You are on page 1of 22

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2018, 9999, 1–22 NUMBER 9999 ()

FACILITATING THE EMERGENCE OF CONVERGENT INTRAVERBALS


IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM
ANDRESA A. DESOUZA, WAYNE W. FISHER AND NICOLE M. RODRIGUEZ
UNIVERSITY OF NEBRASKA MEDICAL CENTER’S MUNROE-MEYER INSTITUTE

Convergent intraverbals represent a specific type of intraverbal in which multiple components of


one speaker’s verbal behavior control a specific verbal response from another speaker
(e.g., Speaker 1: What wooly, horned animal lives in the high country? Speaker 2: Bighorn
sheep). To foster the development of advanced language, Sunderbeg and Sundberg (2011) pro-
posed prerequisite skills that may engender the emergence of novel, convergent intraverbals. We
used a multiple-probe design with both nonconcurrent (across participants) and concurrent
(across stimulus sets) components to evaluate the effects of training these prerequisite skills on
the emergence of convergent intraverbals with four children with autism. Participants showed
the emergence of convergent intraverbals at mastery levels after they displayed mastery perfor-
mance on all of the prerequisite skills identified by Sundberg and Sundberg, lending support to
their characterization as prerequisites. We discuss these findings in terms of operant mechanisms
that may facilitate the development of generative language.
Key words: autism spectrum disorder, convergent intraverbals, generative language, multiple
control, verbal behavior

In Verbal Behavior, Skinner (1957) described response do not match) and produces general-
a number of verbal operants that differed from ized conditioned reinforcement. An example of
one another in terms of their functional proper- intraverbal behavior is the response “in the bed-
ties (e.g., the mand “toy” evoked by deprivation room” to the question, “Where do you sleep?”
from access to play materials and reinforced by which then produces the generalized reinforcing
the delivery of a preferred toy). Skinner consequence “Nice work! You’re right.” Accord-
described the intraverbal as an operant under the ing to Sundberg and Sundberg (2011), most of a
control of a stimulus that does not have point- person’s daily verbal interaction involves intra-
to-point correspondence to the verbal response verbal relations. Intraverbal behavior also is
(i.e., the antecedent stimulus and the verbal strongly required in the school environment in
the form of answering questions, telling stories,
This study was conducted by the first author in partial solving problems, describing events, and engag-
fulfillment of the requirements for the PhD degree in the ing in social reciprocal interaction with peers.
Applied Behavior Analysis Program at University of Therefore, a weak intraverbal repertoire may
Nebraska Medical Center. We thank Amanda Zangrillo,
Caio Miguel, and Kathryn Peterson for their feedback on negatively impact academic achievement and
an earlier version of this manuscript. We also thank Ami social skills development.
Kaminski, Kendall Lanning, Sydney Readman, and Briana The difficulty or complexity of intraverbal
Licht for assistance with data collection.
Internal funding from the University of Nebraska Med- interactions can range from simple to advanced
ical Center’s Chancellor’s Office provided partial support and can be of infinite number (Sundberg &
for this research. Sundberg, 2011). Early intraverbal skills are
Andresa A. DeSouza is now at the University of Mis-
souri – St. Louis, College of Education. fairly simple and commonly observed in chil-
Address correspondence to Wayne W. Fisher, Center dren around 1 ½ to 2 years old. Around this
for Autism Spectrum Disorders, 985450 Nebraska Medi- age, children start learning songs (e.g., after the
cal Center, Omaha, Nebraska 68198 (email:
wfisher@unmc.edu) parent says, “The itsy bitsy. .. ,” the child says
doi: 10.1002/jaba.520 “spider”), providing sounds that animals make
© 2018 Society for the Experimental Analysis of Behavior
1
2 ANDRESA A. DESOUZA et al.

(e.g., “The cow says. ..” “moo”), and complet- “fruit”, “red”, and “vine”. Moreover, responses
ing reinforcing phrases (e.g., “Ready, set. ..” under convergent control also might involve
“go”). By age 2, typically developing children divergent control (Axe, 2008). For example, in
learn to respond with their name to the ques- the question, “Tell me some fruits that are
tion “What’s your name?” and perform simple red,” one could respond correctly with “straw-
word associations (e.g., the parent says “Socks berry and raspberry.” When an individual
and. .. ,” the child says “shoes”). Their conver- responds to some but not all of the functional
sation skills are not yet established, but they antecedent stimuli involved in convergent
already possess a strong listener and speaker intraverbals, the resulting errors are typically
vocabulary as well as echoic, mand, and tact referred to as stimulus overselectivity (Lovaas,
repertoires. Language quickly develops between Koegel, & Schreibman, 1979; Lovaas, Schreib-
the ages of 2 and 3 and consists mostly of man, Koegel, & Rehm, 1971) or restricted stim-
intraverbal responses. According to Sundberg ulus control (Dube & McIlvane, 1999). For
and Sundberg (2011), early intraverbal interac- example, the child who responds “John” to
tions observed in typically developing children both “What is your first name?” and “What is
tend to be simple, but they provide the founda- your last name?” demonstrates stimulus overse-
tion for advanced intraverbal behavior. lectivity by attending only to the stimulus
One of the challenges involved in the devel- “name” and not to the other relevant stimuli
opment of advanced intraverbals is the fact that (i.e., “first” and “last”).
intraverbals are often controlled by multiple Several studies have implemented procedures
verbal antecedent stimuli (Sundberg & Sund- to decrease the errors that occurred during the
berg, 2011). Michael, Palmer, and Sundberg teaching of convergent intraverbal relations.
(2011) suggested that outside the analog envi- For example, Braam and Poling (1983) used a
ronment of experimental conditions, verbal delayed prompting procedure to transfer con-
responses are the product of many variables trol from nonverbal stimuli (pictures) to verbal
that interact with one another. Michael stimuli (signs) to teach intraverbal classification
et al. described two primary types of multiple of school things (e.g., ruler, book), school do
control: divergent control and convergent control. (e.g., read, write), and school people (e.g., names
With divergent multiple control, a unique or of school personnel) versus home things
singular antecedent variable evokes several ver- (e.g., bathtub, stove), home do (e.g., clean,
bal responses. For example, when presented cook), and home people (e.g., names of family
with the instruction, “Tell me some fruits,” members) to two 17-year-olds with hearing
one could respond correctly with “banana, impairments and intellectual disabilities. The
strawberry, and grape” or with “orange, blue- training procedures brought intraverbal
berry, and mango.” In this example, the single responding under the control of two distinct
verbal antecedent stimulus “fruits” provided classes of verbal antecedent stimuli
divergent control for several verbal responses. (e.g., “school” vs. “home” and “things” vs. “do”
Conversely, in convergent control, two or more vs. “people”) for both participants. Other stud-
antecedent variables evoke a single correct ies have shown the efficacy of including a dif-
response. For example, when presented with ferential observing response (DOR) to ensure
the instruction, “Tell me a fruit that is red and that participants are attending to the relevant
grows on a vine,” one could respond correctly features of the antecedent vocal stimulus. For
with a single word, “strawberry.” In this exam- example, in a study by Kisamore, Karsten,
ple, the single response was under the conver- Mann, and Conde (2013), a DOR in which
gent control of the verbal antecedent stimuli the experimenter prompted the participant to
EMERGENCE OF CONVERGENT INTRAVERBALS 3

