You are on page 1of 16

The Interplay of Self-Control and Peer

Influence in Filipino Delinquency


1*
Dan Jerome Barrera, 2 Dante Capistrano, 3 Amabelle T. Feliciano, 4 Anna Marie C. Alaban,
5
Haiasi Mariam Agusin, 6 Nathaniel G. Locsin, 7 Jiemark G. Villabeto

ABSTRACT

Gottfredson and Hirschi (1990) argued that self-control is the individual level cause
of crime and delinquency and that it will render the relationship between differential
association and delinquency spurious. Contrary to these claims, previous research
reveals that self-control cannot diminish the effects of differential association on
delinquency as both have comparable effects and are even found complementary.
These issues, however, are still unexamined in the Philippines. Thus, this study
explores the relationships of self-control, differential association, and delinquency in a
sample of Filipino college students in Dumaguete City, Philippines. Results reveal that
self-control and differential association have direct effects on delinquency, but self-
control does not render differential association-delinquency relationship spurious. In
fact, differential association mediates the effects of self-control on delinquency. These
results suggest that self-control and differential association are complementary, thus
indicating that integration of these concepts might be a better way of modeling Filipino
delinquency.

Keywords: Self-control, differential association, delinquency, college students,


Dumaguete City, Philippines.

INTRODUCTION spurious as both are deemed to be the results of


low self-control (Gottfredson & Hirschi, 1990).
Do individuals who have locked with These and other tangential issues are
delinquents become deviants, or do they lock important in criminology that they have been the
with delinquents because they are already subject of debate between social learning and
deviants themselves? Two of the theories that have self-control theory camps (Akers, 1996; Akers,
competing answers to these questions are social 2008; Gottfredson & Hirschi, 1990; Hirschi, 1996;
learning (Akers, 2009) and self-control theories of Hirschi & Gottfredson, 2008). Aside from this,
delinquency (Gottfredson & Hirschi, 1990). Social these propositions have been empirically tested by
learning theory posits that criminal behavior is researchers. Some writers (e.g. Evans et al., 1997)
learned through peer networks. On the other found support for the propositions of self-control
hand, self-control theory argues that self-control theory that individuals who have low self-control
is the cause of both crime and gravitation to and are already delinquents tend to self-select
delinquent peers. Thus, the relationship between delinquent peers, while some did not (e.g. Huang
having delinquent peers and delinquency is likely & Akers, 2003; Young, 2011). However, some

1,2,3,4,5,6,7
College of Criminal Justice Education, Negros Oriental State University Prism, Volume 20 Issue 2, (July - Dec. 2015)
Main Campus II, Brgy. Bajumpandan, Dumaguete City, 6200, Philippines
*danjerome.barrera@norsu.edu.ph
70 The Interplay 0f Self-Control

researchers found that both theories are correct when they are with delinquent peers. The rest
– persons with low self-control tend to lock with of the paper discusses the literature on social
those who have low self-control and delinquent, learning and self-control theories of delinquency
but self-control could not account for all of the and the results of the current study.
criminogenic effects of delinquent peers on
delinquency (Chapple, 2005; McGloin&Shermer, Social learning theory of delinquency
2009; Wright, Caspi, Mofitt & Silva, 1999; Wright,
Caspi& Mofitt, 2001). Social learning theory is an elaboration
However, these studies were conducted of Sutherland’s (1947) differential association
in Western countries, mostly in the US. The theory, which argues that criminal behavior is
issue is still unexamined in the Philippines. The learned. Akers (2009) elaborated the theory by
Philippines provides a unique context in testing adding concepts on operant conditioning which
the competing propositions of the two theories. posits that behavior is a function of differential
Unlike Westerners, most Filipinos are known to reinforcements – rewards and punishments.
manifest self-control, resilience (katatagang-loob), However, the concept of differential association
non-violence (kawalang-karahasan), prudence together with deinition and imitation remains
(kahinahunan), calmness (kakalmahan) than the core of the theory. Akers (2009) summarizes
anger and violence (Tiangco, 2005). Self-control his theory into the following:
play a signiicant role in Filipinos’ personal and
The probability that persons will engage in
social life. Thus, the interplay of self-control and
criminal and deviant behavior is increased
peer inluence might be different in the Philippine
and the probability of their conforming to the
setting.
norm in decreased when they differentially
In illing this gap, the current study examines
associate with others who commit criminal
the relationship of self-control, differential
behavior and espouse deinitions favorable
association, and delinquency among Filipino
to it, are relatively more exposed in-person
college students in Dumaguete City, Philippines.
or symbolically to salient criminal/deviant
In particular, we speculate that the effect of self-
models, deine it as desirable or justiied in a
control on differential association-delinquency
situation discriminative for the behavior, and
relationship can be modeled in three ways. First,
have received in the past and anticipate in the
the relationship between differential association
current or future situation relatively greater
and delinquency is spurious since both are
reward than punishment for the behavior. (p.
caused by low self-control. In effect, when self-
50)
control is entered simultaneously with differential
association into the model, the effect of the latter Thus, the differential association is
will disappear. Second, the relationship between the context in which other criminogenic
self-control and delinquency is mediated by peer learning processes – imitations, deinition,
deviance. That is, the effect of self-control on and reinforcement – occur. It pertains to the
delinquency is eliminated or attenuated once the “individual’s direct interaction with others who
differential association is entered into the model. engage in certain kinds of behavior (criminal/
Finally, the effect of self-control on delinquency deviant or conforming) and exposes the individual
is moderated by differential association. Persons to the norms, values, and attitudes supportive of
with low self-control tend to commit more crimes these behaviors” (Akers & Jennings, 2009:325).
Barrera, Capistrano, Feliciano, Alaban, Agusin, Locsin, and Villabeto 71

