You are on page 1of 11

Antm. Behav.

, 1969, 17, 641-651

CLEANING BEHAu IN DROSOPHILA MELANOGASTER


BY ANDREW L. SZEBENYI, S.J.
Biology Department, Le Moyne College, Syracuse, New York 13214

The objective of this paper is to give a descriptive as soon as dust particles stick to them' (p. 85).
account of a behaviour pattern complex in From the author's own observations the
Drosophila melanogaster, generally known as following general statements can be made.
grooming or cleaning behaviour, and to present Cleaning behaviour is performed with the legs
methods which make the descriptive and com- by two different basic movements: the sweeping
parative study of this behaviour complex movements of the legs over the body surface,
possible. and the rubbing motion of the legs along the
Very little is known about grooming or tarsal joints. It has been observed in D. melano-
cleaning behaviour in Diptera, Apart from the gaster on several occasions that removal of
fact that cleaning movements do occur, most macroscopic particles was actually achieved by
other aspects of this behaviour, such as the these movements. A small piece of yeast particle
nature of the eliciting stimulus, spontaneity adhering to the body surface has been seen to
of movements, stereotypy of patterns, the im- be picked up by the sweeping movements, while
portance of sensory feedback during perform- the rubbing that followed caused the particle
ance, and to a large extent even the biological to travel along the tarsal joints and to drop off
significance of the behaviour remained hitherto at the end. The direction of the movement of the
obscure. particle seemed to be determined by the direction
Recently, Connolly (1968) described social of bristles on the legs where they all point away
facilitation of preening behaviour in D. melano- from the body.
gaster and showed that facilitation is probably It is not at all certain, however, that removal
induced through vision, and it is unaffected by of larger particles is the only result of this
physical contact between flies, or by the amount behaviour. It is quite possible that microscopic
of dirt on the body. Another published work on particles adhering to sensory receptors, Or the
cleaning behaviour in Diptera is that of Heinz need for even distribution of wax-coating on the
(1949) describing the cleaning movements in body surface play some role in eliciting the
terms of a number of components, and investi- movements. Further investigation is needed to
gating some of the conditions which may be determine the causes and the effects of this
responsible for releasing or strengthening clean- behaviour.
ing activity. In the same article, Heinz also The behaviour presents a rich source of
reported his observations on cleaning behaviour material for study, and many aspects of it may
in twenty-eight Drosophila mutants, and found become important not only as a mere behaviour
the movements to be identical with those of the study but also as a new source for behaviour-
wild type. It should be noted, however, that the genetic analysis. To investigate these aspects
system of components used by Heinz was rather and possibilities of this widespread and probably
limited and did not extend to more precise highly adaptive behaviour complex, a survey
quantitative aspects of the behaviour which has been initiated. What was sought, first of all,
may account for this statement. was some means of obtaining records which
Apart from these two studies, there are only would make it possible to analyse and compare
a few sporadic references in the literature to the cleaning performance in different individuals
cleaning behaviour in Diptera. Lorenz (1956) and populations in appropriate qualitative and
remarked in a conference on instinct: 'in quantitative terms.
Drosophila wingless mutants are still in full The progressive phases of these preliminary
possession of the normal movements of cleaning investigations were the following: (1) analysis of
wings' (p. 55). Tinbergen (1965) referred to this cleaning behaviour into recurrent components
observation as a possible example of spontaneous and the construction of ethograms based on
behaviour, cautioning however, that the appar- the performance of D. melanogaster wild type
ent absence of external stimuli is n o t in itself flies; (2) discovery of the structural organization
a positive proof of spontaneity. He also main- of the behaviour based on the ethograms and
tained th.at 'flies will always preen their wings on continued direct observations. (Thes~ two