repeat the complete antecedent stimulus routinely asked to produce novel, multiply con-
(e.g., “Name the same as tall. You say it.”), trolled, intraverbal responses without ever
proved effective in teaching intraverbal receiving direct training for those specific intra-
responses involving antonym and synonym verbal responses. In fact, the frequency with
relations for four of six typically developing which children are asked to produce novel ver-
preschoolers. bal responses to multiple, novel verbal stimuli
More recently, Kisamore, Karsten, and during routine conversation would make it
Mann (2016) compared the effects of trial-and- unwieldy to build a child’s intraverbal repertoire
error training, DOR, and DOR plus trial- through direct reinforcement of each potential
blocking procedures to teach multiply con- target response. In addition, children with
trolled intraverbal responses (e.g., “What’s a autism present with language delays that, in
drink that’s brown”) to seven children between most cases, require intensive intervention for
4 and 18 years old diagnosed with autism spec- prolonged periods (LeBlanc, Esch, Sidener, &
trum disorder. During the trial-and-error train- Firth, 2006; Roane, Fisher, & Carr, 2016).
ing, the experimenter provided praise and a Hence, it is important that behavior analysts
tangible item following a correct response and discover the variables that facilitate the emer-
re-presented the discriminative stimulus fol- gence of novel, multiply controlled intraverbal
lowed by the controlling prompt at a 0-s delay skills in children with autism, because the incor-
following incorrect responses. During the DOR poration of those variables into early interven-
procedure, the experimenter presented the ante- tion programs could increase the effectiveness
cedent stimulus (e.g., “What’s an animal that’s and efficiency of programs designed to build
red?”), prompted the child to emit the DOR intraverbal competencies in children with
(e.g., “Say animal red”), waited for the partici- autism.
pant to emit the DOR, and re-presented the Sundberg and Sundberg (2011) delineated
antecedent stimulus. The experimenter imple- several prerequisites that may facilitate the
mented the DOR plus trial blocking for partici- acquisition of intraverbals, particularly when
pants who did not reach mastery levels with the training intraverbals under convergent control.
trial-and-error or DOR procedures. For the First, when training intraverbals involving a set
trial-blocking component, the experimenter of stimuli (e.g., red, yellow, green, fruits, vege-
presented 20 consecutive trials for each target tables), the tact and listener repertoires of all
and then systematically faded to irregular block individual targets should be well established,
sizes (Saunders & Spradlin, 1990). The results discriminated, and generalized in the child’s
indicated that, although some participants repertoire. Second, all individual targets should
acquired at least one set of targets with the be evoked under divergent control (e.g., “Tell
trial-and-error procedure, most participants me some vegetables” versus “Tell me some
required additional procedures (e.g., DOR, fruits”). Finally, the child should be able to
DOR plus trial blocking) to acquire multiply respond correctly as a listener when the target
controlled intraverbals. involves multiple control (e.g., “Touch the fruit
The Kisamore et al. (2016) study showed that is yellow” versus “Touch the vegetable that
that young adults and children with develop- is yellow”). Evaluating the prerequisites that are
mental disabilities can learn intraverbals involv- hypothesized to be necessary for the emergence
ing multiple control via direct training methods of untrained, multiply controlled intraverbals is
(i.e., differential reinforcement combined with important for all children, but it is particularly
other procedures). However, during typical important for children with autism because
social and educational interactions, children are they often fail to respond correctly under
4 ANDRESA A. DESOUZA et al.

conditions that require convergent stimulus questions that involved multiple components
control (Sundberg & Sundberg, 2011). There- and therefore required responses under conver-
fore, our goal for this study was to evaluate if gent control (e.g., “What animal has a long
the specific prerequisite skills hypothesized by neck?” or “What animal moves real slow?”).
Sundberg and Sundberg would facilitate the
acquisition and emergence of novel, complex
Setting and Materials
convergent intraverbals in children with autism.
We conducted sessions in a quiet area in the
child’s session room. The room was equipped
with a table, chairs for the participant and the
METHOD
experimenter, data sheets, leisure items, edible
Participants reinforcers, and 13- by 10-cm picture cards.
Four children from 4 to 5 years old diag- We selected preferred items and edibles by ask-
nosed with autism and attending a university- ing the participant for what he or she would
based center participated (see Table 1). All par- like to work for on a regular basis prior to and
ticipants could echo single and multiple words, during sessions. We conducted one to six ses-
follow simple instructions, tact pictures of vari- sions, two to five times per week. Sessions
ous items and their features, and respond to lasted from 5 to 20 min. We provided a break
simple intraverbal questions (e.g., “Where do every time the child requested one.
you wash your hands?” and “What are some We included two sets of picture cards per
animals”). To be included in the study, partici- participant, except for James whose family
pants had to (a) reliably echo words when moved to another part of the country before the
prompted by the experimenter (e.g., say completion of the second set (see Table 2). We
“cookie”); (b) demonstrate simple intraverbal modified Set B for William slightly because he
responses (e.g., respond to questions such as, responded correctly to the probe question, “A
“What do you wash?”) and intraverbal- tool used for scooping is a __,” during probe
categorization responses (intraverbal responses sessions. We included four targets (e.g., zebra,
under divergent control; for example, “What emu) per set. We classified each target according
are some colors?”); and (c) demonstrate to two primary classes: for example, animal type,
restricted stimulus control when asked hereafter referred to as Class 1 (e.g., mammals

Table 1
Participants’ Gender, Ages, and Test Scores

Scores
Intraverbal Subtest
Participants’ Demographics and Diagnosis Total Scorea Group 7b Group 8b EESAc PPVT-4d EVT-2d
James (male, 5 years, ASD) 54 5 3 100 96 94
Thomas (male, 4 years 10 mo., ASD) 50 4 1 99.5 96 92
Kelly (female, 5 years 6 mo., ASD) 56 5 2 100 88 97
William (male, 5 years 2 mo., ASD) 56 4 4 100 86 89

Note. ASD = Autism spectrum disorder; EESA = Early Echoic Skills Assessment; PPVT-4 = Peabody Picture Vocabu-
lary Test–Fourth Edition; EVT-2 = Expressive Vocabulary Test – Second Edition
a
Highest possible score on the VB-MAPP Intraverbal Subtest is 80 points.
b
Highest possible score on each group is 10 points.
c
Highest possible score on the EESA is 100.
d
Mean score for standard scores is 100 with a standard deviation of 15.
EMERGENCE OF CONVERGENT INTRAVERBALS 5

Table 2 performed to criteria during intraverbal probes


Targets for Set A and Set B for the first set, we initiated training for the sec-
ond set of stimuli with the first participant and
Class 1
baseline probes for all sets of stimuli for the sec-
Set A Set B
ond participant. We continued this process of ini-
Class 2 Mammals Birds Utensils Tools
tiating training and baseline probes until all
Set A participants performed to criteria for both sets of
Savanna Zebra Emu
Rain Forest Gorilla Toucan stimuli. We randomized and counterbalanced the
Set B order in which we trained each set per partici-
Scooping Spoon Shovel
Cutting Knife Saw pant. In other words, the first participant initiated
Set B-modifieda training with Set A, whereas the second partici-
Spreading Spatula Trowel
Cutting Knife Saw pant initiated training with Set B.
a
For William only.
Dependent Variables and Response
vs. birds), and bios type, hereafter referred to as Measurement
Class 2 (e.g., savanna vs. rain forest). Therefore, The primary dependent variable included the
each target in a set overlapped with other targets percentage of trials with correct convergent
by either Class 1 (e.g., zebra and gorilla are both intraverbals. We defined a convergent intraver-
mammals) or Class 2 (e.g., zebra and emu are bal as a correct vocal response (e.g., “zebra”) to a
both from the savanna) resulting in two overlap- vocal discriminative stimulus that included the
ping features between classes. The items in Set names of Class 1 and Class 2 for that specific
A differed from the items in Set B both in terms exemplar (e.g., “A mammal from the savanna is
of class and feature. We counterbalanced each a ___;” see Table 3 for a description of the tar-
set across participants to control for potential get responses and corresponding discriminative
sequence effects and the possibility that one set stimuli). We scored a trial as correct if the par-
was more difficult than the other. ticipant provided an exemplar from the stimulus
set within 5 s from the presentation of the vocal
discriminative stimulus. Because our purpose
Experimental Design for this study was to investigate the effects of
We used a multiple-probe design (Horner & teaching prerequisite skills involving a specific
Baer, 1978) with both nonconcurrent (across par- set of stimuli on the emergence of convergent
ticipants) and concurrent (across sets of stimuli) intraverbals related to the same set of stimuli,
components to evaluate the effects of the inde- we recorded all intraverbals emitted by the par-
pendent variable on the emergence of convergent ticipant, but scored only intraverbals within the
intraverbals. We first implemented convergent stimulus set as correct responses, similar to Kisa-
intraverbal probes with the first participant across more, Carr, and LeBlanc (2011).
both sets of stimuli to evaluate the levels of cor- Secondary dependent variables included the
rect intraverbal responses prior to any training. percentage of correct responses during baseline
After the initial baseline probes, the first partici- and training procedures of multiple-tact (MT),
pant began training with the first set of stimuli multiple-listener (ML), intraverbal categoriza-
while the second set was periodically probed for tion, listener compound discrimination (LCD),
the emergence of intraverbals (i.e., probes of the and the percentage of trials with DORs during
second set occurred concurrently to probes of the LCD and intraverbal probe sessions. During
first training set). Once the first participant MT, we scored a trial as correct if the participant
6 ANDRESA A. DESOUZA et al.