It can occur with “intimate personal groups” or models examined while imitation was signiicant
primary groups and secondary groups (Akers, in 73% of the models. Moreover, these two
2009:60). Primary groups include family and predictors had the largest effects on delinquency
friends while secondary groups and reference among the four concepts. In sum, Pratt et al. (2010)
groups include neighbors, schools, churches, and noted that the strong effect size of differential
the media. The family is acknowledged as most association and deinitions to delinquency/crimes
inluential during childhood; however, during was stronger than that of deterrence (Pratt et
adolescence and adulthood, peers and secondary al., 2006) but comparable to that of self-control,
or reference groups become more important thereby conirming the results of a previous meta-
in inluencing individuals in conforming and analysis of the effect of self-control on crime and
nonconforming behavior. delinquency (see Pratt & Cullen, 2000).
These groups can inluence an individual
to commit delinquent acts through “what they Self-control theory’s self-selection thesis
do” and “what they think” – the behavioral/
interactional and normative dimensions of Self-control is perhaps one of the most
differential association (Akers, 2009; Warr& popular and most cited theories in criminology
Stafford, 1991). Regarding the behavioral (Google citations as of November 4, 2015 = 7998),
dimension, individuals are provided with models not to mention it being the most controversial
of which delinquents acts may be modeled. These theory in recent decades (Goode, 2008). It has
models also encourage delinquent behaviors caught this much attention because of its bold
through punishments and rewards toward claims in crime causation, methodology, and
conforming and nonconforming behavior. As to policy.
the normative dimension, individuals are exposed One source of much controversy is the claim
to models with delinquent deinitions or views, of its theorists on the role of self-control in crime
thereby further encouraging them to commit causation. Gottfredson and Hirschi (1990: 232)
delinquent acts and altering their normative boldly claimed that self-control is “the individual-
deinitions. In short, differential association level cause of crime” that can explain crime at all
facilitates the mechanisms through which an places and all times. People who have low self-
individual learn and commit crimes. control are “impulsive, insensitive, physical (as
Empirically, the theory has been tested over opposed to mental), risk-taking, short-sighted,
a hundred of times and generated overwhelming and non-verbal, and they will, therefore, tend to
support from most studies (Akers & Jensen, 2006; criminal and analogous behavior”. They further
Pratt et al., 2010). Pratt et al. (2010) analyzed claimed that self-control could explain not only
the results of 133 studies that tested the theory variations in crime and analogous behaviors but
on a total of 118,403 individuals from 1973 to also those in accidents, bad health, and negative
2003 and found considerable support for the marriages, work, and friendship ties. With this,
theory. However, they found that its four central they implied that social factors believed to be
concepts did not perform equally in predicting correlates of crime are just social consequences
crime/delinquency. In general, differential of having low self-control. These views are largely
association and deinitions outperform imitation at a variance of the conventional thinking about
and differential reinforcement. The differential crime, and if proven, “most sociological theories
association was signiicant in 90% of the statistical can be relegated to the criminological dustbin”
72 The Interplay of Self-Control

(Evans et al., 1997: 479). that self-control is not the only cause of crime. As
In line with this thinking, Gottfredson and pointed above, this claim is empirically supported
Hirschi (1990) explicitly attacked Sutherland’s - self-control and differential association have
differential association theory and Aker’s social comparable strong effects on delinquency (see
learning theory, labeling these theories as cultural Pratt & Cullen, 2000 and Pratt et al., 2010 for
deviance theory. For them, these learning theories meta-analyses). Differential association retains its
are “redundant or superluous” (p.71) and “thus effects on delinquency even after controlling for
clearly false” (p.156). Their claims are based on self-control and vice-versa. Little research work
this premise: shows, however, that differential association
variables have stronger effects than self-control
People who lack self-control tend to dislike
(e.g. Beaver et al., 2015; Hwang & Akers, 2003).
settings that require discipline, supervision,
However, most of these studies employed
or other constraints on their behavior; such
perceptual measures of peer deviance, which have
settings include school, work, and for that
been found to be misperceived by respondents,
matter, home. These people, therefore, tend
especially by persons with low self-control
to gravitate to “the street” or, at least in
(Boman, Stogner, Miller, Grifin &Krohn, 2011;
adolescence, with the same-sex peer group
Meldrum &Boman, 2013; Rebellon&Modecki,
(Gottfredson&Hirschi, 1990: 157).
2014; Young, Barnes, Meldrum &Weerman, 2011).
They concluded that self-control is the Consequently, direct peer delinquency measures
primary determinant of membership in delinquent appear to be better than perceptual measures
peer groups, and thus the relationship between in measuring peer delinquency (Meldrum
peer deviance and delinquency is spurious since &Boman, 2013) and the effect of self-control on
both are caused by low self-control. Individuals delinquency is greater than peer deviance if it is
who are impulsive and possess other elements of assessed through direct measures (Meldrum et
low self-control tend to have dificulty in making al., 2009; Rebellon&Modecki, 2014).
and maintaining conforming friends and will end To reduce bias associated with perceptual
up befriending low-self-controlled individuals, measures, some researchers proposed the error
thereby forming a group of individuals who correlation method using latent variable modeling
have low self-control and who commit crimes (Matsueda& Anderson, 1998; Rebellon, 2012).
(Gottfredson&Hirschi, 1990). However, subsequent research comparing the
effects of perceptual measures adjusted using this
The interplay of self-control and social method and direct peer delinquency measures
learning variables revealed that error correlation strategy fails to
eliminate the bias associated with perceptual
Gottfredson and Hirschi’s (1990) argued that measures (Boman, Rebellon& Meldrum, 2016;
self-control would render social learning variables- Rebellon&Modecki, 2014). Also, an alternative
delinquency relationship spurious. In their view, operationalization of perceptual measure
birds of the same feather lock together. On the by asking respondents for individual peer’s
other hand, Aker’s (2008: 85) countered this by involvement in delinquency failed to reduce the
quoting Benjamin Franklin, “If you lie down with said bias (Meldrum &Flexon, 2015). Nevertheless,
dogs, you get up with leas,” implying that peer within the perspective of situational action theory
deviance has direct effects on delinquency and (SAT), the use of perceptual measures of peer
Barrera, Capistrano, Feliciano, Alaban, Agusin, Locsin, and Villabeto 73

