You are on page 1of 17

Journal of International Consumer Marketing

ISSN: (Print) (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/wicm20

Consumer Intention to Buy Plant-Based Meat


Alternatives: A Cross-Cultural Analysis

Yousra Bakr, Hayat Al-Bloushi & Mohamed Mostafa

To cite this article: Yousra Bakr, Hayat Al-Bloushi & Mohamed Mostafa (2022): Consumer
Intention to Buy Plant-Based Meat Alternatives: A Cross-Cultural Analysis, Journal of
International Consumer Marketing, DOI: 10.1080/08961530.2022.2122103

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/08961530.2022.2122103

Published online: 14 Sep 2022.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 570

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Citing articles: 1 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=wicm20
Journal of International Consumer Marketing
https://doi.org/10.1080/08961530.2022.2122103

Consumer Intention to Buy Plant-Based Meat Alternatives: A


Cross-Cultural Analysis
Yousra Bakra, Hayat Al-Bloushib and Mohamed Mostafac
a
McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario, Canada; bCollege of Business Studies Ardiya Safat, The Public Authority for Applied
Education and Training – PAAET, Kuwait City, Kuwait; cGulf University for Science and Technology, Hawally, Kuwait

ABSTRACT KEYWORDS
The purpose of this study is to explore factors affecting attitudes toward plant-based meat Plant-based meat alternatives;
alternatives (PBMA) as well as consumer intentions to buy PBMA in Canada as a Western plant-based diet; meat
country and Kuwait as an Arab country. The proposed research model extends the theory attachment; food neophobia;
theory of planned behavior;
of planned behavior (TPB) by incorporating factors such as environmental concerns, health
sustainable food choices
consciousness, and animal welfare, food choice factors (health, sustainability, cruelty-free,
and sensory appeal), meat attachment and food neophobia. Data were collected through
online surveys in Canada (n = 304) and Kuwait (n = 313). The partial least square structural
equation modeling (PLS-SEM) technique was used for data analysis and hypotheses testing.
Results show that attitudes, subjective norms, and behavioral control have a significant effect
on purchase intentions. Results also show that environmental concerns, cruelty free as a
food factor choice, meat attachment, and food neophobia significantly affect attitudes toward
PBMA. There were significant differences between Canada and Kuwait in the strength of the
effect of attitude and behavioral control on purchase intentions.

1.  Introduction Counts, n.d.), it is now more vital than ever to


promote more sustainable food options.
According to the Food and Agriculture
Plant-based meat alternatives (PBMA) are
Organization, meat production and consumption
foods made from plants or plant-derived ingre-
are responsible for several climate and environ-
dients, produced in a manner that mimics the
mental issues (MacLeod et  al. 2013). Livestock
taste, smell, and texture of real meat (Choudhury
production is a key factor in deforestation where
et  al. 2020). Popular brands of PBMA include
it accounts for 70% of all agricultural land and Beyond Meat and Impossible Foods. When com-
30% of land surface on the planet (Steinfeld et  al. pared to meat production, the production of
2006; The Good Food Institute 2019a). Livestock PBMA uses 47%–99% less land, emits 30%–90%
production produces more emissions than the less greenhouse gas, uses 72%–99% less water
transport sector, accounting for 18% of green- and causes 51%–91% less water pollution (The
house gas emissions as well as over 60% of Good Food Institute 2019b). Besides being more
anthropogenic ammonia emissions which con- environmentally sustainable, PBMA have—in
tributes to acid rain and acidification of ecosys- many ways—less harmful effects on human health
tems (Steinfeld et  al. 2006). Moreover, livestock when compared to meat (Reinberg 2021) and
production is widely regarded as a major source constitute a tremendous step toward animal wel-
of water pollution and a threat to biodiversity fare (Webber, n.d.). In general, global market
(Steinfeld et  al. 2006; The Good Food Institute revenues from PBMA have seen an increase from
2019a). With meat consumption hovering over $3.64 billion in 2016 to $7.75 billion in 2021 and
350 million tons per year and forecasted to reach is forecasted to increase to $16.65 billion in 2026
as high as 570 million tons by 2050 (The World (Statista 2021). This increase is mainly led by

CONTACT Yousra Bakr yousra.bakr@gmail.com McMaster University, 1280 Main St W, Hamilton, ON L8S 4L8, Ontario, Canada.
© 2022 Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
2 Y. BAKR ET AL.

vegan and vegetarian consumers whose numbers intentions of PBMA in Kuwait, in comparison
increase year after year (Sentient Media 2021). to Canada as a Western country. The study
However, if regular meat-eaters, who still consti- extends the theory of planned behavior by add-
tute the majority of the global population, reduce ing variables related to meat attachment, food
their meat consumption and substitute meat with neophobia, relevant attitudes toward the envi-
plant-based alternatives just once a week, the ronment, health, and animal welfare, as well as
collective impact on the environment, health, and several food choice factors. The results of the
animal welfare would be immense (Michel et  al. study would add to the growing body of litera-
2021). Accordingly, understanding the motiva- ture on PBMA, and would contribute to under-
tions and barriers to the acceptance of PBMA standing the readiness of Arab meat-eaters for
amongst meat-eaters is of crucial importance. mainstream adoption of PBMA. The following
In recent years, there has been an academic inter- section presents a review of literature and intro-
est in the acceptance of vegan foods, including duces the research model.
PBMA. Published studies explore the motivations
and barriers to reducing meat consumption as well
2.  Literature review
as adopting vegan and vegetarian lifestyles (Simons
et  al. 2021; Reipurth et  al. 2019). Prior research has 2.1.  Motivations to adopt a plant-based diet
identified primary motivations to adopting
Motives to adopting a plant-based diet can be
plant-based diets as environmental concerns (Wang
classified into egoistic motives, such as health
and Scrimgeour 2021; Davitt et al. 2021; Michel et al.
benefits, and altruistic motives, such as environ-
2021), health concerns (Estell, Hughes, and
mental concerns and animal welfare (Phua, Jin,
Grafenauer 2021), as well as animal welfare (Profeta
and Kim 2020). Attitudes related to these factors
et  al. 2021; Ploll and Stern 2020). Prior research has
can affect the general attitude toward PBMA.
also identified barriers to be meat attachment (Graça,
Calheiros, and Oliveira 2015; Circus and Robison
2019), food neophobia (Wang and Scrimgeour 2021;
Hwang et  al. 2020), among other factors. 2.1.1.  Health consciousness
While most published studies focus on Health consciousness is defined as “the tendency
European and western countries, there is a lack to focus attention on one’s health” (Iversen and
of research that explores how consumers in Arab Kraft 2006). Dietary choices are commonly linked
countries perceive PBMA. There is also a lack of to health and well-being (Fox and Ward 2008),
understanding as to what motivates the adoption and studies have demonstrated a positive rela-
of PBMA in these countries, despite that meat tionship between health consciousness and healthy
plays a central role in the region’s food culture. food choices. For example, Chen (2009) reports
Increasing urbanization and the consumer shift a positive relationship between health conscious-
toward high-protein and low carb diets have led ness and attitude toward organic foods. Royne
to an increase in meat demand, especially in Gulf et  al. (2014) have also reported that health con-
countries which host the most affluent consumers sciousness has a positive effect on attitude toward
of the Arab region (Zarrouki 2017). For example, dietary supplements. Plant-based products tend
Kuwait was ranked second worldwide in terms to be associated with being healthy in consumers’
of meat consumption in 2017, with an average minds (Rondoni et  al. 2021) and health concerns
consumption per capita of 119.2 kg (GDN Online have been routinely cited as a main motive for
2017). At the same time, the forecasts for the the reduction of meat consumption (Kemper
plant-based market in the MEA region valued at 2020) and for following plant-based diets (Ghaffari
$240.96 million in 2020 promises a 6.01% increase et  al. 2022). This discussion suggests the follow-
from 2021 to 2026 (Mordor Intelligence 2020). ing hypothesis:
Given this gap in research, the purpose of this H1: Health consciousness has a positive impact on
study is to explore the acceptance and purchase attitude towards PBMA
Journal of International Consumer Marketing 3

Figure 1.  Conceptual model.