Table 3
Prerequisite Skill and Major Features

Procedure Type of Control Type of Response Prompt Strategy Discriminative Stimulus (e.g.)
Intraverbal probe Convergent Speaker None “A mammal from the savanna is a___”
Multiple-tact (MT) Simple Speaker Echoic “Name it”
“It is a __”
“It’s from the ___”
Multiple-listener (ML) Simple/ Listener Point “Point to the zebra”
Divergent “Point to all mammals”
“Point to all from the savanna”
Intraverbal categorization Divergent Speaker Picture “Tell me some mammals”
“Tell me some things from the savanna”
Listener compound Convergent Listener Point “Point to the mammal from the savanna”
discrimination (LCD)

responded within 5 s of each discriminative stimulus set (e.g., zebra and gorilla for mam-
stimulus for the following: (a) the tact of the tar- mals) and both targets from Class 2 of the
get (e.g., said “zebra” in response to presentation stimulus set (e.g., zebra and emu for savanna)
of the picture and the vocal stimulus, “Name within 10 s from the presentation of the vocal
it”); (b) the tact of Class 1 associated with the discriminative stimulus with no more than 5 s
exemplar depicted in the picture card (e.g., said between the emission of each exemplar
“mammal” in response to presentation of the (Sautter, LeBlanc, Jay, Goldsmith, & Carr,
picture and the vocal stimulus, “It’s a __”); and 2011) and no more than 5 s between the dis-
(c) the tact of Class 2 of the exemplar depicted criminative stimulus and the emission of the
in the picture card (e.g., said “savanna” in first response.
response to presentation of the picture and the During LCD, we scored a response correct if
vocal stimulus, “It’s from the __”). the participant independently pointed to the
During ML, we scored a listener response specified picture card within 5 s of the presen-
correct if the participant independently pointed tation of the vocal conditional discriminative
to all specified picture cards within 10 s of the stimulus (e.g., “Point to the mammal from the
presentation of the vocal conditional discrimi- rain forest”). We also collected data on DORs,
native stimulus containing either a target, Class which we defined as vocal responses that
1, or Class 2 name (e.g., “Point to the zebra;” matched the names of Class 1 and Class
“Point to all mammals;” “Point to all from the 2 described in the vocal discriminative stimulus
savanna”) with no more than 5 s between the (e.g., “bird from rain forest”) and was emitted
selection of each exemplar and no more than within 5 s of the vocal discriminative stimulus.
5 s between the vocal conditional discrimina- We collected data on DORs because they have
tive stimulus and the emission of the first selec- been used in other studies to teach intraverbals
tion response. We scored an intraverbal under convergent control (e.g., Kisamore et al.,
categorization trial correct if the participant 2016). We intended to use a similar procedure
provided a vocal response that contained multi- as a fallback position, in case participants did
ple exemplars in response to a vocal discrimina- not demonstrate the emergence of convergent
tive stimulus containing either a Class 1 or intraverbals to criteria levels after the last pre-
Class 2 name (e.g., “Tell me some mammals,” requisite skill. If this had happened, we would
and, “Tell me some things from the savanna”). have used the DOR data collected during
We scored a trial as correct if the participant probe sessions as baseline data for the subse-
provided both targets from the Class 1 of the quent training.
EMERGENCE OF CONVERGENT INTRAVERBALS 7

Interobserver Agreement and Procedural (i.e., listener) and expressive (i.e., tact) reper-
Integrity toires for each participant (similar to those
Four trained observers scored data indepen- included in Kisamore et al., 2016).
dently for at least 32% of all probe, baseline, and Inclusion criteria assessment. We conducted
training sessions for all participants to measure the Early Echoic Skills Assessment (Esch, 2008)
interobserver agreement (IOA). We scored an to assess participants’ ability to echo single and
agreement each time the primary observer and the multiple words because we used echoic
secondary observer both recorded the same correct prompts during teaching procedures to evoke
or incorrect response for each trial. We scored a correct responses. We concluded that partici-
disagreement each time the primary observer pants met the inclusion criteria if they scored at
recorded a correct response whereas the secondary least 85 out of 100 possible points. In addition,
observer recorded an incorrect response, and vice we conducted the Intraverbal Subtest
versa. We calculated IOA by dividing the number (Sundberg & Sundberg, 2011) to evaluate par-
of point-by-point agreements by the number of ticipants’ intraverbal repertoires. The Intraver-
point-by-point agreements plus disagreements and bal Subtest is an 80-item assessment with tasks
converting the quotient to a percentage. IOA aver- ranging from simple intraverbals (e.g., “A kitty
aged 99% (range, 92%-100%) for James, 97% says. ..”) to advanced intraverbals involving ver-
(range, 75%-100%) for Thomas, 99% (range, bal conditional discrimination (e.g., “Why do
83%-100%) for Kelly, and 96% (range, 75%- people wear glasses?”). We sought to identify
100%) for William. participants who correctly responded to simple
An independent observer measured procedural intraverbals and intraverbal categorization ques-
integrity using a checklist during at least 32% of tions (e.g., “Tell me some numbers”), but dem-
sessions in all phases for all participants. We calcu- onstrated restricted stimulus control when
lated procedural integrity by recording if the exper- asked questions involving multiple components
imenter performed the steps for each trial. We (e.g., James responded “flower” for the ques-
scored a trial incorrect if the experimenter made an tion, “What do you smell with?”; Thomas
error in any of the steps for that trial. The observer responded “cake, pizza” for the question,
recorded the following steps: (a) correct presenta- “Where do you eat?”; Kelly responded with her
tion of probe instructions and probe practice (for full name for the question, “What’s your last
probe sessions only), (b) correct presentation of name?”; and William responded with “in the
the antecedent stimulus, (c) correct presentation of kitchen” for the question, “When do you set
programmed prompts, and (d) correct presentation the table?”). Participants met the inclusion cri-
of consequences. Procedural integrity averaged teria if they scored at least 50 but not more
99% (range, 92%-100%) for James, 98% (range, than 70 out of 80 possible points and
85%-100%) for Thomas, 99% (range, 92%- responded correctly to at least two intraverbal
100%) for Kelly, and 99% (range, 92%-100%) categorization questions.
for William. Convergent intraverbal probes. We conducted
convergent intraverbal probe sessions prior to
and following each prerequisite skill to test for
Procedures the emergence of convergent intraverbals. At
Language assessment. We administered the the beginning of each session, we secured the
Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test–Fourth Edi- participant’s attending and said:
tion (Dunn & Dunn, 2007) and the Expressive
Vocabulary Test–Second Edition (Williams, I am going to ask you some questions. I
will not tell you if you are right or wrong,
2007) to gain information about the receptive
8 ANDRESA A. DESOUZA et al.