delinquency along with self-control has been Wilcox, 2007; Wright, Caspi& Mofitt, 2001).
suggested to be acceptable. As Hirtenlehner, The effects of low self-control on delinquency
Pauwels and Mesko (2015: 541) note, “… are stronger when individuals have peers who
according to SAT it is the subjectively perceived commit delinquent acts and espouse delinquent
moral context and the perceived provocation or deinitions.
temptation in a setting that renders the exercise Thus, we attempt to model the relationship
of self-control relevant.” of self-control, differential association, and
However, instead of pitting head-to-head, delinquency among Filipino college students in
some researchers treated these rival theories as three ways:
complementary. Gottfredson and Hirschi (1990) 1. The relationship between differential as-
argued that persons with low self-control tend sociation and delinquency is spurious since
to self-select delinquent peers. And based on the both are determined by low self-control.
prediction of social learning theory, differential When self-control is included into the model
association provides a context where an individual of differential association-delinquency re-
is presented with models who are delinquents, lationship, the latter relationship is dimin-
provide rewards to delinquent behaviors, and ished.
espouse attitude favorable to delinquency (Akers, 2. The relationship between self-control and
2009). It is likely possible then that differential delinquency is mediated by differential as-
association mediates the effects of self-control sociation. When the differential association
on delinquency. In this model, low self-control is included into the model of self-control-de-
leads to “locking” with deviant peers which later linquency relationship, the latter relation-
turns to delinquency. Indeed, previous research ship is diminished.
found that differential association mediated 3. The relationship between self-control
the low self-control-delinquency link (see e.g. and delinquency is moderated by differen-
Chapple, 2005; Malouf, Stuewig& Tangney, tial association. In particular, the effects of
2012; McGloin&Shermer, 2009; Wright et al., self-control on delinquency are increased
1998). Self-control indirectly affects delinquency by having peers who commit crimes and es-
through association of delinquent peers. pouse delinquent norms.
Also, the interplay of self-control and
differential association might follow an METHODOLOGY
interaction model. Wright et al. (2001) proposed
a life-course interdependence model to describe Data Gathering
this complementary role between self-control
and differential association. It could be that the Data used in this study were the results of a
effects of self-control on delinquency vary by self-report survey of Filipino college students (n
levels of peer inluence, and vice versa. Previous = 211) at four universities (1 state and 3 private)
research supports this model: differential in Dumaguete City, Philippines. The current
association aggravates the effects of self-control sample size possesses the necessary power to
on delinquency and vice versa (Cochran, Jones, test the models built in this study because the
Jones & Sellers, 2016; Ford & Blumenstein, 2012; study requires a minimum of 184 respondents to
Higgins & Makin, 2004; Hirtenlehner et al., 2015; achieve a power of 0.95 with 0.15 (medium) effect
Meldrum, Young, &Weerman, 2009; Ousey& size, 0.05 signiicance level, and 12 independent
74 The Interplay of Self-Control

variables as parameters.1 The respondents were self-control, and the social learning theories were
identiied conveniently from the social science proposed as general theories of crime to explain
classes which were selected before the survey. all crimes, at all places and times, and in all types
Initially, necessary consent from each of the of samples, including the convenient sample of
universities was sought. Three social science this study. Thus, the randomness of the current
classes were then selected at the state university sample is not a primary concern in testing the
and one class from each of the three private theories. However, caution should be taken in
universities. These social science classes comprise generalizing the results to a larger population of
students from different degree programs and year Filipino students.
levels. Respondents were asked to participate in
the self-report survey and were informed that Measures
their participation in the survey was voluntary
in nature. The survey period started in July and Delinquency. Delinquency was measured
ended in August 2016. using a modiied form of the normative deviance
The majority of the respondents were female scale developed by Vazsonyi et al. (2001). The
(56%), and most of them were about 18 years (Table normative deviance scale was developed to
1); more than half of them came from families/ measure delinquency with the aim of overcoming
households with monthly income of P10,000 cultural differences in measuring delinquency.
and below. Although there are concerns on the It measures both serious and non-serious forms
non-representativeness of the current sample, of delinquency. However, since the target

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of the variables in this study