641
642 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 17, 4

phases of the investigation furnished the characteristics of the movements. Next, the
material for the present paper.) (3) Meanwhile a performance is analysed into components, and
dissection of the behaviour into relevant quan- each of these components is given a verbal
titative aspects has been undertaken, and a symbol. The symbols should be sufficiently
method for quick and accurate determination of simple and short since they are to be pronounced
cleaning behaviour in terms of quantitative as fast as the movements occur. The next step is
parameters of populations has been developed. to carry out systematic observations during
This third phase of the investigation is now being which the verbal symbols are recorded on tape.
tested, and the results will be brought to light At this point, one has to be completely familiar
in the near future. with both the movements and their corres-
All these phases are primarily descriptive ponding symbols to avoid lag or confusion
but they lay the foundation for meaningful during the recording. If the movements flow
experimental and comparative study of the into each other, it is sufficient to mark the end of
behaviour. a component by the beginning of the next, and
The present account is based on studies of to say 'stop' at the end of a series of movements.
D. melanogaster wild-type flies which were No recorded symbol after the word 'stop'
obtained at Syracuse University from a popu- means no observable movement. The next
lation cage in Dr Marvin Druger's laboratory. step is to play back the tape and to write down
The cage contains mixed strains derived from the symbols. This gives a written record of
single females caught at various localities in patterns. For more elaborate ethograms the
Syracuse in October 1964. The cage was started duration of each component is to be measured
with 15 males and 15 females of each of 15 from the tape with the aid of a timer. The
strains in 23 December 1964. The flies used for ethogram obtained in this manner gives most
observation and record in this account were of the essential parameters of the behavionr
maintained in culture bottles since November (patterns, duration, frequency) in a form which
1967. They were raised on agar molasses media is easily and directly comparable.
at a temperature of 23~ and at 62 per cent It is found that one can carry out visual
humidity in a constant temperature and humid- observation at different levels of complexity,
ity room. Observations were carried out in the and that the study of some aspects of cleaning
same room. For observation the flies were either behaviour does not require a specially detailed
transferred into clean glass vials or were trans- record. It is, therefore, practical to work out a
ferred into food vials, and a 5-min 'settling down' number of systems of symbols of different com-
period was allowed before each observation or plexity. The selection of the appropriate system
recording. will depend on the nature of the problem at
Ethograms: The Descriptive Method hand. For instance, the study of front versus
back cleaning preference, that is, a type of
Looking at unetherized flies, one is immediately cleaning which is performed with the first pair
struck by the high frequency of occurrence, and
of legs, while the second pair is moved forward
by the complexity and great variety of move- for cleaning, versus a type of cleaning which is
ments of cleaning behaviour. In most instances, performed with the third pair of legs while the
the movements are rapid, and they follow each
second pair is moved back for cleaning can be
other in quick succession which renders detailed described quite adequately by a very simple
observation an exacting task, and also limits the
system of symbols (system I).
precision of direct visual observation. The It should be remarked here that the choice of
alternative to direct visual observation is to symbols is entirely subjective. The symbols are
make use of motion pictures. This latter method, simply useful in converting fly movements into
although excellent for analysis of movements, generalizations for human understanding of the
is not the best to give routine permanent record fly's behaviour, and in making a written record
because it does not render the quantitative
characteristics of the performance immediately possible.
comparable. This can be achieved better by System I
permanent written records or ethograms. For- Cleaning behaviour is performed with the
tunately, there is a simple and inexpensive way legs, and may refer to various body parts and
to obtain such ethograms. The following is to the legs themselves. Thus, the components
recommended: cleaning behaviour is observed can be described by giving a number to the legs
l~n[il one b~om~s familiar with the general involved in cleaning (first, second; and third
SZEBENYI: CLEANING BEHAVIOUR IN DROSOPHILA MELANOGASTER 643
pair), and by giving a letter symbol to the main along the tarsal joints, this component is an
body parts cleaned (H = head, T = thorax, A exception.
: a b d o m e n , and W--= wings). No more than Experience in the use of symbols shows that
three legs may be involved in cleaning activity recording from direct visual observation cannot
at the same time because the fly requires at least go far beyond system I. The following systems
three legs for support. It can often be predicted are more suitable for analysis of motion pictures.
which leg is going to be used for cleaning by System H
observing the preparatory movements of the
supporti~g legs shifting the centre of gravity The description of components is extended to
of the body to the required position. The system finer subdivisions of body parts. The leg cleaning
of symbols is the following. components are the same as in the previous
system. For sake of simplicity, the body clean-
Body cleaning movements (sweeping motions). ing components are not all prefixed by the
1H, any cleaning movement directed toward any number of legs involved in the activity but most
part of the head. The movements are performed of them have their own letter symbol.
with the first pair of legs. 2H, same as above
but the movements are performed with the Head cleaning movements. F, all cleaning
second pair of legs (a rare component). 1T and movements directed towards the front part of
2T, any cleaning movement directed towards the head, antennae, front part of the eyes,
the thorax and performed with the first or second ocelli (while head is bent down), and proboscis.
pair of legs respectively. 3A, any cleaning move- The proboscis is not extended while the region
anent directed toward any part of the abdomen is being cleaned. I, all cleaning movements
and performed with the third pair of legs. 3W, directed towards the lateral sides of the head
wing cleaning movements performed with the and eyes. The head is not turned during these
third pair of legs. 3TW, a symbol representing movements. H, a characteristic head cleaning
the sweeping movement of one of the third pair component. The movements clean the dorsal
of legs over the dorsal side of thorax and wings. and ventral sides of the head at the same time
3HTW, same as above except that the move- while the head is rotated to various degrees to
ment also includes the head. 3T, the initial the right or to the left. P, proboscis cleaning com-
phase of 3TW; the wings are, however, not ponent. The proboscis is extended for cleaning.
touched. The component may appear in itself All these components are performed with the
without going into the 3TW movement. first pair of legs. 2H, a rare component (see
system I).
Leg cleaning movements (rubbing motions). 1, Thorax cleaning movements. 1V, the ventral
the rubbing motion of the first pair of legs.
side of the thorax is cleaned with the first pair
No other legs are involved in this component.
12, the movements include both first legs and of legs. The fly usually takes up a posture at an
angle to the horizontal and balances on four
one member of the second pair. The legs ap-
legs forming three supporting points, one of the
proach each other on one side. 21, the move-
ments include one of the first and one of the second legs being extended parallel with one of
the third legs on the same side. 2T, any cleaning
second pair of legs on one side. 2112, the above
movement directed towards the thorax and
two components flow into each other. The 21
performed with the second pair of legs. One
part is often very short. 32, the movements
include both third legs and one member of the may distinguish between 2TD, 2TS, and 2TV
depending on whether the dorsal, lateral, or
second pair which is moved back for cleaning.
ventral sides of the thorax are being cleaned.
The performance is slightly to one side. The 3T, the dorsal side of the thorax is cleaned with
legs cleaning usually touch the ground and form
additional support for the body. 3, rubbing one of the third pair of legs. The component is
identical with the initial phase of TW move-
movements performed with the third pair of
ments.
legs. No other legs are involved in this com-
ponent. 1C, performed with one of the first Abdomen cleaning movements. D, S, V, and G.
legs cleaning the coxal region of the other These symbols refer to dorsal, lateral, and ventral
member of the first pair. The component is sides of the abdomen, and to genital and anal
associated with the rigid extension of the leg regions ((3) cleaned with the third pair of legs.
cleaned. While the leg cleaning movements The anal plates and the reproductive organs may
l~ormally mean ~h~ rubbin~ motio~ of the le~s or may not be ~xLen0r during cleaning.
644 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 17, 4

