You are on page 1of 11

Improvements in Spatial Ability as a Result

of Targeted Training and Computer-Aided


Design Software Use: Analyses of Object
Geometries and Rotation Types

RICHARD M. ONYANCHA Lord, 1990; Smith, 1964). Spatial ability has been shown to improve
Department of Mechanical Engineering during a CAD course or through training (Sorby and Baartmans,
Rose-Hulman Institute of Technology 2000; Sorby, 2005, Hsi. Linn, and Bell, 1997; Miller, 1992; Kinsey
et al., 2007). The targeted training can take different forms such as
MATTHEW DEROV semester long spatial skills course or short targeted training. Sorby
Department of Mechanical Engineering and Baartmans (2000) developed a course at Michigan Technolog-
University of New Hampshire ical University entitled “Introduction to Spatial Visualization.” Stu-
dents who scored poorly on standardized spatial ability tests were
BRAD L. KINSEY invited to enroll in the course; however, the course was open to any
Department of Mechanical Engineering interested student. The course includes topics such as isometric and
University of New Hampshire orthographic sketching, flat pattern development, and rotation of
objects. Data analyses showed that the spatial ability of the students
who enrolled in the course (i.e., the Experimental Group) was
ABSTRACT significantly higher than that of the students who opted not to take
the course (i.e., the Control Group). Also, the retention rates at
Spatial ability, which affects retention and achievement in engi- Michigan Tech increased from 70.0 percent to 76.8 percent for
neering and science disciplines, has been shown to improve male students and from 71.7 percent to 87.4 percent for female stu-
through training and simply from enrollment in a Computer- dents, for the Control (N  173, 120 men and 53 women) and
Aided Design (CAD) course. However, detailed analyses of the Experimental (N  169, 82 men and 87 women) groups respec-
specific object geometries and rotation types that cause difficulty tively (Sorby, 2005). Finally, the overall Grade Point Average
for students on standardized spatial ability tests or which ones (GPA) of students who opted to take the spatial ability training
improve based on training has not been performed. This infor- course was significantly better, 2.64 and 3.00 for the Control and
mation may assist in determining the optimal training methods to Experimental Groups respectively (Sorby, 2005). Subsequent re-
improve the spatial ability skills of students and thus provide the search has investigated the effect of such training on non-engineering
most significant benefits for the training time committed. In this students (Sorby, 2007), K-12 students (Sorby, 2006) and women
paper, scores from a standardized spatial ability test, which was (Sorby, 2001) in engineering. Similarly, Study (2006) provided re-
administered before and after training and/or enrollment in a mediation over a semester long course to minority students and
CAD course, are compared. The results show that the targeted found an improvement in their spatial ability test scores between
training produced a significant improvement in the spatial ability the beginning and end of the semester.
test scores of students and that these improvements occur across As opposed to semester-long training, more targeted training
all object geometries and rotation types. efforts that require less time commitment have also been investi-
gated. Hsi et al. (1997) identified at-risk students in an introduc-
Keywords: computer-aided design, spatial ability, training methods tory design course due to poorly developed spatial skills and pro-
vided targeted training. Data analyses showed that pre-course
gender differences were eliminated and that the overall course
I. INTRODUCTION grade was improved as a result of the targeted spatial ability training.
However, such gender improvement was not found in other studies
(Sorby, 2005; Kinsey et al., 2007). Women typically score lower on
Spatial ability has been defined as the ability to generate, retain, spatial ability tests than men (Sorby, Leopold, and Gorska, 1999).
retrieve, and transform well-structured visual images (Lohman, While investigating the improvements noted in spatial ability
1993). These skills are extremely important in engineering practice scores before and after training, the question of which shapes and
and many other professions as well such as Chemistry, Physics, etc. rotations were the most difficult for students and showed the most
(Devon, Engel, and Turner, 1998; Coleman and Gotch, 1998; significant improvement due to training has not been studied. This
Carter, LaRussa, and Bodner, 1987; Pallrand and Seeber, 1984; may be important for the design of appropriate training programs

April 2009 Journal of Engineering Education 157


Table 1. Number of subjects in each group.