Variable Mean/n Standard Skewness Min-Max
deviation/%
Gender (Female =1) .56 0-1
Age 18.58 2.123 1.517 15-28
Household/Family Monthly Income 3.64 1.755 .274 1-7
Below P1,000 (1) 22 10.9%
P1,001-P5,000 (2) 38 18.9%
P5,001-P10,000 (3) 48 23.9%
P10,001-P20,000 (4) 26 12.9%
P20,001-P30,000 (5) 28 13.9%
P30,001-P50,000 (6) 27 13.4%
Above P50,000 (7) 12 6%
Religiosity 16.5073 2.50806 -.594 7-20
Self-control Attitudinal Scale 56.65 12.413 -.313 23-87
Self-control Social Bond Scale 159.14 20.447 -.264 101-200
Peer Deviance 2.50 3.965 1.959 0-16
Peer Norms 1.82 4.230 3.331 0-32
Delinquency 3.78 7.325 2.946 0-39
Barrera, Capistrano, Feliciano, Alaban, Agusin, Locsin, and Villabeto 75

respondents of this study were college students risk seeking, physical activities, self-centered, and
of which some were young adults, the scale was temper. Each of these components is composed
modiied by excluding some items that would of four items such as “I often act on the spur
not be considered as deviant when committed by of the moments without stopping to think.”; “I
adults. Acts that were excluded include lying about frequently try to avoid projects that I know will be
one’s age to get into a bar and stayed out all night dificult.”; “I like to test myself now and then by
without informing parents. Consequently, the doing something a little risky.”; “If I had a choice,
modiied normative deviance scale comprised of I would almost always rather do something
39 items such as “Have you ever:” avoided paying physical than something mental.”; “I try to look
for something (e.g. movies, bus, food, etc.), sold out for myself irst, even if it means making things
any drugs, used drugs (e.g. cocaine, marijuana, dificult for other people.”; and “Often, when I’m
etc.), and hit or threaten to hit a person. Responses angry at people, I feel more like hurting them than
to the items were “Yes = 1” or “No =0”. All items talking to them about why I am angry.” Responses
were summed to create the delinquency index to these items range from “Somewhat agree = 1”
of which higher scores indicate a higher number to “Strongly disagree = 4.” In the current sample,
of delinquent acts committed. However, caution the scale is highly reliable with Cronbach alpha =
is given in interpreting the results, because the 0.90. All responses were summed of which higher
deviance scale asked for lifetime deviance. The scores indicate high self-control.
order of the causal linkages may be in reverse as Self-control social bond scale. Self-control
it is most likely that delinquency affects the key was also measured using a social bond scale for
variables in the study (Akers, 2009; Thornberry et two reasons. First, several studies have shown
al., 1994). that attitudinal scales tend to underestimate the
effects of self-control on crime (see Engel, 2012
Independent variables for a meta-analysis). Second, Hirschi (2004:
543) re-deined self-control as “the tendency to
Self-control theory consider the full range of potential costs of a
particular act,” arguing that self-control and social
Self-control attitudinal scale. control are in fact the same. With this, Hirschi
Gottfredson and Hirschi (1990) proposed that rejected his and Gottfredson’s original trait-based
the individual-level cause of crime is self-control. conceptualization of self-control and favored a
Over the years, self-control has been measured wider set of internal set of inhibitions – mainly
using two types of scales – attitudinal and composed of four elements of the social bond,
behavioral. However, the behavioral scales (e.g. namely: attachment, commitment, involvement,
Keane et al., 1993) received criticisms saying that and belief. Hirschi admitted that they borrowed
they contribute to the tautology of the theory (see their original conceptualization of self-control
Akers, 1991) for using acts analogous to crime from psychology and this move created several
(e.g. failure to wear a seatbelt) to predict crime. problems. First, the original self-control suggests
Therefore, to overcome the tautology criticism, specialization in criminal behaviors which is
the use of attitudinal scales is preferred. In this contrary to their theoretical propositions. Second,
study, we used the attitudinal self-control scale of theirs was a personality approach which also
Grasmick et al. (1993). Their self-control measure contradicts their argument that personality traits
has six components – impulsivity, simple tasks, are less important in explaining delinquency.
76 The Interplay of Self-Control

Third, they suggested that “offenders act as they Development Study measures reported by
do” and “they are what they are because of their Thornberry et al. (1994). For peer deviance,
original conceptualization” (Hirschi, 2004: 542). the respondents were asked how many of their
Finally, the individual self-control traits have a friends in the past six months committed the eight
comparable predictive ability as that of when all delinquency items which included “Hit someone
are combined. with the idea of hurting them” and “Damaged or
With this, the study employed a modiied destroyed someone else’s property on purpose.”
form of the self-control social bond measure Response categories for these items were “Most
used by Dodson (2009) which she modiied from of them = 2,” “Some of them = 1,” and “None
the work of Gibbs et al. (2008). The Attachment of them = 0.” Cronbach alpha for this scale is
component comprised of 17 items, for example: 0.944. All items were summed to create a scale
“I care a lot about what my parents think of me.” with higher scores indicating bigger proportion
The Commitment component was composed of of delinquent friends.
17 items which included “Grades are important to Peer norms. This variable relects the
me.” The Belief component also comprised of 14 respondents’ perception about their peer’s
items, for example: “Rules restricting alcohol use reaction if they (the respondents) would commit
on campus should not be strictly enforced” (reverse delinquent acts. In measuring peer norms, the
coded). Finally, the Involvement component was respondents were asked, “What would your
made up of 3 items which included “Join campus friends say if they know that you have committed
activities in clubs, associations, societies or other the following acts?" This question was followed
organizations that focus on career interest and/or by six delinquency items, such as “Attacked
academic discipline.” Responses to the irst three someone with a weapon or with the idea of
components ranged from “Strongly agree = 4” to seriously hurting them” and “Stole something
“Strongly disagree = 4,” while the involvement worth more than Php 100.” Response categories
component’s responses ranged from “Always = for these items were “Say it was wrong = 1,” “Say
4” to “Never = 1”. The scale’s Cronbach alpha nothing = 2,” and “Say it was okay = 3.” The
is 0.876. All items were summed into a scale of scale’s Cronbach alpha is 0.944. Higher scores
which higher scores indicate higher self-control. indicate higher delinquent peer norms.