2.1.2.  Environmental concern attitude toward or intention to buy plant-based


Dunlap and Jones (2002, p. 485) define food. This discussion suggests the following
Environmental Concerns as “the degree to which hypothesis:
people are aware of problems regarding the envi-
H2: Environmental concerns have a positive impact
ronment and support efforts to solve them.” on attitude towards PBMA
Research shows that consumers’ environmental
awareness can influence the perceived value of
products and therefore, can influence their choice 2.1.3.  Animal welfare
of products (Papista and Krystallis 2013). It has Concerns for animal welfare are one of the most
also been shown that consumers with higher con- cited altruistic motives to adopting plant-based
cern for the environment were more likely to buy diets (Fox and Ward 2008; Ploll and Stern 2020).
green products and consider the effect of their In general, avoidance of animal products is
purchases on the environment (Mostafa 2007). related to concerns over animal welfare (Izmirli
Yadav and Pathak (2016) have also found that and Phillips 2011). Since meat consumption is a
there is a significant positive relationship between behavior associated with attitudinal ambivalence
consumers’ environmental concern and their atti- (Berndsen and van der Pligt 2004), Rothgerber
tudes toward green products. In the context of (2013) have reported several strategies used by
plant-based foods, environmental concerns have meat-eaters to justify the behavior such as denial
been one of the main motives to follow a of animal suffering, or the belief that humans are
plant-based diet which incorporates more sus- superior to animals. Similarly, Bilewicz, Imhoff,
tainable food choices (Profeta et  al. 2021; Michel and Drogosz (2011) have reported that the per-
et  al. 2021; Fox and Ward 2008). A significant ceived level of similarity between animals and
percentage of those who tried plant-based foods humans is different between consumers who fol-
before believe they are more sustainable and more low an omnivore versus a vegetarian diet, where
environmentally friendly (Davitt et  al. 2021). meat-eaters “deny animals certain characteristics”
Miguel, Coelho, and Bairrada (2020) and Hwang mostly related to secondary emotions such as
et  al. (2020) have reported a significant positive love and melancholy. On the other hand, con-
relationship between environmental concerns and sumers who support anti-speciesism believe that
4 Y. BAKR ET AL.

all species deserve the same moral consideration H6: Cruelty-free as a food choice measure has a pos-
and animals should not be consumed for human itive impact on attitude towards PBMA
benefit (Espinosa and Treich 2021). Accordingly, H7: Sensory appeal (taste, texture, smell and look)
consumers with higher attitudes toward animal as a food choice measure has a positive impact on
welfare are less likely to be meat-eaters than flex- attitude towards PBMA
itarians, which means that attitudes toward ani-
mal welfare might be a strong predictor to one’s 2.3.  Meat attachment
diet (De Backer and Hudders 2015). This discus-
sion suggests the following hypothesis: One of the main barriers against the adoption of
PBMA is known as meat attachment, which is
H3: Animal welfare concerns have a positive impact the affective bond between consumers and meat
on attitude towards PBMA
(Graça, Calheiros, and Oliveira 2015). Graça,
Calheiros, and Oliveira (2015) developed a meat
2.2.  Food choice measures attachment questionnaire (MAQ) that consists of
four dimensions: hedonism (meat consumption
Steptoe, Pollard, and Wardle (1995) developed a as a source of pleasure), affinity (degree of liking
measure for motives underlying food choice which for meat), entitlement (meat consumption as a
includes nine factors: health, mood, convenience, human right) and dependence (degree of reliance
sensory appeal, natural content, price,weight con- on meat consumption). The MAQ was used to
trol, familiarity and ethical concerns. Lindeman assess consumers’ willingness to reduce meat con-
and Väänänen (2000) later expanded the scale to sumption and adopt a more plant-based diet in
include dimensions related to animal welfare, polit- several studies. Graça, Calheiros, and Oliveira
ical views, religion, and environmental protection. (2015) report that meat attachment has a negative
Studies utilized dimensions from the food choice impact on the willingness to adopt a more
questionnaire to assess motivations for healthy plant-based diet. Similarly, consumers with low
eating, as well as consumption of green and meat attachment are more likely to be following
organic products (Tandon et  al. 2020). In the con- some form of a plant-based diet, as opposed to
text of plant-based foods, Wang and Scrimgeour consumers with high meat attachment scores who
(2021) examined the impact of health and envi- are more likely to be meat-eaters (Circus and
ronmental concerns dimensions on the adoption Robison 2019). There is also a negative associa-
of plant-based diets where ethical concerns had tion between the level of meat attachment and
a significant positive impact on willingness to meat substitution (Profeta et  al. 2021; Wang and
adopt a more plant-based diet. Similar results Scrimgeour 2021; Bryant et  al. 2020). This dis-
were reported by Haverstock and Forgays (2012) cussion suggests the following hypothesis:
where animal rights, environmental, and health
H8: Meat attachment has a negative impact on atti-
dimensions were significant motivating factors for
tude towards PBMA
animal product limiters. Other studies explored
the effect of sensory appeal (taste and texture) on
the acceptance of plant-based foods/diets, and it 2.4.  Food neophobia
was a significant factor affecting food perceptions Food neophobia is the inclination to avoid
and choice (Pandey, Ritz, and Perez-Cueto 2021; unknown foods (Pliner and Hobden 1992). Food
Profeta et  al. 2021). In this study, two dimensions neophobia is considered a barrier to trying for-
from the original food choice questionnaire will eign and new foods (Lähteenmäki and Arvola
be tested—health and sensory appeal—in addition 2010) which include novel meat substitutes.
to animal welfare and sustainability: Several studies have assessed the impact of food
neophobia on the adoption of meat substitutes
H4: Health as a food choice measure has a positive
impact on attitude towards PBMA
such as plant-based foods, cultured meat, hybrid
meat, and insect-based meat substitutes (Michel
H5: Sustainability as a food choice measure has a et  al. 2021; Profeta et  al. 2021; Hwang et  al.
positive impact on attitude towards PBMA 2020). Michel et  al. (2021) and Forestell, Spaeth,
Journal of International Consumer Marketing 5