but I want you to do your best. If I tell repertoires when learners already have an echoic
you a fruit that is red is an ___, you can repertoire. Therefore, we decided to implement
say ‘Apple.’ Now it’s your turn: A fruit MT before ML. The order of the remaining
that is red is an ___. procedures followed the sequence of prerequi-
sites suggested by Sundberg and Sundberg
We started the probe with a practice trial to (2011). We implemented convergent intraver-
ensure that errors were not due to the child bal probes prior to initiating baseline exposure
misunderstanding the requirements of the task. to each prerequisite skill. If at any point in the
We then started the probe session by present- study a participant performed at 92% correct
ing a vocal discriminative stimulus involving or higher across two consecutive sessions during
Class 1 and Class 2 of targets (e.g., “A bird convergent intraverbal probes for a stimulus
from the rain forest is a ___”). We moved to set, we terminated training for that participant
the next trial after the participant provided a with that specific set.
response (correct or incorrect) or 5 s had Prerequisite-skills baseline. We set the crite-
elapsed without any response. We delivered rion for mastery performance at 92% or higher
praise and an edible or preferred item at the of correct independent responses for two con-
end of about every two trials in which appro- secutive sessions for all prerequisite skills. We
priate behavior (e.g., attending, hands in lap, conducted baseline and training sessions when
attempting to respond to questions) occurred necessary of one prerequisite skill at a time until
to maintain responding, but we did not deliver participants demonstrated mastery criterion. We
differential consequences for correct and incor- conducted the baseline sessions for each prereq-
rect responses. We presented each vocal dis- uisite skill using procedures identical to those
criminative stimulus three times for a total of described above for assessing the emergence of
12 trials per session. We defined mastery per- convergent intraverbals (i.e., each discriminative
formance as 92% or more of correct intraver- stimulus presented three times per session with
bals across two consecutive probe sessions. reinforcers presented at the end of about every
Prerequisite skill sequence. We conducted two trials in which appropriate behavior
baseline and, if necessary, training for a occurred) except that we presented a unique dis-
sequence of prerequisite skills until participants criminative stimulus for each prerequisite skill.
demonstrated mastery during convergent intra- Table 3 lists the vocal discriminative stimuli
verbal probes. We conducted baseline sessions presented for each prerequisite skill. During
for each prerequisite skill followed by training each MT trial, we presented a single picture
sessions if the participant emitted fewer than (e.g., zebra) along with one of the corresponding
92% correct responses during baseline sessions vocal discriminative stimuli listed in Table 3
for that prerequisite skill unless the participant (e.g., “Name it,” “It’s a __.”). During each ML
had already met mastery on the convergent trial, we presented all four pictures from a given
intraverbal probes. The sequence of prerequisite set (e.g., zebra, gorilla, emu, toucan) along with
skills progressed as follows: (a) MT, (b) ML, one of the corresponding vocal conditional dis-
(c) intraverbal categorization, and (d) LCD. criminative stimuli listed in Table 3
According to Petursdottir and Carr (2011), (e.g., “Point to all birds.”). During each intra-
there is little empirical support for teaching lis- verbal categorization trial, we presented one of
tener responses before speaker responses. the corresponding vocal discriminative stimuli
According to the authors, if anything, teaching listed in Table 3 (e.g., “Tell me some birds.”).
speaker responses first might be more beneficial Finally, during each LCD trial, we presented all
for the acquisition of both speaker and listener four pictures from a given set (e.g., zebra,
EMERGENCE OF CONVERGENT INTRAVERBALS 9

gorilla, emu, toucan) along with one of the cor- target only (“Name it”). Once participants
responding vocal conditional discriminative responded to criterion for the tact of the target,
stimuli listed in Table 3 (e.g., “Point to a bird we introduced baseline and training
from the savanna.”). (if necessary) for tacts of Class 1 (“It’s a __”)
Training the prerequisite skills. For training of and Class 2 (e.g., “It’s from the__.”). During
MT, ML, and LCD, we used a progressive training sessions of Class 1 and Class 2, a trial
prompt delay (PD) similar to that used by block consisted of three trial types: a target tact
Miguel and Kobari-Wright (2013; i.e., 0 s, 1 s, (“Name it”), a Class 1 tact (“It’s a___.”), and a
2 s, 3 s, 4 s, then 5 s). For intraverbal categori- Class 2 tact (e.g., “It’s from the ___.”).
zation, we used a progressive PD similar to that Whereas we always presented the task involving
used by Miguel, Pettursdottir, and Carr (2005; the target tact first, we randomized the order of
i.e., 0 s then 5 s). For sessions with a 0-s PD, Class 1 and Class 2 tasks within a trial block.
we presented the vocal discriminative stimulus During training for Class 1 and Class 2 tacts,
and then immediately provided the specified the echoic prompt included both the vocal dis-
prompt (see Table 3 for a description of criminative stimulus and the target response
prompts). For sessions with a 1-s PD, we pre- (e.g., “It’s from the savanna”). If the participant
sented the vocal discriminative stimulus and responded correctly (e.g., “savanna”) either
after 1 s provided the specified prompt. We prior to or following the echoic prompt but did
repeated the same procedures for the remaining not echo the vocal discriminative stimulus
intervals of the PD procedure. We increased (e.g., “It’s from the [savanna]”), we praised the
the delay if the participant emitted 92% or correct response and prompted the participant
higher correct independent or prompted to echo the vocal discriminative stimulus plus
responses at a given delay level for two consecu- the correct response, to increase discrimination
tive sessions. Contingent on three consecutive of the verbal antecedent stimulus (e.g., “That’s
errors within a session, we moved back to the right! You can also say ‘It’s from the
previous PD level. For all trials (except during savanna.’”); however, the participant was not
0-s PD), we delivered: (a) praise and a preferred required to echo this statement to contact the
edible or item contingent on each independent reinforcement contingency. We scored a trial as
correct response (i.e., FR 1); (b) praise alone correct even if the participant did not echo the
contingent on each prompted correct response; discriminative stimulus (e.g., said only “rainfor-
and (c) a correction procedure contingent on est” in response to, “It is from the ____.”).
each error, during which we re-presented the During training sessions of Class 1 and Class
trial, provided the specified prompt at a 0-s PD 2, edibles and preferred items were only deliv-
until the participant emitted the correct ered if the participant responded correctly to
response, and then moved to the next trial. the tact of the Target, Class 1, and Class 2 for
During sessions with a 0-s PD, we delivered a specific trial block. We delivered praise, edi-
praise and a preferred edible or item following ble, or a preferred item immediately after a cor-
prompted correct responses. We randomized rect response. We conducted four target tasks
and counterbalanced trials before every session. (for baseline and training of target only) or four
We concluded training if a participant reached trial-blocking tasks (for baseline and training of
mastery criterion (i.e., two consecutive sessions Class 1 and Class 2) presented three times each,
at or above 92% independent correct for a total of 12 trials per session.
responses) at any PD. Multiple-listener (ML). During ML, we
Multiple-tact (MT). MT started with baseline requested a selection response of the target
and training (if necessary) for the tact of the (e.g., “Point to the zebra”), Class 1 (e.g., “Point
10 ANDRESA A. DESOUZA et al.

to all mammals”), and Class 2 (e.g., “Point to During a remedial trial, we re-presented the
all from the rainforest”). For all sessions, we trial and waited for an independent correct
placed all four picture cards on the table in response. If the participant responded incor-
front of the child and delivered the vocal condi- rectly or did not respond within the specified
tional discriminative stimulus. We randomized PD, we prompted the response and re-
and counterbalanced the position of the picture presented the trial. We repeated this procedure
cards before every session. During training up to five times or until the participant emitted
(if necessary), we issued gesture prompts an independent correct response, whichever
(i.e., the experimenter pointed to the correct came first. Only the first presentation during a
picture card) according to the scheduled given trial counted for data collection purposes
PD. We presented four target tasks, two Class (i.e., prior to any correction procedures). We
1 tasks, and two Class 2 tasks three times each delivered praise only for correct independent
for a total of 24 trials per session. responses during the remedial-trial procedure.
Intraverbal categorization. During intraverbal
categorization, we introduced intraverbal catego-
RESULTS
rization of Class 1 (e.g., “Tell me some mam-
mals”) and Class 2 (e.g., “Tell me some things Across-Participant Comparison
from the rainforest”). During training Figure 1 depicts the percentages of indepen-
(if necessary), we presented a tact prompt using a dent correct convergent intraverbals (henceforth
5-s PD, and we then presented an echoic prompt called correct convergent intraverbals) for all four
if participants did not respond within 5 s or participants during intraverbal probe sessions
responded incorrectly to the tact prompt. We for the first set of targets conducted with each
presented two Class 1 and two Class 2 tasks three participant. We introduced training on Set A
times each for a total of 12 trials per session. first for James and William and on Set B first
Listener compound discrimination (LCD). For for Thomas and Kelly. We selected this order
all sessions, we placed all four picture cards on using randomization with counterbalancing
the table in front of the child and delivered the across pairs of participants. All participants
vocal conditional discriminative stimulus emitted zero correct convergent intraverbals
(e.g., “Point to the mammal from the rainfor- during all pretraining probe sessions. After MT,
est”). We randomized and counterbalanced the correct convergent intraverbals increased to 8%
position of the picture cards before every ses- (one correct trial) for each participant. After
sion. During training (if necessary), we imple- ML, James showed the same level of correct
mented a gesture prompt (i.e., experimenter convergent intraverbals (8%), Thomas showed
pointed to the correct response) according to zero correct convergent intraverbals, and Kelly
the specified PD. We presented four com- (33%) and William (50%) showed modest
pound discrimination tasks three times each for improvements in correct convergent intraver-
a total of 12 trials per session. bals. After intraverbal categorization, three of
Remedial trials procedure (James & Thomas). the participants showed small increases in cor-
We added remedial trials during training for rect convergent intraverbals (James, 25%;
several prerequisite skills for James and Thomas Thomas, 17%; Kelly, 50%) and one showed
(see Supporting Information 1 through 3) no change (William, 50%). However, after
when we verified, through visual analysis, that LCD, all four participants performed at mas-
there were no improvements in the level of cor- tery levels for convergent intraverbals.
rect responses across several sessions. Remedial The bars in Figure 1 denote the percentage of
trials followed the error correction procedure. trials in which participants emitted unprompted
EMERGENCE OF CONVERGENT INTRAVERBALS 11