Wing cleaning movements. B, the wing base some of the legs. Each drawing is accompanied
is cleaned with one of the third pair of legs which by the symbol of the component it represents
is inserted at the inside angle of the wing. The according to system II.
movements are usually slow and protracted Figure 2 is an example of ethograms of clean-
and are accompanied by slight opening and ing behaviour in the D. melanogaster wild-type
raising of the wing. K, a rather rapid move- studied. The ethogram was obtained by analysis
ment which consists of a sharp kick against the of motion pictures, using the described method.
underside of the wing. The wing is lifted by the The movie was taken at 24 frames per sec, and
movement. VS, SS, DS. The symbols represent was played back at 6 frames per sec. The dur-
the sweeping movements of the third pair of ation of each component was measured at the
legs over the wing surfaces. The ventral sweep low speed, and the true durations were calcu-
(VS) is often not distinguishable from move- lated for the ethogram. The symbols follow
ments cleaning the dorsal side of the abdomen system II. The ethogram is a sample observation
(D). The dorsal sweep (DS) is similar to TW of 280 sec duration of which 4.9 sec were spent
but the movement does not include the thorax. in locomotion, 140.9 sec in cleaning, and 134.2
The lateral or side sweep (SS) refers to the move- sec in motionless state. The performance is
ment along the outer edges of the wings in a presented at three levels: the top level shows
parallel fashion. TW, and HTW, the same com- locomotion, the middle level cleaning, and the
ponents as 3TW and 3HTW in system I. W, the bottom level the rest periods during which no
most characteristic wing cleaning component. observable movement has occurred.
The wing is taken between the two third legs The Problem of Error
which move in a see-saw fashion along its dorsal
In preparing an ethogram, the final written
and ventral surfaces. Several phases can be
record is based on a series of transformations.
distinguished in this component. At an early
In case of direct visual observation, the event is
phase, the wing is still in a near horizontal
first seen, and what is seen is expressed and
position, at the end phase, the wing is vertical.
recorded on tape by verbal symbols. Playing
All these wing cleaning components are per-
back the tape, what is heard is timed and written
formed with the third pair of legs. Various
down. At each step in this series of transform-
modifications of the W component can be
ations from the actual event to the written record
observed clearly in D. melanogaster vestigial
there are a number of sources of error due partly
mutants, and in other wing mutants of this
to the observer and partly to the instruments
species, such as unfolded and pupal. This is
used by the observer, in this case the tape re-
probably the component referred to by Konrad
corder and the timer. If the visual observation is
Lorenz (1956, p. 55).
indirect, that is, if one makes use of motion
System III pictures, additional sources of error are intro-
In addition to systems of symbols used in the duced by the movie camera and the projection
previous account, the problem of symmetry equipment. Thus, the written record is rather far
may be dealt with by using subscripts R, L, removed from the actual event it is supposed to
and P meaning right, left and parallel, depending represent. In order to see how closely the written
on the symmetry of the movements. For instance, record approximates the event, it is necessary
WR, DSP, and 12L would mean cleaning of the to form some ideas about the magnitude and the
right wing according to the W component, direction of errors introduced by the trans-
sweeping movement along the dorsal surfaces formations.
of the wings performed on both sides simultan- It is customary to distinguish between system-
eously, and the performance of the one-two atic and random errors where the former refers
component on the left side. to the accuracy, the latter to the precision of the
Figure 1 shows the most commonly occurring results. It seems that systematic errors inherent
cleaning behaviour components in D. melano- in the equipment are of no real significance
gaster wild-type flies. The drawings, obtained because the comparison of records requires
by tracing single frames of motion pictures, no more than relative accuracy, and because it
are schematic, and in them several structural was found that random fluctuations in the re-
details were omitted to place emphasis on pos- cording and timing instruments were in practice
tures and movements. For the same reason, negligible. The major cause for concern is the
the wings are shown only in outline. The addition error introduced by the observer at the point of
of v~n~tion on the wings would have. obsc_ur~d tra nfforrnatior~ of visual impressions into verbal
9ZEBENYI: C L E A N I N G B E H A V i O U R i N DROSOPHILA MELANOGASTER 64~J