and Kane (2012) found that consumers with a 2021). There is a significant relationship between
negative attitude toward plant-based diets tend perceived behavioral control and willingness to
to have high food neophobia scores, which means adopt a plant-based diet (Wang and Scrimgeour
that food neophobia predicts low purchase inten- 2021; Pandey, Ritz, and Perez-Cueto 2021). This
tions of plant-based foods (Bryant et  al. 2020). discussion suggests the following hypotheses:
This discussion suggests the following hypothesis:
H10: Attitude towards PBMA has a positive impact
H9: Food neophobia has a negative impact on attitude on intention to buy PBMA
towards PBMA
H11: Subjective norms (normative beliefs) have a
positive impact on intention to buy PBMA
2.5.  Theory of planned behavior
H12: Behavioral control (availability, price and cook-
The theory of planned behavior (TPB) has been ing ability) has a positive impact on intention to
one of the most influential theories to explain buy PBMA
human behavior since its development (Ajzen
2011). The theory has four main components:
2.6.  Multi-group differences
behavior, attitude, subjective norms and behav-
ioral control, where attitude, subjective norms In this study, the researchers choose to compare
and behavioral control affect behavioral inten- between Canada and Kuwait. Based on Hofstede
tions (Ajzen 1991). The TPB has been used (2001), there are significant cultural differences
to explain consumers’ dietary choices includ- between both countries. Canada has a signifi-
ing the consumption of fruits and vegetables, cantly higher score in Individualism (score of 80)
organic produce, healthy food, among others versus a score of 25 in Kuwait. On the other
(Yazdanpanah and Forouzani 2015). In the con- hand, Kuwait has significantly higher scores in
text of plant-based foods, the TPB explains the uncertainty avoidance (score of 80) and power
adoption of plant-based diets, as well as a variety distance (score of 90) as opposed to 48 and 39
of plant-based and vegan products (Wang and in Canada respectively.
Scrimgeour 2021; Ploll and Stern 2020). Previous cross-cultural studies on food choices
Studies testing the impact of subjective norms confirm that cultural aspects affect food choices,
on the adoption of plant-based diets or foods “specifically with individualistic, feminine and
report inconsistent results. Some studies could tolerant societies” (Djekic et  al. 2021). In general,
not establish a significant relationship between food choice is a behavior that is heavily moder-
social influences and attitudes or social influences ated by norms and culture (Montanari 2006). A
and behavior intention (Pandey, Ritz, and difference in food choice motives is also expected
Perez-Cueto 2021; Miguel, Coelho, and Bairrada to exist between different countries. For example,
2020). However, de Lanauze and Sirieix (2022) Phillips et  al. (2012) reported differences between
report that social influences are a crucial factor European and Asian students’ attitudes toward
in the adoption and maintenance of a plant-based animal welfare and perceptions of animal rights
diet. Wang and Scrimgeour (2021) and Contini as a result of different socio-political situations.
et  al. (2020) also report a significant relationship Unlike most western countries, animal welfare is
between subjective norms and willingness to not yet a political priority in most Middle Eastern
adopt a plant-based diet. countries (Aidaros 2014), which can affect con-
Perceived behavioral control is the perceived sumers’ perceptions toward animal welfare, as
difficulty of performing a certain behavior which well as the effect on purchase decisions and food
is determined by a set of control beliefs or factors choices. Similarly, Rahman and Luomala (2021)
that can facilitate or impede the behavior report that differences in the cultural dimension
(Fishbein and Ajzen 2009). Barriers to the adop- of individualism versus collectivism have a posi-
tion of plant-based foods can include price, famil- tive impact on environmental responsibility which
iarity, cooking ability, availability, among others had a positive effect on the consumption of green
(Contini et  al. 2020; Aschemann-Witzel et  al. products. Different levels of uncertainty avoidance
6 Y. BAKR ET AL.

would also affect the level of food neophobia, constructs which were developed by the research-
which can affect the attitudes toward PBMA (Seo, ers following the guidelines of Ajzen (2006) and
Kim, and Jang 2018; Djekic et  al. 2021). Francis et  al. (2004) and were validated during the
Research on the Theory of Planned Behavior pretesting stage of the questionnaire. The measure-
across cultures also reported that the effect of ment scale for Health Consciousness is adapted
the different constructs on attitude and behavior from Gould (1990). The scale for Environmental
is different in different countries (Hassan, Shiu, Concerns is adapted from Dunlap and Jones
and Parry 2016; Park 2000). A lower score in (2002). The scale for Animal Welfare is from
individualism is associated with a stronger effect Miguel, Coelho, and Bairrada (2020) and the scale
of subjective norms on behavior intention, as for Food Choices factors is adapted from Steptoe,
noted by Park (2000). Similarly, a higher score Pollard, and Wardle (1995). The Meat Attachment
in power distance would cause differences in per- scale is adapted from the original scale developed
ceived behavioral control as well as subjective by Graça, Calheiros, and Oliveira (2015). Finally,
norms as reported by Hassan, Shiu, and Parry the Food Neophobia scale is adapted from Pliner
(2016). Therefore, this study proposes that: and Hobden (1992). A soft launch was conducted
in both countries for 50 participants each. No
H13. There are significant differences in the effect
of PBMA purchase intention between Canada and changes were made to the scales since the datasets
Kuwait. yielded acceptable reliability scores. The question-
naire also included questions about the partici-
Figure 1 presents the research model with the
pants’ demographics, dietary habits, shopping
hypotheses.
habits, as well as previous engagement with PBMA.
3.  Method
4.  Results
Data were collected via online surveys in Canada
and Kuwait. Data in Canada was collected by Following Latif, Perez, and Sahibzada (2020), the
Asking Canadians and data in Kuwait was col- data analysis reported in this study is guided by
lected by TGM Research. Participants targeted the following six steps:
were residents of Kuwait or Canada, aged 18 years
or older. The final datasets included 304 partic- 1. Assessing the common/mono method bias.
ipants from Canada, and 313 participants from 2. Checking the reliability and validity of the
Kuwait. The Canadian sample was 51% male, constructs.
48% female and 1% non-binary, while the Kuwaiti 3. Performing the descriptive analysis.
sample was 56% male, 42% female, 1% non-binary, 4. Assessing the hypothesized relationships.
and 2% preferred not to say. In Kuwait, 74% of 5. Performing mediation analysis.
the participants followed an omnivore diet, and 6. Performing multi-group analysis.
26% followed a flexitarian diet, while in Canada,
76% followed an omnivore diet and 24% followed To conduct the PLS-SEM analysis, R version
a flexitarian diet. In Canada, 68% have tried 4.1 (R Development Core Team 2021) with
PBMA before versus 47% in Kuwait. SeminR package was used. We also used IBM
The questionnaire was originally drafted in SPSS version 25.0 software package to conduct
English, then translated to Arabic by a researcher the descriptive analysis and the Harman’s single
fluent in both languages. To ensure accuracy of factor analysis, respectively. We discuss here the
translation, back-to-back translation was used. steps outlined above in some detail.
Participants were given the choice to choose the
language of preference. All constructs were mea-
sured through 5-point Likert scales except for 4.1.  Common method bias
Attitude which was measured by a 5-point seman- Common method bias (CMB) is recognized across
tic differential scale. Most scales were adapted different research fields as a threat to results integ-
from previous research except for the TPB rity. This bias occurs “when both the independent
Journal of International Consumer Marketing 7

and dependent variables are measured within one alternatives between Canada and Kuwait. Most of
survey, using the same response technique” (Kock, the independent sample t-tests conducted yielded
Berbekova, and Assaf 2021, p. 3). The most com- non-significant results between the two samples.
mon test for CMB is Harman’s single factor test
(Fuller et  al. 2016). The test suggests that CMB is
4.4.  Structural model
present if all measured items produce one factor
accounting for at least 50% of the variance We assessed the hypothesized direct relationships.
(Podsakoff et  al. 2003). In this study, Harman’s test Results are shown in Figure 2 and detailed in
revealed that a single factor explained only 20.84%, Table 3. Path coefficient estimates with T-stat
20.83% and 20.44% in the Canadian, Kuwaiti and above 1.96 and with confidence intervals that do
overall samples, respectively, which implies that the not include zero are considered significant at 0.05
CMB does not represent a serious threat to the confidence level. Results show that attitudes
integrity of the study results. toward PBMA are positively influenced by envi-
ronmental concerns (H2), and Cruelty Free as a
food choice factor (H6) at a significant level. As
4.2.  Reliability and validity
expected, there is a significant negative relation-
The reliability of the constructs was assessed using ship between Meat Attachment and Food
both Cronbach’s alpha and composite reliability Neophobia and Attitudes, therefore H8 and H9
(CR). Werts, Linn, and Joreskog (1974) suggested were supported. Results show negative insignifi-
the use of CR because it is not influenced by the cant relationships between Health Consciousness,
number of items on the scale. Initial assessment Animal Welfare, and Health as a food choice
of the overall sample resulted in eliminating items factor, thus H1, H3 and H4 were not supported.
with factor loadings lower than 0.6. Table 1 pres- Although the relationships between Attitudes and
ents the country-specific and the overall sample Sustainability and Attitudes and Sensory Appeal
results of scales’ reliability and validity as well as as food choice factors were positive, they did not
factor loadings for the remaining items. The table reach statistical significance at the traditional 0.05
shows that almost all reliability alpha values are level. Thus, H5 and H7 cannot be supported.
higher than the recommended threshold of 0.7. Theory of Planned Behavior relationships were
The table also shows that all average variance all significant, where Attitudes toward PBMA,
extracted (AVE) and CR values are all greater than Normative Beliefs and Behavioral Control had
or very close to 0.5 and 0.7, respectively. Taken positive effects on Purchase Intentions, therefore
together, the results reported in Table 1 confirm H10, H11, and H12 were supported. The same
the convergent validity of the scales. results were reported for both the Canadian and
Cross-loadings were used to assess discrimi- Kuwaiti samples.
nant validity and variance inflation factor (VIF)
was used to assess multicollinearity. All VIF val-
4.5.  Mediation analysis
ues were less than the recommended threshold
of 5. All factor loadings are higher than their For the overall sample results show that, apart
cross-loadings, which corroborates discriminant from the insignificant mediation between animal
validity. The heterotrait-monotrait (HTMT) welfare and purchase intention and between sus-
method proposed by Fornell and Larcker was tainability and purchase intention, all other medi-
also used to assess discriminant validity. Table 2 ated relationships are statistically significant. The
reports the HTMT results, which also corrobo- findings for each country-specific analysis seem
rates the discriminant validity of the scales. to generally agree with the results reported for
the overall sample. For example, for the Canadian
sample, the mediating role of meat attachment,
4.3.  Descriptive analysis
food neophobia, health consciousness, and
Descriptive analysis results revealed striking simi- cruelty-free are all significant. Roughly the same
larity regarding the perception of plant-based meat findings were reported in the Kuwaiti sample.
8 Y. BAKR ET AL.