that can provide targeted instruction to mitigate any spatial ability ability. The Web-based software recorded the radio button the stu-
concerns. If certain questions are providing difficulty to students, dent selected for each of the multiple choice test questions. To en-
then an array of training activities directed towards these concerns sure anonymity, an encrypted university identification code was
could be developed. To provide data with respect to this question, used as opposed to the student’s name for data analyses.
portions of the Purdue Spatial Visualization Test (PSVT) (Guay,
1977) were administered to mechanical engineering freshmen, who A. Participants
were enrolled in a CAD course within the College of Engineering The Web-based test was administered to 190 Mechanical
and Physical Sciences at the University of New Hampshire in the Engineering students (Males  174 and Females  16) at the
beginning of the fall semesters in 2006 and 2007. Students whose University of New Hampshire during the fall semesters of 2006
average percentage correct PSVT score was less than 60 percent and 2007. These students were all enrolled in a freshman level
were invited to participate in a special training program. This 3D CAD course. The subjects were divided into three groups
60 percent criterion was selected to obtain a reasonable sample size based on how they scored on the PSVT at the beginning of the
for testing and based on a standard assessment of competence typi- semester, i.e., Low Group (those whose percentage correct
cally used. Those who responded as willing to participate in the PSVT score was less than or equal to 60 percent), Intermediate
training formed the Experimental Group and the rest were included Group (those who scored between 60 percent and 80 percent),
in the Control Group. The subjects were again tested using the and High Group (those who scored above 80 percent). The stu-
same instrument at the end of the semesters and their scores were dents in the Low Group were invited to receive targeted train-
compared to those obtained at the beginning of the semesters. ing. Approximately half of the students opted for the training
The hypothesis for this research was that students with initially (Experimental Group), while the remaining students did not
poor spatial ability skills who were involved in targeted training (Control Group). This critical score (60 percent) was selected
would perform better than students who opted not to be involved based on the distribution of scores. Note that these groups were
in training. An additional goal of the research was to identify the self-selected.
question types that caused the improved spatial ability scores and Since the spatial ability of women improves at different
those that students continued to struggle with. This information rates than men (Sorby, 2005; Kinsey et al., 2007) and there
would thus identify the types of problems to target for further were a disproportionate number of women in the various
training. groups, the data for males only were used when comparing the
The results obtained show that there is no difference between the scores. Table 1 shows the distribution of the subjects in the
Experimental and Control Groups at the beginning of the semester different groups.
for all categories of object geometries and/or rotation types for the
PSVT questions. However, at the end of the semester, students in B. Web-Based Test
the Experimental Group scored statistically better than those in the The subset of PSVT questions which were administered to stu-
Control Group. These improvements existed for almost all question dents consisted of 20 questions from the mental rotation of the ob-
types, both easy and difficult. Furthermore, the Experimental ject section and 20 questions from the mental rotation of perspec-
Group’s average percentage correct PSVT score increased such that tive section (Guay, 1977). The test consists of three dimensional
there was no statistical difference between a group of students who representations of different objects in both solid and no hidden line
initially scored higher than them at the beginning of the semester. representations. See Figures 1 and 2 for examples of PSVT rota-
This demonstrates the effectiveness of the targeted training. tions questions and Figure 3 for an example of a PSVT perspective
question, where the student selects the correct perspective of the
object if it was viewed from the corner of the transparent cube with
I. METHODOLOGY the dot. The questions were subdivided further according to the ob-
ject’s geometry and nature of the rotation in the questions. This was
A Web-based version of the Purdue Spatial Visualization Test done so that an understanding could be obtained as to how the
(Guay, 1977) was used to obtain a measure of a student’s spatial training affected the student performance with different object

