic expec- compromised after brain injury. In a recent study by
tations, building in breaks, or self-monitoring of Sawchyn et al.39 individuals with moderate to severe
fatigue/attention levels. Finally, the “key ideas log” brain injury tended to underappreciate cognitive
involves teaching people to quickly write or tape record impairments, whereas individuals with mild brain
questions or ideas which they want to address later, so injury often appeared to report much great difficulties
that they may continue with the task at hand. As with in cognition than were observed by others. Working
supports and modifications, it is important to allocate on attentional skills can often assist the injured indi-
sufficient time to effectively establish a strategy. vidual in gaining some insight into the nature of their
Typically, the research literature in this area is cognitive difficulties and in managing their emotion-
restricted to case reports which describe the proce- al response to cognitive challenges (often frustration
dures and outcomes for an individual client. However, or hopelessness). Mateer 1, 40 emphasizes the impor-
one approach which has been studied in a small tance of integrating cognitive interventions with inter-
group format is time pressure management (TPM).18 ventions designed to manage such emotional respons-

Vol. 42 - No. 1 EUROPA MEDICOPHYSICA 65


MICHEL ATTENTION REHABILITATION FOLLOWING STROKE AND TRAUMATIC BRAIN INJURY

es and to correct negative beliefs about cognitive abil- 2. Ben-Yishay Y, Piasetsky EB, Rattock J. A systematic method for
ameliorating disorders in basic attention. In: Meier MJ, Benton
ity that lead to catastrophic reactions and avoidance AL, Diller L editors. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation. New York:
of cognitive challenges. In particular, blending prin- Churchill Livingstone; 1987.p.165-81.
ciples of cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) with cog- 3. Mateer CA, Sohlberg MM, Crinean JJ. Focus on clinical research:
perceptions of memory function in individuals with closed-head
nitive interventions has been found to be beneficial. injury. Head Trauma Rehabil 1987;2:74-84.
This approach builds self-confidence in cognitive abil- 4. Park NW, Ingles JL. Effectiveness of attention rehabilitation after

A
ities, and assists the individual in feeling that their an acquired brain injury: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychology
2001;15:199-210.
own actions (metacognitive strategies, external aids, 5. Sohlberg MM, Mateer CA. Attention Process Training I and II.

C
etc.) can influence how successful they are in man- Wake Forest, NC: Lash and Associates Publishing; 2001.
6. Sturm W, Wilmes K, Orgass B. Do specific attention deficits need
aging everyday cognitive demands. specific training? Neuropsychol Rehabil 1997;7:81-103.
7. Choi J, Medalia A. Factors associated with a positive response to

T ® DI
cognitive remediation in a community psychiatric sample. Psychiatr
Serv 2005;56:602-4.
Conclusions 8. Boman IL, Lindstedt M, Hemmingsson H. Cognitive training in
home environment. Brain Inj 2004;18:985-95.
9. Sohlberg MM, Avery J, Kennedy M, Ylvisaker M, Coelho C, Turkstra
Twenty-five years of research has demonstrated L et al. Practice guidelines for direct attention training. J Med

H E
that while attentional capacities are sensitive to brain
injury, they are also amenable to intervention. One
method of rehabilitation is direct training of basic
Speech Lang Pathol 2003;11:19-39.
10. Sohlberg MM. Can disabilities resulting from attentional impair-
ments be treated effectively? In: Halligan PW, Wade DT editors.
IG M
Effectiveness of rehabilitation for cognitive deficits.Oxford: Oxford
attentional processes. Numerous studies have sup- University Press; 2005. p.91-102.
11. Lincoln NB, Majid MJ, Weyman N. Cognitive rehabilitation for
ported the ability of attentional process training to attention deficits following stroke. Cochrane Database Syst Rev
improve attentional functioning in individuals in the 2000;(4):CD002842.
postacute stage. However, its ability to generalize to 12. Cappa SF, Benke T, Clarke S, Rossi B, Stemmer B, van Heugten CM.
EFNS Guidelines on cognitive rehabilitation: report of an EFNS Task
R A

untrained abilities or functional capacity has yet to Force. Eur J Neurol 2003;10:11-23.
be conclusively demonstrated. Psychostimulant med- 13. Cicerone KD, Dahlberg C, Malec JF, Langenbahn DM, Felicetti T,
Kneipp S et al. Evidence-based cognitive rehabilitation: updated
ication appears to be another avenue for rehabilitation,
Y V

review of the literature from 1998 through 2002. Arch Phys Med
primarily impacting processing speed, and, therefore, Rehabil 2005;86:1681-91.
may be particularly useful when used in combina- 14. Schottke H. [Rehabilitation of attention deficits after stroke -
Effectivity of a neuropsychological training program for attention
P R

tion with training attentional processes. While a num- deficits]. Verhaltenstherapie 1997;7:21-3. German.
ber of studies have demonstrated neurophysiologi- 15. Sturm W, Willmes K. Efficacy of a reaction training on various
cal changes (e.g. fMRI, EEG, ERP) changes with atten- attentional and cognitive functions in stroke patients. Neuropsychol
Rehabil 1991;1:259-80.
O E

tion training, evidence is just beginning to accumulate 16. Cicerone KD, Dahlberg C, Kalmar K, Langenbahn DM, Malec JF,
for the effectiveness of biofeedback as a treatment. Bergquist TF et al. Evidence-based cognitive rehabilitation: rec-
ommendations for clinical practice. Arch Phys Med Rehabil
There are a few studies which suggest that training
C IN