Table 1. Factor loadings, reliability and convergent validity.


Canada Kuwait Overall
Λ Alpha CR AVE Λ Alpha CR AVE Λ Alpha CR AVE
HC1 0.910 0.906 0.929 0.727 0.761 0.857 0.890 0.617 0.856 0.878 0.911 0.672
HC2 0.909 0.795 0.846
HC3 0.896 0.805 0.841
HC4 0.874 0.796 0.838
HC5 0.646 0.770 0.710
EC1 0.727 0.863 0.902 0.650 0.749 0.757 0.822 0.481 0.708 0.807 0.866 0.565
EC2 0.875 0.693 0.808
EC3 0.838 0.692 0.777
EC4 0.880 0.602 0.757
EC5 0.692 0.721 0.702
AW1 0.741 0.838 0.892 0.674 0.844 0.858 0.904 0.702 0.814 0.848 0.897 0.686
AW2 0.860 0.829 0.844
AW3 0.856 0.870 0.856
AW4 0.822 0.807 0.797
S1 0.959 0.912 0.958 0.919 0.948 0.852 0.930 0.870 0.951 0.883 0.944 0.895
S2 0.959 0.917 0.941
H1 0.918 0.897 0.916 0.734 0.848 0.883 0.918 0.738 0.864 0.887 0.921 0.745
H2 0.919 0.896 0.889
H3 0.709 0.806 0.807
H4 0.865 0.883 0.889
CF1 0.949 0.854 0.931 0.871 0.932 0.793 0.905 0.826 0.940 0.820 0.916 0.845
CF2 0.918 0.886 0.899
SA1 0.836 0.867 0.890 0.674 0.721 0.865 0.890 0.671 0.830 0.866 0.906 0.708
SA2 0.609 0.791 0.802
SA3 0.914 0.867 0.868
SA4 0.889 0.887 0.862
MA1 0.735 0.869 0.896 0.522 0.747 0.857 0.885 0.494 0.735 0.860 0.888 0.500
MA10 0.759 0.725 0.744
MA11 0.731 0.754 0.743
MA12 0.770 0.669 0.712
MA2 0.791 0.748 0.759
MA3 0.804 0.760 0.775
MA6 0.577 0.551 0.573
MA9 0.574 0.640 0.584
FN1 0.792 0.854 0.886 0.570 0.814 0.637 0.737 0.340 0.800 0.760 0.810 0.436
FN2 0.630 0.400 0.399
FN4 0.863 0.678 0.820
FN5 0.881 0.675 0.827
FN6 0.697 0.315 0.450
FN8 0.625 0.447 0.507
A1 0.939 0.934 0.958 0.883 0.893 0.873 0.922 0.797 0.918 0.906 0.941 0.842
A2 0.926 0.873 0.901
A3 0.953 0.913 0.933
BC1 0.681 0.664 0.815 0.597 0.758 0.723 0.842 0.639 0.677 0.688 0.823 0.610
BC2 0.786 0.794 0.816
BC3 0.841 0.845 0.840
NB1 0.935 0.892 0.933 0.823 0.882 0.881 0.927 0.809 0.916 0.894 0.934 0.825
NB2 0.929 0.926 0.932
NB3 0.855 0.890 0.876
PI1 0.954 0.958 0.973 0.922 0.922 0.916 0.947 0.856 0.943 0.944 0.964 0.899
PI2 0.968 0.941 0.959
PI3 0.958 0.913 0.943

4.6.  Multi-group analysis 5.  Discussion


Finally, we tested whether there is a significant 5.1.  Predictors of attitude toward PBMA
difference between Canada and Kuwait regarding
This study attempted to explore the acceptance
the intention to buy plant-based meat alternatives.
and purchase intentions of PBMA in Kuwait and
The results show that significant differences were
Canada. Besides the TPB framework, the research
only found for attitudes and behavioral control,
model tested the effects of relevant attitudes
otherwise, all other differences were not signifi-
(health consciousness, environmental concerns
cant, implying that H13 cannot be accepted at the
and animal welfare) as well as food choice factors
traditional 0.05 significance level. This result might
(health, sustainability, cruelty free, and sensory
indicate that the plant-based meat alternatives are
appeal) and meat attachment and food neophobia
perceived quite similarly in Canada and Kuwait.
Table 2.  Discriminant validity—Heterotrait-Monotrait ratio (HTMT)—Overall sample.
HC EC AW S H CF SA MA FN A BC NB PI
Health Consciousness (HC)
Environmental Concerns (EC) 0.595
Animal Welfare (AW) 0.561 0.741
Sustainability (S) 0.491 0.519 0.558
Health (H) 0.647 0.490 0.522 0.717
Cruelty Free (CF) 0.459 0.548 0.713 0.837 0.646
Sensory Appeal (SA) 0.452 0.459 0.604 0.521 0.672 0.531
Meat Attachment (MA) 0.161 0.155 0.162 0.163 0.177 0.160 0.270
Food Neophobia (FN) 0.370 0.310 0.321 0.253 0.337 0.266 0.260 0.224
Attitude (A) 0.134 0.348 0.249 0.290 0.148 0.335 0.125 0.393 0.252
Behavioral Control (BC) 0.308 0.226 0.153 0.222 0.313 0.219 0.138 0.113 0.406 0.361
Normative Beliefs (NB) 0.066 0.100 0.069 0.266 0.103 0.194 0.042 0.145 0.142 0.283 0.189
Purchase Intentions (PI) 0.204 0.330 0.279 0.411 0.289 0.376 0.160 0.412 0.285 0.720 0.428 0.427
Journal of International Consumer Marketing
9
10 Y. BAKR ET AL.