I 12 -21 IC 52

f i ~ .

3 G I H 3T

-
%
P B IV TOP VIEW IV SIDE VIEW

f
< L.
"l iJ

K SS VS W EARLY AND LATE PHASES

Fig. I. The most commonly occurring cleaning behaviour components in D. melanogaster


wild type flies. For the definition of symbols see system II in text,
~46 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 17, 4

I_ ,.oO

oo ~
12
I
I
r

II
o o

Z ~
0 ~ No

O--Z
~Zo
UW
o-0
SZEBENYI: CLEANING BEHAViOUR iN DROSOPHILA MELANOGAST~ER d4~

symbols, and at the point of timing the duration Table I.


of components from the tape. Each of these
transformations had to be tested for the mag- Sequence of 20 Sampleaverage 2or
nitude and direction of error, and for any components of 10 measure- cr - - x 100
cumulative effect. ments of the same
First, the precision of timing was tested. A components (sec)
randomly selected portion of the tape was
1 0-9 0'06 13-3
played back ten times, and each time the dura-
tion of behaviour components was measured with 2 4"8 0.08 3-3
an electric timer which reads in sec and tenths
of sec. To aid the precision of measurements, the 3 1'8 0.06 6.7
sequence of the components was first written 4 1.6 0"07 8.8
down. In this way each component could be
anticipated. To aid accuracy, the bar switch 5 1"2 0'07 11-7
of the timer was eliminated and a sensitive 6 1"8 0"10 11'1
microswitch was installed in its place getting
rid of most of the lag associated with pressing 7 0"6 0"04 13"3
down on the bar switch. The portion of the tape
contained twenty behaviour components of a 8 7-5 0'06 1"6
total duration of approximately 118.5 sec. of 9 1"5 0"11 14'7
cleaning. The components were measured in a
cumulative fashion, that is, the timer was not 10 3.7 0'06 3"2
reset to zero after each measurement but only after 11 8"6 0"07 1-6
all the twenty components had been timed.
From these cumulative data the actual duration 12 4"5 0"15 6'7
of each component was computed by subtraction.
The ten measurements of each component were 13 2'6 0"14 10'8
then processed for sample average, standard 14 14"2 0"2 2'8
deviation, and relative percentage error expressed
2a 15 36'0 0"18 1"0
as = • 100 where N is the average of the ten
x 16 1"1 0'06 10"9
measurements of the same component. The
relative percentage error was calculated to show 17 0"5 0'05 20'0
the relationship between duration of component 18 6"5 0'08 2"5
and error of measurement. The results were
rather encouraging. The range of relative per- 19 1"7 0'09 10'6
centage error was between 1 per cent and 20 20 17"5 0"10 1"1
per cent with an average value close to 8 per
cent. The error is clearly related to duration of
component and not to any cumulative effect of events. The portion of the film contained twenty-
two behaviour components. The average relative
successive measurements. Table I gives the
percentage error was found to be close to 16 per
results of these computations.
The next step was to see the precision of cent, approximately twice as great as in the
transformation of visual observation to verbal previous case. Thus, it may be concluded that
the process of transformation from visual ob-
symbols. This was obtained in the following
manner. Cleaning behaviour was recorded servation to verbal symbols, on the average,
introduces approximately 8 per cent error. This
on film which was then replayed ten times, and
random error was found to be relative to the
each time the components were recorded on
duration of components, and was free of
tape with the aid of verbal symbols. Each in-
dependent recording was then measured for cumulative effects.
duration of components, and the measurements The Organization of Cleaning Behaviour
were subjected to the same statistical analysis as At first sight, as illustrated by the ethogram,
in the previous case. To aid the precision of cleaning behaviour is not only a complex
recording, a number of preruns were made with phenomenon but the observation of its per-
the film to become familiar with the sequence of formance gives the impression of a bewildering
648 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, i?, 4