Figure 1. Percentage of independent correct convergent intraverbals and differential observing responses (DORs)
during probe sessions for the first training set across participants. Arrows indicate when each prerequisite skill was imple-
mented. MT = multiple-tact; ML = multiple-listener; IV Categ = intraverbal categorization; LCD = listener compound
discrimination.
12 ANDRESA A. DESOUZA et al.

correct DORs. James emitted DORs on 8% of Like Thomas, Kelly showed zero correct
trials after intraverbal categorization training, convergent intraverbals during all pretraining
and 100% and 83% during the last two intra- probes for both Set A and Set B (bottom two
verbal sessions during which he demonstrated panels, Figure 2). She also showed slightly
mastery performance. Thomas emitted DORs higher percentages of correct convergent intra-
on some trials after intraverbal categorization verbals following MT on her second set (Set A,
(17%) and after LCD (17% and 33%). Kelly 50%) than on her first set (Set B, 8%), and this
emitted zero DORs across all pretraining and difference increased following ML (Set A, 67%;
posttraining sessions. William emitted increas- Set B, 33%) and increased further following
ing percentages of DORs following each prereq- intraverbal categorization (Set A, 100%; Set B,
uisite skill (MT, 50%; ML, 83%; intraverbal 50%). It should be noted that Kelly showed
categorization, 100%; LCD, 100%). mastery performance with her second set fol-
lowing intraverbal categorization and did not
require LCD for her second set (Set A). Finally,
Within-Participant Analyses Kelly showed zero DORs throughout pretrain-
Figures 2 and 3 show the percentages of cor- ing and posttraining with her second set (Set
rect convergent intraverbals during intraverbal A), just as she did with her first set (Set B).
probes, for both Sets A and B, for Thomas, William also showed zero correct convergent
Kelly, and William. Intraverbal probe data for intraverbals during all pretraining probes for
Set B are not reported for James because his both Set A and Set B (top two panels,
family moved to another part of the country Figure 3). Unlike Thomas and Kelly, William
before we started training with his second set. showed higher levels of correct convergent
For each participant, the top panel shows the intraverbals with his second set (Set B, 42%)
data from their first training set (duplicating the relative to his first set (Set A, 8%) following
data shown in Figure 1 for that participant) and only MT. Following both ML and intraverbal
the bottom panel shows the data from Set B. categorization, correct convergent intraverbals
As can be seen in the top two panels of remained low (8% after both of these condi-
Figure 2, Thomas showed zero correct conver- tions). Similar to Thomas, William showed
gent intraverbals during all pretraining probes mastery performance with both his first and
for both Set A and Set B. In addition, he second set only after LCD. Finally, William
showed slightly higher percentages of correct showed lower levels of DORs with his second
convergent intraverbals following MT on his set (Set B: MT, 25%; ML, 17%; intraverbal
second set (Set A, 25%) than on his first set categorization, 0%; LCD, 58% and 75%) rela-
(Set B, 8%); this pattern continued following tive to his first set (Set A: MT, 50%; ML,
ML (Set A, 50%; Set B, 0%) and following 83%; intraverbal categorization, 100%; LCD,
intraverbal categorization (Set A, 50%; Set B, 100% and 100%).
17%). However, as with his first training set
(Set B), Thomas showed mastery performance
with his second set (Set A) only after LCD. Prerequisite-Skills Acquisition
Finally, Thomas showed increased DORs fol- Figure 4 shows training trials to criteria for
lowing LCD with his second set (Set A, 50% all prerequisite skills for all participants. For all
and 83%) relative to his first set (Set B, 17% training sets across all participants, MT
and 33%), but he otherwise showed low levels required the most trials to reach mastery perfor-
of DORs in all prior probe sessions for mance. Among the participants, Kelly required
both sets. the lowest number of trials to criterion for MT
EMERGENCE OF CONVERGENT INTRAVERBALS 13

Figure 2. Percentage of independent correct convergent intraverbals and differential observing responses (DORs)
during probe sessions across sets for Thomas and Kelly. Arrows indicate when each prerequisite skill was implemented.
MT = multiple-tact; ML = multiple-listener; IV Categ = intraverbal categorization; LCD = listener compound
discrimination.

during training of the second set (i.e., Set A) Of all the participants, three performed to
with 120 trials. Thomas demonstrated the criteria during baseline sessions of prerequisite
highest number of trials to criterion for MT skills after MT for at least one prerequisite skill
during training of the second set (i.e., Set A) (Figure 4, prerequisite skills marked with an
with 360 trials. asterisk). When participants responded at
14 ANDRESA A. DESOUZA et al.

Figure 3. Percentage of independent correct convergent intraverbals and differential observing responses (DORs)
during probe sessions across sets for William. Arrows indicate when each prerequisite skill was implemented. MT =
multiple-tact; ML = multiple-listener; IV Cat = intraverbal categorization; LCD = listener compound discrimination.

mastery levels during baseline for a prerequisite baseline or after direct training with a PD or
skill, no training was implemented for that spe- PD-plus-remedial trial. Readers interested in
cific prerequisite skill, and therefore, no data examining the data from all training sessions
are depicted for that skill. James displayed mas- for all participants can view those figures in the
tery performance during baseline for Supporting Information section of the website
ML. Thomas showed correct responses at levels associated with this article.
below mastery performance for all baseline ses-
sions across all prerequisite skills. Kelly showed
mastery performance during baseline for ML DISCUSSION
and intraverbal categorization of Sets B and A, Four children with autism, aged 4 to
and LCD of Set B. William displayed mastery 5, showed the emergence of convergent intra-
performance for ML and LCD of Sets A verbals after receiving training on a sequence of
and B, and intraverbal categorization of Set prerequisite skills that Sundberg and Sundberg
B. Although some participants showed mastery (2011) suggested would facilitate the acquisi-
performance of the prerequisite skills during tion of intraverbals under multiple control. The
baseline sessions, we observed the emergence of outcomes of the current study suggest that,
convergent intraverbals at mastery level only whereas training the individual component
after LCD (except for Kelly during her second skills (e.g., MT, ML) resulted in mastery per-
set [Set A], who performed to criterion during formance of those skills, correct convergent
convergent intraverbals after baseline sessions of intraverbals first emerged (without direct train-
intraverbal categorization on that set). ing) for each participant only after they mas-
All participants learned to emit the prerequi- tered the last component skill, LCD. In
site skills at mastery performance either during addition, we replicated this finding across sets
EMERGENCE OF CONVERGENT INTRAVERBALS 15

Figure 4. Number of training trials to criterion for all prerequisite skills for all participants. The asterisks denote the
prerequisite skill in which participants responded to criterion during baseline sessions and no training was implemented.
MT = multiple-tact; ML = multiple-listener; IV Categ = intraverbal categorization; LCD = listener compound
discrimination.
16 ANDRESA A. DESOUZA et al.