158 Journal of Engineering Education April 2009


geometries and rotation types. Note that in Figure 1, the object in-
cludes both a single inclined plane and a cylindrical surface and is a
solid image of the object undergoing a single, 180 rotation about
the standard isometric axes of the object. Alternatively, Figure 2 is a
multiple inclined plane object shown in a line representation under-
going a double rotation (both 90 rotations). Finally, Figure 3 is of
an object with an oblique plane and is also shown in a line represen-
tation. The questions were presented to all of the students in the
same order with the type of question (i.e., object geometries, rota-
tion type, and whether the object was a solid or line representation)
randomly mixed.
Table 2 shows the various object geometries (e.g., Right Angle
Only, Cylindrical, etc.) and rotation (e.g., Single Rotation, 180
Rotation, etc.) question types in the PSVT. Only questions from
the original PSVT were used in this study. The questions in the
Figure 1. Question from the PSVT mental rotation of an object subset used were selected to achieve a balance for the question
section (Guay, 1977) which includes the top right angle only object types. However, a limited number of Right Angle Only and
and the bottom object that contains both a cylindrical surface and an Oblique questions existed in the PSVT and all were used in our
inclined plane. study. The number of questions in each type is also provided in
Table 2. Note that only the rotation of view PSVT questions were
used when determining the Single or Double Rotation types
while the object geometry type and the 90 or 180 Rotations were
determined from all 40 questions of the PSVT subset. Also, note
that some questions included more than one object type (e.g., the
object in Figure 1 includes both an Inclined Plane and Cylindrical
surface). Thus, the number of object geometry questions does not
sum to 40.

C. Targeted Spatial Ability Training


The training consisted of two tools, the Physical Model Rotator
(PMR) and Alternative View Screen (AVS). The PMR, which is
shown in Figure 4, rotates an object in synchronous motion with
the same object in the CAD software. This assists the student in in-
terpreting the three dimensional object from the two dimensional
representation in the software. The AVS software provides two
Figure 2. Question from the PSVT mental rotation of an object representations of the object in the CAD software and rotates them
section (Guay, 1977) which includes the top right angle only object in synchronous motion. This assures that the student is not having
and the bottom object that contains multiple inclined planes. difficulty perceiving the various representations (i.e., solid, hidden
line, no hidden line, etc.) of the object. See Figure 5 for a screen
capture from the AVS software. Further details related to these de-
vices can be found in Towle (2005) and Onyancha, Towle, and
Kinsey (2007).
The targeted training consisted of two 1-hour sessions working
with the AVS system, and two 1-hour sessions working with the
PMR over a four week period. The students were provided with
written instructions, introductory support in the use of the tools,
and additional help as required from a graduate teaching assistant
for all the four sessions. The training exercises consisted of four ac-
tivities each session, such as creating engineering drawings, rotating
to a specified view (e.g., isometric), and using the devices to check
the results (Towle, 2005). The students were again tested at the end
of the semester with the same PSVT questions used at the begin-
ning of the semester.

II. RESULTS
Figure 3. Question from the PSVT mental rotation of perspec-
tive section (Guay, 1977) which includes an object with an Figure 6 compares the average percentage correct PSVT
oblique plane. scores at the beginning and end of the semester for the various

April 2009 Journal of Engineering Education 159


Table 2. Information regarding various object geometry and rotation types.

Figure 4. Picture of the Physical Model Rotator device (Towle, Figure 6. Comparison of the average percentage correct PSVT
2005; Onyancha, Towle, and Kinsey, 2007). scores of the students in the various groups at the beginning and at
the end of the semester.

imental Group performed significantly better on the PSVT


compared to the Control Group (p  0.013). Furthermore, the
Experimental Group improved their scores to the point that
there was no statistically significant difference between them
and the Intermediate Group (p  0.22) at the end of the semes-
ter, while at the beginning of the semester there was a difference
(p  0.000). The data and p-values comparing the beginning of
the semester to the end of the semester average percentage cor-
rect PSVT scores for all 40 questions are provided in Table 3. As
is evident from this data, the only group whose spatial ability
scores did not significantly improve from the beginning of the
semester to the end of the semester was the High Group. Inde-
pendent samples t-tests were performed to compare the average
percentage correct PSVT scores between the various groups.
The p-values comparing the Experimental Group versus Con-
trol Group and the Experimental versus Group Intermediate
Figure 5. Screen capture from the Alternative View Screen de- Group are provided in Table 3. In addition, paired samples t-tests
vice (Towle, 2005; Onyancha, Towle, and Kinsey, 2007). were performed to compare the beginning of the semester to the
end of semester scores for the individual groups. All p-values
groups (i.e., High, Intermediate, Experimental (trained), and reported in this paper are two-tailed. Finally, Cohen’s d-values
Control (not trained)). As is evident from this figure, at the be- are presented to assess effect size.
ginning of the semester, the Experimental and Control Groups In order to determine if there were specific question types
had equivalent PSVT scores (p  0.8 based on an independent that caused these effects to occur, the various object geometries
samples t-test). However, by the end of the semester, the Exper- and rotation types were considered (see Table 4 and 5). Again,