Social learning theory Interaction terms

Peer deviance. Akers (2009) proposed that Four interaction terms were created to
four elements of the social learning process measure the interaction between self-control and
increase delinquency. These are differential differential association. These interaction terms
association, deinitions, imitation, and differential were used to test whether measures of differential
reinforcement. However, these components association alleviate or aggravate the effects of self-
do not have a similar magnitude of effect on control on delinquency. Self-control (attitudinal
delinquency, with differential association having and social bond) and differential association
the strongest effect comparable to that of self- (peer deviance and peer norms) variables were
control (Pratt et al., 2010). Thus, this study used centered before creating multiplicative terms
differential association scales (i.e. peer deviance (e.g. self-control X peer deviance). Thus, four
and peer norm qualities) from Rochester Youth interaction terms were created: (1) Self-control
Barrera, Capistrano, Feliciano, Alaban, Agusin, Locsin, and Villabeto 77

attitudinal scale X Peer Deviance, (2) Self-control mediates the relationship between self-control
attitudinal scale X Peer norms, (3) Self-control and delinquency; (3) differential association
social bond X Peer deviance, and (4) Self-control moderates the relationship between self-control
social bond X Peer norms. and delinquency. Table 3 presents the negative
binomial regression models of delinquency using
Control variables self-control, differential association, interaction
terms, and control variables as predictors.
Four known individual and social correlates In general, the results suggest that there
of delinquency were included in the models as is no current evidence supporting Gottfredson
control variables. The variables were sex (Male and Hirschi’s (1990) contention that the
= 0, Female = 1), Age (in years), monthly relationship between differential association and
family/household income (Below P1,000 = delinquency is spurious. In fact, it appears that
1; P1,001-P5,000 = 2; P5,001-P10,000 = 3; self-control theory and social learning theory are
P10,001-P20,000 = 4; P20,001-P30,000 = 5; complementary: differential association mediates
P30,001-P50,000 = 6; and Above P50,000 = 7), the effects of self-control on delinquency.
and religiosity. Religiosity was measured using a
scale comprising of ive items (Cronbach alpha = The spuriousness of the differential
0.673). Items include “My ideas about religion are association-delinquency relationship
one of the most important parts of my philosophy
of life” and “Believing as I do about religion is very Model 4 provides evidence refuting
important to being the kind of person I want to Gottfredson and Hirshi’s (1990) speculation that
be” taken from Cannon (2005). Responses ranged self-control will diminish the effects of differential
from “Strongly agree = 4” to “Strongly disagree association on delinquency. As shown, the effects
= 1.” of differential association variables continue to
exert signiicant effects on delinquency despite
RESULTS including self-control measures simultaneously in
the model. Thus, current evidence suggests that
Negative binomial regression was used the relationship between differential association
to model delinquency because the dependent and delinquency is not spurious.
variable is basically a count data which exhibits
over-dispersion. Over-dispersed count data are Mediating effects of differential association in
best modeled by count regression models such as the self-control-delinquency
negative binomial regression (Gardner, Mulvey & relationship
Shaw, 1995). In all models, the data have an over-
dispersion parameter of not less than 1.7. Thus, Model 4 shows that it is the differential
negative binomial regression best its the current association that diminishes the effects of self-
data. control on delinquency. Differential association,
Recall that using self-control and differential however, does not negate the relationship
association, we speculated that delinquency between self-control and delinquency. The model
can be modeled in three ways: (1) self-control implies that differential association mediates such
diminishes the effects of differential association relationship because of a prior signiicant direct
on delinquency; (2) differential association effect of self-control measures on delinquency.
78 The Interplay of Self-Control

Table 2. Negative binomial regression models of delinquency among Filipino college students. The 95% Wald
conidence interval of the coeficients are reported in parentheses
Independent variable Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5
Key Independent Variables
Self-control Attitudinal -.026* -.003 -.004
Scale [-.047, -.004] [-.023, .018] [-.024, .016]
Self-control Social Bond -.019** .003 .003
Scale [-.033, -.005] [-.011, .017] [-.012, .017]
Peer Deviance .177** .177** .242**
[.097, .257] [.096, .258] [.143, .341]
Peer Norms .078* .083* .082
[.007, .150] [.008, .158] [-.021, .184]
Interaction Terms
SC Attitudinal x Peer .006
Deviance [-.001, .012]
SC Attitudinal x Peer Norms .000
[-.007, .006
SC Social Bond x Peer .003
Deviance [-.001, .007]
SC Social Bond x Peer Norms -.002
[-.007, .003]
Control Variables
Age -.017 -.023 .017 .022 .044
[-.137, .103] [-.147, .101] [-.083, .118] [-.081, .125] [-.060, .148]
Sex -.293 -.132 .156 .128 .192
[-.836, .251 [-.692, .428] [-.330, .641] [-.370, .626] [-.300, .683]
Monthly Household/Family .215** .203* .150* .151* .134
Income [.056, .375] [.041, .365] [.009, .290] [.007, .295] [-.008, .277]
Religiosity -.065 .004 -.065 -.072 -.038
[-.167, .036] [-.095, .103] [-.157, .027] [-.169, .026] [-.136, .060]
Constant 3.398 3.932* .400 .032 .142
[-.066, 6.863] [.283, 7.582] [-2.054, 2.854] [-3.403, 3.468] [-2.395, 2.679]
Likelihood ration X2 14.583* 16.328** 61.50** 61.736** 70.622**
Legend:*p<0.05, **p<0.01