Table 3.  Direct relationships: path coefficient estimates, significance, and hypotheses results.
Canada Kuwait Overall sample
Hypotheses
β T β T β T results
H1. Health consciousness → attitude −0.068 −0.953 −0.095 −1.305 −0.104 2.165 Rejected
H2. Environmental concerns → attitude 0.318 4.536 0.210 3.343 0.202 4.314 Confirmed
H3. Animal welfare → attitude −0.020 −0.292 −0.125 −1.693 −0.047 −0.909 Rejected
H4. Health → attitude −0.168 −2.178 −0.015 −0.168 −0.105 −2.002 Rejected
H5. Sustainability → attitude −0.043 −0.534 0.027 0.306 0.080 1.528 Rejected
H6. Cruelty free → attitude 0.163 2.281 0.185 2.408 0.140 2.853 Confirmed
H7. Sensory appeal → attitude 0.069 0.856 0.057 0.569 0.075 1.599 Rejected
H8. Meat attachment → attitude −0.353 −6.785 −0.348 −7.376 −0.360 −10.770 Confirmed
H9. Food neophobia → attitude −0.248 −4.334 −0.161 −3.159 −0.201 −4.947 Confirmed
H10. Attitude -> purchase intentions 0.648 16.275 0.452 9.239 0.562 17.468 Confirmed
H11. Normative beliefs -> purchase 0.183 4.012 0.184 3.212 0.224 6.547 Confirmed
intentions
H12. Behavioral control -> purchase 0.096 2.560 0.287 5.945 0.157 4.910 Confirmed
intentions

Figure 2.  Path diagram of significant relationships in the structural model—overall sample. Dashed lines indicate non-significant
coefficients.

on attitudes toward PBMA. Results revealed that with previous research on meat attachment and
Meat Attachment had the strongest significant its effect on willingness for substitution (Profeta
effect on Attitudes toward PBMA (β = −0.360 et  al. 2021; Wang and Scrimgeour 2021; Bryant
in overall sample) where the relationship was et  al. 2020). In general, meat has a strong pres-
negative. Similar results were reported separately ence in both Arab and Western cuisines (Hamdan
in Canada and Kuwait at β values of −0.353 and et  al. 2021). Meat is perceived as a source of plea-
−0.348 respectively. These results are consistent sure and meat consumption is associated with a
Journal of International Consumer Marketing 11

sense of entitlement and perceived as an “unques- (β = 0.140 in the overall sample). This empha-
tionable right” (Graça et  al. 2015). Therefore, sizes the proposition that attitude toward meat
this result is not surprising. Similarly, the pres- consumption is an ambivalent attitude (Berndsen
ent results report a significant negative effect of and van der Pligt 2004) where consumers don’t
Food Neophobia on Attitudes toward PBMA (β = perceive eating meat as harmful to animal wel-
−0.201 in the overall sample). Food neophobia is fare, yet their attitude toward PBMA tends to
generally associated with willingness to try new be—in part—predicted by considering cruelty
experiences as well as consumption of vegetables free as a food choice factor.
and healthier diets (Forestell, Spaeth, and Kane
2012). Individuals with a high food neophobia
5.2.  Theory of planned behavior and purchase
score tend to consume less fruits and vegetables
intention of PBMA
as reported by Costa, Silva, and Oliveira (2020),
which explains the results of the present study. The results of the study strongly emphasize the
This study highlights the effect of concerns applicability of the Theory of Planned Behavior
about the environment as an altruistic motive in predicting willingness to buy PBMA. Results
to the adoption of PBMA. The results report show that Attitude toward PBMA is the strongest
that Environmental Concerns had a significant predictor of Purchase Intentions (β = 0.562 in the
positive effect on Attitudes toward PBMA overall sample). This matches previous studies
(β = 0.202 in the overall sample). This result is where attitudes had a positive significant effect
consistent with previous studies related to the on purchase intentions of plant-based yogurt
adoption of more sustainable products, includ- alternatives (Pandey, Ritz, and Perez-Cueto 2021),
ing green and plant-based foods (Profeta et  al. and on following a plant-based diet (Wang and
2021; Michel et  al. 2021; Mostafa 2007). Despite Scrimgeour 2021). Our results also show a sig-
that, it appears that Sustainability is not yet a nificant positive relationship between Subjective
major decisive factor for food choice where the Norms and Purchase Intentions, which supports
relationship between Sustainability as a food previous research on the effect of subjective
choice factor and Attitudes toward PBMA was norms on food choices and dietary behaviors (de
not significant. Surprisingly, other motives like Lanauze and Sirieix 2022; Contini et  al. 2020).
Health Consciousness and Animal Welfare had Finally, our results report a positive relationship
a negative effect on Attitudes toward PBMA at between Behavioral Control and Attitude toward
a nonsignificant level. The negative relationship PBMA, where the study particularly addressed
between Health Consciousness and Attitude PBMA availability, affordability, and cooking abil-
infer that in both Canada and Kuwait, the sam- ity. These results also match results reported by
ples may be perceiving meat as a healthier alter- previous studies (Wang and Scrimgeour 2021;
native and a main source of dietary protein, Pandey, Ritz, and Perez-Cueto 2021). When
when compared to PBMA. This is consistent assessing differences between the Canadian and
with what Haverstock and Forgays (2012) Kuwaiti samples, Subjective norms had a slightly
reported about health concerns being one of stronger influence on Attitudes in Kuwait versus
the reasons why former vegetarians brought Canada, which supports results reported by Park
back meat into their diet. Similarly, the negative (2000) that lower scores in individualism are
(nonsignificant) relationship between Animal associated with stronger effect of subjective norms
Welfare and Attitudes may be explained by meat on behavior, despite that our multi-group analysis
consumers having a sense of entitlement to eat- revealed that the difference between both coun-
ing meat, and perceiving animal slaughter as a tries did not reach a significant level. On the
natural part of an animal’s purpose in life other hand, multi-group analysis revealed that
(Bilewicz, Imhoff, and Drogosz 2011; Rothgerber there is a significant difference in the relationship
2013). Despite that, results report that Cruelty between Attitudes and Intentions, and Behavioral
Free as a food choice factor had a significant Control and Intentions between Canada and
positive effect on Attitude toward PBMA Kuwait, where in Canada there was a relatively
12 Y. BAKR ET AL.

higher effect of Attitude on Purchase Intention time. Yet, the results imply that consumers tend
and in Kuwait there was a relatively higher influ- to perceive PBMA as a less healthy alternative
ence of Behavioral Control on Purchase Intentions. than Meat which can act as a barrier to the adop-
This matches evidence reported by Hassan, Shiu, tion of PBMA in the future. This calls for increas-
and Parry (2016), that cross-country variations ing awareness about the negative health effects of
exist in the effect of Attitude and Perceived exaggerated meat consumption, which can also
Behavioral control on Purchase Intentions. help with reducing consumer attachment to meat.
Most importantly, the significant effect of behav-
ioral control on purchase intentions calls for
6.  Implications and recommendations increasing awareness of PBMA and supplementing
the market with the necessary information to
6.1.  Marketing Implications
increase consumers’ self-efficacy to purchase these
Despite that results show an above average accep- products, including making PBMA more available
tance of PBMA in both Kuwait and Canada, and more affordable to the average consumer.
there seems to be barriers to the acceptance of
plant-based meat substitutes. With the rise in the
6.2.  Theoretical implications and future research
number of vegans and vegetarians in the MENA
region, and with the increase in the availability The present study has a number of theoretical and
of PBMA in the markets, it is crucial to under- research implications. First, the study addresses a
stand how consumers in Arab countries perceive research gap which is the lack of research on the
these products and what factors predict their acceptance of Arab consumers of more sustainable
acceptance. One major barrier is meat attach- food choices like PBMA. Previous studies on the
ment, whose roots are deeply embedded into the region have focused on organic and green foods
food culture in both Arab and Western countries. as well as agri-food products (Timpanaro et  al.
Therefore, marketing efforts should be directed 2022; Mostafa 2007) and—to our knowledge—
toward presenting PBMA as an equivalent substi- there are no studies exploring the acceptance of
tute to meat in terms of sensory appeal, cooking PBMA as a sustainable food choice in the Arab
methods, as well as eating rituals, which can con- region, which makes this study novel. The results
vince consumers with high meat attachment to of the present study support the cultural relevance
view these products as a satisfactory substitute to of the TPB framework, the meat attachment and
meat. Another major barrier to the adoption of the food neophobia scales, and the applicabil-
PBMA is subjective norms. Unfortunately, since ity of our research model to different cultures.
meat consumption is considered as the accepted Despite that food is heavily moderated by culture,
food norm in society, this may hinder efforts the present study infers that the acceptance of
for convincing the larger segment of meat eat- PBMA as well as the predictors of attitude toward
ers to substitute meat with PBMA. Accordingly, PBMA seem to be universal in nature, since the
marketers need to capitalize on the minority multi-group analysis did not reveal significant
influence. In this situation, using celebrities and differences between Kuwait and Canada across
well-known influencers can act as a catalyst for the majority of the model relationships.
attitude change. Future research can assess the applicability of
As for motivators, more efforts should be the model on other countries in the MENA
directed toward educating consumers about the region, with more focus on how subcultural fac-
environmental effects of meat production versus tors like religion would affect the acceptance of
plant-based alternatives to capitalize on the effect PBMA. The results also reveal a number of find-
of environmental concerns on attitudes toward ings that need further exploration. The negative
PBMA. It seems that for meat eaters, other relationship between Health Consciousness and
motives like Animal Welfare and Health Attitudes toward PBMA calls for further research
Consciousness do not seem to have a significant on how consumers perceive PBMA from a health
effect on attitudes toward PBMA at the present perspective, in comparison to meat. The
Journal of International Consumer Marketing 13