of stimuli with two of the three participants to that children with typical development can
whom we exposed the training sequence a sec- show the emergence of complex intraverbal
ond time with a new set of target stimuli. responses without direct training (Perez-Gonza-
Thus, in six of seven applications of the train- lez, Herszlikowicz, & Williams, 2008). How-
ing sequence hypothesized to facilitate conver- ever, to our knowledge, only a few studies have
gent intraverbal responding, convergent demonstrated that young children with autism
intraverbals emerged at mastery levels without can show the emergence of multiply controlled
direct training after mastery of the final prereq- intraverbal responses without direct training
uisite skill, LCD. In the seventh application (e.g., Frampton & Shillingsburg, 2018). The
(Kelly, Set A), convergent intraverbals emerged outcomes of the current study represent a
at mastery levels after the participant learned potentially important finding because the emer-
the first three of the four hypothesized prereq- gence of novel, complex intraverbal responses is
uisite skills. Taken together, these results sug- critical to the development of conversational
gest that the skills trained in this study can speech, a skill area that is often markedly
facilitate the emergence of novel, complex impaired in children on the autism spectrum
intraverbal behavior in young children with (Goldsmith, LeBlanc, & Sautter, 2007).
autism. Second, results of the current investigation
The emergence of intraverbals, in particular, provide at least some support for the hypothesis
the emergence of complex intraverbals, has not that the development of novel intraverbal
received as much attention from the scientific behavior is dependent upon the acquisition of
community as the emergence of other verbal certain listener and speaker responses (Poon &
operants (e.g., mands and tacts; DeSouza, Butler, 1972; Sundberg & Sundberg, 2011).
Akers, & Fisher, 2017). The results of the cur- That is, we trained four prerequisite skills
rent study extend the literature on the emer- involving listener and speaker responses
gence of multiply controlled intraverbal hypothesized to facilitate the development and
behavior in several ways. First, prior studies emergence of convergent intraverbal responding
have demonstrated that individuals with typical (Sundberg & Sundberg, 2011), and each par-
and atypical development can acquire complex ticipant first showed the emergence of the tar-
intraverbal responses under the control of mul- get intraverbals at criterion level after training
tiple conditional and discriminative stimuli via of these four hypothesized prerequisite skills.
direct training using transfer-of-stimulus- Prior research has provided correlation data
control procedures or prompted DORs supporting the notion of a general sequence in
(e.g., Braam & Poling, 1983; Kisamore et al., the development of intraverbal behavior
2013; Kisamore et al., 2016). In addition, pre- (Sundberg & Sundberg, 2011). The current
vious research has shown that training proce- findings add to those data by providing experi-
dures can be arranged in ways to promote the mental results demonstrating that training of
emergence of simple intraverbal responses in these prerequisite skills resulted in the emer-
individuals with typical and atypical develop- gence of novel intraverbals in children who
ment (e.g., DeSouza & Rehfeldt, 2013; Gran- reportedly had not previously emitted intraver-
nan & Rehfeldt, 2012; Miguel et al., 2005; bal behavior at this level of complexity.
Petursdottir & Haflidadottir, 2009), provided It should be noted that one participant,
certain conditions are met (e.g., provided emer- Kelly, showed the emergence of the second set
gent responses share the response form with a of convergent intraverbal targets after training
different previously taught operant; Dounavi, on just the first three hypothesized prerequisites
2014). Finally, prior investigations have shown (MT, ML, intraverbal categorization). This
EMERGENCE OF CONVERGENT INTRAVERBALS 17

finding should not be surprising, as one might prior investigations with a variety of target
expect broader and more rapid generalization responses (e.g., Grow, Kodak, & Carr, 2014;
from the prerequisites to the target convergent Kobari-Wright & Miguel, 2014; Reichow &
intraverbals following repeated exposure to the Wolery, 2011). The current results also suggest
training sequence with multiple exemplars, as that these training methods may be useful for
has been demonstrated with other conditional promoting emergent responses in addition to
discriminations (e.g., Saunders & Spradlin, the target responses exposed directly to these
1990) and emergent relations (e.g., Rose, training procedures.
Souza, & Hanna, 1996). Nevertheless, it is In MT, we trained participants to tact the
possible that other training procedures, ones target stimuli (e.g., zebra) as well as two charac-
not following the hypothesized sequence, teristics of the target (e.g., mammal [Class 1]
would have similarly facilitated the emergence and from the savanna [Class 2]). During ML,
of novel, convergent intraverbals in these chil- we taught participants to select specific stimuli
dren. That is, the current results show that when we presented the name of the target and
training the hypothesized prerequisite skills their classes in separate trials (e.g., “Point to
proved sufficient for inducing novel and com- the zebra,” “Point to all mammals,” and “Point
plex emergent stimulus relations in young chil- to all from the savanna”). For intraverbal cate-
dren with autism, but the results do not gorization, we taught participants to emit two
demonstrate that training using this specific intraverbal responses corresponding to Class-1
sequence is necessary nor a more efficient and trials (e.g., “Tell me some mammals,” with the
effective way to promote the emergence of such correct response being “zebra” and “gorilla” in
relations. Future research should examine no specific order) and Class-2 trials (e.g., “Tell
whether the direct training of other prerequisite me some things from the savanna,” with the
skills or the training of the current prerequisite correct response being “zebra” and “emu”).
skills in a different sequence would produce Finally, during LCD, participants responded as
equivalent or better effects relative to the emer- a listener in a conditional discrimination task
gence of convergent intraverbals. involving compound stimuli (e.g., “Point to the
Third, the training procedures we used to mammal from the savanna”).
train each prerequisite skill proved effective in We trained the MT response first. This pre-
teaching these prerequisite skills to all four par- requisite skill required the most trials to pro-
ticipants with little modification (i.e., James duce mastery performance for every participant
and Thomas required the addition of a with every target set. Nevertheless, mastery of
remedial-trial procedure). The training proce- this skill alone produced minimal effects on the
dures began with the continuous delivery of the levels of correct intraverbals of the first training
vocal discriminative stimulus (e.g., “Name it”; set for all participants (Figure 1). However, we
“Point to zebra”) and a controlling prompt observed higher levels of correct intraverbals
(i.e., an echoic prompt for vocal responses, a following MT training with the second target
point prompt for selection responses, and a pic- set relative to the first set for all three partici-
torial prompt for intraverbal categorization pants trained on a second set (i.e., Kelly,
responses). In subsequent sessions, we inserted Thomas, and William). Moreover, once they
a progressively increasing delay between the mastered the MT targets, three of the partici-
vocal discriminative stimulus and the control- pants (James, Kelly, & William) mastered ML
ling prompt (and we added the remedial-trial in baseline without direct training, suggesting
procedure for James and Thomas). Variations that tact training facilitated the emergence of
of these procedures have proven effective in corresponding listener skills, consistent with a
18 ANDRESA A. DESOUZA et al.

number of prior studies (e.g., Delfs, Conine, “gorilla” go together as “mammals”; cf. Miguel
Frampton, Shillingsburg, & Robinson, 2014; et al., 2008). Finally, LCD may have facilitated
Gilic & Greer, 2011; Miguel, Petursdottir, conditional responding based on conjoint
Carr, & Michael, 2008). membership of a single stimulus in two stimu-
After mastering the MT skills, Kelly showed lus classes (i.e., convergent stimulus control of
mastery performance on each subsequent pre- listener responding). That is, responses under
requisite skill during baseline, but she did not convergent control may be easier to learn first
show mastery performance on the convergent in a listener format (relative to an intraverbal
intraverbal probes until after exposure to each of format), because with a listener format the par-
the prerequisite skills with the first target set or ticipant has access to some of the controlling
until after exposure to each of the first three pre- stimuli (i.e., the pictures) until the participant
requisite skills for the second target set. William emits the convergent response (e.g., all pictures
showed the same response pattern except that remained present in front of the participants
he required some training to master the intra- until they selected the picture of a mammal
verbal categorization skills with the first set of from the savanna).
targets. The fact that these two participants Our speculation is partially supported by
mastered all (Kelly) or almost all (William) of results from prior investigations. Several studies
the prerequisite skills in baseline, but they did have addressed the effects of simple tact training
not master the convergent intraverbals until on the emergence of other operants, such as
after exposure to all (or all but one) of the pre- mands (e.g., Finn, Miguel, & Ahearn, 2012;
requisite skills, suggests that mere exposure to Wallace, Iwata, & Hanley, 2006), intraverbals
the prerequisite skill targets may have facilitated (e.g., Dounavi, 2014; Petursdottir, Olafsdot-
the emergence of convergent intraverbals for tir, & Aradottir, 2008), and listener responding
these two participants. Conversely, it could be (e.g., Greer, Stolfi, Chavez-Brown, & Rivera-
that one or more of the prerequisite skills taught Valdes, 2005; Miguel & Kobari-Wright, 2013).
later in the sequence (i.e., LCD) were crucial for Studies involving tact and listener responding
the emergence of convergent intraverbals. For suggest that conditions can be created in which
example, it is possible that participants would the acquisition of one response will promote
have displayed the emergence of convergent the emergence of the other response (Fiorile &
intraverbals after learning a subset of the prereq- Greer, 2007; Greer, Yaun, & Gautreaux, 2005;
uisite skills (e.g., after learning to name the Nuzzolo-Gomez & Greer, 2004). In other
stimuli via tact training and then to condition- words, by teaching the tact of a specific target,
ally select the stimuli based on class membership the participant would demonstrate the emer-
during LCD). Future research should investi- gence of the listener response toward that target
gate the effects of simpler training sequences on without direct training, and vice versa. The per-
the emergence of convergent intraverbals. formance of our participants during ML follow-
We speculate that following MT, ML may ing MT is consistent with studies that
have facilitated naming (Horne & Lowe, 1997) demonstrated the emergence of listener
or bidirectional naming (Miguel, 2016) of the responding following tact training.
target stimuli (i.e., learning to respond in an The effects of MT on the emergence of
equivalent manner to corresponding words and untrained intraverbal categorization also have
pictures). In addition, intraverbal categorization been evaluated by a few studies (Miguel et al.,
may have facilitated functional, stimulus– 2005; Partington & Bailey, 1993). The results
stimulus relations between the topographically of both Miguel et al. (2005) and Partington
defined classes (e.g., learning that “zebra” and and Bailey (1993), demonstrated that
EMERGENCE OF CONVERGENT INTRAVERBALS 19