Moderating effects of differential association have no signiicant effect on delinquency. That


in the self-control-delinquency relationship is, differential association does not moderate
the relationship between self-control and
Model 5 shows the interaction effects of delinquency. Thus, the complementary roles
self-control and differential association measures of self-control and differential association in
on delinquency. As shown, all interaction terms only through the mediating effect of differential
Barrera, Capistrano, Feliciano, Alaban, Agusin, Locsin, and Villabeto 79

association in the self-control-delinquency link. theory (Akers, 2009; Akers & Jennings, 2009) and
its previous tests (Pratt et al., 2010). Overall, these
DISCUSSION direct effects echo previous indings that both
self-control and differential association are strong
In this study, we examined the relationships predictors of delinquency.
between self-control, differential association, and Although commonly seen as competing
delinquency among Filipino college students theories and tested head-to-head, the theories
in Dumaguete City. Based on self-control and are in fact complementary based on the current
social learning theories and previous research, evidence. Self-control did not extinguish the
we speculated that such relationships can be effects of differential association on delinquency,
modelled in three ways: (1) the relationship thereby negating the arguments of Gottfredson
between differential association and delinquency and Hirschi (1990). In fact, differential association
is spurious; (2) the relationship between self- mediates the relationship between self-control
control and delinquency is mediated by differential and delinquency, supporting previous research
association; and (3) the relationship between (e.g. Chapple, 2005; Wright et al., 1998). That is,
self-control and delinquency is moderated by those who have low self-control tend to self-select
differential association. In general, both self- delinquent peers who hold favorable attitudes
control and differential association have direct toward deviance and who in turn increase the
effects on delinquency. The spuriousness thesis probability of committing delinquent acts. These
of Gottfredson and Hirschi’s (1990), however, is results suggest that self-control and social learning
not supported by current evidence. In fact, self- theories are complementary, thereby supporting
control and social learning are complementary claims by some scholars (e.g. Akers, 2008; Evan
– differential association mediates the effects et al., 1997; Meldrum, Young &Weerman, 2009).
of self-control on delinquency. In spite of this, Thus, as pointed out by Meldrum, Young and
Filipinos are known to possess high self-control Weerman (2009: 368), “theoretical integration
and value prudence, calmness, and non-violence of self-control theory and social learning theory
in personal and social settings (Tiangco, 2005). may lead to a more precise understanding of the
Both self-control and differential association contours of criminal offending.”
have direct effects on delinquency. Speciically, These indings also point out important
self-control (attitudinal and bond) measures practical implications in the Philippine setting.
are negatively related to delinquency even after It is important to develop high self-control
controlling for socio-demographic variables, among Filipino children through, say, proper
thereby supporting the predictions of Gottfredson parenting (e.g. parental supervision, prompt
and Hirschi (1990), Hirschi (2004), and previous identiication of wrongs, and non-violent
research (Pratt et al., 2000). This means to say, discipline; Gottfredson&Hirschi, 1990). However,
respondents who lacked self-control tended developing high self-control is not enough in
to commit more delinquent acts. On the other turning adolescents from delinquency. Delinquent
hand, differential association (peer deviance and peers and beliefs that encourage it still affect
peer norms) measures are positively related to them in spite of being able to control themselves.
delinquency. Those who had delinquent peers Thus, not only do Filipino adolescents need high
who espoused deviance tended to commit more self-control; they also need training on selecting
delinquency. This inding supports social learning non-deviant peers.
80 The Interplay of Self-Control

Although the current study ills a signiicant by (Rebellon&Modecki, 2014). Examining this
gap in the literature, caution is given to readers line of inquiry in the Philippines will shed more
in interpreting the results. Since the sample was light on the relationships investigated.
not randomly generated, the results cannot be
generalized to a larger population. It is, therefore, CONCLUSION
interesting to examine whether the relationships
found in this study hold true among a more The current indings imply that self-control
representative sample of Filipinos. Nevertheless, and differential association have direct effects
the current study might serve as a seed for further on delinquency among a sample of Filipino
theory-building in the country. Gottfredson and college students in Dumaguete City. In addition,
Hirschi (1990) also speculated that there is a differential association mediates the effects of
measurement overlap between the respondents’ self-control on delinquency. That is, those who
reported delinquency and their reported have low self-control will self-select delinquent
delinquency of peers. In reporting their peers’ peers who can encourage and facilitate offending.
delinquency, it is possible that they are reporting Further, the current indings are contrary to
theirs. Several possibilities may give rise to this spuriousness thesis of self-control theory,
event: thereby suggesting that self-control and social
(1) The respondent may have been at the learning theory are complementary. Theoretical
scene, himself engaging in the activity; (2) integration hinged on the life-course perspective
the respondent may impute his own quali- (e.g. Wright et al., 2001) might be a proitable way
ties to his friends; (3) the respondent may of explaining Filipino delinquency.
impute friendship to people like himself;
4) the respondent’s friends may have told ACKNOWLEDGMENT
him about delinquencies he did not himself
witness; or (5) the respondent may have We would like to express our gratitude to
heard about his friend’s delinquencies Enrique Oracion of Silliman University, Rubie
from people who witnessed or heard about Rubio of Negros Oriental State University-Main
them.”(Gottfredson&Hirschi, 1990: 157) Campus, Cyndi Archival of Negros Oriental State
University-Siaton Campus, and the editors of the
In supporting these speculations, studies PRISM journal for comments on earlier drafts
found that respondents misperceived peer deviance of this paper. Likewise, we would like to thank
(Boman, Stogner, Miller, Grifin &Krohn, 2011; the anonymous reviewers for the constructive
Meldrum &Boman, 2013; Rebellon&Modecki, comments and suggestions without which this
2014; Young, Barnes, Meldrum &Weerman, 2011). paper would not be better.
Also, some researchers found that such indirect
peer deviance measures underestimate the effects Notes
of self-control. For example, Meldrum, Young,
and Weerman (2009) found that self-control had 1. This estimation was done using GPower
stronger effects than peer deviance when the 3.1.9.2 software.
latter was assessed using direct measures (i.e.
using the self-reported delinquency by peers of
the respondents). The same indings were found
Barrera, Capistrano, Feliciano, Alaban, Agusin, Locsin, and Villabeto 81