2000;81:1596-615.
of specific skills, such as driving, can be a successful 17. Sohlberg MM, McLaughlin KA, Pavese A, Heidrich A, Posner MI.
method for retraining those with attention deficits. Evaluation of attention process training and brain injury education
in persons with acquired brain injury. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol
The availability of specialized virtual reality programs 2000;22:656-76.
provides an exciting new avenue for such training. 18. Fasotti L, Kovacs F, Eling PA, Brouwer WH. Time pressure man-
M

Finally, modifications to the environment, the imple- agement as a compensatory strategy training after closed head
injury. Neuropsychol Rehabil 2000;10:47-65.
mentation of strategies, emotional support, and exter- 19. Cicerone KD. Remediation of ‘working attention’ in mild trau-
nal supports are important parts of a rehabilitation matic brain injury. Brain Inj 2002;16:185-95.
20. Park NW, Proulx G, Towers W. Evaluation of the Attention Process
program, especially as the client returns to their home Training Programme. Neuropsychol Rehabil 1999;9:135-54.
environment. Whatever approach is taken, it is vital 21. Sturm W, Longoni F, Weis S. Functional reorganisation in patients
that it be guided by the individual’s strengths and with right hemisphere stroke after training of alertness: a longitu-
dinal PET and fMRI study in eight cases. Neuropsychologia
weaknesses and be implemented in full cooperation 2004;42:434-50.
with the client. 22. Conners CK. Forty years of methylphenidate treatment in attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Atten Disord 2002;6 Suppl 1:S17-
30.
23. Whyte J, Vaccaro M, Grieb-Neff P. Psychostimulant use in the
References rehabilitation of individuals with traumatic brain injury. J Head
Trauma Rehabil 2002;17:284-99.
1. Sohlberg MM, Mateer CA. Cognitive Rehabilitation, an Integrative 24. Whyte J, Hart T, Vaccaro M, Grieb-Neff P, Risser A, Polansky M et
Neuropsychological Approach. New York: Guildford Press; 2001. al. Effects of methylphenidate on attention deficits after traumat-

66 EUROPA MEDICOPHYSICA March 2006


ATTENTION REHABILITATION FOLLOWING STROKE AND TRAUMATIC BRAIN INJURY MICHEL

ic brain injury: a multidimensional, randomized, controlled trial. cognitive skills performance to activities of daily living in stroke
Am J Phys Med Rehabil 2004;83:401-20. patients. Am J Occup Ther 1988;42:449-55.
25. Loo SK, Barkley RA. Clinical utility of EEG in attention deficit 34. Kewman DG, Seigerman C, Kinter H, Chu S, Henson D, Reeder C.
hyperactivity disorder. Appl Neuropsychol 2005;12:64-76. Simulation training of psychomotor skills: teaching the brain-
26. Egner T, Gruzelier JH. The temporal dynamics of electroen- injured to drive. Rehabil Psychol 1985;30:11-27.
cephalographic responses to alpha/theta neurofeedback training 35. Sivak M, Hill CS, Henson DL, Butler BP, Silber SM, Olson PL.
in healthy subjects. J Neurotherapy 2004;8:43-57. Improved driving performance following perceptual training in per-
27. Thatcher RW, Walker RA, Gerson I, Geisler FH. EEG discriminant sons with brain damage. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 1984;65:163-7.

A
analyses of mild head trauma. Electroencephalogr Clin Neuro- 36. Wilson C, Robertson IH. A home-based intervention for atten-
physiol 1989;73:94-106. tional slips following head injury: a single case study. Neuropsychol
28. Laibow RE, Stubblebine AN, Sandground H. EEG-NeuroBio- Rehabil 1992;2:193-205.

C
Feedback treatment of patients with brain injury: Part 2: Changes 37. Rizzo AA, Schultheis M, Kerns KA, Mateer CA. Analysis of assets
in EEG parameters versus rehabilitation. J Neurother 2001;5:45-68. for virtual reality applications in neuropsychology. Neuropsychol
29. Stathopoulou S, Lubar JF. EEG changes in traumatic brain injured Rehabil 2004;14:207-39.
patients after cognitive rehabilitation. J Neurotherapy 2004;8:21-51. 38. LoPresti EF, Mihailidis A, Kirsch N. Assistive technology for cog-

T ® DI
30. Baribeau J, Ethier M, Braun C. A neurophysiological assessment of nitive rehabilitation: State of the art. Neuropsychol Rehabil
selective attention before and after cognitive remediation in patients 2004;14:5-39.
with severe closed head injury. J Neurolog Rehabil 1989;3:71-92. 39. Sawchyn JM, Mateer CA, Suffield JB. Awareness, emotional adjust-
31. Penkman L, Mateer CA. The specificity of attention retraining in ment, and injury severity in postacute brain injury. J Head Trauma
traumatic brain injury. J Cognitive Rehabil 2004;22:13-26. Rehabil 2005;20:301-14.
32. Backman L, Dixon RA. Psychological compensation: a theoretical 40. Mateer CA, Sira CS, O’Connell ME. Putting Humpty Dumpty togeth-

33.

H E
framework. Psychol Bull 1992;112:259-83.
Carter LT, Oliveira DO, Duponte J, Lynch SV. The relationship of
er again, the importance of integrating cognitive and emotional
interventions. J Head Trauma Rehabil 2005;20:58-70.
IG M
R A
Y V
P R
O E
C IN
M

Vol. 42 - No. 1 EUROPA MEDICOPHYSICA 67

You might also like