significant relationship between subjective norms Consent to participate


and purchase intentions also calls for future All participants involved in this research have provided
research on the effect of minority influence when informed consent as part of filling out the questionnaire.
it comes to the adoption of PBMA, in compar-
ison to the majority influence of society’s norms.
Consent to publish
All authors approved the final version of the manuscript
7.  Limitations and consent to publish in the JICM.
Despite that the use of survey panels have advan-
tages including cost effectiveness, fast data col- Disclosure statement
lection, and international reach (Hays, Liu, and
The authors have no relevant financial or non-financial
Kapteyn 2015), it restricted the samples to the interests to disclose.
internet population, which is a limitation of this
study. Another limitation is the use of scales that
were originally developed for English-speaking References
populations when conducting research in an Aidaros, H. 2014. Drivers for animal welfare policies in the
Arabic speaking country like Kuwait. However, Middle East. Revue Scientifique et Technique (International
given the scarcity of research on the MENA Office of Epizootics) 33 (1):85–9. 10.20506/rst.33.1.2262.
region, this choice was necessary, and to ensure Ajzen, I. 1991. The theory of planned behavior.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes
the scales validity and reliability, back-to-back
50 (2):179–211. doi: 10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T.
translation and pretesting were employed. Ajzen, I. 2006. Constructing a theory of planned behavior
Conducting a cross-sectional study can also be questionnaire. https://people.umass.edu/aizen/pdf/tpb.
a potential limitation, especially when assess- measurement.pdf.
ing attitudes toward foods. Some foods have an Ajzen, I. 2011. The theory of planned behavior: Reactions
acquired taste, and food neophobia decrease and and reflections. Psychology & Health 26 (9):1113–27.
10.1080/08870446.2011.613995.
attitudes improve as familiarity with the foods
Aschemann-Witzel, J., R. F. Gantriis, P. Fraga, and F. J. A.
increase over time. This means that a longitudinal Perez-Cueto. 2021. Plant-based food and protein trend
study would be recommended for future research. from a business perspective: Markets, consumers, and
the challenges and opportunities in the future. Critical
Reviews in Food Science and Nutrition 61 (18):3119–28.
Authors’ contributions doi: 10.1080/10408398.2020.1793730.
Yousra Bakr: conceptualization, writing original draft; Hayat Berndsen, M., and J. van der Pligt. 2004. Ambivalence to-
Al-Bloushi: methodology and data collection; Mohamed wards meat. Appetite 42 (1):71–8. doi: 10.1016/
Mostafa: data analysis, supervision. All authors participated S0195-6663(03)00119-3.
equally. Bilewicz, M., R. Imhoff, and M. Drogosz. 2011. The hu-
manity of what we eat: Conceptions of human uniqueness
among vegetarians and omnivores. European Journal of
Availability of data Social Psychology 41 (2):201–9. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.766.
Bryant, C., K. Szejda, N. Parekh, V. Deshpande, and
The data that support the findings of this study are openly B. Tse. 2020. A survey of consumer perceptions of
available in 4TU.ResearchData at http://doi.org/10.4121/20067578, plant-based and clean meat in the USA, India, and
http://doi.org/10.4121/20067581, http://doi.org/10.4121/20067584. China. Frontiers in Sustainable Food Systems 4. doi:
Other tables will be provided by the researchers upon request. 10.3389/fsufs.2019.00011.
Chen, M. F. 2009. Attitude toward organic foods among
Taiwanese as related to health consciousness, environ-
Compliance with ethical standards
mental attitudes, and the mediating effects of a healthy
The authors have no potential conflict of interests to dis- lifestyle. British Food Journal 111 (2):165–78. doi:
close. This study has been reviewed and approved by the 10.1108/00070700910931986.
McMaster Research Ethical Board (MREB) at McMaster Choudhury, D., S. Singh, J. S. H. Seah, D. C. L. Yeo, and
University to ensure compliance with the Tri-Council Policy L. P. Tan. 2020. Commercialization of plant-based meat
Statement and the McMaster Policies and Guidelines for alternatives. Trends in Plant Science 25 (11):1055–8.
Research Involving Human Participants. 10.1016/j.tplants.2020.08.006.
14 Y. BAKR ET AL.