160 Journal of Engineering Education April 2009


Table 3. Comparison of the average percentage correct PSVT scores on all 40 questions for the students in the various groups at the be-
ginning and at the end of the semester. Also, included are the p-values from independent and paired samples t-tests and Cohen’s d-values
to assess effect size.

the p-values comparing the Experimental Group versus Control is consistent with past research results that show spatial ability
Group and the Experimental Group versus Intermediate Group scores improving based on CAD training (Miller, 1992; Kinsey
are provided in Tables 4 and 5 as well as independent samples et al., 2007). These improvements were observed for all questions
t-tests to determine if a given group improved their spatial abili- (both easy and difficult categories) except for easy rotation ques-
ty scores from the beginning to the end of the semester. tions (see Figures 7 and 8 and data in Table 7). If individual ques-
In order to further evaluate these results and to provide graphical tion types are investigated (see Tables 4 and 5), these trends hold
representations of the improvements, the questions were grouped except for lack of improvements in easy Right Angle Only and
into Easy and Difficult Categories for both the object and rotation Cylindrical questions and difficult Oblique objects, where im-
question types based on the average PSVT scores for the various provements from the beginning to the end of the semester were
question types shown in Table 6. not found for the Intermediate and Control Groups. Also, the In-
With these groupings, the various groups were compared termediate Group did not improve on the easy single rotation
with respect to their beginning and end of semester scores for questions.
the Easy and Difficult Categories of questions. Figures 7 and 8 The Experimental Group that received both targeted training as
show the result of these analyses. As these results show, the Ex- well as CAD training was the only group that had a statistically sig-
perimental Group performed better than the Control Group on nificant improvement for all question types and categories (see
all question types. Furthermore, their improvements on all ques- Tables 3–5 and 7). Alternatively, the only category that the High
tion types allowed them to improve their scores such that there Group improved on was the difficult rotation category, i.e., the 180
was no statistically significant difference between them and the and double rotations. These last two points are further discussed
Intermediate Group. Table 7 has the data for these analyses as below.
well as p-values comparing the beginning of the semester to the
end of the semester spatial ability scores for the various cate- B. Experimental Group Improvements due to Targeted Training
gories and groups. If individual question types in Tables 4 and 5 are considered, at
While it was somewhat subjective to include the single inclined the beginning of the semester, there was no statistically significant
plane problems in the Easy Category for the object shapes, multi- difference between the Experimental (trained) and Control (not
ple colleagues from both psychology and engineering provided the trained) Groups who both scored below 60 percent initially on the
same designations for all cases when asked to categorize the ques- PSVT. (See the p-value for Expermimental vs. Control in Tables 4
tions. The end of the semester scores in Table 4 justify this deci- and 5.) However, by the end of the semester, there was a statistically
sion more than the beginning of the semester scores in Table 6. significant difference with the Experimental Group scoring higher
Even if the single inclined plane problems were included in the than the Control Group on all question types except for Right
Difficult Category, Figure 7 was not considerably affected by this Angle Only and Single Rotation problems, which are two of the
designation. easiest question types. (It is noteworthy that a marginally significant
effect (p  0.065) was obtained for the Double Rotation ques-
tions.) For the easy and difficult categories (see Table 7), the Exper-
III. DISCUSSION imental Group did significantly better in every category by the end
of the semester. Thus, the training produced a significant increase
A. Improvements Based on CAD Training in the spatial ability scores of the students.
The spatial ability scores of students in the Intermediate and The improvements by the Experimental Group from the be-
Control Groups who received CAD software training (but not ginning of the semester to the end of the semester were so consid-
the targeted spatial ability training with the PMR and AVS) im- erable that their spatial ability scores were statistically equivalent
proved over the course of the semester with respect to the total av- to those of the Intermediate group by the end of the semester on
erage correct PSVT scores (see Figure 6 and data in Table 3). This all question types except the Single Rotation (p  0.008) and 180