participants were able to respond with higher The fact that three of the four participants in
levels of correct responding during intraverbal the current study displayed DORs without
probe sessions relative to baseline sessions. being prompted to do so is at least somewhat
However, with all participants, MT alone did consistent with the naming account of emer-
not result in mastery performance, which gent stimulus relations. In addition, rates of the
required the subsequent addition of a transfer- DOR appeared to be somewhat correlated with
of-stimulus-control procedure. The current corrected convergent intraverbal responding
study partially replicated these prior results (see within those participants that displayed DORs
Figure 4), in that three of the participants (James, Thomas, and William). That is, these
(James, Thomas, and William [first training three participants showed near-zero or zero
set]) had low levels of correct responses during levels of the DOR during pretraining and rates
the intraverbal categorization baseline (see Sup- of the DOR generally increased with or follow-
porting Information 1, 2, 3, 6, 7) and required ing training. However, rates of the DOR across
direct training to master this prerequisite skill. participants showed little to no correlation with
Fourth, because we collected data on the correct convergent intraverbals, as evidenced by
participant’s use of DORs, but we did not spe- the fact that all participants showed mastery
cifically prompt participants to emit a DOR on performance of the convergent intraverbals
each trial, the results provide some data rele- despite the fact that Kelly showed zero rates,
vant to the role of naming or the naming rela- Thomas showed low rates, and James and Wil-
tion in the development of emergent stimulus liam showed high rates of the DOR. Taken
relations (Horne & Lowe, 1997). In the together, these results do not support the sup-
applied literature, researchers have prompted or position that naming is necessary for the emer-
required DORs to overcome stimulus overse- gence of convergent intraverbals, but it is quite
lectivity in matching-to-sample tasks (Dube & possible that naming facilitated emergent
McIlvane, 1999; Fisher, Kodak, & Moore, responding for James and William. However, it
2007; Walpole, Roscoe, & Dube, 2007), and is possible that participants emitted DORs
to directly train multiply controlled intraverbals silently, as private verbal responses, during trials
(Kisamore et al., 2016). When DORs are spon- when we did not observe overt DORs. Alterna-
taneously emitted, they may facilitate the devel- tive experimental manipulations would be war-
opment of naming or the naming relation ranted to examine this hypothesis and
(Horne & Lowe, 1997). demonstrate the effects of DOR in the emer-
In the basic and theoretical literatures on gence of convergent intraverbal responses.
stimulus class formation, naming is hypothe- One limitation of the current investigation
sized to be a higher-order response involving was that we trained the prerequisite skills in a
bidirectional stimulus relations between objects given sequence with each participant (MT, fol-
or events and the speaker and listener respond- lowed by ML, then intraverbal categorization,
ing they engender (Horne & Lowe, 1997). For and finally LCD). We chose this sequence
example, when a verbally competent individual based on the theoretical framework provided by
sees a picture (e.g., of a zebra), it typically Sundberg and Sundberg (2011), which pro-
evokes one or more tacts pertaining to relevant vided a conceptually sound rationale for why
stimulus classes (e.g., “zebra”, “mammal”), and the sequence of prerequisite skills would facili-
each tact, in turn, may evoke relevant listener tate the emergence of convergent verbal
responding, such as identifying other pictures responding. However, this limitation leaves
from a common stimulus class (looking at the open the possibility that had we conducted
gorilla because it also is a mammal). training only with a simple tact of the target
20 ANDRESA A. DESOUZA et al.

(e.g., “Emu”) and LCD for an amount of time series of skills that, when implemented to crite-
approximately equal to that spent on all four rion, can facilitate the emergence of intraverbals
prerequisite skills, it is possible that convergent under multiple control in children with autism.
intraverbal responding may have similarly
emerged at mastery level. Because of this limi-
tation, we are unable to conclude that it is nec- REFERENCES
essary to train each of the prescribed Axe, J. B. (2008). Conditional discrimination in the intra-
prerequisite skills and to do so in the prescribed verbal relation: A review and recommendations for
future research. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 24,
sequence to demonstrate the emergence of con- 159-174. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf03393064
vergent intraverbals. Braam, S. J., & Poling, A. (1983). Development of intra-
Another limitation is that all participants verbal behavior in mentally retarded individuals
correctly tacted at least one of the targets of all through transfer of stimulus control procedures: Clas-
sification of verbal responses. Applied Research in
sets during baseline sessions. As such, the par- Mental Retardation, 4, 279-302. https://doi.org/10.
ticipants’ history of reinforcement with these 1016/0270-3092(83)90030-9
“known” targets may have influenced their per- Delfs, C. H., Conine, D. E., Frampton, S. E.,
Shillingsburg, M.A., & Robinson, H. C. (2014).
formances of some prerequisite skill (e.g., ML) Evaluation of the efficiency of listener and tact
and facilitated subsequent performances. How- instruction for children with autism. Journal of
ever, the fact that participants had some of Applied Behavior Analysis, 47, 793-809. https://doi.
org/10.1002/jaba.166
these targets in their repertoire did not influ-
DeSouza, A., Akers, J., & Fisher, W. W. (2017). Empiri-
ence their performance during pretraining cal application of Skinner’s verbal behavior to inter-
intraverbal probes as demonstrated by zero ventions for children with autism: A review. The
levels of correct intraverbals during those ses- Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 33, 212-228. https://doi.
org/10.1007/s40616-017-0093-7
sions (Figure 1). Future researchers studying DeSouza, A., & Rehfeldt, R. A. (2013). Effects of
convergent intraverbals should select unknown dictation-taking and match-to-sample training on list-
targets or use abstract stimuli to control for the ing and spelling responses in adults with intellectual
disabilities. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 46,
effect of the history of reinforcement. 792-404. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.75
Finally, we did not conduct baseline sessions Dounavi, K. (2014). Tact training versus bidirectional
of ML, intraverbal categorization, and LCD intraverbal training in teaching a foreign language.
before MT. We omitted these baselines to min- Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 47, 165-170.
https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.86
imize participants’ exposure to the target stim- Dube, W. V., & McIlvane, W. J. (1999). Reduction of
uli. However, without these baselines, we stimulus overselectivity with nonverbal differential
cannot completely rule out the possibility that observing responses. Journal of Applied Behavior
Analysis, 32, 25-33. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.
the participants mastered at least some of the 1999.32-25
prerequisite skills prior to the study; therefore, Dunn, M., & Dunn, L. M. (2007). Peabody picture vocab-
conclusions regarding the emergence of ulary test (4th ed.). Circle Pines, MN: AGS.
untrained responses observed during baseline of Esch, B. E. (2008). Early echoic skills assessment. In
M. L. Sundberg (Ed.), Verbal behavior milestones
prerequisite skills should be made with caution. assessment and placement program: The VB-MAPP
Future research should conduct baseline ses- (p. 24). Concord, CA: AVB Press.
sions of all prerequisite skills before MT. Finn, H. E., Miguel, C. F., & Ahearn, W. H. (2012).
The emergence of untrained mands and tacts in chil-
The results of the current study demonstrate dren with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior
the emergence of convergent intraverbals in Analysis, 45, 265-280. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.
four children with a diagnosis of autism. The 2012.45-265
current study adds to the literature on Fiorile, C. A., & Greer, R. D. (2007). The induction of
naming in children with no prior tact responses as a
behavior-analytic facilitation of generative and function of multiple exemplar histories of instruction.
emergent responses. In particular, it provides a The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 23, 71-87.
EMERGENCE OF CONVERGENT INTRAVERBALS 21