variety of movements apparently without any of cotoured fabric where at any area in the range
definable system. The comparison of many of patterns a certain colour dominates others.
ethograms and continued direct visual obser- In spite of these ambiguities, there are some
vation, however, begin to reveal some consis- well-definable features of the cleaning bout.
tencies of performance, such as the regular Its beginning is often clear cut. The fly may be
sequence of the two basic types of movements engaged in various kinds of activities when
(sweeping and rubbing) separating the behaviour suddenly cleaning begins with an eruption of
into body and leg cleaning types; the character- movements. During the cleaning performance,
istic shifts of the centre of gravity of the body other behaviour types may intrude, most
prior to a given type of cleaning involving one, commonly walking and motionless state. If care
two or three legs; well-definable postures is taken not to disturb the flies, the motionless
which often accompany some sets of cleaning state becomes more and more predominant over
movements; the relatively high frequency of cleaning, and finally cleaning disappears alto-
occurrence of some components over others; gether. Then cleaning may start again, or some
the stereotyped nature of some movements and other behaviour type takes over. Because of the
patterns; the repetitive character of components intrusion of other behaviours each cleaning bout
and of subdivisions of components. may be subdivided into smaller activity units
These are no more than scattered observa- on the behaviour level.
tions. Nevertheless, they indicate that cleaning As it has been mentioned earlier, cleaning
behaviour in its complexity does have organ- behaviour is performed by two basic types of
ization. The behaviour is found to be built up movements, the sweeping and the rubbing
of simple movements and of the repetition of movements. Based on this distinction, each
these movements forming complexes which are cleaning activity unit may be broken up into
recognized as behaviour components, while the the repeated alternations of the two movement
components themselves are repeated forming types, that is, into a series of alternating body
major structural elements of the behaviour. and leg cleaning complexes. This subdivision of
One of the most fundamental features of this the behaviour is probably artificial. It is sug-
behaviour is its repetitive character. gested that one set of body cleaning movements
During the course of a longer period of time, is followed by one set of leg cleaning movements,
such as during the course of a day, cleaning and they together represent the natural unit
behaviour is being repeatedly performed. Each cycle of cleaning activity. The concept of unit
cleaning bout may be regarded as a complete cycle is consistent with the view that the primary
unit on the behaviour level. It is not known function of the behaviour may be the removal of
whether these bouts are spread out in time foreign material from the body surfaces, and
evenly, having approximately the same duration thus the two movement types are part of the
and the same length of intermission periods same natural unit of cleaning activity. That this
between them, or whether there exists some may be so is indicated by the ethogram in the
variation of behaviour density in time, and if so, 53, 56, 69, 73, 152, 169, 171, 188 and 225 sec
whether such variation is in any way correlated regions. The ethogram was selected to show
with particular times of the day. The random some of the exceptions to this interpretation.
variation of behaviour density cannot be a Front cleaning activity--that is cleaning per-
priori excluded either. In any case, at this higher formed with the first pair of l e g s - i s often pre-
level of organization, the behaviour consists ceded by an initial set of rubbing movements, as
of the repetitive performance of cleaning bouts. can be seen in the ethogram in the 0 to 33,
There is some difficulty about the unambig- 108, 135, 148 and 187 sec regions. In addition,
uous definition of cleaning bouts in time. The the H, W, and VS components often terminate
sources of uncertainty are partly the indefinite the cleaning activity without being followed by
end of the bout, and partly the fact that other the rubbing movements of the legs, as in the
behaviour types, namely, walking, feeding, 16, 33, 103 and 205 sec regions.
mating, motionless state, and so on, often intrude Each unit cycle may be subdivided into be-
during the performance of a cleaning bout. haviour components depending on the leg or
It is more realistic to speak of the dominance legs involved in the cleaning activity, or in the
of a given behaviour type over others at any body part toward which the behaviour is directed.
given period of time in the range of the bouts. In this paper seven leg-cleaning and twenty
The situation is similar to a woven meshwork body cleaning components have been described.