April 2009 Journal of Engineering Education 161


Table 4. Comparison of the average percentage correct PSVT scores of the students in the various groups at the beginning and at the end of
the semester for the object question types. Also, included are the p-values from independent and paired samples t-tests.

Rotation questions (p  0.052). At the beginning of the semester, The Experimental Group improved on every object and rota-
the Intermediate Group’s scores were statistically higher than the tion type question in the PSVT. (See p-values comparing the be-
Experimental Group’s scores. (See p-values for Experimental vs. ginning and end of the semester scores.) They were the only group
Intermediate) For the easy and difficult categories (see Table 7), to improve significantly on the Right Angle Only, Cylindrical,
the Experimental Group scored equivalently to the Intermediate Oblique, and 90 Rotation questions. These improvements were
Group in every category by the end of the semester. the cause of the change in their scores relative to the other groups.

162 Journal of Engineering Education April 2009


Table 5. Comparison of the average percentage correct PSVT scores of the students in the various groups at the beginning and at the end of
the semester for the rotation question types. Also, included are the p-values from independent and paired samples t-tests.

The improvements by the Experimental Group were similar to questions (both object and rotation). However, for the easy
the improvements observed through the semester long course at questions, the High Group did not improve. This represents a
Michigan Tech (Sorby, 2005). In their study, the average percent- ceiling effect where the High Group did not improve from the
age correct on the PSVT increased from 48.3 percent at the begin- beginning to the end of the semester. In fact, in all except the
ning of the semester to 73.7 percent at the end of the semester. Single Incline questions of the easy question cases (e.g., Right
It is noteworthy that the scores for the High Group were signifi- Angle Only, Cylindrical, etc.), the average percentage correct
cantly higher than any other group at the beginning and end of the for the High Group decreased on the Easy Category questions
semester on all question types ( p  0.05). Similarly, the Intermedi- from the beginning of the semester to the end of the semester.
ate Group’s scores were significantly higher than the Control This is likely due to the students spending less time and making
Group at the beginning and end of the semester ( p  0.01). So the careless errors on these less challenging questions. However, for
improvements by the Experimental Group were the only ones that the Difficult Category questions where they were engaged, the
changed the relative differences between the groups. scores for the High Group improved (e.g., the Multiple Incline,
180 Rotation, and Double Rotation questions). These im-
C. Ceiling Effect of Spatial Ability Scores provements were statistically significant on the Difficult Rota-
It is interesting to note that all groups, even the High tion Category questions (p  0.01). Additional ceiling effects
Group, improved their spatial ability scores for the difficult may have occurred for the Intermediate and Control groups

April 2009 Journal of Engineering Education 163


Table 6. Groupings of various questions into Easy and Difficult Categories based on average percentage correct PSVT scores at the
beginning of semester.

Figure 7. Comparison of the average percentage correct PSVT Figure 8. Comparison of the average percentage correct PSVT
scores of the students in the various groups at the beginning and at scores of the students in the various groups at the beginning and at
the end of the semester for the (a) Easy and (b) Difficult Object Cat- the end of the semester for the (a) Easy and (b) Difficult Rotation
egories of questions. Categories of questions.