REFERENCES Criminology. Advanced online publication.


doi:0306624X15583670
Akers, R. L. (1991). Self-control as a general theory
of crime. Journal of Quantitative Boman, J. H., Stogner, J. M., Miller, B. L., Grifin,
Criminology, 7(2), 201-211. O. H., &Krohn, M. D. (2011). On the
operational validity of perceptual peer
Akers, R. L. (1996). Is differential association/ delinquency: Exploring projection and
social learning cultural deviance theory? elements contained in perceptions. Journal
Criminology, 34(2), 229-247. of Research in Crime and Delinquency,
49(4), 601-621.
Akers, R. L. (2008). Self-control and social
learning theory. In E. Goode (Ed), Out of Boman IV, J. H., Rebellon, C. J., & Meldrum, R.
control: Assessing the general theory of C. (2016). Can Item-Level Error Correlations
crime. In E. Goode (Ed), Out of control: Correct for Projection Bias in Perceived
Assessing the general theory of crime. Peer Deviance Measures? A Research Note.
Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Journal of Quantitative Criminology,
32(1), 89-102.
Akers, R. L. (2009). Social learning and social
structure: A general theory of crime and Cannon, K. D. (2005). “Ain't no faggot gonna
deviance. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction rob me!”: Anti-gay attitudes of criminal
Publishers. justice undergraduate majors. Journal of
Criminal Justice Education, 16(2), 226-243.
Akers, R. L. & Jennings, W. (2009).Social
learning theory. In J. Miller (Ed.), 21st Chapple, C. L. (2005). Self-control, peer relations,
Century criminology: A reference handbook. and delinquency. Justice Quarterly, 22(1),
Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications, Inc. 89-106.

Akers, R. L. & Jensen, G.F. (2006). The empirical Cochran, J. K. Jones, S., Jones, A.M., &
status of social learning theory of crime and Sellers, C.S. (2016). Does Criminal
deviance: The past, present, and future. In Propensity Moderate the Effects of Social
F.T. Cullen, J.P. Wright& K.R. Blevins (Eds), Learning Theory Variables on Intimate
Taking stock: The status of criminological Partner Violence? Deviant Behavior.
theory: Advances in criminological theory, Advanced online publication. DOI:
Vol 15. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction. 10.1080/01639625.2015.1060793

Beaver, K. M., Al-Ghamdi, M. S., Kobeisy, A. N., Dodson, K. D. (2009). Tracing the evolution
Alqurashi, F. H., Schwartz, J. A., Connolly, of Gottfredson and Hirschi’s concept of
E. J., & Gajos, J.M. (2015). The effects of self-control: A conceptual and empirical
low self-control and delinquent peers on analysis. (Unpublished doctoral
alcohol, tobacco, and drug use in a sample dissertation). Indiana University of
of Saudi Arabian youth.International Journal Pennsylvania, Pennsylvania, USA.
of Offender Therapy and Comparative
82 The Interplay Of Self-Control

Engel, C. (2012). Low self-control as a source of Higgins, G.E. & Makin, D.A. (2004). Does social
crime: A meta-study. Bonn, Germany: Max learning theory condition the effects
Planck Institute for Research on Collective of low self-control on college students’
Goods. software piracy? Journal of Economic Crime
Management, 2(2), 1-22.
Evans, T. D., Cullen, F. T., Burton Jr, V. S.,
Dunaway, R. G., Benson, M. L. (1997). The
social consequences of self-control: Testing Hirschi, T. (1996). Theory without ideas: Reply to
the general theory of crime. Criminology, Akers. Criminology, 34(2), 249-256.
35(3), 475-504.
Hirschi, T. & Gottfredson, MR. (2008). Critiquing
Ford, J. A., & Blumenstein, L. (2012). Self-control the critics: The authors respond. In E. Goode
and substance use among college students. (Ed), Out of control: Assessing the general
Journal of Drug Issues, 43(1), 56-68. theory of crime. Stanford, CA: Stanford
University Press.
Gardner, W., Mulvey, E. P. & Shaw, E. C. (1995).
Regression analyses of counts and rates: Hirtenlehner, H., Pauwels, L., & Mesko, G. (2015).
Poisson, overdispersedpoisson, and negative Is the criminogenic effect of exposure to
binomial models. Psychological Bulletin, peer delinquency dependent on the ability
118(3), 392-404. to exercise self-control? Results from three
countries.Journal of Criminal Justice, 43(6),
Gibbs, J. J., Dodson, K. D., Cho, B., & Clevenger, 532-543.
S. (2008). Developing and testing a
revised measure of self-control in the Hwang, S., & Akers, R. L. (2003). Substance use
general theory of crime—inal report. by Korean adolescents: A cross-cultural
Unpublished manuscript.Indiana University test of social learning, social bonding, and
of Pennsylvania, Indiana, PA. self-control theories. In R. L. Akers & G. F.
Jensen (Eds.), Advances in criminological
Goode, E. (2008). Out of control: Assessing the theory: Vol. 11. Social learningtheory and
general theory of crime.Stanford, CA: the explanation of crime: A guide for the
Stanford University Press. new century (pp. 39–64). New Brunswick,
NJ: Transaction.
Gottfredson, M.R. &Hirschi, T. (1990). A general
theory of crime. Stanford, CA: Stanford Keane, C., maxim, P.S., &Teevan, J.J. (1993).
University Press. Drinking and driving, self-control, and
gender: Testing a general theory of
Grasmick, H. G., Tittle, C. R., Bursick, R. J. crime. Journal of Research in Crime and
& Arneklev, B.J. (1993). Testing the core Delinquency, 30(1), 30-46.
empirical implications of Gottfredson and
Hirschi’s general theory of crime. Journal of Malouf, E., Stuewig, J. & Tangney, J. (2012). Self-
Research in Crime and Delinquency, 30(1), control and jail inmates’ substance misuse
5-29. post-release: Mediation by friends’ substance
Barrera, Capistrano, Feliciano, Alaban, Agusin, Locsin, and Villabeto 83