SZEBENYI: CLEANING BEHAVIOUR IN DROSOPHILA MEZANOGASTER 649

A given behaviour component may occur several reduced to the parallel state, :while the motor
times within the same unit cycle, and there may units are one dimensional (Fig. 3).
be several different components in each activity The bilateral character of the components may
type. also appear as movements to the right or to the
The components are made of repeated sets of left of the body part cleaned, as in the most
movements. The leg cleaning components, for typical head cleaning component (H). Here
instance, are built up of a number of repeated the head is rotated to the right or to the left
rubbing movements. The individual rubbing while the first pair of legs carry out two dimen-
movements are similar and they represent the sional sweeping movements over the dorsal
lowest level of organization of leg cleaning be- and ventral surfaces of the head (Fig. 3).
haviour. Since the rubbing motions are mostly Thus it is found that cleaning behaviour in
along a straight line, they may be regarded as D. melanogaster wild-type flies is not a random
one dimensional motor units. aggregate of a great variety of movements, but
The organization of the body cleaning com- the behaviour is organized into a hierarchy of
ponents is more complex because of the bi- repetitive complexes. The dgily cycles of the
lateral symmetry of the body surfaces, and behaviour(?) are composed of cleaning bouts
because the sweeping of surfaces requires two which are broken up by the intrusion of other
dimensional movements. The repetitive char- behaviour types into activity units. Each activity
acter of the behaviour is extended to both of unit is composed of sets of body and leg clean-
these aspects. The most characteristic wing ing movements (unit types) which together
cleaning component (W) may serve as an ex- probably form the natural units of cleaning on
ample. The wing on the right or on the left side the behaviour level, the unit cycles. Each unit
is taken between the third pair of legs near the type consists of sets of behaviour components.
wing base. It is held at a slight angle to the Because of the repetitive nature of the be-
horizontal. The legs then perform one sweeping haviour, the movement complexes associated
movement from base to tip of the wing and at with the components may be classified according
the same time many small sweeping movements to three aspects of repetitive symmetry: bilateral
over the wing surfaces between the inner and alternating, bilateral parallel, and unilateral
outer edges. The motor units are thus two performances. Each of these complexes is built
dimensional. Toward the end of the component, up of repetitions of simple movements along
the wing is bent and is nearly vertical. This set of one or both coordinates of a Cartesian space,
movements is then often repeated on the same forming two- or one-dimensional motor units,
wing several times, and then on the other wing, the fundamental building blocks of the be-
or on alternate wings. Thus the repetition of haviour.
similar elements within the same component Summary
occurs at a number of different levels of organ- A descriptive account of cleaning behaviour in
ization: bilateral repetitions of the entire D. rnelanogaster wild-type is given. The be-
component, unilateral repetitions of sets of haviour complex is analysed into seven leg
movements, and finally the repetitions of two cleaning and twenty body cleaning components.
dimensional simple movements (Fig. 3). A method of obtaining ethograms is presented.
Not all body cleaning components have the The method consists of a series of transform-
same complexity of organization. For instance, ations from event to written record with the aid
the dorsal sweep (DS), another wing cleaning of verbal symbols recorded on tape. Three
component, has three aspects of repetitive systems of symbols of increasing degree of
symmetry: the bilateral alternating, the bilateral complexity are proposed, and their usefulness
parallel and the unilateral repetitive perform- is illustrated by an actual ethogram. The problem
ances. It lacks the two-dimensional motor of error introduced by the series of transform-
units, there being only repeated, smooth, one- ations is discussed. It is found that ethograms
dimensional sweeps over the dorsal surfaces may be constructed by the method with reason-
of the wings (Fig. 3). able degree of precision. The overall average
The side sweep (SS), another wing cleaning relative percentage error is found to be approx-
component, is even simpler. It consists of a few imately 16 per cent, one half of which is caused
times repeated, uninterrupted sweeps along the by the transformation from event to verbal
outer edges of both wings at the same time. Thus record, the other half by the process of timing
the repetitive symmetry of the component is the duration of components from the tape. The
650 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 17, 4
A