164 Journal of Engineering Education April 2009


Table 7. Comparison of the average percentage correct PSVT scores of the students in the various groups at the beginning and at the end of
the semester for the categories of questions. Also, included are the p-values from independent and paired samples t-tests.

who did not improve their spatial ability scores for easy ques- Analyses of Variance (ANOVAs) were not performed as the
tions such as the Right Angle Only, Cylindrical Surface, and data showed an obvious main effect between groups and begin-
90 rotation questions. ning and end of semester scores. Thus, the t-tests were conducted
to provide more specific information related to the improvements
D. Additional Comments found.
Based on Stanley and Hopkins (1972), practice effects for tests ad- Note that the PSVT is only one measure of spatial ability. Other
ministered within three months will improve scores by 0.2 times the measures could be used to further assess the improvements based on
standard deviation or less. For our study, the standard deviations for the CAD software use and targeted training.
the groups that improved from the beginning of the semester to the
end of the semester (i.e., the Intermediate, Experimental, and Control
Groups) were 11.4, 15.1, and 17.9 respectively. Thus, the improve- IV. CONCLUSIONS
ments in the spatial ability scores were 0.50, 1.66, and 0.56 times the
standard deviation respectively. While some of the improvement Spatial ability has been shown to correlate with retention and
could be attributed to practice effects, improvements in the spatial achievement in engineering and science courses. Thus, improving
ability scores due to the CAD and targeted training did occur based on the spatial ability skills of students is essential. The data analyses
this analysis, in particular for the Experimental Group. show the significant improvements in spatial ability which were

April 2009 Journal of Engineering Education 165


achieved by the students who underwent training with the PMR Lohman, D.F. 1993. Spatial ability and gender. First Spearman
and AVS devices. These improvements caused them to have signif- Seminar, University of Plymouth, July 21.
icantly higher spatial ability scores by the end of the semester than Lord, T.R. 1990. Enhancing learning in the life sciences through spa-
the Control Group who declined training and equivalent scores to tial perception. Innovative Higher Education 15 (1): 5–16.
students who scored higher than them at the beginning of the se- Miller, C. 1992. Enhancing visual literacy of engineering students
mester. The questions that they improved their scores on compared through the use of real and computer generated models. Engineering Design
to the other groups included questions that were designated as both Graphics Journal 56 (1): 27–38.
easy and difficult. Thus, the training improved their abilities across Onyancha, R.M., E. Towle, and B.L. Kinsey. 2007. Improvement of
an array of questions. However, the scores of the students potential- spatial ability using innovative tools: Alternative view screen and physical
ly could still be improved. Therefore, further training on difficult model rotator. Engineering Design Graphics Journal 71 (4): 1–8.
questions (e.g., mulitple incline and oblique objects and 180 and Pallrand, G.J., and F. Seeber. 1984. Spatial ability and achievement in
double rotations) may be beneficial. introductory physics. Journal of Research in Science Teaching 21 (5): 507-16.
It is possible that there is a limit to the potential improvements Smith, I.M. 1964. Spatial ability-its educational and social significance.
of students with spatial ability concerns as measured by a particular London: University of London.
instrument such as the PSVT. The spatial ability of the High Sorby, S., C. Leopold, and R. Gorska. 1999. Cross-cultural compar-
Group that scored above 80 percent correct on the PSVT questions isons of gender differences in the spatial skills of engineering students. Jour-
only improved on difficult rotation questions from the beginning of nal of Women and Minorities in Science and Engineering 5 (2): 279–91.
the semester to the end of the semester. In the research by Study Sorby, S., and B. Baartmans. 2000. The development and assessment of
(2006), remediation improved the spatial ability of students in the a course for enhancing the 3-d spatial visualization skills of first year engi-
experimental group, but further 3D CAD training did not improve neering students. Journal of Engineering Education 89 (3): 301–07.
the scores of the experimental group while the scores of the control Sorby, S. 2001. A course in spatial visualization and its impact on the
group continued to increase. However, the targeted training de- retention of female engineering students. Journal of Women and Minorities
scribed in this paper may provide significant improvements in the in Science and Engineering 7 (2): 153–72.
least amount of time and with the least resources spent and thus Sorby, S. 2005. Assessment of a “new and improved” course for the de-
would be desirable for students. velopment of 3-d spatial skills. Engineering Design Graphics Journal 69 (3):
Finally, as was found in past research, the spatial ability scores of 6–13.
students improved from use of CAD software during a course. Study, N. 2006. Using remediation to improve the visualization abilities
These improvements were most pronounced in questions that were in minority engineering and technology students. In Proceedings of the 2006
difficult for students at the beginning of the semester. American Society of Engineering Education Conference. Chicago, Illinois.
Sorby, S. 2006. Developing 3-d spatial skills for K-12 students. Engi-
neering Design Graphics Journal 70 (3): 1–11.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Sorby, S. 2007. A longitudinal study of the impact of spatial skills train-
ing for non-engineering students. In Proceedings of the 2007 American Soci-
Support from the National Science Foundation, Engineering ety of Engineering Education Conference. Honolulu, Hawaii.
Education and Centers Division (EEC- 0343862) is gratefully ac- Stanley, J.C., and K.D. Hopkins. 1972. Educational and psychological
knowledged. Also, the assistance of Edward O’Brien from the measurement. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Pyschology Department at UNH is appreciated. Towle, E. 2005. Design, implementation, and validation of a device to
improve the spatial ability of engineering students. MS Thesis, University
of New Hampshire.
REFERENCES