Circus, V. E., and R. Robison. 2019. Exploring perceptions Francis, J., M. P. Eccles, M. Johnston, A. E. Walker, J. M.
of sustainable proteins and meat attachment. British Food Grimshaw, R. Foy, E. F. S. Kaner, L. Smith, and D. Bonett.
Journal 121 (2):533–45. doi: 10.1108/BFJ-01-2018-0025. 2004. Constructing questionnaires based on the theory
Contini, C., F. Boncinelli, E. Marone, G. Scozzafava, and of planned behavior: A manual for health services re-
L. Casini. 2020. Drivers of plant-based convenience foods searchers. Centre for Health Services Research, University
consumption: Results of a multicomponent extension of of Newcastle upon Tyne. https://openaccess.city.ac.uk/id/
the theory of planned behavior. Food Quality and eprint/1735/1/TPB%20Manual%20FINAL%20May2004.pdf.
Preference 84:103931. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2020.103931. Fuller, M., M. Simmering, G. Atinc, Y. Atinc, and B. Babin.
Costa, A., C. Silva, and A. Oliveira. 2020. Food neophobia 2016. Common method variance detection in business
and its association with food preferences and dietary research. Journal of Business Research 69 (8):3192–8. doi:
intake of adults. Nutrition & Dietetics: The Journal of the 10.1016/j.jbusres.2015.12.008.
Dietitians Association of Australia 77 (5):542–9. GDN Online. 2017. Kuwait: Kuwait 2nd most meat con-
10.1111/1747-0080.12587. suming country in the world. Gulf Daily News. May 15.
Davitt, E. D., D. M. Winham, M. M. Heer, M. C. Shelley, Accessed January 12, 2022. https://www.gdnonline.com/
and S. T. Knoblauch. 2021. Predictors of plant-based Details/204941/Kuwait-2nd-most-mea
alternatives to meat consumption in midwest university t-consuming-country-in-the-world.
students. Journal of Nutrition Education and Behavior 53 Ghaffari, M., P. G. K. Rodrigo, Y. Ekinci, and G. Pino. 2022.
(7):564–72. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2021.04.459. Consumers’ motivations for adopting a vegan diet: A
De Backer, C. J. S., and L. Hudders. 2015. Meat morals: mixed-methods approach. International Journal of
Relationship between meat consumption consumer atti- Consumer Studies 46 (4):1193–208. doi: 10.1111/ijcs.12752.
tudes towards human and animal welfare and moral be- Gould, S. J. 1990. Health consciousness and health behav-
havior. Meat Science 99:68–74. 10.1016/j.meats- ior: The application of a new health consciousness scale.
ci.2014.08.011. American Journal of Preventive Medicine 6 (4):228–37.
de Lanauze, G. S., and L. Sirieix. 2022. Impact of social doi: 10.1016/S0749-3797(18)31009-2.
influences and adoptive community on behaviors: An Graça, J., M. M. Calheiros, and A. Oliveira. 2015. Attached
exploratory study of young French vegetarians. to meat? (Un)Willingness and intentions to adopt a more
International Journal of Consumer Studies 46 (2):419–33. plant-based diet. Appetite 95:113–25. 10.1016/j.ap-
doi: 10.1111/ijcs.12689. pet.2015.06.024.
Djekic, I., E. Bartkiene, V. Szűcs, M. Tarcea, I. Klarin, M. Hamdan, M. N., M. A. Ramli, N. M. Huri, N. N. H. Abd
Černelić-Bizjak, K. Isoldi, A. El-Kenawy, V. Ferreira, D. Rahman, and A. Abdullah. 2021. Will Muslim consumers
Klava, et  al. 2021. Cultural dimensions associated with replace livestock slaughter with cultured meat in the
food choice: A survey based multi-country study. market? Trends in Food Science & Technology 109:729–32.
International Journal of Gastronomy and Food Science doi: 10.1016/j.tifs.2021.01.034.
26:100414. doi: 10.1016/j.ijgfs.2021.100414. Hassan, L. M., E. Shiu, and S. Parry. 2016. Addressing the
Dunlap, R. E., and R. E. Jones. 2002. Environmental con- cross-country applicability of the theory of planned be-
cern: Conceptual and measurement issues. In Handbook havior (TPB): A structured review of multi-country TPB
of environmental sociology, 482–524. Westport CT: studies. Journal of Consumer Behaviour 15 (1):72–86. doi:
Greenwood Press. 10.1002/cb.1536.
Espinosa, R., and N. Treich. 2021. Animal welfare: Haverstock, K., and D. K. Forgays. 2012. To eat or not to
Antispeciesism, veganism and a “life worth living”. Social eat. A comparison of current and former animal product
Choice and Welfare 56 (3):531–48. doi: 10.1007/ limiters. Appetite 58 (3):1030–6. 10.1016/j.ap-
s00355-020-01287-7. pet.2012.02.048.
Estell, M., J. Hughes, and S. Grafenauer. 2021. Plant protein Hays, R. D., H. Liu, and A. Kapteyn. 2015. Use of Internet
and plant-based meat alternatives: Consumer and nutri- panels to conduct surveys. Behavior Research Methods 47
tion professional attitudes and perceptions. Sustainability (3):685–90. 10.3758/s13428-015-0617-9.
13 (3):1478. doi: 10.3390/su13031478. Hofstede, G. 2001. Culture’s consequences: Comparing values,
Fishbein, M., and I. Ajzen. 2009. Predicting and changing behaviors, institutions, and organizations across nations.
behavior: The reasoned action approach. New York: Taylor Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
and Francis. Hwang, J., J. You, J. Moon, and J. Jeong. 2020. Factors
Forestell, C. A., A. M. Spaeth, and S. A. Kane. 2012. To affecting consumers’ alternative meats buying intentions:
eat or not to eat red meat. A closer look at the relation- Plant-based meat alternative and cultured meat.
ship between restrained eating and vegetarianism in col- Sustainability 12 (14):5662. doi: 10.3390/su12145662.
lege females. Appetite 58 (1):319–25. 10.1016/j.ap- Iversen, A. C., and P. Kraft. 2006. Does socio-economic
pet.2011.10.015. status and health consciousness influence how women
Fox, N., and K. Ward. 2008. Health, ethics and environment: respond to health-related messages in media? Health
A qualitative study of vegetarian motivations. Appetite Education Research 21 (5):601–10. 601-610. 10.1093/her/
50 (2–3):422–9. 10.1016/j.appet.2007.09.007. cyl014.
Journal of International Consumer Marketing 15

Izmirli, S., and C. J. C. Phillips. 2011. The relationship Park, H. S. 2000. Relationships among attitudes and sub-
between student consumption of animal products and jective norms: Testing the theory of reasoned action
attitudes to animals in Europe and Asia. British Food across cultures. Communication Studies 51 (2):162–75.
Journal 113 (3):436–50. doi: 10.1108/00070701111116482. doi: 10.1080/10510970009388516.
Kemper, J. A. 2020. Motivations, barriers, and strategies for Phillips, C. J., S. Izmirli, S. J. Aldavood, M. Alonso, B. I.
meat reduction at different family lifecycle stages. Appetite Choe, A. Hanlon, A. Handziska, G. Illmann, L. Keeling,
150:104644. 10.1016/j.appet.2020.104644. M. Kennedy, et  al. 2012. Students’ attitudes to animal
Kock, F., A. Berbekova, and A. Assaf. 2021. Understanding welfare and rights in Europe and Asia. Animal Welfare
and managing the threat of common method bias: 21 (1):87–100. doi: 10.7120/096272812799129466.
Detection, prevention and control. Tourism Management Phua, J., S. V. Jin, and J. Kim. 2020. Pro-veganism on
86:104330. doi: 10.1016/j.tourman.2021.104330. Instagram: Effects of user-generated content (UGC) types
Lähteenmäki, L., and A. Arvola. 2010. Food neophobia and and content generator types in Instagram-based health
variety seeking: Consumer fear or demand for new food marketing communication about veganism. Online
products. In Food, people and society: A European per- Information Review 44 (3):685–704. doi: 10.1108/
spective of consumers’ food choices, ed. L. J. Frewer, E. OIR-06-2019-0213.
Risvik, and H. Schifferstein, 161–75. Heidelberg: Springer. Pliner, P., and K. Hobden. 1992. Development of a scale to
Latif, K., A. Perez, and U. Sahibzada. 2020. Corporate so- measure the trait of food neophobia in humans. Appetite,
cial responsibility (CSR) and customer loyalty in the 19 (2):105–20. doi: 10.1016/0195-6663(92)90014-W.
hotel industry: A cross-country study. International Ploll, U., and T. Stern. 2020. From diet to behavior:
Journal of Hospitality Management 89:102565. doi: Exploring environmental- and animal-conscious behavior
10.1016/j.ijhm.2020.102565. among Austrian vegetarians and vegans. British Food
Lindeman, M., and M. Väänänen. 2000. Measurement of Journal 122 (11):3249–65. doi: 10.1108/BFJ-06-2019-0418.
ethical food choice motives. Appetite 34 (1):55–9. 10.1006/ Podsakoff, P., S. MacKenzie, L. Jeong-Yeon, and N.
appe.1999.0293. Podsakoff. 2003. Common method biases in behavioral
MacLeod, M., P. Gerber, A. Mottet, G. Tempio, A. Falcucci, research: A critical review of the literature and recom-
C. Opio, T. Vellinga, B. Henderson, and H. Steinfeld. 2013. mended remedies. The Journal of Applied Psychology 88
Greenhouse gas emissions from pig and chicken supply (5):879–903. 10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879.
chains: A global life cycle assessment. Food and Agriculture Profeta, A., M. C. Baune, S. Smetana, S. Bornkessel, K.
Organization of the United Nations. Accessed November Broucke, G. Van Royen, U. Enneking, J. Weiss, V. Heinz,
22, 2021. https://www.fao.org/3/i3460e/i3460e.pdf. S. Hieke, et  al. 2021. Preferences of German consumers
Michel, F., A. Knaapila, C. Hartmann, and M. Siegrist. 2021. for meat products blended with plant-based proteins.
A multi-national comparison of meat eaters’ attitudes Sustainability 13 (2):650. doi: 10.3390/su13020650.
and expectations for burgers containing beef, pea or al- R Development Core Team. 2021. R: A language and envi-
gae protein. Food Quality and Preference 91:104195. doi: ronment for statistical computing. R Foundation for
10.1016/j.foodqual.2021.104195. Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria. www.R-project.
Miguel, I., A. Coelho, and C. M. Bairrada. 2020. Modelling org.
attitude towards consumption of vegan products. Rahman, S., and H. Luomala. 2021. Demystifying horizon-
Sustainability 13 (1):9. doi: 10.3390/su13010009. tal/vertical cultural difference in green consumption: A
Montanari, M. 2006. Food is culture. Trans. A. Sonnenfeld. cross-cultural comparative study. Journal of International
New York: Columbia University Press. C o n s um e r Mar k e t ing 3 3 ( 5 ) : 5 4 3 – 5 8 . d oi :
Mordor Intelligence. 2020. MEA plant-based meat and 10.1080/08961530.2020.1857669.
dairy products market | 2021–26 | Industry share, size, Reinberg, S. 2021. How healthy are the new plant-based
growth. Mordor Intelligence. Accessed January 13 2022. ‘fake meats’? WebMD. June 17. Accessed January 10, 2022.
https://www.mordorintelligence.com/industry-reports/ https://www.webmd.com/diet/news/20210617/how-health
middle-east-and-africa-plant-based-meat-and-dairy-prod- y-are-the-new-plant-based-fake-meats#1.
ucts-industry. Reipurth, M. F. S., L. Hørby, C. G. Gregersen, A. Bonke,
Mostafa, M. M. 2007. A hierarchical analysis of the green and F. J. A. P. Cueto. 2019. Barriers and facilitators to-
consciousness of the egyptian consumer. Psychology and wards adopting a more plant-based diet in a sample of
Marketing 24 (5):445–73. doi: 10.1002/mar.20168. Danish consumers. Food Quality and Preference 73:288–
Pandey, S., C. Ritz, and F. J. A. Perez-Cueto. 2021. An 92. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2018.10.012.
application of the theory of planned behavior to predict Rondoni, A., C. Grebitus, E. Millan, and D. Asioli. 2021.
intention to consume plant-based yogurt alternatives. Exploring consumers’ perceptions of plant-based eggs
Foods 10 (1):148. doi: 10.3390/foods10010148. using concept mapping and semantic network analysis.
Papista, E., and A. Krystallis. 2013. Investigating the types Food Quality and Preference 94:104327. doi: 10.1016/j.
of value and cost of green brands: Proposition of a con- foodqual.2021.104327.
ceptual framework. Journal of Business Ethics 115 (1):75– Rothgerber, H. 2013. Real men don’t eat (vegetable) quiche:
92. doi: 10.1007/s10551-012-1367-6. Masculinity and the justification of meat consumption.
16 Y. BAKR ET AL.