use and moderation by age. Addictive L. E., &Madensen, T. D. (2006). The


Behaviors, 37(11), 1198-1204. empirical status of deterrence theory: A
meta-analysis. In F. T. Cullen, J. P. Wright &
Matsueda, R. L., & Anderson, K. (1998). The K. R. Blevins ( Vol. Eds.), Taking stock: The
dynamics of delinquent peers and delinquent status of criminological theory—Advances
behavior. Criminology, 36(2), 269-308. in criminological theory ( Vol. 15) (pp.
367–396). New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction.
McGloin, J.M. &Shermer, L.O. (2009). Self-control
and deviant peer network structure. Journal Pratt, T. C., Cullen, F. T., Sellers, C. S., Winfree
of Research in Crime and Delinquency, Jr., TL., Madenses, T. D., Daigle, L. E. Fearn,
46(1), 35-72. N.E. &Gau, J.M. (2010). The empirical status
of social learning theory: A meta-analysis.
Meldrum, R. C., &Boman, J. H. (2013). Similarities Justice Quarterly, 27(6), 765-802.
and differences between perceptions
of peer delinquency, peer self-reported Rebellon, C. J. (2012). Differential association and
delinquency, and respondent delinquency: substance use: Assessing the roles of
An analysis of friendship dyads. Journal of discriminant validity, socialization, and
Criminal Justice, 41(6), 395-406. selection in traditional empirical tests.
European Journal of Criminology, 9(1), 73-
Meldrum, R. C., &Flexon, J. L. (2015). Is 96.
peer delinquency in the eye of the beholder?
Assessing alternative operationalizations of Rebellon, C.J. &Modecki, K.L. (2014). Accounting
perceptual peer delinquency. Criminal Justice for projection bias in models of delinquent
and Behavior. Advanced online publication. peer inluence: The utility and limits of
Doi: 10.1177/0093854815569729. latent variable approaches. Journal of
Quantitative Criminology, 30 (2), 163-186.
Meldrum, R. C., Young, J.T.N., Weerman, F.M.
(2009). Reconsidering the effect of self- Sutherland, E. H. (1947). Principles of
control and delinquent peers: Implications criminology. Philadelphia: Lippincott.
of measurement for theoretical signiicance.
Journal of Research in Crime and Tiangco, J. A. N. Z. (2005). Understanding the
Delinquency, 46(3), 353-376. Filipino philosophy of resiliency: katatagang-
loob [emotional strength/resiliency] and
Ousey, G. C., & Wilcox, P. (2007). The interaction its phenomenological considerations.In R.
M. Gripaldo (Ed.), Filipino cultural traits.
Washington, D.C: The Council for Research
Pratt, T. C., & Cullen, F. T. (2000). The empirical in Values and Philosophy.
status of Gottfredson and Hirschi’s
general theory of crime: A meta-analysis. Thornberry, T. P., Lizotte, A.J., Krohn, M. D.,
Criminology, 38(3), 931-964. Farnworth, M. & Jang, S. J. (1994).
Delinquent peers, beliefs, and delinquent
Pratt, T. C., Cullen, F. T., Blevins, K. R., Daigle, behavior: A longitudinal test of interactional
84 The Interplay Of Self-Control

theory. Criminology, 32(1), 47-83.

Vazsonyi, A.T., Pickering, L.E., Junger, M. &


Hessing, D. (2001). An empirical test of
a general theory of crime: A four-nation
comparative study of self-control and the
prediction of deviance. Journal of research
in crime and Delinquency, 38(2), 91-131.

Warr, M. & Stafford, M. (1991). Inluence of


delinquent peers: what they think or what
they do? Criminology, 29(4)-851-866.

Wright, B.R.E., Caspi, A., Mofitt, T.E., Silva,


P.A. (1999). Low self-control, social bonds,
and crime: Social causation, social selection,
or both? Criminology, 37(3), 479-514.

Wright, B. R., Caspi, A. & Mofitt, T.E. (2001).


The Effects of Social Ties on Crime Vary by
Criminal Propensity: A Life-Course Model of
Interdependence. Criminology, 39(2):321-
348.

Young, J. (2011). How do they ‘end up together’?


A social network analysis of self-control,
homophily, ad adolescent relationships.
Journal of Quantitative Criminology,
27(3), 251-273.

Young, J. T., Barnes, J. C., Meldrum, R. C.,


& Weerman, F. M. (2011). Assessing
and explaining misperceptions of peer
delinquency. Criminology, 49(2), 599-630.

You might also like