Carter, C.S., M.A. LaRussa, and G.M. Bodner. 1987. A study of AUTHORS’ BIOGRAPHIES
two measures of spatial ability as predictors of success in different levels
of general chemistry. Journal of Research in Science Teaching 24 (7): Richard M. Onyancha is an assistant professor at Rose-Hulman
645–57. Institute of Technology. He received his Bachelor’s degree from the
Coleman, S.L., and A.J. Gotch. 1998. Spatial perception skills of University of Nairobi, Kenya in 1987, his Master’s degree from
chemistry students. Journal of Chemical Research 75 (2): 206–09. Brunel University, London, in 1990, and his Ph.D. from the Uni-
Devon, R., R. Engel, and G. Turner. 1998. The effects of spatial visual- versity of New Hampshire in 2007. His awards include the 2007
ization skill training on gender and retention in engineering. Journal of Chair’s Award for the Best Paper, Engineering Design and Graph-
Women and Minorities in Engineering 4 (4): 371–80. ics Division, ASEE.
Guay, R. 1977. Purdue spatial visualization test: Rotations. West Address: Department of Mechanical Engineering, Rose-
Lafayette, IN: Purdue Research Foundation. Hulman Institute of Technology, 5500 Wabash Avenue, Terre
Hsi, S., M. Linn, and J. Bell. 1997. The role of spatial reasoning in en- Haute, IN 47803; e-mail: richard.m.onyancha@rose-hulman.
gineering and the design of spatial instruction. Journal of Engineering Edu- edu.
cation 86 (2): 151–58.
Kinsey, B.L., E. Towle, E.J. O’Brien, and C. Bauer. 2007. Analysis of Matthew Derov received his Bachelor’s and Master’s degrees
self-efficacy and ability related to spatial tasks and the effect on retention from the University of New Hampshire in 2006 and 2008 respec-
for students in engineering. International Journal of Engineering Education tively, in Mechanical Engineering. He is currently employed by
24 (3): 488–94. SolidWorks Corporation.

166 Journal of Engineering Education April 2009


Brad L. Kinsey is an associate professor in the Mechanical Engi- the 2007 Chair’s Award for the Best Paper, Engineering Design
neering Department at the University of New Hampshire. He re- and Graphics Division, ASEE, and the 2006 Ralph R. Teetor
ceived his Bachelor’s degree from the University of Michigan in Award from the Society of Automotive Engineers.
1992 and his Master’s and Doctoral degrees from Northwestern Address: Mechanical Engineering Department, University of
University in 1998 and 2001 respectively, all in Mechanical Engi- New Hampshire, Kingsbury Hall, Room S256, 33 College Road,
neering. His awards include a CAREER Award from NSF in Durham, NH 03824; telephone: (1) 603.862.1811; e-mail:
2007, the UNH Assistant Professor of the Year Award in 2006–07, bkinsey@unh.edu.

April 2009 Journal of Engineering Education 167

You might also like