Psychology of Men & Masculinity 14 (4):363–75. doi: The Good Food Institute. 2019b. Plant-based meat for a
10.1037/a0030379. growing world. The Good Food Institute. Accessed January
Royne, M. B., A. K. Fox, G. D. Deitz, and T. Gibson. 10, 2022. https://gfi.org/wp-content/uploads/2021/02/
2014. The effects of health consciousness and familiarity GFI-Plant-Based-Meat-Fact-Sheet_Environmental-
with DTCA on perceptions of dietary supplements. The Comparison.pdf.
Journal of Consumer Affairs 48 (3):515–34. doi: 10.1111/ The World Counts. n.d. World consumption of meat. The
joca.12051. World Counts. Accessed January 10, 2022. https://www.
Sentient Media. 2021. Increase in veganism: Why is veg- theworldcounts.com/challenges/consumption/
anism on the rise in 2021? Sentient Media. April 28. foods-and-beverages/world-consumption-of-meat/story.
Accessed January 12, 2022. https://sentientmedia.org/ Timpanaro, G., P. Guarnaccia, S. Zingale, V. T. Foti, and
increase-in-veganism/. A. Scuderi. 2022. The sustainability role in the purchas-
Seo, S., K. Kim, and J. Jang. 2018. Uncertainty avoidance ing choice of agri-food products in the United Arab
as a moderator for influences on foreign resident dining Emirates and Italy. AIMS Agriculture and Food 7 (2):212–
out behaviors. International Journal of Contemporary 40. doi: 10.3934/agrfood.2022014.
Hospitality Management 30 (2):900–18. doi: 10.1108/ Wang, O., and F. Scrimgeour. 2021. Willingness to adopt a
IJCHM-03-2016-0152. more plant-based diet in China and New Zealand:
Simons, J., C. Vierboom, J. Klink-Lehmann, I. Härlen, and Applying the theories of planned behavior, meat attach-
M. Hartmann. 2021. Vegetarianism/veganism: A way to ment and food choice motives. Food Quality and
feel good. Sustainability 13 (7):3618. doi: 10.3390/ Preference 93:104294. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2021.104294.
su13073618. Webber, J. n.d. Demand for vegan meat is growing world-
Statista. 2021. Market revenue of plant-based meat world- wide due to concerns about animal welfare. Livekindly.
wide from 2016 to 2026. Statista. December. Accessed Accessed January 10, 2022. https://www.livekindly.co/
January 11, 2022. https://www-statista-com.libaccess.lib. vegan-meat-animal-welfare/.
mcmaster.ca/outlook/cmo/food/meat/meat-substitutes/ Werts, C., R. Linn, and K. Joreskog. 1974. Interclass reli-
worldwide. ability estimates: Testing structural assumptions.
Steinfeld, H., P. Gerber, T. Wassenaar, V. Castel, M. Rosales, Educational and Psychological Measurement 34 (1):25–33.
and C. de Haan. 2006. Livestock’s long shadow: doi: 10.1177/001316447403400104.
Environmental issues and options. Food and Agriculture Yadav, R., and G. S. Pathak. 2016. Young consumers’ inten-
Organization of the United Nations. Accessed November tion towards buying green products in a developing na-
22, 2021. https://www.fao.org/3/a0701e/a0701e00.pdf. tion: Extending the theory of planned behavior. Journal
Steptoe, A., T. M. Pollard, and J. Wardle. 1995. Development of Cleaner Production 135:732–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jcle-
of a measure of the motives underlying the selection of pro.2016.06.120.
food: The food choice questionnaire. Appetite 25 (3):267– Yazdanpanah, M., and M. Forouzani. 2015. Application of
84. 10.1006/appe.1995.0061. the Theory of Planned Behavior to predict Iranian stu-
Tandon, A., A. Dhir, P. Kaur, S. Kushwah, and J. Salo. 2020. dents’ intention to purchase organic food. Journal of
Behavioral reasoning perspectives on organic food pur- Cleaner Production 107:342–52. doi: 10.1016/j.jcle-
chase. Appetite 154:104786. 10.1016/j.appet.2020.104786. pro.2015.02.071.
The Good Food Institute. 2019a. Environmental ben- Zarrouki, K. 2017. Sector trend analysis: Livestock and meat
efits of plant-based meat products. The Good Food trends in the Gulf Cooperation Council. Agriculture and
Institute. Accessed November 22, 2021. https://gfi. Agri-Food Canada. March. Accessed January 13, 2022.
org/wp-content/uploads/2021/02/GFI-Plant-Based-Mea https://www.agr.gc.ca/resources/prod/Internet-Internet/
t-Fact-Sheet_Environmental-Comparison.pdf. MISB-DGSIM/ATS-SEA/PDF/6873-eng.pdf.

You might also like