Fisher, W. W., Kodak, T., & Moore, J. W. (2007). Kisamore, A. N., Karsten, A. M., Mann, C. C., &
Embedding an identity-matching task within a Conde, K. A. (2013). Effects of a differential observ-
prompting hierarchy to facilitate acquisition of condi- ing response on intraverbal performance of preschool
tional discriminations in children with autism. Jour- children: A preliminary investigation. The Analysis of
nal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40, 489-499. https:// Verbal Behavior, 29, 101-108. https://doi.org/10.
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2007.40-489 1007/bf03393127
Frampton, S. E., & Shillingsburg, M. A. (2018). Teach- Kobari-Wright, V. V., & Miguel, C. F. (2014). The
ing children with autism to explain how: A case for effects of listener training on the emergence of cate-
problem solving? Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, gorization and speaker behavior in children with
51, 236-254. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.445 autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 47, 431-
Gilic, L., & Greer, R. D. (2011). Establishing naming in 436. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.115
typically developing two-year-old children as a func- LeBlanc, L. A., Esch, J., Sidener, T. M., & Firth, A. M.
tion of multiple exemplar speaker and listener experi- (2006). Behavioral language interventions for chil-
ences. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 27, 157-177. dren with ASD: Comparing applied verbal behavior
https://doi.org/10.1007/bf03393099 and naturalistic training approaches. The Analysis of
Goldsmith, T. R., LeBlanc, L. A., & Sautter, R. A. Verbal Behavior, 22, 49-60. https://doi.org/10.1007/
(2007). Teaching intraverbal behavior to children bf03393026
with autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, Lovaas, O. I., Koegel, R. L., & Schreibman, L. (1979).
1, 1-13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2006.07.001 Stimulus overselectivity in autism: A review of
Grannan, L., & Rehfeldt, R. A. (2012). Emergent intra- research. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 1236-1254.
verbal responses via tact and match-to-sample instruc- https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.86.6.1236
tion. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 45, 601- Lovaas O. I., Schreibman, L., Koegel, R., & Rehm, R.
605. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2012.45-601 (1971). Selective responding by autistic children to
multiple sensory input. Journal of Abnormal
Greer, R. D., Stolfi, L., Chavez-Brown, M., & Rivera-
Psychology, 77, 211-222. https://doi.org/10.1037/
Valdes, C. (2005). The emergence of the listener to
h0031015
speaker component of naming in children as a func-
Michael, J., Palmer, D. C., & Sundberg, M. L. (2011).
tion of multiple exemplar instruction. The Analysis of
The multiple control of verbal behavior. The Analysis
Verbal Behavior, 21, 123-134. https://doi.org/10.
of Verbal Behavior, 27, 3-22. https://doi.org/10.1007/
1007/bf03393014
bf03393089
Greer, R. D., Yaun, L., & Gautreaux, G. (2005). Novel
Miguel, C. F. (2016). Common and intraverbal bidirec-
dictation and intraverbal responses as a function of a
tional naming. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 32,
multiple exemplar instructional history. The Analysis
125-138. https://doi.org/10.1007/
of Verbal Behavior, 21, 99-116. https://doi.org/10.
s40616-016-0066-2
1007/bf03393012
Miguel, C. F., & Kobari-Wright, V. V. (2013). The
Grow, L. L., Kodak. T., & Carr, J. E. (2014). A compari- effects of tact training on the emergence of categori-
son of methods for teaching receptive labeling to chil- zation and listener behavior in children with autism.
dren with autism spectrum disorders: A systematic Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 46, 669-673.
replication. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 47, https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.62
600-605. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.141 Miguel, C. F., Petursdottir, A. I., & Carr, J. E. (2005).
Horne P. J., & Lowe, C. F. (1997). Toward a theory of The effects of multiple-tact and receptive-
verbal behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of discrimination training on the acquisition of intraver-
Behavior, 68, 271-296. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab. bal behavior. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 21, 27-
1997.68-271 41. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf03393008
Horner, R. D., & Baer, D. M. (1978). Multiple-probe Miguel, C. F., Petursdottir, A. I., Carr, J. E., &
technique: A variation of the multiple baseline. Jour- Michael, J. (2008). The role of naming in stimulus
nal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 11, 189-196. https:// categorization by preschool children. Journal of the
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1978.11-189 Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 89, 283-405.
Kisamore, A. N., Carr, J. E., & LeBlanc, L. A. (2011). https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2008-89-383
Training preschool children to use visual imagining Nuzzolo-Gomez, R., & Greer, D. (2004). Emergence of
as a problem-solving strategy for complex categoriza- untaught mands or tacts of novel adjective–object
tion tasks. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 44, pairs as a function of instructional history. The Analy-
255-278. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2011.44-255 sis of Verbal Behavior, 20, 63-76. https://doi.org/10.
Kisamore, A. N., Karsten, A. M., & Mann, C. C. (2016). 1007/bf03392995
Teaching multiply controlled intraverbals to children Partington, J. W., & Bailey, J. S. (1993). Teaching intra-
and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Jour- verbal behavior to preschool children. The Analysis of
nal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 49, 826-847. https:// Verbal Behavior, 11, 9-18. https://doi.org/10.1007/
doi.org/10.1002/jaba.344 bf03392883
22 ANDRESA A. DESOUZA et al.

Perez-Gonzales, L. A., Herszlikowicz, K., & Williams, G. Analysis of Behavior, 54, 239-250. https://doi.org/10.
(2008). Stimulus relations analysis and the emergence 1901/jeab.1990.54-239
of novel intraverbals. The Psychological Record, 58, Sautter, R. A., LeBlanc, L. A., Jay, A. A.,
95-129. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395605 Goldsmith, T. R., & Carr, J. E. (2011). The role of
Petursdottir, A. I., & Carr, J. E. (2011). A review of rec- problem solving in complex intraverbal repertoires.
ommendation for sequencing receptive and expressive Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 44, 227-244.
language instruction. Journal of Applied Behavior https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2011.44-227
Analysis, 44, 859-876. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba. Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Cambridge, MA:
2011.44-859 Prentice Hall.
Petursdottir, A. I., & Haflidadottir, R. D. (2009). A com-
parison of four strategies for teaching a small foreign- Sundberg, M. L., & Sundberg, C. A. (2011). Intraverbal
behavior and verbal conditional discrimination in
language vocabulary. Journal of Applied Behavior
Analysis, 42, 685-690. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba. typically developing children and children with
2009.42-685 autism. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 27, 23-43.
https://doi.org/10.1007/bf03393090
Petursdottir, A. I., Olafsdottir, A. R., & Aradottir, B.
(2008). The effects of tact training and listener train- Wallace, M. D., Iwata, B. A., & Hanley, G. P. (2006).
ing on the emergence of bidirectional intraverbal rela- Establishment of mands following tact training as a
tions. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41, 411- function of reinforcer strength. Journal of Applied
415. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2008.41-411 Behavior Analysis, 39, 25-34. https://doi.org/10.1901/
Poon, W., & Butler, K. G. (1972). Evaluation of intraver- jaba.2006.119-04
bal responses in five-to-seven year-old children. Jour- Walpole, C. W., Roscoe, E. M., & Dube, W. V. (2007).
nal of Speech and Hearing Research, 15, 303-307. Use of a differential observing response to expand
https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.1502.303 restricted stimulus control. Journal of Applied Behav-
Reichow B., & Wolery, M. (2011). Comparison of pro- ior Analysis, 40, 707-712. https://doi.org/10.1901/
gressive prompt delay with and without instructive jaba.2007.707-712
feedback. Journal of Autism and Developmental Williams, K. (2007). Expressive vocabulary test (2nd ed.).
Disorders, 44, 327-340. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba. Minneapolis, MN: NCS Pearson.
2011.44-327
Roane, H. S., Fisher, W. W., & Carr, J. E. (2016). Received December 9, 2016
Applied behavior analysis as treatment for autism Final acceptance August 29, 2018
spectrum disorder. Journal of Pediatrics, 175, 27-32. Action Editor, James Carr
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2016.04.023
Rose, J. C., Souza, D. G., & Hanna, E. S. (1996). Teach-
ing reading and spelling: Exclusion and stimulus
equivalence. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29, SUPPORTING INFORMATION
451-469. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1996.29-451 Additional Supporting Information may be
Saunders, K. J., & Spradlin. J. E. (1990). Conditional dis-
crimination in mentally retarded adults: The develop- found in the online version of this article at the
ment of generalized skills. Journal of the Experimental publisher’s website.

You might also like