[3

W --r

IL ,,~, [3 I c

[35 C

$9
A

H 1
[3
Fig. 3. Diagrammatic representation of the repetitive organization of four
cleaning behaviour components in D. melanogaster wild type flies. The
components are in sequence W, DS, SS, and H. (For the definition of these
symbols see text.) The movement complexes associated with these com-
ponents may be classified according to bilateral repetitive symmetry (A),
unilateral repetitive symmetry (B), and bilateral parallel symmetry (C).
The components are built up of repetition of simple movements performed
along the two axes of a Cartesian space (two-dimensional motor units), or
along a single line (one-dimensional motor units). The vertical arrows re.
present the direction of the main movement, while the double-headed
arrows indicate the direction of small movements superimposed upon the
main movement. The broken arrows show the direction of movement of
body parts cleaned, as in the case of the H component.
SZEBENYI: CLEANING BEHAVIOUR IN DROSOPHILA MELANOGASTER 651

magnitude of this error is dependent on the of the Sigma Xi for a generous research grant
duration of components, and is free of cumula- which made this work possible.
tive effects. REFERENCES
The organization of Cleaning behaviour is Connolly, K. (1968). The social facilitation of preening
discussed. The behaviour is not a random aggre- behaviour in Drosophila melanogaster. Anita.
gate of a great variety of movements, but it is Behav., 16, 385-391.
organized into a hierarchy of repetitive com- Heinz, I-I-J. (1949). Vergleichende Beobachtungen fiber
die Putzhandlungen bei Dil3teren im allgemeinen
plexes from the daily cycles down to the smallest und bei Sarcophaga carnaria L. im besonderen.
building blocks of the behaviour, the m o t o r Z. TierpsychoL, 6, 330-371.
units. One of the most fundamental features of Lorenz, K. (1956). The objectivistic~theory of instinct.
In L'instinct clans le comportement des animaux
this behaviour is its repetitive character. et de l'homme (ed. P. P. Grassr). Paris: Masson
Acknowledgments et Cie.
Tinbergen, N. (1965). Animal Behavior, pp 85-86.
The author would like to express his sincere New York: Life Nature Library, Time Inc.
gratitude to D r M. Druger and to G. Smith for (Received 3 July 1968; revised 2 February 1969;
their professional assistance and to the Society Ms. number: A724)

You might also like